Focus Prosody Varies by Phrase-Initial Tones in Seoul Korean: Production, Perception, and Automatic Classification

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Focus Prosody Varies by Phrase-Initial Tones in Seoul Korean: Production, Perception, and Automatic Classification languages Article Focus Prosody Varies by Phrase-Initial Tones in Seoul Korean: Production, Perception, and Automatic Classification Yong-cheol Lee 1,* and Sunghye Cho 2,* 1 Department of English Language and Literature, Cheongju University, Cheongju 28503, Korea 2 Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] (Y.-c.L.); [email protected] (S.C.) Received: 22 September 2020; Accepted: 11 November 2020; Published: 18 November 2020 Abstract: Production and perception experiments were conducted to examine whether focus prosody varies by phrase-initial tones in Seoul Korean. We also trained an automatic classifier to locate prosodic focus within a sentence. Overall, focus prosody in Seoul Korean was weak and confusing in production, and poorly identified in perception. However, Seoul Korean’s focus prosody differed between phrase-initial low and high tones. The low tone group induced a smaller pitch increase by focus than the high tone group. The low tone group was also subject to a greater degree of confusion, although both tone groups showed some degree of confusion spanning the entire phrase as a focus effect. The identification rate was, therefore, approximately half in the low tone group (23.5%) compared to the high tone group (40%). In machine classification, the high tone group was also more accurately identified (high: 86% vs. low: 68%) when trained separately, and the machine’s general performance when the two tone groups were trained together was much superior to the human’s (machine: 65% vs. human: 32%). Although the focus prosody in Seoul Korean was weak and confusing, the identification rate of focus was higher under certain circumstances, which avers that focus prosody can vary within a single language. Keywords: focus prosody; Seoul Korean; tonal contrast; production; human perception; machine perception 1. Introduction Focus highlights a particular element in a sentence (Bolinger 1972; Xu and Xu 2005). It is normally modulated by prosodic prominence to emphasize its importance in communication. However, prosodic prominence marking focus varies according to each language’s prosodic system. For example, in English, prominence is marked by a post-lexical pitch accent on the head (i.e., a stressed syllable) of a word (Beckman and Pierrehumbert 1986; Cohan 2000; Jun 2011; Ladd 1996). Additionally, prosodic focus in American English is well identified by a machine classifier with a high accuracy (92%) (Cho et al. 2019). In Mandarin Chinese, it is also cued by the head of a word, but is distinctively characterized by the tone shape of individual lexical tones (Lee et al. 2016; Liu 2009; Yuan 2004). However, in Seoul Korean (SK), focus is expressed through prosodic phrasing in which a new phrase boundary is inserted before the focused word and prominence occurs throughout the entire focused word (Jun 2011; Lee 2012). Based on Jun’s (Jun 2005, 2014) prosodic typology, English and Mandarin Chinese belong to head-prominence languages. However, SK is labeled as an edge-prominence language because prominence is realized by marking the edge of a prosodic constituent. In order to understand the