Evaluating Stereoacuity with 3D Shutter Glasses Technology Huang Wu1*, Han Jin2, Ying Sun3, Yang Wang1, Min Ge1, Yang Chen1 and Yunfeng Chi1

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Evaluating Stereoacuity with 3D Shutter Glasses Technology Huang Wu1*, Han Jin2, Ying Sun3, Yang Wang1, Min Ge1, Yang Chen1 and Yunfeng Chi1 Wu et al. BMC Ophthalmology (2016) 16:45 DOI 10.1186/s12886-016-0223-3 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Evaluating stereoacuity with 3D shutter glasses technology Huang Wu1*, Han Jin2, Ying Sun3, Yang Wang1, Min Ge1, Yang Chen1 and Yunfeng Chi1 Abstract Background: To determine the stereoacuity threshold with a 3D laptop equipped with 3D shutter glasses, and to evaluate the effect of different shape and size of test symbols and different type of disparities to stereoacuity. Methods: Thirty subjects with a visual acuity in each eye of at least 0 logMAR and a stereoacuity of at least 32 arcsec (as assessed in Fly Stereo Acuity Test) were recruited. Three target symbols—tumbling "E", tumbling "C", and "□"—were displayed, each with six different sizes representing a visual acuity ranging from 0.5 to 0 logMAR when tested at 4.1 m, and with both crossed and uncrossed disparities. Two test systems were designed - fixed distance of 4.1 m and one for variable distance. The former has disparities ranging from 10 to 1000 arcsec. Each subject completed 36 trials to investigate the effect of different symbol sizes and shapes, and disparity types on stereoacuity. In the variable distance system, each subject was tested 12 times for the same purposes, both proximally and distally (the point where the 3D effect just appears and where it just disappears respectively), and the mean value was calculated from the mean proximal and distal distances. Results: No significant difference was found among the groups in the fixed distance test system (Kruskal-Wallis test; Chi-square = 29.844, P = 0.715). Similarly, no significant difference was found in the variable distance system (Kruskal-Wallis test; proximal: Chi-square = 5.687, P = 0.338; distal: Chi-square = 5.898, P = 0.316; mean: Chi-square = 6.152, P =0.292). Conclusions: Evaluating stereoacuity using this measurement system was convenient and effective. Changes in target shape and size and disparity types had no significant effect on stereoacuity. It would be helpful to choose optimal targets according to different purposes using computer-assisted 3D measurements. Keywords: Stereopsis, Shutter glasses, Horizontal disparity Background quantified as the minimum geometric disparity that Stereopsis is a higher function of two-eye coordination, elicits the perception of depth termed stereoacuity, mea- which enables a precise judgment of distance. Generally, sured in seconds of arc (arcsec) [1]. Stereoacuity is it is possible to determine distance with monocular vi- sometimes measured from a distance of 3–6 m, e.g., the sion from looming, motion parallax, and pictorial depth Frisby–Davis distance stereotest [3–9]. The majority of cues such as occlusion, perspective, texture gradients, measurements are performed at a closer range, usually relative size, and shadows. However, the most precise 0.4 m, with e.g., the Titmus Fly Test using polarization distance determination is achieved through stereopsis, technology [9–14], and the TNO Stereoacuity Test using which is a specific type of binocular depth perception red and green glasses [9, 10, 15–19]. These traditional resulting from the horizontal separation of the two eyes methods are widely used in clinical practice and experi- and the subsequent ability to recognize retinal disparity mental research. Since the pattern and number of test [1]. Stereopsis is important for carrying out specific pictures are relatively fixed, it can be overwhelming and tasks, including better motor control and quicker and painstakingly slow to study a large number of influen- more accurate cognitive information [2]. Stereopsis is cing factors for stereopsis. With the development and advances in three-dimensional (3D) computer technol- * Correspondence: [email protected] 1 ogy over recent years, researchers have designed new Department of Optometry, Second Hospital of Jilin University, No. 218, – Ziqiang Street, Nanguan District, Changchun 130041, China stereopsis tests with modern techniques [20 25]. At this Full list of author information is available at the end of the article © 2016 Wu et al. Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. Wu et al. BMC Ophthalmology (2016) 16:45 Page 2 of 8 point, no definite conclusions have been drawn on whether the size or the shape of the target would affect the test result and how to choose an ideal symbol to ex- plore stereopsis on the display screen. Thus, we estab- lished a stereoacuity measurement system using 3D shutter glasses technology and designed different test targets in order to assess the effects of different factors on stereoacuity. We also applied two methods to evalu- ate those factors: the fixed distance test and the variable distance test. The aim of our study was to investigate the effect of different target shapes and sizes on stereoa- cuity, and it would be the condition to choose optimal targets according to different purposes with computer- aided 3D measurement. Fig. 1 Photograph showing the 3D laptop (ASUS G750Y47JX) equipped with NVidia 3D Vision 2 Wireless Glasses Kit (Expressway Methods Santa Clara, CA, USA) Subjects The study was conducted at the Second Hospital of Jilin University in China. A total of 30 subjects, aged 20–28 midpoint of the L and R images, which was 100 px away (22.9 ± 2.4) years, were included, comprising 11 males from other symbols. The test distance was set at 4.1 m, and 19 females. The refractive status of the right eyes and the symbol sizes represented 0.5, 0.4, 0.3, 0.2, 0.1, and were as follows: spherical error +0.50 DS to −4.00 DS 0 logMAR respectively, under these conditions. (−1.42 ± 1.16 DS), cylinder error 0 to −1.00 DC (−0.64 ± Seven symbols were drawn in a line. One symbol, which 0.18 DC), spherical equivalent +0.50 DS to −4.00 DS was the stereo target, was chosen randomly from the five (−1.48 ± 1.14 DS). The refractive status of the left eyes middle symbols and rendered with 3D depth. The stereo were as follows: spherical error +0.50 DS to −4.00 DS target consisted of two component images, each of which (−1.50 ± 1.20 DS), cylinder error 0 to −1.75 DC (−0.92 ± was visible with one eye only as the 3D shutter glasses 0.47 DC), spherical equivalent +0.50 DS to −4.00 DS blocked the other eye. The distance between the two ob- (−1.59 ± 1.14 DS). None of the study participants had jects represented the horizontal disparity. Crossed dispar- severe ametropia, amblyopia [26], strabismus [27], or an- ity occurred when both eyes saw an image on the opposite isometropia [28]. The minimum corrected visual acuity side. If the right image of the symbol was seen by the left of each eye was 0 logMAR. The stereoacuity, as mea- eye, and the left image was seen by the right eye, the target sured by the Fly Stereo Acuity Test (Vision Assessment would be perceived as a single image through the 3D Corporation, Elk Grove Village, IL, USA), was at least shutter glasses. It would appear in a different plane that 32 arcsec. was closer to the observer, compared to the other sym- bols on the same line. Conversely, if the right side of Computer system the symbol was seen by the right eye, and the left side We designed a notebook computer system, using a laptop was seen by the left eye, the target would be seen dent- (ASUS G750Y47JX, 17.3" 16:9 full HD 3D (1920 × 1080 ing into the screen, compared to the other symbols, 120 Hz)) running Windows 8.1 and NVidia 3D Vision 2 and this is called uncrossed disparity. The depth of extru- Wireless Glasses Kit (Expressway Santa Clara, CA, USA). sion of the stereo target was determined by the degree of The NVidia 3D Vision Photo Viewer Software was used disparity. Thus the larger the disparity, the more visible to view 3D pictures (Fig. 1). the stereo perception would be. When the disparity de- creased to a certain threshold, the observer would not be Test targets able to distinguish between the stereo target and the other A program written in C# produced all test targets. Three symbols. The aim of our test was to detect the minimum types of symbols were drawn: tumbling "E", tumbling "C", disparity at which the study participant could still see the and "□" (Fig. 2). Each type of symbol had six different stereo target. sizes, containing strokes that were 19, 15, 12, 9, 7, and 6 pixels (px) wide. Hence, the complete symbols had sizes The fixed distance test system of 95 × 95, 75 × 75, 60 × 60, 45 × 45, 35 × 35, and 30 × 30 In our study, the test distance was set at 4.1 m, and hence px. All symbols were set at 100 px apart, both horizontally 1 px corresponded to a disparity of 10 arcsec. The fixed and vertically. The symbols with 3D effect were slightly distance test system contained three grades of difficulty. different; the perceived images of the symbols were at the The first grade consisted of a single page that contained Wu et al. BMC Ophthalmology (2016) 16:45 Page 3 of 8 Fig.
