Phonological Conditions on Affixation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Phonological Conditions on Affixation UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics Title Phonological Conditions on Affixation Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7tc6m7jw Author Paster, Mary Publication Date 2006 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Phonological Conditions on Affixation by Mary Elizabeth Paster B.A. (Ohio State University) 2000 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 2002 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Sharon Inkelas, Co-Chair Professor Andrew Garrett, Co-Chair Professor Kristin Hanson Spring 2006 Phonological Conditions on Affixation © 2006 by Mary Elizabeth Paster Abstract Phonological Conditions on Affixation by Mary Elizabeth Paster Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professor Sharon Inkelas and Professor Andrew Garrett, Co-Chairs This dissertation presents results of a cross-linguistic survey of phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy (PCSA). One hundred thirty-seven examples are discussed, representing 67 languages. Three major generalizations emerge from the survey. First, PCSA occurs at the same edge of the stem as the trigger: PCSA in prefixes is triggered at the left edge of the stem, while PCSA in suffixes is triggered at the right edge. Second, PCSA is sensitive to underlying rather than surface forms. This is demonstrated in several examples where phonological processes render opaque the conditions determining allomorph distribution. Finally, despite its characterization in recent literature, PCSA is not always optimizing. In numerous examples, words are no more phonologically well-formed than they would be if there were no allomorphy, or if the distribution of allomorphs were reversed. The generalizations arising in this survey distinguish between competing frameworks for modeling phonological conditions on affixation. The first is the ‘P >> M’ approach, where PCSA is modeled by ranking Phonological constraints over Morphological constraints in Optimality Theory. This model predicts that PCSA should 1 be phonologically optimizing and that allomorphy may be sensitive to phonological conditions anywhere in the word. In the alternative approach, advocated here, PCSA is modeled by incorporating phonological elements of stems into the subcategorization frames of affixes. Subcategorization frames specify the type of stem to which affixes will attach, including syntactic, morphological, and (crucially) phonological features of stems. The distribution of suppletive allomorphs results from different requirements imposed by each allomorph on stems. This approach predicts that allomorphy should be sensitive to input rather than surface phonological elements and that PCSA should be sensitive only to elements at the edge of the stem where the affix attaches. These predictions are upheld in the survey. In the introduction and conclusion, I situate these findings in the broader context of the literature on the phonology-morphology interface. I also consider predictions of the models discussed above for other types of phonological effects in morphology, showing that research in those areas converges with the results presented here. 2 Phonological Conditions on Affixation Table of contents Chapter 1. Introduction ........................................................................................... 1 1.1 Phonological conditions on affixation ............................................................... 3 1.1.1 The P >> M model ....................................................................................... 6 1.1.1.1 Description of the model ....................................................................... 7 1.1.1.2 Predictions for phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy ...... 8 1.1.2 The subcategorization model ....................................................................... 11 1.1.2.1 Description of the model ....................................................................... 11 1.1.2.2 Predictions for phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy ...... 12 1.2 Phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy .......................................... 16 1.2.1 Previous discussions .................................................................................... 17 1.2.2 Survey methodology .................................................................................... 22 Chapter 2. Segmentally conditioned suppletive allomorphy ................................ 25 2.1 Survey results ..................................................................................................... 25 2.1.1 Allomorphy vs. morphophonology .............................................................. 27 2.1.2 Examples ...................................................................................................... 31 2.1.2.1 Assimilation/harmony ............................................................................ 31 2.1.2.2 Dissimilation/disharmony ...................................................................... 39 2.1.2.3 Syllable structure optimization .............................................................. 53 2.1.2.4 Syllable contact constraint satisfaction .................................................. 72 2.1.2.5 Non-optimization ................................................................................... 76 2.1.2.6 Opaque conditioning .............................................................................. 98 2.1.3 Summary ...................................................................................................... 103 2.2 Analysis ............................................................................................................. 104 2.3 Conclusion ......................................................................................................... 109 Chapter 3. Tone/stress conditioned suppletive allomorphy .................................. 111 3.1 Survey results ..................................................................................................... 111 3.1.1 Examples ...................................................................................................... 113 3.1.1.1 Stress effects .......................................................................................... 