Aberle, D. F., 105 Accumulator Counting Mechanism, 265

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Aberle, D. F., 105 Accumulator Counting Mechanism, 265 Index Aberle, D. F., 105 Aristotle, 130 Accumulator counting mechanism, 265 Armstrong, W., 19, 29 Adaptation, 7, 18 Art objects, 59 conflict resolution and, 82 Ashby, F. G., 268 human uniqueness and, 204–214 Assemblies Addition, 255–256 attention and, 193–197 Adelson, E. H., 194 behavior and, 192–193 Africa, 56–58, 112 response synchronization and, 190–197 Agriculture, 7–8, 181 Attention, 193–197 Agta people, 91 Auditory cortex, 185 Aiello, L. C., 16, 59, 69 Aunger, R., 9 Albazo study, 148–152, 156–159 Aureli, F., 82 Albright, T. D., 194 Austad, S. N., 70 Alcohol, 12 Australia, 58 Alemu, Abesha, 149–150 Austronesian languages, 19, 24–25 Alexander, R. D., 89, 110, 114, 220 Autism, 211 Algebra, 248 Auto-domestication, 6–7 Allman, J., 70 Axelrod, R., 114 Altruism, 94–96 cooperation and, 105–130 Bacteria, 46 egalitarian syndrome and, 87–92 Baillargeon, R., 226 helping another in pain, 235–239 Balakrishnan, J. D., 268 morals and, 85–86 (see also Morals) Baldwin effect, 5–6 Amino acids. See Genetics Balikci, A., 84 Anatomically modern humans (AMH), 57–59 Barkow, J. H., 12, 71 Anderson, Linda, 227, 229 Baron-Cohen, S., 211 Andrews, P., 56 Barracuda, 28 Anthropologists, 55–56, 120 Barth, Fredrik, 126, 152 Ants, 11–12, 107 Barton, M., 169 Aoki, K., 33 Bechara, A., 229 Arabic enumeration, 209 Becker, G., 106 282 Index Bees, 107 Binmore, K. G., 114, 116–117 Behavior, xi–xii, xvi Binocular rivalry, 193–197 affiliative, 204 Bipedal gait, 181 alcohol and, 12 Birds, 10, 251, 256–257 assemblies and, 192–193 Bismark Islands, 24, 26 Baldwin effect and, 5–6 Blade technology, 57 cheaters and, 36, 85, 90–91, 114, 177 Blakemore, S., 176 cognitive causal chains (CCCs) and, 152–164 Blurton-Jones, N., 90, 241 (see also Cognition) Boehm, Christopher, 6, 11, 35, 79–103, 279 cooperative, 105–130, 235–241 Boesch, C., 206 dialects and, 177–178 Borges, Jorge Luis, 141 dominance and, 204 Bougainville Islands, 24 egalitarian syndrome and, 86–92 Bowles, S., 89, 91–92 functionalism and, 105–106 Boyd, Robert, 3, 6, 35, 176, 279 helping another in pain, 235–239 on cooperation, 105–132 infantile processing and, 209–214 on evolutionary quantum leaps, 54 inhibitions and, 11–12, 221–225, 241–242 on natural selection, 79, 84, 92, 94 metarepresentations and, 183–190 Boysen, S. T., 230, 232, 255 morals and, 11, 82–96, 219–245 (see also Brain. See also Cognition Morals) ancestral comparisons and, 182 mystical rituals and, 148–152, 156–159 assembly coding and, 190–197 natural selection and, 79–103 (see also binocular rivalry and, 193–197 Natural selection) conscience and, 85 numerosity and, 247–271 cortex, 11, 169–172, 185–187, 193–197 passion and, 221–225 Decade of the Brain and, 203 political, 81–96 encephalization quotient and, 62, 64 preconditional patterns and, 60–66 fossil studies and, 181 perseveration and, 225–234 human/primate gulf and, 181–182 primates and, 79–80 infant abilities and, 182 reciprocity and, 114–117, 120–125 inhibitory response and, 221–225, 241–242 response synchronization and, 