Recommended publications
  • Real-Time 2D to 3D Video Conversion Using Compressed Video Based on Depth-From Motion and Color Segmentation
    International Journal of Latest Trends in Engineering and Technology (IJLTET) Real-Time 2D to 3D Video Conversion using Compressed Video based on Depth-From Motion and Color Segmentation N. Nivetha Research Scholar, Dept. of MCA, VELS University, Chennai. Dr.S.Prasanna, Asst. Prof, Dept. of MCA, VELS University, Chennai. Dr.A.Muthukumaravel, Professor & Head, Department of MCA, BHARATH University, Chennai. Abstract :- This paper provides the conversion of two dimensional (2D) to three dimensional (3D) video. Now-a-days the three dimensional video are becoming more popular, especially at home entertainment. For converting 2D to 3D, the conversion techniques are used so able to deliver the 3D videos efficiently and effectively. In this paper, block matching based depth from motion estimation and color segmentation is used for presenting the video conversion scheme i.e., automatic monoscopic video to stereoscopic 3D.To provide a good region boundary information the color based segmentation is used for fuse with block-based depth map for assigning good depth values in every segmented region and eliminating the staircase effect. The experimental results can achieve 3D stereoscopic video output is relatively high quality manner. Keywords - Depth from Motion, 3D-TV, Stereo vision, Color Segmentation. I. INTRODUCTION 3DTV is television that conveys depth perception to the viewer by employing techniques such as stereoscopic display, multiview display, 2D-plus depth, or any other form of 3D display. In 2010, 3DTV is widely regarded as one of the next big things and many well-known TV brands such as Sony and Samsung were released 3D- enabled TV sets using shutter glasses based 3D flat panel display technology.
    [Show full text]
  • Vers Un Modèle Unifié Pour L'affichage
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Savoirs UdeS VERS UN MODÈLE UNIFIÉ POUR L’AFFICHAGE AUTOSTÉRÉOSCOPIQUE D’IMAGES par Clément Proust Mémoire présenté au Département d’informatique en vue de l’obtention du grade de maître ès sciences (M.Sc.) FACULTÉ DES SCIENCES UNIVERSITÉ DE SHERBROOKE Sherbrooke, Québec, Canada, 24 février 2016 Le 24 février 2016 Le jury a accepté le mémoire de Clément Proust dans sa version finale Membres du jury Professeur Djemel Ziou Directeur Département d’informatique de l’Université de Sherbrooke Professeur Bessam Abdulrazak Président-rapporteur Département d’informatique de l’Université de Sherbrooke Professeur Marie–Flavie Auclair–Fortier Membre interne Département d’informatique de l’Université de Sherbrooke i Sommaire Dans un premier chapitre, nous décrivons un modèle de formation d’image affichée sur un écran en revenant sur les concepts que sont la lumière, la géométrie et l’optique. Nous détaillons ensuite les différentes techniques d’affichage stéréoscopique utilisées à l’heure actuelle, en parcourant la stéréoscopie, l’autostéréoscopie et plus particuliè- rement le principe d’imagerie intégrale. Le deuxième chapitre introduit un nouveau modèle de formation d’image stéréoscopique. Ce dernier nous permet d’observer deux images d’une paire stéréoscopique soumises à des éventuelles transformations et à l’effet d’une ou de plusieurs optiques particulières, pour reproduire la perception de trois dimensions. Nous abordons l’aspect unificateur de ce modèle. En effet il permet de décrire et d’expliquer de nombreuses techniques d’affichage stéréoscopique exis- tantes. Enfin, dans un troisième chapitre nous discutons d’une méthode particulière de création de paires d’images stéréoscopiques à l’aide d’un champ lumineux.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Audio Cues and Sound Source Stimuli on Looming