details of the current study, we first briefly overview SK’s prosodic system. We then provide a detailed description of how prosodic focus is manifested in SK. Finally, we present our research goals. Languages 2020, 5, 64; doi:10.3390/languages5040064 www.mdpi.com/journal/languages Languages 2020, 5, 64 2 of 19 Languages 2020, 5, x FOR PEER REVIEW 2 of 19 SK has neither lexical stress nor lexical pitch accents (Ueyama and Jun 1998; Song 2005). Instead, in defaultSK prosodichas neither phrasing, lexical eachstress content nor lexical word pitch usually accents forms (Ueyama a small prosodicand Jun 1998; unit called Song an2005). Accentual Instead, Phrasein default (AP) (prosodicJun 1998 )phrasing, that is tonally each markedcontent atword the post-lexical usually forms level. a Thesmall typical prosodic melody unit of called an AP an isAccentual either LH-LH Phrase or HH-LH,(AP) (Jun depending 1998) that onis tonally an AP-initial marked onset at the segment. post-lexical The level. AP-initial The typical tone is melody high whenof an the AP initial is either onset LH-LH has the or HH-LH, feature [ +dependingaspirated /ontense], an AP-initial while it isonset low segment. when the The initial AP-initial onset has tone theis featurehigh when [ aspirated the initial/tense]. onset Intermediate has the feature H-L [+aspi tonesrated/tense], (either one orwhile both) it is are low omitted when fromthe initial APs thatonset − containhas the less feature than [ four−aspirated/tense]. syllables, resulting Intermediate in several H-L tonal tones patterns: (either one L-H, or L-LH,both) are LH-H, omitted H-H, from H-LH, APs HH-H.that contain Regarding less the than AP four melody syllables, of SK, resulting recent studies in several (Cho andtonal Lee patterns: 2016; Cho L-H, 2017 L-LH,) showed LH-H, that H-H, AP’s H- pitchLH, targetHH-H. is Regarding much higher the when AP melody it begins of withSK, recent a high-tone studies inducing (Cho and segment Lee 2016; and Cho the high2017) pitch showed is retainedthat AP’s toward pitch thetarget final is much syllable higher of an when AP. The it be tonalgins with pattern a high-tone of an AP inducing with a high-inducing segment and the initial high consonantpitch is retained even shows toward a high the plateau, final syllable particularly of an whenAP. The the tonal number pattern of syllables of an AP within with ana high-inducing AP is two or three,initial as consonant demonstrated even in shows Figure a1 high. This plateau, seems to particul be whyarly the when AP with the anumber high-inducing of syllables initial within segment an AP doesis two not or have three, a clear as demonstrated HH-LH pattern in Figure in Figure 1. This1, unlike seems the to basicbe why melody the AP of with TH-LH a high-inducing in SK. Please initial see Chosegment and Lee does(2016 not) for have further a clear discussion HH-LH pattern of the results. in Figure 1, unlike the basic melody of TH-LH in SK. PleaseUSV see Symbol Cho and Macro(s) Lee (2016) for further discussion of the results. Description 0241 Ɂ \textglotstopvari LATIN CAPITAL LETTER GLOTTAL STOP 0242 ɂ \textglotstopvarii LATIN SMALL LETTER GLOTTAL STOP 0243 Ƀ \textbarcapitalb LATIN CAPITAL LETTER B WITH STROKE 0244 Ʉ \textbarcapitalu LATIN CAPITAL LETTER U BAR 0245 Ʌ \textturnedcapitalv LATIN CAPITAL LETTER TURNED V 0246 Ɇ \textstrokecapitale LATIN CAPITAL LETTER E WITH STROKE 0247 ɇ \textstrokee