Ethological Considerations in the Experimental Study of Lizard Behavior

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ethological Considerations in the Experimental Study of Lizard Behavior University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Faculty Publications and Other Works -- Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Ecology and Evolutionary Biology January 1978 Ethological Considerations in the Experimental Study of Lizard Behavior Neil Greenberg University of Tennessee - Knoxville, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_ecolpubs Part of the Behavioral Neurobiology Commons, Medical Sciences Commons, and the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Greenberg, Neil, "Ethological Considerations in the Experimental Study of Lizard Behavior" (1978). Faculty Publications and Other Works -- Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_ecolpubs/17 This is brought to you for free and open access by the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications and Other Works -- Ecology and Evolutionary Biology by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. BeMwitw <m4 Neaf'Olotnl of Li.lotU N. G.....,nberg and P. D. lber-n, eda. NDI:H, 1978. Ethological Considerations in the Experimental Study of Lizard Behavior NeU Greenberg* Laboratory of Brain Evolution and Behavior National Institute of Mental Health SUMMARY. The importance of an ethological approach to the experimental study of an un­ familiar species is described and several of its problems discussed. The selection of units of be.. havior is a crucial first step in the development of a behavior inventory. The correlation of a behavioral unit with a particular context is necessary to ascribe function to that unit and to de� velop an etbogram. Me thods of studying lizards under controlled conditions are described and discussed. Constraints on behavior that must be considered in an experimental study include the microclimate and its thermal qualities, food and water, shelter utilization, and social behavior. Ritualized display patterns are proposed as sources of hypotheses about the neurophysiological control of social communications. INTRODUCTION The importance of the ethogram in the interpretation of an experimental study of This report will first discuss some issues a species is stressed by most ethologists (e.g., in an ethological approach to the study of an Lorenz, 1935, trans. 1970 :111; Tinbergen, unfamiliar species and will then describe 1951; Thorpe, 1973). Failure to consider all some observations that bear on the. develop­ aspects of normal behavior has diminished ment of an experimental design for the study the usefulness of many experimental studies of lizard behavior in the laboratory. (Adams, 1973; Eibl�Eibesfeldt, 1970; The observations described, both my own Thorpe, 1973). and those gleaned from the literature, are presented to indicate the complexity of the environmental constraints on lizard behavior. Units of Behavior They constitute something of a guide for re­ searchers-be they students of reptiles de­ .A problem in developing ethograms has voted to a favorite group or those who see proven to be the objective delineation of lizards as a possible model for experimentally separate units in what is a continuum · of testing hypotheses about the causation and behavioral patterns. To minimize the arbi­ function of behavior. trary nature of behavioral classification, the investigator attempts to identify reliable, THE ETHOGRAM AND UNITS easily recognizable diagnostic features. OF BEHAVIOR Ideally, behavioral units should be of the Naturalistic observation is the essence of type that a neurophysiologist could deal with. ethology and the fountainhead of hypotheses Barlow (1968) asserts that, in this regard, about the causation and function of behavior only quantifiable behavior is of interest be­ as well as the foundation on which experi­ cause the behavior must be mathematically mental tests of these hypotheses must be modeled if it is to beo c nsidered in neuro­ built. physiological terms. But preoccupation with quantification can be hazardous, .interfering •Present address: Department of Zoology, Univer­ sity of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 87916. with the Gestalt perception of a phenomenon 203 204 Greenberg that is crucial to generating creative hy­ been treated as a single unit, when they may, potheses (Lorenz, 1971). For