113 3.1.1.2 Stem allomorphy .................................................................................... 119 3.1.1.3 Tone effects ........................................................................................... 126 3.1.1.4 Non-optimization ................................................................................... 131 3.1.2 Summary ...................................................................................................... 135 3.2 Analysis ............................................................................................................. 136 3.3 Conclusion ......................................................................................................... 139 Chapter 4. Prosodically conditioned suppletive allomorphy ................................ 140 4.1 Survey results ..................................................................................................... 140 4.1.1 Examples ...................................................................................................... 142 4.1.1.1 Foot parsing ........................................................................................... 144 4.1.1.2 Minimality ............................................................................................. 159 i 4.1.1.3 Non-optimization ................................................................................... 165 4.1.2 Summary ...................................................................................................... 172 4.2 Historical development of prosodically conditioned suppletive allomorphy: The case of Pama-Nyungan ergative suffix allomorphy ................................... 174 4.2.1 Examples ...................................................................................................... 176 4.2.2 Previous discussions .................................................................................... 196 4.2.3 A history of PN ergative allomorphy ........................................................... 201 4.3 Analysis of prosodically conditioned suppletive allomorphy ........................... 205 4.3.1 The Output Optimization approach ............................................................. 205 4.3.2 Survey results revisited ................................................................................ 207 4.3.3 Examples of non-optimizing syllable-counting allomorphy (SCA) ............ 208 4.3.4 Examples of SCA possibly resulting from TETU ....................................... 211 4.3.5 The Subcategorization approach .................................................................. 212 4.3.6 Using Subcategorization to model SCA....................................................... 214 4.3.7 Estonian revisited ......................................................................................... 214 4.3.8 Summary of analysis .................................................................................... 216 4.4 Conclusion ........................................................................................................
Recommended publications
  • Tlingit Phonology in a Generative Framework
    TLINGIT PHONOLOGY IN A GENERATIVE FRAMEWORK: AN EXAMINATION OF PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSES AND ABSTRACT REPRESENTATION by GAELAN^/de WOLF B.A., University of British Columbia, 1971 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS In THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES Department of Linguistics We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA August, 1977 GAELAN de WOLF, 1977 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the Head of my Department or by his representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Gaelan de Wolf Department of T.-mpn-i «t-i rs The University of British Columbia 2075 Wesbrook Place Vancouver, Canada V6T 1W5 Date /V? /ff? - ii - ABSTRACT The Saussurean Paradox described by Labov (1971), in which "the social aspect of language can be studied by the theorist asking himself questions, while the individual aspect can only be studied by a social survey", apparently mirrors a predicament occurring in the structuralist and generative models of linguistics. For, while descriptive and structuralist linguistic models seek to mirror the reality of particular languages, a generative model of linguistics, in a search for universals, attempts to discover the underlying reality of all languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Inflectional and Derivational Hebrew Morphology According to the Theory of Phonology As Human Behavior
    BEN- GURION UNIVERSITY OF THE NEGEV FACULTY OF HUMINITIES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES DEPARTMENT OF FOREIGN LITERATURES AND LINGUISTICS INFLECTIONAL AND DERIVATIONAL HEBREW MORPHOLOGY ACCORDING TO THE THEORY OF PHONOLOGY AS HUMAN BEHAVIOR THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS LINA PERELSHTEIN UNDER THE SUPERVISION OF: PROFESSOR YISHAI TOBIN FEBRUARY 2008 BEN- GURION UNIVERSITY OF THE NEGEV FACULTY OF HUMANITIES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES DEPARTMENT OF FOREIGN LITERATURES AND LINGUISTICS INFLECTIONAL AND DERIVATIONAL HEBREW MORPHOLOGY ACCORDING TO THE THEORY OF PHONOLOGY AS HUMAN BEHAVIOR THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS LINA PERELSHTEIN UNDER THE SUPERVISION OF PROFESSOR YISHAI TOBIN Signature of student: ________________ Date: _________ Signature of supervisor: _____________ Date: _________ Signature of chairperson of the committee for graduate studies: ______________ Date: _________ FEBRUARY 2008 ABSTRACT This research deals with the phonological distribution of Hebrew Inflectional and Derivational morphology, synchronically and diachronically. The scope of this study is suffixes, due to the fact that final position bears grammatical information, while initial position bears lexical items. In order to analyze the gathered data, the theory of Phonology as Human Behavior will be employed. The theory classifies language as a system of signs which is used by human beings to communicate; it is based on the synergetic principle of maximum communication with minimal effort. This research shows that the similarity within Modern Hebrew inflectional and derivational suffix system is greater than the derivational Modern Hebrew – Biblical Hebrew system in terms of a specialized suffix system and that the phonological distribution of Hebrew suffixes is motivated by the principles of the theory.