    The Effect of Audio Cues and Sound Source Stimuli on Looming Perception Sonia Wilkie Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science Queen Mary, University of London January 28, 2015 Statement of Originality I, Sonia Wilkie, confirm that the research included within this thesis is my own work or that where it has been carried out in collaboration with, or supported by others, that this is duly acknowledged below and my contribution indicated. Previously published material is also acknowledged below. I attest that I have exercised reasonable care to ensure that the work is original, and does not to the best of my knowledge break any UK law, infringe any third partys copyright or other Intellectual Property Right, or contain any confidential material. I accept that the College has the right to use plagiarism detection software to check the electronic version of the thesis. I confirm that this thesis has not been previously submitted for the award of a degree by this or any other university. The copyright of this thesis rests with the author and no quotation from it or in- formation derived from it may be published without the prior written consent of the author. Signature: Date: Abstract Objects that move in depth (looming) are ubiquitous in the real and virtual worlds. How humans interact and respond to these approaching objects may affect their continued survival in both the real and virtual words, and is dependent on the individual's capacity to accurately interpret depth and movement cues.
    [Show full text]
  • 3D Television - Wikipedia
    3D television - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/3D_television From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia 3D television (3DTV) is television that conveys depth perception to the viewer by employing techniques such as stereoscopic display, multi-view display, 2D-plus-depth, or any other form of 3D display. Most modern 3D television sets use an active shutter 3D system or a polarized 3D system, and some are autostereoscopic without the need of glasses. According to DisplaySearch, 3D televisions shipments totaled 41.45 million units in 2012, compared with 24.14 in 2011 and 2.26 in 2010.[1] As of late 2013, the number of 3D TV viewers An example of three-dimensional television. started to decline.[2][3][4][5][6] 1 History 2 Technologies 2.1 Displaying technologies 2.2 Producing technologies 2.3 3D production 3TV sets 3.1 3D-ready TV sets 3.2 Full 3D TV sets 4 Standardization efforts 4.1 DVB 3D-TV standard 5 Broadcasts 5.1 3D Channels 5.2 List of 3D Channels 5.3 3D episodes and shows 5.3.1 1980s 5.3.2 1990s 5.3.3 2000s 5.3.4 2010s 6 World record 7 Health effects 8See also 9 References 10 Further reading The stereoscope was first invented by Sir Charles Wheatstone in 1838.[7][8] It showed that when two pictures 1 z 17 21. 11. 2016 22:13 3D television - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/3D_television are viewed stereoscopically, they are combined by the brain to produce 3D depth perception. The stereoscope was improved by Louis Jules Duboscq, and a famous picture of Queen Victoria was displayed at The Great Exhibition in 1851.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereoscopic 3D in Games
    DEGREE PROJECT, IN COMPUTER SCIENCE , SECOND LEVEL STOCKHOLM, SWEDEN 2015 Stereoscopic 3D in games HENNING BENNET AND DAVID LINDSTRÖM KTH ROYAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY SCHOOL OF COMPUTER SCIENCE AND COMMUNICATION (CSC) EXAMENSARBETE VID CSC, KTH Stereoskopisk 3D i spel Stereoscopic 3D in games Bennet, Henning & Lindström, David E-postadress vid KTH: [email protected] & [email protected] Exjobb i: Datalogi Program: Civilingenjör Datateknik Handledare: Arnborg, Stefan Examinator: Arnborg, Stefan Uppdragsgivare: Fabrication Games Datum: 2015-06-17 Sammanfattning Stereoskopisk 3D i spel I den här rapporten undersöks stereoskopisk 3D. Vi utreder hur ett spel ska anpassas för att ta fram en så bra och tydlig stereoskopisk 3D-effekt som möjligt och så att betraktaren upplever ett tydligt djup utan att uppleva ett obehag på grund av effekten. Rapporten tittar djupare på vilka tekniska aspekter man behöver ta hänsyn till vid spelutveckling i stereoskopisk 3D. Samt vilka prestandabegränsningar