example, time in fact, represent multiple behavioral pat­ sampling can often be misleading unless the terns which have coalesced; neck and limb environmental or social context is considered, movements may represent two alternative and that is not always possible (Driver, mechanisms which have become combined to 1968). As Vowles (1975) points out, a pre­ effect a display. Some species have several occupation with quantification may be an bobbing movements for different displays understandable response to the subjective which utilize limb and neck movement to and introspective approach of early different degrees or even dispense with one psychology. type of movement. Another difficulty in of be­ The selection of units of behavior must be defining units havior is that motor patterns and sequences suitable to the questions being asked. The are too easily defined in functional hard decisions of "lumping" and "splitting," terms which combine explanation and description. familiar to taxonomists, must also be made This can interfere with comparative when behavior patterns are to be organized inter­ pretations and replications of the into manageable groups. This is true at the original study. level of individual units in a coordinated sequence as well as in more complex be­ havioral categories. It is often helpful to The Evolutionary Perspective begin with a broad overview of general ac­ tivity patterns and then subdivide them until One goal of comparative studies is the de­ the appropriate "resolution" is achieved velopment of the evolutionary perspective (Fig. 1). A neurophysiologist may profitably that leads to an appreciation of the genetic deal with units as tine as a muscle group and (and to that extent "innate") contribution to its associated actions (e.g., the "functional behavior (Brown, 1969). Comparative stud­ unit" of Liem, 1967, cited by Barlow, 1968: ies are also useful to investigators in sug­ 217), while a population biologist may be gesting hypotheses and in analyzing the concerned with a complete communicative adaptive aspects of natural units of behavior. act and its consequences. Both analogy and homology are important While too fine a preliminary analysis can considerations in thinking comparatively obscure a behavioral pattern with trivial de­ about behavior. tails, if the units of behavior utilized in the In evolutionary biology, Mayr (1969) study of a species are too broad, difficulties asserts that only homologous characters are may emerge that could not have been antici­ of taxonomic importance. Homology origi­ pated until the comparative perspective was nally referred to an anatomical correspon­ brought to bear on a problem. For example, dence between structures in two animals details of behavior that might suggest a sharing a similar body plan, but, in current physiological mechanism or the evolutionary use, the evolutionary explanation for hom­ sources of signal behavior can be overlooked. ology has usurped the original meaning of 1974). This . can be a problem in studies of display the term (Beer, behavior where the individual components Two characters under consideration by a of the display and/or their time course are biologist may differ markedly in form or often not as clearly delineated as the general function and yet be related through deriva­ form of the display. A notable exception is tion from a common ancestral precursor. the comparative study of the vertical 41bob- Such relatedness may provide valuable in­ . bing" movements of displaying males of the sights into the causation of dissimilar be­ genus Sceloporu8 by Purdue and Carpenter havioral patterns and the manner in which (1972). Such analysis is useful because the animals evolve. body movements in the ubiquitous bobbing The idea of behavioral homology, however, displays of iguanid lizards have generally is a continuing source of confusion. The con- FORAGING STAlKING \"\ walking (walk·ing) ' . l anerttve body-lean display � and pause head tipped * down. lunge - 1'7·head r � .....t"'4 head tippecl � PERCHING • a anterior food-nudge head exposed negative I t;l:J body up perpendicular COVERT BEHAVIOR • low-mod fully emerged orientation [ fadlitated tl� at threshold { oblique stance � sleeping and ______ .,._ covert activity lateral expansion and (cotd 'SLibstrilte) body-up low-mod u }� (or warm substrate) svbs.trate FEEDING J warm" body-down ventral adpression head·down [ + . rtree•ing··· food-grab postenor