192–193 (see also Morals) sanctioning of, 83–86, 94–97 language and, 11, 13–16 (see also Language) selfish, 7, 45–51, 82–97, 105–130, 135–137 mathematics and, 251–255 (see also sociobiology and, 107–109 Mathematics) tradition and, 55, 276–277 memory and, 193–197, 252–253 Beise, J., 7 modular specificity and, 12 Berger, Michelle, 241 motor skills and, 9–10, 15, 182–183, Berlin, B., 13–15 190–197 Bernstein, Leonard, 143 natural selection and, 11 Berntson, G. G., 255 neural substrates and, 169–178 Beynon, A. D., 62 preconditional patterns and, 60–66 “Big Mistake” hypothesis, 110–111 predisposition and, 15–16 Binary systems, 252 primary visual area and, 170–172 Index 283 as repository, 12 Church, Russ, 235–236, 252–254, 256 representational structure and, 187–190 Cladistic character, 24–25 size and, 11–12, 16, 53–54, 61–62, 70, Clothing, 5–6, 13, 34 169–172 Clutton-Brock, T. H., 62 storage and, 184 Coevolution, 13–15 symbolic thought and, 55, 57 Cognition, 12–16 theory of mind and, 11, 34, 172–178 assembly coding and, 190–197 Brannon, E. M., 256, 258 attention and, 193–197 Bride price, 118 binding mechanism and, 184 Bromage, T. G., 62 binocular rivalry and, 193–197 Brooks, A. S., 57, 69 biological adaptation and, 204–214 Brown, P., 9, 12, 211 cognitive causal chains (CCCs) and, 152–164 Buka, 24 computation and, 184–185 Bunn, H. T., 71 cortical processing and, 185–187 Burial, 59 cultural cognitive causal chains (CCCCs) Byrne, R., 71, 174, 204 and, 157–161, 163 cultural triggers and, 182–183 Call, J., 204, 206, 214 effector systems and, 184–185 Campbell, Donald T., 79, 85, 92–93, 95 human uniqueness and, 203–217 Cannibalism, 29 imagination and, 183 Carey, Susan, 225, 229, 266, 268, 270 infants and, 14–15, 175–176, 209–214 Carpenter, M., 210 invisible displacement task and, 226–228 Carroll, R. L., 49 learning and, 54 Cartesian coordinate system, 209 metarepresentations and, 183–190 Case systems, 26–27 modality and, 184–185 Cashdan, E. A., 79, 81 morals and, 219–245 (see also Morals) Castelo-Branco, M., 192, 194 motor skills and, 9–10, 15, 182–183, Catania, A. C., 258 190–197 Caterpillars, 31–32 neural substrates and, 169–178 Causality, 147 numerosity and, 247–271 (see also cognitive causal chains (CCCs) and, 152–164 Mathematics) cultural cognitive causal chains (CCCCs) preconditional patterns and, 60–66 and, 157–161, 163 perseveration and, 225–234 mystic rituals and, 148–152, 156–159 representational structure and, 187–190 Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., 2–3, 54, 79, 123, 136, response synchronization and, 190–197 147 social complexity and, 204 Changeaux, J.-P., 11 spike timing and, 192 Cheaters, 36, 85, 90–91, 114, 177 symbolic thought and, 55, 57 Cheney, D. L., 114 theory of mind and, 11, 34, 172–178 Children. See Humans wild child and, 203–204 China, 17 Colby, A., 230 Chloroplasts, 5 Coleman, J., 106 Chomsky, N., 13, 220 Color, 13–15 284 Index Combes, Claude, 4–5, 45–52, 279 brain and, 11–12, 181–201 (see also Brain) Competition, 93–94 burial, 59 Complexity, 2, 17, 20–22 cheaters and, 36, 85, 90–91, 114, 177 language and, 204–214 chimpanzee tyranny and, 53–54 learning and, 54 cladistic character and, 