LATIN SMALL LETTER E WITH STROKE USV Symbol Macro(s) Description 0248 Ɉ \textbarcapitalj LATIN CAPITAL LETTER J WITH STROKE 0249 0266 ɉ ɦ \textbarj\m{h} LATIN SMALLLATIN LETTERSMALL LETTER J WITH STROKE H WITH HOOK 024A Ɋ \texthtcapitalq\texthookabove{h} LATIN CAPITAL LETTER SMALL Q WITH HOOK TAIL \texthth 024B ɋ \texthtq LATIN SMALL LETTER Q WITH HOOK TAIL 0267 ɧ \texthookabove{\textheng} LATIN SMALL LETTER HENG WITH HOOK 024C Ɍ \textbarcapitalr LATIN CAPITAL LETTER R WITH STROKE \texththeng 024D ɍ \textbarr LATIN SMALL LETTER R WITH STROKE 0268 ɨ \B{i} LATIN SMALL LETTER I WITH STROKE 024E Ɏ \textbarcapitaly\textbari LATIN CAPITAL LETTER Y WITH STROKE 024F 0269 ɏ ɩ \textbary\m{i} LATIN SMALLLATIN LETTERSMALL LETTER Y WITH IOTA STROKE 0250 ɐ \textturna\textiota LATIN SMALL LETTER TURNED A \textiotalatin 0251 ɑ \textscripta LATIN SMALL LETTER ALPHA \textniiota 0252 ɒ \textturnscripta LATIN SMALL LETTER TURNED ALPHA 026A ɪ \textsci LATIN LETTER SMALL CAPITAL I 0253 ɓ \m{b} LATIN SMALL LETTER B WITH HOOK 026B ɫ \textltilde LATIN SMALL LETTER L WITH MIDDLE TILDE \m{b} 026C ɬ \texthtb\textbeltl LATIN SMALL LETTER L WITH BELT 026D ɭ \textbhook\textrethookbelow{l} LATIN SMALL LETTER L WITH RETROFLEX HOOK 0254 ɔ \m{o}\textrtaill LATIN SMALL LETTER OPEN O Figure 1. Mean pitch values of all speakers by the length of the target words. The x-axis shows syllable Figure026E 1. Meanɮ \textopenopitch\textlyoghlig values of all speakers by the length of the target words.LATIN The SMALL x-axis LETTER shows LEZH syllable positionspositions in in the the target \textoopentarget\textOlyoghlig sentences, sentences, and and the the y-axis y-axis shows shows the the mean mean pitch pitch values. values. The The first first three three syllables syllables 0255 026F ɕ ɯ \textctc\textturnmtopic LATIN SMALLLATIN LETTERSMALL LETTER C WITH TURNED CURL M areare for for/i.t /i.te.nɕe.nnɯ/n/‘now- ‘now-TOPIC’,’, and and the the last last four four syllables syllables are are for fo/rmal.ha.se.jo /mal.ha.se.jo// ‘say’ ‘say’ in in the the carrier carrier 0256 0270 ɖ ɰ \M{d}\textturnmrleg LATIN SMALLLATIN LETTERSMALL LETTER D WITH TURNED TAIL M WITH LONG LEG sentencesentence/i.d /i.dʑe.nɯne.n n ________ mal.ha.se.jomal.ha.se.jo// ‘Now‘Now saysay _____.’, _____.’, and and the the syllables syllables in in the the middle middle (inside (inside the the 0271 ɱ \textrtaild\m{m} 1 LATIN SMALL LETTER M WITH HOOK dashed lines) are the target words. Modified1 from Cho and Lee(2016). dashed lines) are\textdtail the\textltailm target words. Modified from Cho and Lee (2016). 0257 ɗ \m{d} LATIN SMALL LETTER D WITH HOOK Figure02722 depictsɲ SK’s\m{j} focus realizations when a focused word startsLATIN with SMALL a LETTER low-tone N WITH LEFT inducing HOOK Figure 2 depicts\texthtd SK’s\textltailn focus realizations when a focused word starts with a low-tone inducing segment. Focus conditions\textdhook\textnhookleft were produced in an experimental setting in which six native speakers of segment.0258 Focusɘ conditions\textreve were produced in an experimental settingLATIN in SMALL which LETTER six REVERSED native E speakers of SK read stimuli0273 inɳ isolation\m{n} for broad focus and produced the same stimuliLATIN SMALL in a LETTER Q&A N WITH dialogue RETROFLEX HOOK for SK0259 read stimuliə in \schwaisolation\textrtailn for broad focus and produced the sameLATIN stimuli SMALL LETTER in SCHWA a Q&A dialogue for discourse-new focus.