    [Show full text]
  • 4 the Syllable Structure of Shaoxing
    The phonology of Shaoxing Chinese Zhang, J. Citation Zhang, J. (2006, January 31). The phonology of Shaoxing Chinese. LOT dissertation series. LOT, Utrecht. Retrieved from https://hdl.handle.net/1887/4279 Version: Not Applicable (or Unknown) Licence agreement concerning inclusion of doctoral thesis in the License: Institutional Repository of the University of Leiden Downloaded from: https://hdl.handle.net/1887/4279 Note: To cite this publication please use the final published version (if applicable). 4 The Syllable Structure of Shaoxing 4.1 Introduction The status of the syllable as a linguistic unit, although not uncontrover- sial, is widely accepted in present-day phonology and phonetics. Syllables may consist of a vowel or diphthong, with onsets and codas of one or more consonants. Syllables may also contain syllabic consonants. Human listeners seem to need syllables as a way of segmenting the speech stream, while speakers use syllables to impose a rhythm of strong and weak beats to language, just like in music. All languages are assumed to have syllables, although the syllable status is sometimes questioned by researchers working on languages with extreme collocations of conso- nants or vowels, such as Bella Coola (Bagemihl 1991) and Gokana (Hyman 1990, 2003).1 Levin (1985) claims that phonetic utterances in all natural human languages are made up of syllables. However, the primary evidence for the syllable seems rather phonological than phonetic, in that “the syllable is the phonological unit which organizes segmental melodies in terms of sonority; syllabic segments are equivalent to sonority peaks within these organizational units” (Blevins 1995: 207). The syllable al- lows the formulation of generalizations both at the segmental level and at higher prosodic levels, which are awkward to express without referring to this constituent.
    [Show full text]
  • Russian-English Dictionary of Linguistic Terminology
    Russian-English dictionary of linguistics, digital linguistics and paleography Русско-английский словарь лингвистики, цифровой лингвистики и палеографии Juliane Besters-Dilger А aдресант addresser aдресат addressee; recipient aдъективный adjectival аккоммодация accommodation актант actant акцентный accentual алгоритм algorithm аллограф allograph алломорф allomorph аллофон allophon алфавит alphabet альвеолярный (согласный) alveolar (consonant) анализ дискурса discourse analysis аннотация annotation аннотировать to annotate антецедент antecedent; reference word антология anthology антоним antonym aорист aorist апикальный (согласный) apical (consonant) апракос aprakos арго slang артикль article артикуляция articulation архаический archaic архаичный archaic архилексема archilexeme ассимиляция assimilation атрибутивный attributive аутентичность autheticity аффективный affective аффикс affix аффиксальный affixal аффиксация affixation аффриката affricate Б безличный impersonal безударный unstressed; unaccented берестяные грамоты birch bark letters; birch bark manuscripts билабиальный bilabial 1 билингв bilingual (noun) билингвал bilingual (noun) билигвизм biligualism близость closeness; proximity боковой lateral будущее (время) future (tense) буква letter буквальный literal В вводное слово modal word велярный velar велярный (согласный) velar (noun) взаимный reciprocal взрывной (согласный) explosive (consonant) вибранта vibrant (consonant) вид aspect вид глагола verbal aspect визуализация visualization визуализировать to visualize винительный
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published By