24–25 social, 54–55 cognitive causal chains (CCCs) and, 152–164 Conflict resolution, 82 (see also Cognition) Conscience, 85 complexity and, 2, 17 Cook, James, 32 conflict resolution and, 82 Cooperation, 130 conscience and, 85 biased cultural transmission and, 118–119 crimes and, 85 “Big Mistake” hypothesis and, 110–111 cumulative nature and, 18, 27–29 extinction rates and, 125–129 Darwinism and, 133–137 functionalism and, 105–106 defining, 10, 54 group selection and, 117–129 design and, 20–22 “Heart on Your Sleeve” hypothesis and, diffusion and, 17, 25–27 109–110 drift and, 119, 123–124 kinship and, 107–108 egalitarian syndrome and, 86–92 lack of consensus on, 105–117 ethnicity and, 69 manipulation hypotheses and, 112–114 family and, 86–92 migration and, 119 feedback and, 2–7, 30–33 morals and, 114–117, 120–125, 235–241 functionless elements and, 29–30 punishment and, 114–117, 120–122 group selection and, 30–33 rates of change and, 127–129 helping another in pain, 235–239 sociobiology and, 107–109 histories and, 206–209 symbolic boundaries and, 128–129 horizontal transmission and, 17, 25–27 variation and, 122–125 human diversity and, 60–61 Cordes, Sara, 11, 247–274, 279 human uniqueness and, 203–217 Cords, M., 82 kinship system and, 6, 21–23, 35, 105–130 Cortex lack of animal, 275–278 auditory, 185 language and, 11–15 (see also Language) binocular rivalry and, 193–197 learning and, 54 brain size and, 169–172 lifespan effects and, 70–72 primary visual area and, 170–172 mathematics and, 247–274 (see also processing mechanisms of, 185–187 Mathematics) visual, 185, 193–197 memes and, 8–9 Cosmides, L., 110, 177 migration and, 119 Couvet, D., 46 morals and, 82–96, 219–245 (see also Morals) Cree people, 92 multiplicity and, 17, 22–24 Crimes, 85 mutualism and, 45–51 Culture, xii–xiii naturalistic approach to, 147–165 acultural issues and, 53–54 natural selection and, 5, 79–103 biased transmission and, 118–119 preconditional patterns and, 60–66 Index 285 predisposition and, 15–16 Dawkins, R., 3, 7–8, 48, 137–139, 147, 235 properties of, 17–33 Deacon, T., 5–6, 12, 57, 69, 87 punishment and, 114–117, 120–122 Dean, M. C., 62 purpose of, 2–16 Decade of the Brain, 203 ratchet, 2, 10, 34–35, 205–206, 209 Decimal system, 25 rates of change and, 127–129 Dehaene, S., 251 reductionism and, 13–15 Dennett, Daniel, 2, 7–8, 16, 133–145, 279 religion and, 13, 31, 110, 122–123, 134, Deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) 147–152, 156–159, 173, 276 memes and, 8–9 replicators and, 137–143 mutualism and, 48–49 Rossel Island and, 18–33, 37nn11,12 replicators and, 137–143 selective conditions for, 69–72 Descartes, Rene, 209 sorcery and, 29–30 Kuhnian error and, 229–234 speculation and, 1 perseveration, 225, 229–234 system complexity and, 20–22 Descent with modification, 3 tools and, 26–27 (see also Tools) Design, 18 tradition and, 55 brain and, 11 triggers of, 182–183 complexity and, 2, 17, 20–22 twin-track theories and, 2–10 feedback and, 2–7 variation and, 12–13, 22–24, 54 hand and, 5, 11, 36n5 vertical transmission and, 17, 24–25 twin-track theories and, 2–4 wares of, 133–134 Developmentalism, 221 weaning and, 7–8 de Waal, Frans B.