adpressoon J food-shake RETREAT �::�:,::;r:a� } t pe j,.L.. '------------------thennalretreat �-----------£------------------------------�------� '---------------------- feeding retreat chew ------------------------ nocturnal retreat ••d swallow Figure 1. A preliminary ethogram of the blue spiny lizard, SceloportUJ cy£�nogentfs. Such a broad overview of daily.ma intenance patterns may be used as � baseline against which subsequent manipulations may be contrasted to indicate distortions in behavior induced by experimental or maintenance � reg1mens. � 206 Greenberg cept resists attempts at rigorous definition heuristic value of such considerations, how­ and yet is of obvious use. Atz
Recommended publications
  • REPTILIA: SQUAMATA: PHRYNOSOMATIDAE Sceloporus Poinsettii
    856.1 REPTILIA: SQUAMATA: PHRYNOSOMATIDAE Sceloporus poinsettii Catalogue of American Amphibians and Reptiles. Webb, R.G. 2008. Sceloporus poinsettii. Sceloporus poinsettii Baird and Girard Crevice Spiny Lizard Sceloporus poinsettii Baird and Girard 1852:126. Type-locality, “Rio San Pedro of the Rio Grande del Norte, and the province of Sonora,” restricted to either the southern part of the Big Burro Moun- tains or the vicinity of Santa Rita, Grant County, New Mexico by Webb (1988). Lectotype, National Figure 1. Adult male Sceloporus poinsettii poinsettii (UTEP Museum of Natural History (USNM) 2952 (subse- 8714) from the Magdalena Mountains, Socorro County, quently recataloged as USNM 292580), adult New Mexico (photo by author). male, collected by John H. Clark in company with Col. James D. Graham during his tenure with the U.S.-Mexican Boundary Commission in late Au- gust 1851 (examined by author). See Remarks. Sceloporus poinsetii: Duméril 1858:547. Lapsus. Tropidolepis poinsetti: Dugès 1869:143. Invalid emendation (see Remarks). Sceloporus torquatus Var. C.: Bocourt 1874:173. Sceloporus poinsetti: Yarrow “1882"[1883]:58. Invalid emendation. S.[celoporus] t.[orquatus] poinsettii: Cope 1885:402. Seloporus poinsettiii: Herrick, Terry, and Herrick 1899:123. Lapsus. Sceloporus torquatus poinsetti: Brown 1903:546. Sceloporus poissetti: Král 1969:187. Lapsus. Figure 2. Adult female Sceloporus poinsettii axtelli (UTEP S.[celoporus] poinssetti: Méndez-De la Cruz and Gu- 11510) from Alamo Mountain (Cornudas Mountains), tiérrez-Mayén 1991:2. Lapsus. Otero County, New Mexico (photo by author). Scelophorus poinsettii: Cloud, Mallouf, Mercado-Al- linger, Hoyt, Kenmotsu, Sanchez, and Madrid 1994:119. Lapsus. Sceloporus poinsetti aureolus: Auth, Smith, Brown, and Lintz 2000:72.
    [Show full text]
  • Preliminary Data on the Age Structure of Phrynocephalus Horvathi in Mount Ararat (Northeastern Anatolia, Turkey)
    BIHAREAN BIOLOGIST 6 (2): pp.112-115 ©Biharean Biologist, Oradea, Romania, 2012 Article No.: 121117 http://biozoojournals.3x.ro/bihbiol/index.html Preliminary data on the age structure of Phrynocephalus horvathi in Mount Ararat (Northeastern Anatolia, Turkey) Kerim ÇIÇEK1,*, Meltem KUMAŞ1, Dinçer AYAZ1 and C. Varol TOK2 1. Ege University, Faculty of Science, Biology Department, Zoology Section, Bornova, Izmir, Turkey 2. Çanakkale Onsekiz Mart University, Faculty of Science - Literature, Biology Department, Zoology Section, Terzioğlu Campus, Çanakkale/Turkey. *Corresponding author, K. Çiçek, E-mail: [email protected] / [email protected] Received: 24. September 2012 / Accepted: 22. October 2012 / Available online: 23. October 2012 / Printed: December 2012 Abstract. In this study, the age structure, growth and longevity of 27 individuals (8 juveniles, 8 males and 11 females) from the Mount Ararat (Iğdır, Turkey) population of Phrynocephalus horvathi were examined with the method of skeletochronology. According to the obtained data, the median age was 3.5 (range= 2-5) for males and 4 (2-5) for females. Both sexes reach sexual maturity after their first hibernation, and no statistically significant difference in age composition was observed between the sexes. According to von Bertalanffy growth curves, asymptotic body length was calculated as 51.29 mm and growth coefficient k - 0.60. Key words: Skeletochronology, growth, longevity, Phrynocephalus horvathi, Northeastern Anatolia. Introduction were measured using dial calipers to the nearest 0.01 mm and re- corded. The genus Phrynocephalus is a core of the Palearctic desert Humerus bones were dissected from specimens, fixed in 70% al- cohol and then washed with distilled water.