Recommended publications
  • Prosodic Processes in Language and Music
    Prosodic Processes in Language and Music Maartje Schreuder Copyright © 2006 by Maartje Schreuder Cover design: Hanna van der Haar Printed by Print Partners Ipskamp, Enschede The work in this thesis has been carried out under the auspices of the Research School of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences (BCN), Groningen Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 60 ISSN 0928-0030 ISBN 90-367-2637-9 RIJKSUNIVERSITEIT GRONINGEN Prosodic Processes in Language and Music Proefschrift ter verkrijging van het doctoraat in de Letteren aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen op gezag van de Rector Magnificus, dr. F. Zwarts, in het openbaar te verdedigen op donderdag 15 juni 2006 om 13:15 uur door Maartje Johanneke Schreuder geboren op 25 augustus 1974 te Groningen Promotor: Prof. dr. J. Koster Copromotor: Dr. D.G. Gilbers Beoordelingscommissie: Prof. dr. J. Hoeksema Prof. dr. C. Gussenhoven Prof. dr. P. Hagoort Preface Many people have helped me to finish this thesis. First of all I am indebted to my supervisor Dicky Gilbers. Throughout this disseration, I speak of ‘we’. That is not because I have some double personality which allows me to do all the work in collaboration, but because Dicky was so enthusiastic about the project that we did all the experiments together. The main chapters are based on papers we wrote together for conference proceedings, books, and journals. This collaboration with Dicky always was very motivating and pleasant. I will never forget the conferences we visited together, especially the fun we had trying to find our way in Vienna, and through the subterranean corridors in the castle in Imatra, Finland.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Theoretical Implications of Cypriot Greek Initial Geminates
    <LINK "mul-n*">"mul-r16">"mul-r8">"mul-r19">"mul-r14">"mul-r27">"mul-r7">"mul-r6">"mul-r17">"mul-r2">"mul-r9">"mul-r24"> <TARGET "mul" DOCINFO AUTHOR "Jennifer S. Muller"TITLE "On the theoretical implications of Cypriot Greek initial geminates"SUBJECT "JGL, Volume 3"KEYWORDS "geminates, representation, phonology, Cypriot Greek"SIZE HEIGHT "220"WIDTH "150"VOFFSET "4"> On the theoretical implications of Cypriot Greek initial geminates* Jennifer S. Muller The Ohio State University Cypriot Greek contrasts singleton and geminate consonants in word-initial position. These segments are of particular interest to phonologists since two divergent representational frameworks, moraic theory (Hayes 1989) and timing-based frameworks, including CV or X-slot theory (Clements and Keyser 1983, Levin 1985), account for the behavior of initial geminates in substantially different ways. The investigation of geminates in Cypriot Greek allows these differences to be explored. As will be demonstrated in a formal analysis of the facts, the patterning of geminates in Cypriot is best accounted for by assuming that the segments are dominated by abstract timing units such as X- or C-slots, rather than by a unit of prosodic weight such as the mora. Keywords: geminates, representation, phonology, Cypriot Greek 1. Introduction Cypriot Greek is of particular interest, not only because it is one of the few varieties of Modern Greek maintaining a consonant length contrast, but more importantly because it exhibits this contrast in word-initial position: péfti ‘Thursday’ vs. ppéfti ‘he falls’.Although word-initial geminates are less common than their word-medial counterparts, they are attested in dozens of the world’s languages in addition to Cypriot Greek.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrastive Modelling of the Intonation of Recapitulatory Echo Interrogative Sentences in Modern American English and Cuban Spanish
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Nottingham ePrints Garcia Romero, Gilberto (2013) Contrastive modelling of the intonation of recapitulatory echo interrogative sentences in modern American English and Cuban Spanish. MA(Res) thesis, University of Nottingham. Access from the University of Nottingham repository: http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/13527/1/AN_INTONATIONAL_ANALYSIS_OF_THE_INTERR OGATIVE_SYSTEMS_IN_MODERN_ENGLISH_AND_CUBAN_SPANISH_2012.pdf Copyright and reuse: The Nottingham ePrints service makes this work by researchers of the University of Nottingham available open access under the following conditions. · Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. · To the extent reasonable and practicable the material made available in Nottingham ePrints has been checked for eligibility before being made available. · Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not- for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. · Quotations or similar reproductions must be sufficiently acknowledged. Please see our full end user licence at: http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/end_user_agreement.pdf A note on versions: The version presented here may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the repository url above for details on accessing the published version and note that access may require a subscription.