Recommended publications
  • Culture in Humans and Other Animals
    Biol Philos (2013) 28:457–479 DOI 10.1007/s10539-012-9347-x Culture in humans and other animals Grant Ramsey Received: 12 March 2012 / Accepted: 30 September 2012 / Published online: 16 October 2012 Ó Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2012 Abstract The study of animal culture is a flourishing field, with culture being recorded in a wide range of taxa, including non-human primates, birds, cetaceans, and rodents. In spite of this research, however, the concept of culture itself remains elusive. There is no universally assented to concept of culture, and there is debate over the connection between culture and related concepts like tradition and social learning. Furthermore, it is not clear whether culture in humans and culture in non- human animals is really the same thing, or merely loose analogues that go by the same name. The purpose of this paper is to explicate core desiderata for a concept of culture and then to construct a concept that meets these desiderata. The paper then applies this concept in both humans and non-human animals. Keywords Behavior Á Culture Á Epigenetic Á Evolution Á Innovation Á Social learning Á Tradition Introduction The study of culture in animals1 is a burgeoning area of research. Biologists, psychologists, and biological anthropologists are increasingly interested in the study of culture and are routinely describing the behavior of animals—from rats to sperm whales—in terms of culture (Laland and Galef 2009). Additionally, the field of 1 To avoid repeated uses of ‘non-human’, I will use ‘animal’ in what follows, not as picking out the Anamalia, but as denoting all non-human animals.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Culture Consequences of Sociality What Is Culture? What Is Culture? Cultural Transmission
    Animal culture Consequences of sociality Animals are exposed to behavior, sometimes novel, of others Do animals display culture? What is culture? “The totality of the mental and physical reactions and activities that characterize the behavior of individuals composing a social group collectively and individually in relations to their natural environment, to other groups, to members of the group itself and of each individual to himself” - Franz Boas (1911) “An extrasomatic (nongenetic, nonbodily), temporal continuum of things and events dependent upon symboling. Culture consists of tools, implements, utensils, clothing, ornaments, customs, institutions, beliefs, rituals, games, works of art, language, etc.” - Leslie White (~1949) What is culture? Cultural transmission “Patterns, explicit and implicit, of and for behavior acquired May occur via copying… …or via direct instruction and transmitted by symbols, constituting the distinctive achievement of human groups” - Kroeber and Kluckhohn (1952) “Learned systems of meaning, communicated by means of natural language and other symbol systems, having representational, directive, and affective functions, and capable of creating cultural entities and particular senses of reality” - Roy D'Andrade (~1984) Tool use in chimpanzees “The universal human capacity to classify, codify and communicate their experiences symbolically…a defining feature of the genus Homo” - Wikipedia (2006) Culturally transmitted behavior must persist beyond life of originator 1 Cultural transmission in macaques Cultural transmission
    [Show full text]
  • The Question of Animal Culture
    THE QUESTION OF ANIMAL CULTURE Bennett G. Galef, Jr. McMaster University In this paper I consider whether traditional behaviors of animals, like traditions of humans, are transmitted by imitation learning. Review of the literature on problem solving by captive primates, and detailed consideration of two widely cited instances of purported learning by imitation and of culture in free-living primates (sweet-potato washing by Japanese macaques and termite fishing by chimpanzees), suggests that nonhuman primates do not learn to solve problems by imitation. It may, therefore, be misleading to treat animal traditions and human culture as homologous (rather than analogous) and to refer to animal traditions as cultural. KEYWORDS:Animal tradition; Animal culture; Imitation and learning; Koshima Island (Japan); Gombe National Park (Tanzania); Nonhuman primates "Imitation is natural to man from childhood, one of his advantages over the lower ani- mals being this, that he is the most imitative creature in the world, and learns at first by imitation." (Aristotle 1941:4486) Received August 28, 1991; accepted October 28, 1991. Address all correspondence to Bennett G. Galef. Jr., Department of Psychology, McMaster Univer- sity, Hamilton, Ontario L85 4Kl, Canada. Copyright 10 1992 by Walter de Gruyter, Inc. New York Human Nature, Vol. 3, No.2, pp, 157-178. 1045-6767/92/$1.00 + ,10 157 The Question of Animal Culture 159 158 Human Nature, Vol. 3, No.2, 1992 It has become increasingly fashionable during the past decade for scien- PROBLEMS OF DEFINITION tists to see evidence of humanlike mental processes in animals as diverse as chickens and chimpanzees. The possibility that animals may be con- Lumpers and Splitters scious, deceitful, self-aware, Machiavellian, etc., is under serious con- sideration.