    [Show full text]
  • HOME RANGE OVERLAP and SPATIAL-TEMPORAL INTERACTIONS in FEMALE FISHERS on the HOOPA INDIAN RESERVATION in CALIFORNIA by Kerry M
    HOME RANGE OVERLAP AND SPATIAL-TEMPORAL INTERACTIONS IN FEMALE FISHERS ON THE HOOPA INDIAN RESERVATION IN CALIFORNIA By Kerry M. Rennie A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of Humboldt State University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in Natural Resources: Wildlife Committee Membership Dr. Micaela Szykman Gunther, Committee Chair J. Mark Higley, Committee Member Dr. Matthew Johnson, Committee Member Dr. William J. Zielinski, Committee Member Dr. Yvonne Everett, Graduate Coordinator July 2015 ABSTRACT HOME RANGE OVERLAP AND SPATIAL-TEMPORAL INTERACTIONS IN FEMALE FISHERS ON THE HOOPA INDIAN RESERVATION Kerry M. Rennie Spatial organization and interactions within animal populations can have important effects on population demography, social dynamics, and genetic composition. Understanding social behavior and relatedness of individuals in a population can advise species management and conservation. Previous research suggests that fishers (Pekania pennanti) are asocial and exhibit intrasexual territoriality, where home ranges rarely overlap with the same sex. On the Hoopa Valley Indian Reservation in California, female fishers appear to overlap with members of the same sex to a greater extent than most studies report. I tested the hypothesis that female fishers tolerate some degree of spatial overlap, but that co- occurrence in the area of overlap is uncommon. I also tested the hypothesis that this overlap is mediated by habitat selection and female philopatry. I found that female fishers share a large proportion of their home ranges and overlap at a high frequency. However, dynamic interactions revealed fishers avoided each other in both space and time. Habitat used in overlap areas differed from what was available to them in non-overlapping areas but lacked a consistent pattern of ii selection among habitat types.
    [Show full text]
  • Dunes Sagebrush Lizard Habitat
    TECHNICAL NOTES U.S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE NATURAL RESOURCES CONSERVATION SERVICE NEW MEXICO September, 2011 BIOLOGY TECHNICAL NOTE NO. 53 CRITERIA FOR BRUSH MANAGEMENT (314) in Lesser Prairie-Chicken and Dunes Sagebrush Lizard Habitat Introduction NRCS policy requires that when providing technical and financial assistance NRCS will recommend only conservation treatments that will avoid or minimize adverse effects, and to the extent practicable, provide long-term benefit to federal candidate species (General Manual 190 Part 410.22(E)(7)). This technical note provides the criteria to ensure that the NRCS practice of Brush Management (314) will avoid or minimize any adverse effects to two Candidate Species for Federal listing: the lesser prairie chicken Tympanuchus pallidicinctus (LEPC), and dunes sagebrush lizard Sceloporus arenicolus (DSL). Species Involved The lesser prairie chicken is a species of prairie grouse native to the southern high plains of the U.S.; including the sandhill rangelands of eastern New Mexico. The dunes sagebrush lizard is native only to a small area of southeastern New Mexico and west Texas, with a habitat range that overlaps the lesser prairie chicken range, but only occurs in the sand dune complexes associated with shinnery oak (Quercus havardii Rydb.). Both species’ habitat includes a component of brush: shinnery oak and/or sand sagebrush (Artemisia filifolia Torr.). See Appendix 1 and 2 for more details on each species. Geographic Area Covered by Technical Note No. 53 encompasses private and state lands within the range that supports the dunes sagebrush lizard and lesser prairie chicken habitat. This includes portions of seven counties in New Mexico: Chaves, Curry, De Baca, Eddy, Lea, Roosevelt, and Quay counties.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Information Natural Treasures Reptiles (Non-Snakes)
    1 Animal Information Natural Treasures Reptiles (Non-Snakes) Table of Contents Red-footed Tortoise…………….………………………………………………………..2 Argentine Black and white Tegu.………………….………………………..……..4 Madagascar Giant Day Gecko.……………………………………….……..………5 Henkel’s Leaf-Tailed Gecko……………………………………………………………6 Panther Chameleon………………………………………………………………………8 Prehensile-tailed Skink………………………………………….……………………..10 Chuckwalla………………………………………………………….……………………….12 Crevice Spiny Lizard……………………………………………………………………..14 Gila Monster……………………………………………..………………………………...15 Dwarf Caiman………….…………………………………………………………………..17 Spotted Turtle……………………………………………………………………………..19 Mexican Beaded Lizard………………………………………………………………..21 Collared Lizard………………………………………………………………………....…23 Red-footed Tortoise Geocheloidis carbonaria 2 John Ball Zoo Habitat – Depending on whether they can be found either in the Natural Treasures Building or outside in the children’s zoo area across from the Budgie Aviary. Individual Animals: 1 Male, 1 Female Male – Morty (Smooth shell) o Age unknown . Records date back to 1985 o Arrived October 11, 2007 o Weight: 8.5lbs Female - Ethel o Age unknown o Arrived June 02, 2011 o Weight: 9.5-10lbs Life Expectancy Insufficient data Statistics Carapace Length – 1.6 feet for males, females tend to be smaller Diet – Frugivore – an animal that mainly eats fruit Wild – Fruit during the wet season and flowers during the dry season o Some soil and fungi Zoo – Salad mix (greens, fruits, veggies) hard boiled eggs, and fish o Fed twice a week Predators Other than humans, there is no information available concerning predators. Habitat Tropical, terrestrial Rainforests and savanna areas. It prefers heavily forested, humid habitats but avoids muddy areas due to low burrowing capacity of these habitats. Region Throughout the South American mainland and North of Argentina. Red-footed Tortoise 3 Geocheloidis carbonaria Reproduction – Polygynous (having more than one female as a mate at a time).