    The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Trans 10 fax: +31 30 253 6000 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://wwwlot.let.uu.nl/ Cover illustration by Silke Hamann ISBN 90-76864-39-X NUR 632 Copyright © 2003 Silke Hamann. All rights reserved. The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Fonetiek en fonologie van retroflexen (met een samenvatting in het Nederlands) Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit Utrecht op gezag van de Rector Magnificus, Prof. Dr. W.H. Gispen, ingevolge het besluit van het College voor Promoties in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 6 juni 2003 des middags te 4.15 uur door Silke Renate Hamann geboren op 25 februari 1971 te Lampertheim, Duitsland Promotoren: Prof. dr. T. A. Hall (Leipzig University) Prof. dr. Wim Zonneveld (Utrecht University) Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Markedness of retroflexes 3 1.2 Phonetic cues and phonological features 6 1.3 Outline of the dissertation 8 Part I: Phonetics of Retroflexes 2 Articulatory variation and common properties of retroflexes 11 2.1 Phonetic terminology 12 2.2 Parameters of articulatory variation 14 2.2.1 Speaker dependency 15 2.2.2 Vowel context 16 2.2.3 Speech rate 17 2.2.4 Manner dependency 19 2.2.4.1 Plosives 19 2.2.4.2 Nasals 20 2.2.4.3 Fricatives 21 2.2.4.4 Affricates 23 2.2.4.5 Laterals 24 2.2.4.6 Rhotics 25 2.2.4.7 Retroflex vowels 26 2.2.5 Language family 27 2.2.6 Iventory size 28 2.3 Common articulatory properties of retroflexion 32 2.3.1 Apicality 33 2.3.2 Posteriority
    [Show full text]
  • Differences in Base of Articulation for Consonants Among Catalan Dialects
    Original Paper Phonetica 2010;67:201–218 Received: September 2, 2010 DOI: 10.1159/000322312 Accepted: October 17, 2010 Differences in Base of Articulation for Consonants among Catalan Dialects Daniel Recasens Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona and Institut d’Estudis Catalans, Barcelona, Spain Abstract Electropalatographic data for several front lingual consonants, i.e., the den- tal /t/, the alveolars /n, l, s, r/ and the alveolopalatals /tʃ, ʃ, ʎ, ɲ/, show differences in constriction anteriority among Catalan dialects varying in the progression Valencian > Eastern, with the Majorcan dialect occupying an intermediate posi- tion. These differences do not conform to speaker- dependent differences in palate morphology and, to the extent that they operate on a varied range of conso- nants, may be attributed to base of articulation. Deviations from this pattern are associated with manner of articulation and symmetry demands. A specific dialect- dependent relationship between tongue dorsum contact and constriction fronting is interpreted assuming the existence of less laminal, more apical dental and alveolar stops, and less dorsal, more laminal alveolopalatals, in Valencian than in the other two dialects. These data are interpreted in terms of the articula- tory characteristics for different tongue front settings which have been proposed in the literature. Copyright © 2011 S. Karger AG, Basel 1. Introduction 1.1. Articulatory Positioning While the terms ‘articulatory setting’ and ‘base of articulation’ enjoy a long tradi- tion in the phonetics literature, very few studies have examined their scientific validity by means of experimental data. Several phoneticians suggest that differences in ‘accent’ among languages stem from the way speakers place their articulatory organs for pro- ducing vowels and consonants in continuous speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Anticipatory Labial Activity in the Production of French Rounded Vowels
    ANTICIPATORY LABIAL ACTIVITY IN THE PRODUCTION OF FRENCH ROUNDED VOWELS *+BŽatrice VAXELAIRE, *¥+Rudolph SOCK, +Jean-Fran•ois BONNOT and +Dominique KELLER *Institut de PhonŽtique de Strasbourg - UniversitŽ Marc Bloch ¥Institut de la Communication ParlŽe de Grenoble, UniversitŽ Stendhal-INPG. +Laboratoire de Psychobiologie du Comportement Moteur et des Sports -UniversitŽ Marc Bloch ABSTRACT (where there was no intrusive vowel between C1 and C2). The In this 1investigation, movement data on articulator unrounded vowels were either [i] or [a] and the rounded gestures are examined, in order to understand timing vowels were either [u] or [y]. Two speakers uttered the principles underlying the production of French rounded speech samples at two speaking rates (normal and fast). X- vowels. The extent of anticipatory labial and lingual rays and a simultaneous audio recording of the speakersÕ activities, tied to vowel production, is analyzed in different production were obtained under medical care. consonantal contexts. X-ray and acoustic data