    [Show full text]
  • An Autosegmental/Metrical Analysis of Serbo-Croatian Intonation *
    AN AUTOSEGMENTAL/METRICAL ANALYSIS OF SERBO-CROATIAN INTONATION * Svetlana Godjevac Abstract Based on the qualitative analysis of the Fo contours of wide range ofutterances (broad focus declaratives, broad focus questions, nar­ row focus declaratives, narrow focus questions, vocative chant, and prompting intonation) utterred by nine native speakers, an autoseg­ mental/metrical analysis of Standard Selbo-Croatian intonation is pro­ posed. This analysis argues for sparse specification of tones, contra Inkelas and Zee (1988), and two levels of prosodic phrasing: the phonological word and the intonational phrase. The phonological word is defined in te!lDS of a lexical pitch accent and an initial word boundary tone, whereas the intonational phrase is a domain defined by pitch range manipulations (expansion, compression, reset, downstep) and final intonational phrase boundary_ tones. 1 Introduction Standard Serbo-Croatian (SC) is a pitch-accent language. All analyses (Browne & Mccawley 1965 (B&M), Inkelas & Zee 1988 (l&Z), Kostic 1983, Lehiste & Ivie 1963, •r would like to express my gratitude to Mary Beckman, Chris Barker, Allison Blodgett, Rebecca Her­ man, Molly Homer, Tsan Huang, Ilse Lehiste, Gina Taranto, and Pauline Welby. I also wish to thank my informants: Dragana Aleksic, Ljubomir Bjelica, Ana Devic, Ksenija Djuranovic, Svetislav Jovanovic, Jasna Kragalott, Svetlana Li.kic, and Branislav Unkovic for their patience and kindness in providing the data. All errors are mine. 79 . SVETLANA GODJEVAC 1986 (L&I), Nikolic 1970, Stevanovic 1989, Gvozdanovic (1980), inter alia) recognize four different types of accents: short falling, long falling, short rising, and long rising. In this paper I present an analysis of surface tones of these accent types in different sentential environments, including broad-focus and narrow-focu.s utterances, citation form, vocative chant, prompting intonation, and questions.
    [Show full text]
  • Automatic Prosodic Variations Modelling for Language and Dialect Discrimination Jean-Luc Rouas
    Automatic prosodic variations modelling for language and dialect discrimination Jean-Luc Rouas To cite this version: Jean-Luc Rouas. Automatic prosodic variations modelling for language and dialect discrimination. IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech and Language Processing, Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers, 2007, 15 (6), 10.1109/TASL.2007.900094. hal-00657977 HAL Id: hal-00657977 https://hal.inria.fr/hal-00657977 Submitted on 9 Jan 2012 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. 1 Automatic prosodic variations modelling for language and dialect discrimination Jean-Luc Rouas Abstract— This paper addresses the problem of modelling recognition [5], notably Adami’s system [6]. More recently, prosody for language identification. The aim is to create a system systems using syllable-scale features have been under research, that can be used prior to any linguistic work to show if prosodic although their aim is to model acoustic/phonotactic properties differences among languages or dialects can be automatically determined. In previous papers, we defined a prosodic unit, the of languages [7] or also prosodic cues [8]. pseudo-syllable. Rhythmic modelling has proven the relevance of Beside the use of prosody to improve the performances of the pseudo-syllable unit for automatic language identification.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Reduction Rds: an Instance of Foot Recursion? Or a Language-Specific Prosodic Unit? Introduction