    [Show full text]
  • Van-Deurs-MA-Thesis.Pdf
    Nonhuman Mammalian Cultural Behaviors and the Animal Cultures Debate By Christian Van Deurs Queens College Department of Biology In fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Spring 2017 Master’s Committee: Advisor: David Lahti Mitchell Baker Larissa Swedell Abstract Although humans are a uniquely cultural species, many animal species have the ability to learn socially as individuals. Of these, a few have exhibited social learning that cascades through a population in a way that could be considered cultural. Chimpanzees, orangutans, humpback whales, and dolphins display clear socially learned behaviors that are practiced on a large scale, with geographical variation, and with no apparent genetic or environmental explanation for these differences. These behaviors have been described as cultural behaviors, despite the lack of a universally accepted definition of culture. An understanding of nonhuman mammalian cultural behaviors and their evolution may guide future research on the varieties and extent of mammalian culture. Introduction Currently there is no clear consensus on culture in nonhuman animals. The question of nonhuman animal culture is not a new one, and the debate has changed in several ways since its inception. Arguments against nonhuman culture were initially based upon a lack of evidence for socially learned behavior, and differences between human and animal cognitive processing. Modern arguments are characterized by disagreements over the definition of culture, mechanisms of transmission, and the question of whether human and other animal cultures are homologous or analogous. In this review, I discuss the currently contentious topics in the animal cultures debate, and describe the best documented examples of cultural behaviors of nonhuman mammal species that have assisted in the recent shift towards acceptance of socially learned and maintained behavioral transmission in nonhuman mammals.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and Conservation of Non-Humans with Reference to Whales and Dolphins: Review and New Directions
    BIOLOGICAL CONSERVATION Biological Conservation 120 (2004) 431–441 www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon Culture and conservation of non-humans with reference to whales and dolphins: review and new directions Hal Whitehead a,*, Luke Rendell b, Richard W. Osborne c, Bernd Wursig€ d a Department of Biology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H J1 b Gatty Marine Laboratory, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St. Andrews, Fife KY16 8LB, UK c The Whale Museum, P.O. Box 945, 62 First Street North, Friday Harbor, WA 98250, USA d Texas A&M University at Galveston, 4700 Avenue U, Building 303, Galveston, TX 77551-5923, USA Received 10 November 2003; received in revised form 4 March 2004; accepted 25 March 2004 Abstract There is increasing evidence that culture is an important determinant of behavior in some non-human species including great apes and cetaceans (whales and dolphins). In some cases, there may be repercussions for population biology and conservation. Rapidly evolving ‘‘horizontal’’ cultures, transmitted largely within generations, may help animals deal with anthropogenic change and even allow them to exploit it, sometimes with negative consequences for both the animals and humans. In contrast, stable ‘‘vertical’’ or ‘‘oblique’’ cultures, transmitted principally between generations, may impede adaptation to environmental change, and confound range recovery, reintroductions and translocations. Conformist stable cultures can lead to maladaptive behavior, which may be mistaken for the results of anthropogenic threats. They can also structure populations into sympatric sub-populations with dis- tinctive cultural variants. Such structuring is common among cetaceans, among which sympatric sub-populations may face different anthropogenic threats or respond to the same threat in different ways.
    [Show full text]
  • CULTURAL TRANSMISSION a View from Chimpanzees and Human Infants
    JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY Tomasello / CULTURAL TRANSMISSION Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The key adaptation is one that enables individuals to understand other individuals as inten- tional agents like the self. This species-unique form of social cognition emerges in human ontogeny at around 1 year of age as infants begin to engage with other persons in various kinds of joint attentional activi- ties involving gaze following, social referencing, and gestural communication. Young children’s joint attentional skills then engender some uniquely powerful forms of cultural learning, enabling the acquisition of language, discourse skills, tool use practices, and many other conventional activities. These novel forms of cultural learning allow human beings to pool their cognitive resources both contemporaneously and over historical time in ways that are unique in the animal kingdom. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION A View From Chimpanzees and Human Infants MICHAEL TOMASELLO Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Germany Primates are highly social beings. They begin their lives clinging to their mothers and nurs- ing, and they spend their next few months, or even years, still in close proximity to her. Adult primates live in close-knit social groups, for the most part, in which members individually recognize one another and form various types of long-term social relationships (see Tomasello & Call, 1994, 1997, for reviews). As primates, human beings follow this same pattern, of course, but they also have some unique forms of sociality that may be character- ized as “ultrasocial” or, in more common parlance, cultural (Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 1993).