som man bör ta hänsyn till vid stereoskopisk 3D. Vi beskriver hur processen och framtagandet av prototypen Kodo med anaglyfisk stereoskopisk 3D såg ut. Prototypen togs fram för att testa och analysera resultatet av stereoskopisk 3D-effekten. Abstract Stereoscopic 3D in games In this report we investigate the technique of stereoscopic 3D. This report investigates the steps needed to create a game adapted for an improved stereoscopic 3D effect. Furthermore we investigate what improvements one should make to avoid the beholder to experience any discomfort due to the effect. The report talks about technical aspects one needs to consider when using stereoscopic 3D, as well as performance issues we might need to take into consideration.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Audio Cues and Sound Source Stimuli on Looming
    The Effect of Audio Cues and Sound Source Stimuli on Looming Perception Sonia Wilkie Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science Queen Mary, University of London January 28, 2015 Statement of Originality I, Sonia Wilkie, confirm that the research included within this thesis is my own work or that where it has been carried out in collaboration with, or supported by others, that this is duly acknowledged below and my contribution indicated. Previously published material is also acknowledged below. I attest that I have exercised reasonable care to ensure that the work is original, and does not to the best of my knowledge break any UK law, infringe any third partys copyright or other Intellectual Property Right, or contain any confidential material. I accept that the College has the right to use plagiarism detection software to check the electronic version of the thesis. I confirm that this thesis has not been previously submitted for the award of a degree by this or any other university. The copyright of this thesis rests with the author and no quotation from it or in- formation derived from it may be published without the prior written consent of the author. Signature: Date: Abstract Objects that move in depth (looming) are ubiquitous in the real and virtual worlds. How humans interact and respond to these approaching objects may affect their continued survival in both the real and virtual words, and is dependent on the individual's capacity to accurately interpret depth and movement cues.
    [Show full text]
  • The Current State of the Consumer 3D Experience
    The Current State of the Consumer 3D Experience By Philip Lelyveld Program Manager, Consumer 3D Experience Lab / Program USC Entertainment Technology Center June 7, 2012 Table of Contents Introduction 3 Theatrical 3D 4 3D TVs 7 3D Cameras, Laptops, Phones, Tablets 12 3D Gaming 13 Head Mounted Displays 15 Theme Parks 15 Other Markets for 3D 16 Conclusion: Looking Backward, Looking Forward 18 Appendix 1 – 3D Lab Presentation / Tour Log 20 Appendix 2 – 3D Lab Event Speaker / Moderator Log 21 Appendix 3 – 3D Audio 22 Appendix 4 – 3D Printers 24 2 The Current State of the Consumer 3D Experience Introduction The introduction of non-anaglyph, digital S3D as a consumer experience started with Disney’s Chicken Little in 2005. But the 2009 theatrical release of Avatar, with James Cameron’s strong marketing of the 3D aspect of the feature, was the benchmark event that defined consumer expectations for a 3D experience. Mr. Cameron and company also laid the groundwork for the rapid decent into the trough of disillusionment by telegraphing what consumers should expect before those expectations could be adequately met. At that moment the production, theatrical exhibition, distribution, and consumer electronics technologies infrastructures were still being developed, and content creation was just ramping up. The ETC’s Consumer 3D Experience Lab and Program has ridden the heart of the Hype Cycle; from a few years after digital stereoscopic 3D’s (S3D’s) launch in cinemas through the peak of inflated expectations, down to the trough of disillusionment, and now upward along the slope of enlightenment. We are approaching, but not yet at, the plateau of productivity for 3D.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning in Virtual 3D Environments: All About Immersive 3D Interfaces