    Reduction in in Phonology 2014, MIT! September 19-21 2014! ! Aleksei Nazarov! University of Massachusetts Amherst! [email protected] Outline Hypothesis: Dutch has prosodic domain larger than syllable, smaller than foot “Reduction Domain” (RD): full vowel σ + optional schwa σs Evidence from morphotactics and vowel reduction RDs: an instance of foot recursion? or a language-specific prosodic unit? Introduction English: only schwa in (non-word final) unstressed position ɑ̀gmɛ̀ntéɪʃən ~ ɑ̀gməntéɪʃən (after Pater 2000) Dutch: both full vowels and schwa in unstressed position má.jo.nɛ́:.zə "mayonnaise" má.jə.stɛ̀it "(Your) Majesty" Introduction Dutch standardly analyzed as a quantity- sensitive trochaic language (Oostendorp 1997 and references therein) LL-final words: penultimate main stress (mà.ka)(ró.ni) “macaroni” LH-final words: antepenultimate main stress le(ó.ni)(dɑ̀s) “Leonidas” Introduction Antepenultimate stress: Non-Finality forces stress to shift to penultimate foot if last syllable is heavy (Oostendorp 1997, references therein) This yields antepenultimate stress when the penult is light (i.e., a foot dependent) leonidɑs → le(ó.ni)(dɑ̀s); *le(ò.ni)(dɑ́́s) Introduction Reality of unstressed full vowels: If all full vowels were stressed, avoidance of final main stress could never yield antepenultimate stress leonidɑs → *le(ò)(ní)(dɑ̀s) leonidɑs ?→ le(ó)(nì)(dɑ̀s) Introduction Standard metrical analyses of Dutch: (mà.jo)(nɛ́:.zə) (má.jə)(stɛ̀it) Van der Hulst & Moortgat (1980) propose: Dutch has nested foot structure Smaller foot
    [Show full text]
  • Acquisition of Hebrew Phonoloy
    Pre-final version – comments are welcome To appear in: Berman, Ruth (ed.) Acquisition and Development of Hebrew: From Infancy to Adolescence. John Benjamins. PATHS AND STAGES IN THE ACQUISITION OF HEBREW PHONOLOGICAL WORD Avivit Ben-David & Outi Bat-El Hadassah Academic College & Tel-Aviv University The acquisition of Hebrew phonology has started gaining attention during the last two decades, with quantitative and theoretical studies on the distribution and development of various phonological structures. In this paper, we follow the acquisition of the phonological word in Hebrew, attending to the prosodic word (number of syllables), the foot (stress patterns), the syllable and its sub-syllabic units (onset and coda), and the segments and their features. For each type of phonological structure, we (a) provide distributional facts in Hebrew, in order to evaluate the role of frequency in phonological development; (b) discuss the constraints active during the different stages of development; and (c) introduce the simplification strategies children employ on their way to faithful targets. At the end we consider the resources children use during their phonological development. Key words: language acquisition, Hebrew, phonology, prosodic word, syllable, onset, coda, stress, trochaic bias, segments, features, frequency, nature vs. nurture. 1. Introduction When we start studying children’s language development, using phonetics as our data source, our first encounter with children’s grammar lies within the domain of phonology, i.e. the mental system that categorizes and organizes the phonetics of speech. Data and generalizations are approached from two angles: (a) the principles active in the children’s phonological system during every stage of development, and (b) the correspondence between the children’s productions and the adults’ targets.