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Calving Interval Alfonso Guillermo Flores Iowa State University
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 1971 A study of calving interval Alfonso Guillermo Flores Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Agriculture Commons, and the Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Flores, Alfonso Guillermo, "A study of calving interval " (1971). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 4452. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/4452 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 72-5200 FLORES, Alfonso Guillermo, 1929- A STUDY OF CALVING INTERVAL. Iowa State University, Ph.D., 1971 Agriculture, animal culture University Microfilms, A XEROX Company, Ann Arbor, Michigan THIS DISSERTATION HAS BEEN MICROFILMED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED A Study of calving interval by Alfonso Guillermo Flores A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Major Subject: Animal Science (Animal Breeding) Approved: Signature was redacted for privacy. In Charge of Major Work Signature was redacted for privacy. For the Major^Department Signature was redacted for privacy. Iowa State University Ames, Iowa 1971 PLEASE NOTE: Some Pages have Indistinct print. Filmed as received. UNIVERSITY MICROFILMS TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION REVIEW OF LITERATURE Fertility Traits in General Calving Interval Postpartum Interval Conception Length Gestation Length Summary of the Reviewed Literature SOURCE OF DATA Source of Data METHODS OF ANALYSIS RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Analysis of Proportions Analyses of Reproductive Traits Analysis I Analysis II SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS LITERATURE CITED ACKNOWLEDGMENTS iii LIST OF TABLES Page Table 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Ontology and Anthropology of Interanimality: Merleau-Ponty From
    AIBR Ontology and anthropology of interanimality: Revista de Antropología Iberoamericana Merleau-Ponty from Tim Ingold's perspective www.aibr.org VOLUMEN 5 Ana Cristina Ramírez Barreto NÚMERO 1 Michoacan University of Saint Nicholas Hidalgo Enero - Abril 2010 Pp. 32 - 57 Received: 11.04.2009 Madrid: Antropólogos Accepted: 25.11.2009 Iberoamericanos en Red. ISSN: 1695-9752 DOI: 10.11156/aibr.050103e E-ISSN: 1695-9752 82 ONTOLOGY AND ANTHROPOLOGY OF INTERANIMALITY ABSTRACT: This essay explores Tim Ingold’s anthropological theory following his references to Merleau-Ponty and the concept of interanimality/interagentivity. It poses some ideas of Ingold’s “poetics of dwelling”, which he highlights from ethnographies of hunter-gatherer peoples, and how these ideas are linked to an ontological consideration which does not dissociate body and person, body and mind, nature and culture, animality and humanity. The paper reviews animal literature in Merleau-Ponty’s philosophy, and Ingold’s critique of “Anthropology of the senses”. It also gives critical clues for the ethical and political implications of this ontology. KEY WORDS: Ingold, Merleau-Ponty, interanimality, interagentivity, perception. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS: This piece of work was possible thanks to the support of the Center for Scientific Research at the Michoacan University of Saint Nicholas of Hidalgo, project 13.8, 2008. A pre- vious version of this manuscript was read in the “Merlaeu-Ponty Alive” International Colloquium (Morelia, 2008). I’m thankful for the comments, criticisms and suggestions of Mauro Carbone, Jaime Vieyra and two of AIBR’s evaluators. My sabbatical leave at the University of California in Santa Cruz enabled me to review and correct the manuscript, for which I would like to thank Olga Nájera-Ramírez and Gabriela Arredondo (teachers of Anthropology at the Chicano/Latino Research Center), as well as the CONACYT, for supporting my project “Charrería, power relations and popular culture: the context from another point of view”.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Culture and Language
    The ITB Journal Volume 9 Issue 1 Article 7 2008 Understanding Culture and Language Mario R.J. Corbin Follow this and additional works at: https://arrow.tudublin.ie/itbj Part of the Anthropology Commons Recommended Citation Corbin, Mario R.J. (2008) "Understanding Culture and Language," The ITB Journal: Vol. 9: Iss. 1, Article 7. doi:10.21427/D7115R Available at: https://arrow.tudublin.ie/itbj/vol9/iss1/7 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Ceased publication at ARROW@TU Dublin. It has been accepted for inclusion in The ITB Journal by an authorized administrator of ARROW@TU Dublin. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-Share Alike 4.0 License ITB Journal Understanding Culture and Language Mario R.J. Corbin Department of Humanities Institute of Technology Blanchardstown “...looking across at our primate relatives learning local traditions, using tools, and manipulating symbols, we can no longer say comfortably that ‘culture’ is the heritage of learned symbolic behaviour that makes humans human.” R. Keesing (1974: 73) 1. Expanding Our Understanding of Culture A consequence of human evolution that has had the most profound impact on human nature and human society was the emergence of culture. A term credited uniquely to humans, however over the course of the last century new developments in animal behaviour have been introduced, indicating perhaps that our understanding of culture is too limited, despite there being a vast amount of theoretical approaches to culture. Before we can further attempt to understand whether or not culture can be attributed to animals, we must first closely examine the concept of culture from a human perspective, including a detailed analysis of the role which language plays in maintaining culture.