    [Show full text]
  • New Behavioral Insights Into Home Range Orientation of the House Mouse (Mus Musculus) by Blythe E
    New Behavioral Insights Into Home Range Orientation of the House Mouse (Mus musculus) By Blythe E. Alexander Submitted to the graduate degree program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology and the Graduate Faculty of the University of Kansas in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. ________________________________ Rudolf Jander ________________________________ Helen M. Alexander ________________________________ Kenneth B. Armitage ________________________________ Linda Trueb ________________________________ Stephen Egbert Date Defended: May 4, 2011 The Dissertation Committee for Blythe Alexander certifies that this is the approved version of the following dissertation: New Behavioral Insights Into Home Range Orientation of the House Mouse (Mus musculus) ________________________________ Rudolf Jander Date approved: ii Abstract Home‐range orientation is a necessity for an animal that maintains an area of daily activity. The ability to navigate efficiently among goals not perceived at the starting point requires the animal to rely on place recognition and vector knowledge. These two components of navigation allow the animal to dynamically update its current position and link that position with the locomotor distance and direction needed to reach a goal. In order to use place knowledge and vector knowledge the animal must learn and remember relevant spatial information obtained from the environment and from internal feedback. The research in this dissertation focuses on behavioral components of topographic orientation, using the house mouse as a model species. Specifically, this research made important discoveries in three main areas: 1) locomotor exploration behavior, 2) the use of learned spatial information for compass orientation, and 3) testable hypotheses based on the controversial cognitive map. In Chapter 1, I used a radial arm maze to find a systematic locomotor component to exploration behavior, which is typically described as random movement.
    [Show full text]
  • Sceloporus Jarrovii)By Chiggers and Malaria in the Chiricahua Mountains, Arizona
    THE SOUTHWESTERN NATURALIST 54(2):204–207 JUNE 2009 NOTE INFECTION OF YARROW’S SPINY LIZARDS (SCELOPORUS JARROVII)BY CHIGGERS AND MALARIA IN THE CHIRICAHUA MOUNTAINS, ARIZONA GRE´ GORY BULTE´ ,* ALANA C. PLUMMER,ANNE THIBAUDEAU, AND GABRIEL BLOUIN-DEMERS Department of Biology, University of Ottawa, 30 Marie-Curie, Ottawa, ON K1N 6N5, Canada *Correspondent: [email protected] ABSTRACT—We measured prevalence of malaria infection and prevalence and intensity of chigger infection in Yarrow’s spiny lizards (Sceloporus jarrovii) from three sites in the Chiricahua Mountains of southeastern Arizona. Our primary objective was to compare parasite load among sites, sexes, and reproductive classes. We also compared our findings to those of previous studies on malaria and chiggers in S. jarrovii from the same area. Of lizards examined, 85 and 93% were infected by malaria and chiggers, respectively. Prevalence of malaria was two times higher than previously reported for the same area, while prevalence of chiggers was similar to previous findings. Intensity of chigger infection was variable among sites, but not among reproductive classes. The site with the highest intensity of chigger infection also had the most vegetative cover, suggesting that this habitat was more favorable for non- parasitic adult chiggers. RESUMEN—Medimos la frecuencia de infeccio´n por malaria y la frecuencia e intensidad de infeccio´n por a´caros en la lagartija espinosa Sceloporus jarrovii de tres sitios en las montan˜as Chiricahua del sureste de Arizona. Nuestro objetivo principal fue comparar la carga de para´sitos entre sitios, sexos y clases reproductivas. Adicionalmente comparamos nuestros hallazgos con estudios previos sobre malaria y a´caros para esta especie en la misma a´rea.