    [Show full text]
  • Prosodic Structure and Intonation in Koasati
    PROSODIC STRUCTURE AND INTONATION IN KOASATI Matthew Gordona, Jack Martinb & Linda Langleyc aUniversity of California, Santa Barbara; bCollege of William & Mary; cMcNeese State University [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] ABSTRACT 2. METHODOLOGY This paper provides a description of prosodic structure and intonational properties of Koasati, an This study is based on a combination of qualitative endangered Muskogean language spoken by observations about Koasati prosody made over approximately 300 people in Louisiana and Texas. several years of fieldwork on the language as well a Words in Koasati group into Accentual Phrases series of elicited recordings targeting particular (AP), which are characterized by an initial and final properties of the prosodic system for both qualitative high tone. Accentual Phrases in turn combine to and quantitative study. The targeted recordings form Intonational Phrases (IP), which are defined by consisted of elicited word lists and sentences a final boundary tone: L% (or for certain speakers recorded from multiple speakers (both male and LH%) in statements, H% in questions, and HL% in female) designed to illustrate the features of lexical commands. In addition to intonational tones, Koasati tone, phrasal intonation patterns, and prosodic also has tonal accents of three types. First, there is constituency. In addition, recordings of two lexical tone associated with certain nouns and certain narratives were consulted for further insight into verbal affixes. Second, tone is used morphologically intonation and prosodic structure. The primary data to convey aspectual distinctions in verbs. Finally, consulted were all recorded at a sampling rate of predictable accents may dock on the penultimate 44.1kHz onto a Marantz (PMD 661) solidstate syllable of the stem and on heavy syllables (those recorder using a handheld unidirectional Sennheiser containing a long vowel or a coda sonorant).
    [Show full text]
  • Epenthesis and Prosodic Structure in Armenian
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Epenthesis and prosodic structure in Armenian: A diachronic account A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies by Jessica L. DeLisi 2015 © Copyright by Jessica L. DeLisi 2015 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Epenthesis and prosodic structure in Armenian: A diachronic account by Jessica L. DeLisi Doctor of Philosophy in Indo-European Studies University of California, Los Angeles, 2015 Professor H. Craig Melchert, Chair In this dissertation I will attempt to answer the following question: why does Classical Armenian have three dierent reexes for the Proto-Armenian epenthetic vowel word- initially before old Proto-Indo-European consonant clusters? Two of the vowels, e and a, occur in the same phonological environment, and even in doublets (e.g., Classical ełbayr beside dialectal ałbär ‘brother’). The main constraint driving this asymmetry is the promotion of the Sonority Sequenc- ing Principle in the grammar. Because sibilants are more sonorous than stops, the promo- tion of the Sonority Sequencing Principle above the Strict Layer Hypothesis causes speak- ers to create a semisyllable to house the sibilant extraprosodically. This extraprosodic structure is not required for old consonant-resonant clusters since they already conform to the Sonority Sequencing Principle. Because Armenian has sonority-sensitive stress, the secondary stress placed on word-initial epenthetic vowels triggers a vowel change in all words without extraprosodic structure, i.e. with the old consonant-resonant clusters. Therefore Proto-Armenian */@łbayR/ becomes Classical Armenian [èł.báyR] ‘brother,’ but Proto-Armenian */<@s>tipem/ with extraprosodic <@s> becomes [<@s>.tì.pém] ‘I rush’ because the schwa is outside the domain of stress assignment.
    [Show full text]
  • PETER AUER a Note on Prosody in Natural Phonology
    Sonderdrucke aus der Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg PETER AUER A note on prosody in natural phonology Originalbeitrag erschienen in: Julian Mendez Dosuma (Hrsg.): Naturalists at Krems: papers from the Workshop on Natural Phonology and Natural Morphology, (Krems, 1 - 7 July 1988). Salamanca: Ed. Univ. de Salamanca, 1990, S. 11-22 A Note on Prosody in Natural Phonology Peter Auer . Naturalness in Phonology: articulation/perception or cognition? The last 15 years have brought about dramatic changes in (Generative) Phonology; We have witnessed the times of segmental Generative Phonology coming to an end. Radically new conceptions of Phonology have developed. Instead of taking the input and output of Phonology as strings of