    [Show full text]
  • Thresholds and Transitions in Hominin Cultural Evolution
    philosophies Article The Encultured Primate: Thresholds and Transitions in Hominin Cultural Evolution Chris Buskes Department of Philosophy, Radboud University, 6500 HD Nijmegen, The Netherlands; [email protected] Received: 30 November 2018; Accepted: 28 January 2019; Published: 1 February 2019 Abstract: This article tries to shed light on the mystery of human culture. Human beings are the only extant species with cumulative, evolving cultures. Many animal species do have cultural traditions in the form of socially transmitted practices but they typically lack cumulative culture. Why is that? This discrepancy between humans and animals is even more puzzling if one realizes that culture seems highly advantageous. Thanks to their accumulated knowledge and techniques our early ancestors were able to leave their cradle in Africa and swarm out across the planet, thereby adjusting themselves to a whole range of new environments. Without culture this would have been impossible. So we may ask once again: if cumulative culture is so useful, why don’t other animals have it? In order to explain this mystery I won’t appeal to the major transitions in human evolution—like walking upright, crafting stone tools and controlling fire, etc.—because that would be question begging. Instead I try to unearth the mechanisms that caused those evolutionary turning points to occur in the first place. It seems that unlike other animals, humans are predisposed to efficiently acquire, store and transmit cultural information in such ways that our cultures can genuinely evolve. Keywords: cultural evolution; cumulative culture; gene–culture coevolution; dual inheritance; universal Darwinism; memetics 1. Introduction Why is Homo sapiens the only species on our planet with cumulative culture? This issue is a profound mystery because it is obvious that culture has many merits.
    [Show full text]
  • The Handbook of Culture and Biology the Handbook of Culture and Biology
    The Handbook of Culture and Biology The Handbook of Culture and Biology Edited by Jose´ M. Causadias Eva H. Telzer Nancy A. Gonzales This edition first published 2018 © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by law. Advice on how to obtain permission to reuse material from this title is available at http://www.wiley.com/go/permissions. The right of Jose´ M. Causadias, Eva H. Telzer and Nancy A. Gonzales to be identified as the authors of the editorial material in this work has been asserted in accordance with law. Registered Office(s) John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, USA Editorial Office 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK For details of our global editorial offices, customer services, and more information about Wiley products visit us at www.wiley.com. Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats and by print-on-demand. Some content that appears in standard print versions of this book may not be available in other formats. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty While the publisher and authors have used their best efforts in preparing this work, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this work and specifically disclaim all warranties, including without limitation any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives, written sales materials or promotional statements for this work.
    [Show full text]
  • The Animal Cultures Debate
    Opinion TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.21 No.10 The animal cultures debate Kevin N. Laland and Vincent M. Janik Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, Bute Building, Queen’s Terrace, St. Andrews, Fife, UK, KY16 9TS Recent interest in animal cultures has been fuelled by relationships among culture-like phenomena in diverse high-profile reports of intra- and interpopulation differ- taxa. A broad definition is likely to be more stimulating ences in the behavioural repertoires of primates and and henceforth we refer to culture (or tradition) as all group- cetaceans, consistent with the existence of socially typical behaviour patterns, shared by members of animal learned traditions. Several studies have mapped spatial communities, that are to some degree reliant on socially differences in behaviour, revealing a mosaic of beha- learned and transmitted information [4]. Here, we criticize vioural phenotypes within species. The dominant cur- the dominant (‘ethnographic’) method for identifying animal rent approach attempts to determine whether this is cultures, which seeks to isolate cultural variation by ruling cultural variation by excluding asocial learning, ecologi- out alternative explanations for behavioural differences, cal or genetic factors. However, claims of animal cultures and instead advocate a more thorough consideration of remain controversial because such comparisons are sub- the interplay between genetics, ecology and culture. ject to weaknesses; thus, new approaches to isolating the influence of culture on behaviour are required. Here Animal cultures we suggest that, rather than attributing behaviour to The idea that animals might acquire components of their explanatory categories, researchers would often be bet- behavioural repertoire by copying others has a long his- ter advised to partition variance in behaviour to alter- tory, dating back to Aristotle.
    [Show full text]