Holding the Hunger Games Hostage at The
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
MANAGEMENT SCIENCE Articles in Advance, pp. 1–17 ISSN 0025-1909 (print) ISSN 1526-5501 (online) http: / / dx.doi.org / 10.1287 / mnsc.2013.1784 ! © 2013 I N F ORMS H olding the H unger Games H ostage at the G ym: A n Evaluation of Temptation Bundling Katherine L. Milkman The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104, [email protected] Julia A. Minson The H arvard Kennedy School, H arvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, [email protected] Kevin G. M. Volpp The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104, [email protected] e introduce and evaluate the effectiveness of temptation bundling—a method for simultaneously tack- W ling two types of self-control problems by harnessing consumption complementarities. We describe a field experiment measuring the impact of bundling instantly gratifying but guilt-inducing “ want” experiences (enjoying page-turner audiobooks) with valuable “should” behaviors providing delayed rewards (exercising). We explore whether such bundles increase should behaviors and whether people would pay to create these restrictive bundles. Participants were randomly assigned to a full treatment condition with gym-only access to tempting audio novels, an intermediate treatment involving encouragement to restrict audiobook enjoyment to the gym, or a control condition. Initially, full and intermediate treatment participants visited the gym 51% and 29% more frequently, respectively, than control participants, but treatment effects declined over time (particu- larly following Thanksgiving). A fter the study, 61% of participants opted to pay to have gym-only access to iPods containing tempting audiobooks, suggesting demand for this commitment device. Key words: commitment devices; temptation bundling; self-control; field experiment; exercise History: Received N ovember 26, 2012; accepted May 21, 2013, Uri Gneezy, behavioral economics. Published online in Articles in Advance. 1. Introduction both economics and psychology into account when With 68% of adult A mericans overweight or obese as seeking to increase exercise rates. of 2008 (Flegal et al. 2010) and 112,000 deaths in the If low exercise rates are in part the result of self- United States per year attributable to obesity (Flegal control problems, as much past research suggests (see, et al. 2007), promoting weight loss is an urgent pub- e.g., Della Vigna and Malmendier 2006), interventions lic health priority. Further, in light of skyrocketing that use psychological tools and insights to tackle healthcare costs caused in part by obesity, programs this obstacle may be particularly valuable and cost designed to encourage weight loss are of tremen- effective. Limited willpower has been shown to play dous interest to most organizations (Finkelstein et al. an important role in decisions made by individuals 2005, 2010). Despite the many benefits that exercise that affect weight gain, such as healthy eating and provides, including promoting weight loss (A ndersen exercise: people intend to exercise and diet tomor- 2010), only 50% of A mericans exercise sufficiently, a row but frequently lack the necessary willpower to percentage that has been steadily declining (Centers act on those good intentions today (Della Vigna and for Disease Control and Prevention 2007). Malmendier 2006, Milkman et al. 2009, Read and van Recent research has highlighted the possibility that Leeu wen 1998, Royer et al. 2012). On the other hand, public policy interventions built on an understanding limited willpower makes it difficult for individuals to of the psychology surrounding the challenges associ- ated with increasing physical activity may be particu- resist engaging in many highly tempting behaviors larly effective. For example, groundbreaking research involving indulgences that induce regret after the fact conducted in the last several years has shown not (for a review, see Milkman et al. 2008). only that incentivizing exercise is an effective way We propose that valuable, healthy behaviors could to increase physical activity but also that incen- be increased while guilt and wasted time from indul- tivizing repeated gym attendance can produce long- gent behaviors are simultaneously decreased through lasting exercise habits that remain after incentives are the use of a previously unstudied intervention: “temp- removed (Charness and Gneezy 2009, Acland and tation bundling.” Temptation bundling involves the Levy 2013). This work underscores the value of taking coupling of instantly gratifying “ want” activities (e.g., 1 Milkman, Minson, and Volpp: An Evaluation of Temptation Bundling 2 Management Science, Articles in Advance, pp. 1–17, © 2013 INFORMS watching the