    [Show full text]
  • Home Range Estimates, Variability, and Sample Size Marc Bekoff University of Colorado
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln USGS Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center Wildlife Damage Management, Internet Center for 1984 Simulation analyses of space use: Home range estimates, variability, and sample size Marc Bekoff University of Colorado L. David Mech USGS Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/usgsnpwrc Part of the Animal Sciences Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Biodiversity Commons, Environmental Policy Commons, Recreation, Parks and Tourism Administration Commons, and the Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecology Commons Bekoff, Marc and Mech, L. David, "Simulation analyses of space use: Home range estimates, variability, and sample size" (1984). USGS Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center. 371. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/usgsnpwrc/371 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Wildlife Damage Management, Internet Center for at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in USGS Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 1984, Vol. 16 (1), 32-37 COMPUTER SIMULATION Simulation analyses of space use: Home range estimates, variability, and sample size MARC BEKOFF University ofColorado, Boulder, Colorado and L. DAVID MECH Patuxent Wildlife Research Center, U. SFish and Wildlife Service, Laurel, Maryland Simulations of space use by animals were run to determine the relationship among home range area estimates, variability, and sample size (number of locations). As sample size in­ creased, home range size increased asymptotically, whereas variability decreased among mean home range area estimates generated by multiple simulations for the same sample size.
    [Show full text]
  • The San Pedro River in Southeastern Arizona
    Lowland Riparian Herpetofaunas: The San Pedro River in Southeastern Arizona Philip C. Rosen School of Natural Resources, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ Abstract—Previous work has shown that southeastern Arizona has a characteristic, high diversity lowland riparian herpetofauna with 62-68 or more species along major stream corridors, and 46-54 species in shorter reaches within single biomes, based on intensive fieldwork and museum record surveys. The San Pedro River supports this characteristic herpetofauna, at least some of which still occurs in the lower basin within the Sonoran Desert. It has about 64 species (55 vouchered to date), with 48-53 species within each of three somewhat ecologically homogeneous portions of the basin. This assemblage is more similar to other lowland herpetofaunas than to an example of a canyon riparian herpetofauna. Most of the characteristic riparian species are not known to be abundant along the San Pedro, and some expected species are apparently absent, suggesting that the herpetofauna may have not yet recovered from the history of grassland, cienega, and bottomland degradation. effort has been most focused in the upper basin, and it is dif- Introduction ficult to entirely separate riparian and non-riparian records, so Remarkably, the riparian herpetofauna of southeastern I have summarized the latter for the upper basin; in the lower Arizona has not been accurately described between Ruthven’s reaches, so little collecting has been done away from the river (1907) and Van Denburgh and Slevin’s (1913) annotations that this was not possible. Museum records were excluded if and records for the Santa Cruz River riparian at Tucson and localities could not be located to an adequate precision, but I the present.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Umwelten in a Changing World
    Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Tartu Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Animal umwelten in a changing world: Zoosemiotic perspectives represents a clear and concise review of zoosemiotics, present- ing theories, models and methods, and providing interesting examples of human–animal interactions. The reader is invited to explore the umwelten of animals in a successful attempt to retrieve the relationship of people with animals: a cornerstone of the past common evolutionary processes. The twelve chapters, which cover recent developments in zoosemiotics and much more, inspire the reader to think about the human condition and about ways to recover our lost contact with the animal world. Written in a clear, concise style, this collection of articles creates a wonderful bridge between Timo Maran, Morten Tønnessen, human and animal worlds. It represents a holistic approach Kristin Armstrong Oma, rich with suggestions for how to educate people to face the dynamic relationships with nature within the conceptual Laura Kiiroja, Riin Magnus, framework of the umwelt, providing stimulus and opportuni- Nelly Mäekivi, Silver Rattasepp, ties to develop new studies in zoosemiotics. Professor Almo Farina, CHANGING WORLD A IN UMWELTEN ANIMAL Paul Thibault, Kadri Tüür University of Urbino “Carlo Bo” This important book offers the first coherent gathering of perspectives on the way animals are communicating with each ANIMAL UMWELTEN other and with us as environmental change requires increasing adaptation. Produced by a young generation of zoosemiotics scholars engaged in international research programs at Tartu, IN A CHANGING this work introduces an exciting research field linking the biological sciences with the humanities. Its key premises are that all animals participate in a dynamic web of meanings WORLD: and signs in their own distinctive styles, and all animal spe- cies have distinctive cultures.