    LEARNING IN VIRTUAL 3D ENVIRONMENTS: ALL ABOUT IMMERSIVE 3D INTERFACES Vojtěch Juřík1,2, Čeněk ŠašInka1,2 1Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Masaryk University, Arna Nováka 1/1, 602 00 Brno, Czech Republic 2HUME lab - Experimental Humanities Laboratory, Masaryk University, Arna Nováka 1/1, 602 00 Brno, Czech Republic Abstract Human-computer interaction have entered the 3D era. The use of immersive virtual 3D environments in the area of education and learning is important field of research interest. 3D virtual worlds can be dynamically modified, updated or customized and more operators can cooperate when solving specific task regardless to their physical presence. With respect to the features of immersive human-computer interaction as well as specific cues included in virtual environments it is necessary to consider every aspect of 3D interface in the way how it influence the process of learning. Especially, the social and communicational aspects in Collaborative Virtual Learning Environments (CVLEs) is the state of the art for the current research. Based on our research of interactive geographical 3D environments, we summarize and discuss the relevant theoretical and technological aspects entering into the issue of interaction with 3D virtual environments. The operators' manipulation and evaluation of the displayed content is discussed regarding such phenomena as fidelity, presence/immersion, informational and computational equivalence, situation awareness, cognitive workload or human error. We also describe specific interface developed for recording and measuring the participants’ behavior in virtual spaces. Further we suggest the methodological background for the research of 3D virtual interfaces with respect to virtual collaboration and learning. Keywords: 3D technology, situation awareness, user interface, human error, human-computer interaction, fidelity, presence 1 INTRODUCTION With the rapid development of informational technologies, the communication and collaboration in virtual worlds (VWs) have entered a 3D era (Boughzala et al., 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • WO 2013/158322 Al 24 October 2013 (24.10.2013) P O P C T
    (12) INTERNATIONAL APPLICATION PUBLISHED UNDER THE PATENT COOPERATION TREATY (PCT) (19) World Intellectual Property Organization International Bureau (10) International Publication Number (43) International Publication Date WO 2013/158322 Al 24 October 2013 (24.10.2013) P O P C T (51) International Patent Classification: (74) Agent: STEIN, Michael, D.; Woodcock Washburn LLP, G06T 15/08 (201 1.01) Cira Centre, 2929 Arch Street, 12th Floor, Philadelphia, PA 19104-2891 (US). (21) International Application Number: PCT/US20 13/032821 (81) Designated States (unless otherwise indicated, for every kind of national protection available): AE, AG, AL, AM, (22) Date: International Filing AO, AT, AU, AZ, BA, BB, BG, BH, BN, BR, BW, BY, 18 March 2013 (18.03.2013) BZ, CA, CH, CL, CN, CO, CR, CU, CZ, DE, DK, DM, (25) Filing Language: English DO, DZ, EC, EE, EG, ES, FI, GB, GD, GE, GH, GM, GT, HN, HR, HU, ID, IL, IN, IS, JP, KE, KG, KM, KN, KP, (26) Publication Language: English KR, KZ, LA, LC, LK, LR, LS, LT, LU, LY, MA, MD, (30) Priority Data: ME, MG, MK, MN, MW, MX, MY, MZ, NA, NG, NI, 61/635,075 18 April 2012 (18.04.2012) US NO, NZ, OM, PA, PE, PG, PH, PL, PT, QA, RO, RS, RU, RW, SC, SD, SE, SG, SK, SL, SM, ST, SV, SY, TH, TJ, (71) Applicant: THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY TM, TN, TR, TT, TZ, UA, UG, US, UZ, VC, VN, ZA, OF CALIFORNIA [US/US]; 1111 Franklin Street, ZM, ZW. Twelfth Floor, Oakland, CA 94607 (US). (84) Designated States (unless otherwise indicated, for every (72) Inventors: DAVIS, James E.; 1156 High Street: SOE3, kind of regional protection available): ARIPO (BW, GH, Santa Cruz, CA 95064 (US).