next episode of a habit-forming tele- Figure 1 Theoretical Inverted Shapes of the Utility Streams Obtained vision show, checking Facebook, receiving a pedi- from Engaging in (A) Wants and (B) Shoulds, Which Are cure, eating an indulgent meal) with engagement in Strategically Combined by Temptation Bundling a “should” behavior that provides long-term benefits (A) but requires the exertion of willpower (e.g., exercising Enjoyment indulgence at the gym, completing a paper review, spending time with a difficult relative). For example, imagine only Time allowing yourself to enjoy the next episode of your Long-term costs (e.g., guilt, wasted time) favorite TV show while exercising, receive a pedi- (B) cure while completing an overdue manuscript review, Long-term benefits (e.g., pride, health) or indulge in the burger you crave when spending Time time with your cranky uncle. Temptation bundling can Utility Utility solve two problems at once by increasing the desire of Pain of execution those with self-control problems to engage in benefi- Note. By strategically combining these utility streams, temptation bundling cial behaviors requiring willpower and reducing the helps those who discount the future heavily both (1) engage in shoulds and likelihood that people will engage in indulgent activ- (2) limit their engagement in wants to moments when the downstream neg- ities that they will later regret. Temptation bundling ative consequences are minimized. may be particularly effective because it exploits com- plementarities that often exist between “wants” and First, our field experiment examines whether tempta- “shoulds” to create added value. The simultaneous tion bundling programs have the potential to induce engagement in wants and shoulds can reduce the guilt behavior change, setting aside the question of whether associated with indulgences and offer a distraction individuals would be “sophisticated” enough about from the unpleasantness of many beneficial activities. their self-control problems to voluntarily seek out such We theorize that people with limited willpower who programs (O’Donoghue and Rabin 1999). Answering are aware of their self-control problems (“sophisti- this first question allows us to establish whether the cates”; O’Donoghue and Rabin 1999) would gain from temptation bundling idea has value. We do this by and value an opportunity to force their future selves to measuring the effectiveness of temptation bundling engage in beneficial should activities while simultane- as a means of increasing exercise frequency. We also ously preventing those future selves from indulging in examine whether individuals are able to effectively pleasurable but ultimately guilt-inducing want activ- self-impose a suggested temptation bundling rule. ities. We test a previously unexplored method for Previous research on mental accounting and goal set- enforcing these preferences by creating a temptation ting indicates that without external referees, people bundling program that bundles a highly tempting often (though not always; see Burger et al. 2011) activity (listening to low-brow, page-turner audio nov- have the capacity to adhere to predetermined rules els) with an activity that requires exerting self-control designed to mitigate self-control problems (Abeler (exercising).1 By bundling access to a hedonic experi- and Marklein 2013; Camerer et al. 1997; Cheema and ence with exercise, exercise is made “tempting” and Soman 2008; Heath et al. 1999; Milkman and Beshears increasingly appealing, while the squandering of time 2009; Shefrin and Thaler 1988; Thaler 1985, 1990, 1999; and resources on a potentially regret-inducing indul- Thaler and Shefrin 1981). By including an intermedi- gent activity is prevented. In short, the inverted shapes ate intervention in our experiment to test the effective- of the utility streams obtained from engaging in want ness of suggested temptation bundling, we are able to and should behaviors are strategically combined by disentangle the effectiveness of merely giving people temptation bundling. This ensures that those who the insight regarding the potential value of this tech- discount the future heavily will engage in shoulds nique from that of creating a structured environment and will limit their engagement in wants to moments that limits their ability to behave myopically. when the downstream negative consequences (e.g., The second question we address is whether indi- guilt and wasted time) are minimized if not eliminated viduals are willing to restrict their own behavior to (see Figure 1). garner the benefits of temptation bundling. We inves- In the present investigation we focus on two ques- tigate this critical question after first establishing the tions pertaining to the value of “temptation bundling.” power