    [Show full text]
  • REPTILIA: SQUAMATA: PHRYNOSOMATIDAE Sceloporus Shannonorum
    813.1 REPTILIA: SQUAMATA: PHRYNOSOMATIDAE Sceloporus shannonorum Catalogue of American Amphibians and Reptiles. Smith, H.M., E.A. Liner, P. Ponce-Campos, and D. Chiszar. 2006. Sceloporus shannonorum. Sceloporus shannonorum Langebartel Shannon's Spiny Lizard Sceloporus heterolepis Boulenger 1894:724 (part; the syntype British Museum (Natural History) (BMNH) 92.2.8.30 from Rancho La Berberia, Sierra de BolaRos, Jalisco, Mexico). Sceloporus shannonorum Langebartel 1959:25. Type-locality, "37 miles by road from Concordia, Sinaloa, near the Durango-Sinaloa border". Holo- Figure 1. Sceloporus shannonorum from 0.6 km W. type, University of Illinois Museum of Natural His- of Revolcadores, Durango, Mexico. Photograph cour- tory (UIMNH) 43060, adult male, collected by J. tesy of Robert G. Webb. Schaffner, 2 September 1957 (examined by HMS). Sceloporus heterolepis shannonorum: Webb 1969: 305. Sceloporus shannorum: Gilboa 1974:107 (incorrect subsequent spelling). CONTENT. No subspecies are recognized. DEFINITION. A medium-sized species, maximum SVL 78 mm in males, 73 mm in females: at least the lateral scales irregular in size, small ones scattered among larger scales; large dorsolateral nuchal scales sharply differentiated from small lateral nuchal scales, the outer row forming a prominent lateral nuc- ha1 fringe and tuft; dorsals 41-49, mean 45.3; lateral Figure 2. Habitat of Sceloporus shannonorum in scale rows oblique, posteriorly converging dorsally Durango, Mexico. Photograph courtesy of Robert G. almost to midline; femoral pores 13-1 9, mean 15.7; Webb. head scales more or less normal for the genus; two canthals; supraoculars in two rows, scales of medial areas of tiny scales separate two paravertebral rows row much the larger.
    [Show full text]
  • Gastrointestinal Helminths of the Crevice Spiny Lizard, Sceloporus Poinsettii (Phrynosomatidae)
    OF WASHINGTON, VOLUME 60, NUMBER 2, JULY 1993 263 Chandler, A. C., and R. Rausch. 1947. A study of book of North American Birds. Vol. 4. Yale Uni- strigeids from owls in north central United States. versity Press, New Haven, Connecticut. Transactions of the American Microscopical So- Newsom, I. E., and E. N. Stout. 1933. Proventriculitis ciety 66:283-292. in chickens due to flukes. Veterinary Medicine 28: Dubois, G., and R. Rausch. 1948. Seconde contri- 462^63. bution a 1'etude des-strigeides—(Trematode) Nord- Pence, D. B., J. M. Aho, A. O. Bush, A. G. Canaris, Americains. Societe Neuchateloise des Sciences J. A. Conti, W. R. Davidson, T. A. Dick, G. W. Natureles 71:29-61. Esch, T. Goater, W. Fitzpatrick, D. J. Forrester, , and . 1950a. A contribution to the J. C. Holmes, W. M. Samuel, J. M. Kinsella, J. study of North American strigeoids (Trematoda). Moore, R. L. Rausch, W. Threfall, and T. A. American Midland Naturalist 43:1-31. Wheeler. 1988. In Letters to the editor. Journal , and . 1950b. Troisieme contribution of Parasitology 74:197-198. al'etude des strigeides. (Trematode) Nord-Amer- Ramalingam, S., and W. M. Samuel. 1978. Hel- icains. Societe Neuchateloise des Sciences Natu- minths in the great horned owl. Bubo virginianus, reles 73:19-50. and snowy owl, Nyctea scandiaca, of Alberta. Ca- Morgan, B. B. 1943. The Physalopterinae (Nema- nadian Journal of Zoology 56:2454-2456. toda) of Aves. Transactions of the American Mi- Rausch, R. 1948. Observations on cestodes in North croscopical Society 62:72-80. American owls with the description of Choano- .
    [Show full text]