linearly ordered segments, as it was the case in classical Generative Phonology, phonological units are now assumed to display an internal hierarchical organization; from year to year, the complexity of this internal organization seems to increase, and new research paradigms such as Autosegmental Phonology, Metrical Phonology, Prosodic Phonology, Dependency Phonology have developed and worked on phonological representations and models that have little to do with, say, SPE phonology. All of these new approaches make use of the theoretical insight that phonological data should be spread onto a number of levels or "tiers") This non-linear approach has made it possible to analyze suprasegmental phenomena, which by their very nature, spread beyond single phonological segments (such as tone, intonation, st0d, musical and pitch accent); in addition, phenomena hitherto considered to be in the realm of the segment turned out to be analyzable in a much more explanative way, when looked upon in the new paradigm - phenomena such as compensatory lengthening or assimilation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Prosodic Account of Consonant Gemination in Japanese Loanwords
    to appear in: Kobozono, Haruo. ed. 2017. The Phonetics and Phonology of Geminate Consonants. Oxford: OUP. A prosodic account of consonant gemination in Japanese loanwords Junko Ito**, Haruo Kubozono*, and Armin Mester** *NINJAL, **UC Santa Cruz 1 Introduction The distribution of geminate consonants in Japanese loanwords is notoriously complex. On the one hand, there are intrinsic factors. Some consonants, in particular obstruents (called sokuon when geminated), are more prone to gemination than sonorants. Segmental features lead to further distinctions: Voiceless obstruents geminate more easily than voiced obstruents (1a), and some types of fricatives geminate more easily than others (1b) (dots indicate syllable boundaries, with initial and final syllable boundaries usually not marked, and an accent mark after a vowel indicates that it is accented). (1) Segmental factors a. Voicing type: voiceless vs. voiced obstruents Gemination No Gemination cap kya'p.pu cab kya'.bu lock ro'k.ku log ro'.gu b. Fricative type: [ʃ] vs. [s], [x] vs. [f] Gemination No Gemination bush bu'ʃ.ʃu bus ba'.su Bach ba'h.ha puff pa'.fu On the other hand, one and the same consonant is more likely to geminate in some phonological contexts than in others (2). (2) Positional factors Gemination No Gemination p cap kya'p.pu captain kya'.pu.ten p apple a'p.pu.ru chapel ͡tʃa'.pe.ru p happy ha'p.pii happiness ha'.pi.ne.su t market ma'a.ket.to marketing ma'a.ke.tin.gu k pack pa'k.ku park pa'a.ku k tax ta'k.ku.su tact ta'.ku.to g frog fu.ro'g.gu log ro'.gu s listen ri's.sun listener ri'.su.naa s message me's.see.d͡ ʒi mess me'.su This paper discusses how the distribution of geminates as opposed to singletons (e.g., /pp/, /dd/, /mm/ vs.
    [Show full text]
  • An Automatic Speech Segmentation Tool Based on Multiple Acoustic Parameters
    An automatic speech segmentation tool based on multiple acoustic parameters Maryualê Malvessi Mittmann°, Plínio Almeida Barbosa* °Universidade do Vale do Itajaí, *Universidade Estadual de Campinas Speech segmentation is required not only for linguistic research based on oral corpora, but had become essential for natural language processing. Many researchers have devel- oped different approaches to deal with the need of automatic segmentation of speech da- ta. In this paper we discuss some of the prosodic parameters used as cues for boundary identification and present an ongoing project for the automatic segmentation of sponta- neous speech developed for Brazilian Portuguese. Keywords: speech segmentation, spontaneous speech, prosodic boundaries, automatic segmentation 1. Introduction Speech corpora are increasingly becoming important resources for different are- as, not only in the field of theoretical and applied linguistics but also for the de- velopment of technologies such as text to speech/speech to text systems. How- ever, information extraction from speech corpora requires the segmentation of the audio signal into discrete and meaningful linguistic units. The main goal of this paper is to propose the main features of a model for the automatic segmenta- tion of spontaneous speech based on prosodic parameters in Brazilian Portu- guese. We also discuss some of the literature concerning this topic. The definition for the basic segmental unit of speech may vary according to the researcher’s interests. They can be either words or linguistic structures smaller or larger than the word. Many tools have been developed for the segmentation of a speech signal into phonetic units smaller than the word, i.e.
    [Show full text]