    [Show full text]
  • The Difference of Distance Stereoacuity Measured with Different Separating Methods
    468 Original Article Page 1 of 9 The difference of distance stereoacuity measured with different separating methods Lingzhi Zhao1, Yu Zhang2, Huang Wu2, Jun Xiao3 1Department of Medical Equipment, 2Department of Optometry, 3Department of Medical Retina, the Second Hospital of Jilin University, Changchun 130041, China Contributions: (I) Conception and design: H Wu, J Xiao; (II) Administrative support: L Zhao; (III) Provision of study materials or patients: All authors; (IV) Collection and assembly of data: All authors; (V) Data analysis and interpretation: H Wu, J Xiao; (VI) Manuscript writing: All authors; (VII) Final approval of manuscript: All authors. Correspondence to: Jun Xiao. Department of Medical Retina, the Second Hospital of Jilin University, No. 218, Ziqiang Street, Nanguan District, Changchun 130041, China. Email: [email protected]. Background: The majority of tests to evaluate stereopsis should separate two eyes first. Whether different binocular separating manner may affect the test result of stereopsis is the main purpose of this study. Red- green anaglyphs, polarized light technology, active shutter 3D system, and auto-stereoscopic technique were chosen to evaluate distance stereoacuity. Methods: Red-green anaglyphs test system was established with an ASUS laptop with the aid of TNO Stereotest glasses. Active shutter 3D system was set up with the same ASUS laptop with the aid of NVidia 3D Vision 2 Wireless Glasses Kit. The polarized 3D system adopted the AOC display. A Samsung naked- eye 3D laptop was used to set up an auto-stereoscopic system. Thirty subjects were recruited. Distance stereoacuity was measured with those computer systems. Results: The auto-stereoscopic system was failed to measure distance stereopsis.
    [Show full text]
  • Book V Camera
    b bb bbbera bbbbon.com bbbb Basic Photography in 180 Days Book V - Camera Editor: Ramon F. aeroramon.com Contents 1 Day 1 1 1.1 Camera ................................................ 1 1.1.1 Functional description ..................................... 2 1.1.2 History ............................................ 2 1.1.3 Mechanics ........................................... 5 1.1.4 Formats ............................................ 8 1.1.5 Camera accessories ...................................... 8 1.1.6 Camera design history .................................... 8 1.1.7 Image gallery ......................................... 12 1.1.8 See also ............................................ 14 1.1.9 References .......................................... 15 1.1.10 Bibliography ......................................... 16 1.1.11 External links ......................................... 17 2 Day 2 18 2.1 Camera obscura ............................................ 18 2.1.1 Physical explanation ...................................... 19 2.1.2 Technology .......................................... 19 2.1.3 History ............................................ 20 2.1.4 Role in the modern age .................................... 31 2.1.5 Examples ........................................... 32 2.1.6 Public access ......................................... 33 2.1.7 See also ............................................ 33 2.1.8 Notes ............................................. 34 2.1.9 References .......................................... 34 2.1.10 Sources
    [Show full text]
  • Moving Towards Consistent Depth Perception in Stereoscopic Projection-Based Augmented Reality
    ICAT-EGVE (2017), pp. 1–8 R. Lindeman, G. Bruder, and D. Iwai (Editors) Moving Towards Consistent Depth Perception in Stereoscopic Projection-based Augmented Reality S. Schmidt1, G. Bruder2 and F. Steinicke1 1Universität Hamburg, Human-Computer Interaction, Germany 2University of Central Florida, USA Abstract Stereoscopic projection-based augmented reality (AR) is a promising technology for creating an effective illusion of virtual and real objects coexisting within the same space. By using projection technology, two-dimensional (2D) textures as well as three-dimensional (3D) virtual objects can be displayed on arbitrary physical objects. However, depending on the geometry of the projection surface, even a single virtual object could be projected with varying depths, orientations, and forms. For these reasons, it is an open question whether or not a geometrically-correct projection leads to a consistent depth perception of the AR environment. We performed an experiment to analyze how humans perceive depths of objects that are stereoscopically projected at different surfaces in a projection-based AR environment. In a perceptual matching task the participants had to adjust the depth of one of two visual stimuli, which were displayed at different depths with varying parallaxes, until they estimated the depths of both stimuli to match. The results indicate that the effect of parallax on the estimation of matching depths significantly depends on the participant’s experience with stereoscopic display. Regular users were able to match the depths of both stimuli with a mean absolute error of less than one centimeter, whereas less experienced users made errors in the range of more than 2cm on average.
    [Show full text]