are acquired With the help of a grid, measurement parameters from two speakers producing sentences at two speaking for vocal tract configurations were determined on mid- rates. Results show that anticipatory extents are comparable sagittal profiles related to: (1) upper lip position, in single consonant contexts, and that loading the obstruent horizontal and vertical displacements; (2) lower lip interval with a sequence of plosives restrains the position, horizontal and vertical displacements; (3) lip anticipatory gestures. Increasing speaking rate provokes opening or the vertical distance between the upper and lower speeding up of articulatory activity and compression of lips; (4) jaw position, vertical and horizontal acoustic durations, but does not significantly modify the displacements; (5) tongue-tip vertical displacement, relative timing of these anticipatory gestures.
    [Show full text]
  • Visualizing Phonetic Segment Frequencies with Density-Equalizing Maps
    Visualizing phonetic segment frequencies with density-equalizing maps Jeff Mielke Department of English, North Carolina State University [email protected] A method is demonstrated for creating density-equalizing maps of IPAconsonant and vowel charts, where the size of a cell in the chart reflects information such as the crosslinguistic frequency of the consonant or vowel. Transforming the IPA charts in such a way allows the visualization of interactions between phonetic features. Density-equalizing maps are used to illustrate a range of facts about consonant and vowel inventories, including the frequency of consonants and vowels and the frequency of common diacritics, and to illustrate the frequency of deletion and epenthesis involving particular consonants and vowels. Solutions are proposed for issues involving genealogical sampling, counting pairs of very similar phones, and counting diacritics in relation to basic symbols. 1 Introduction Density-equalizing maps allow familiar spatial maps to display non-spatial information. For example, the maps in the TheAtlasoftheRealWorld(Dorling, Newman & Barford 2008) represent quantitative information such as population, education, and access to the Internet by altering the area of countries on a world map. The resulting cartograms are likely to be more intuitive than a table of numbers to viewers who have already gained experience with the distribution of countries on a world map, and they enable a visual gestalt impression of similarities and differences between regions. Figure 1 shows two maps from worldmapper.org (Dorling, Barford & Newman 2006), one where area represents land area (as expected in equal-area projection world maps), and one that has been warped so that area represents population.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cross-Generational View of Contact-Related Phenomena in a Philippine Language: Phonology*
    1 A Cross-Generational View of Contact-Related Phenomena in a * Philippine Language: Phonology Lawrence A. Reid University of Hawai`i 1. Introduction The Philippines is a treasure house for the study of the effects of language contact. The extensive borrowing that occurred from Chinese and Malay-speaking traders (Wolff 1973-1974), prior to the coming of the Spanish in 1521, and from other foreign languages such as Spanish and English since then are well-known and often described (Wolff 1976). However, the influence of local Philippine languages on one another is another rich source of data on language contact, and one which has not often been as carefully explored. Probably all Philippine languages have large sets of lexical items which have been borrowed from one or another of the widely-spoken languages such as Filipino (Tagalog), Ilokano, Cebuano, Hiligaynon or Magindanao. The recognition of such borrowings though is sometimes obscured because of the similarity between the phonologies of the source languages and the donor languages.1 The primary purpose of this paper is to characterize and account for some of the massive changes that have taken place in the phonological system of one of the dialects of Central Bontok, that spoken in barangay Guinaang, over the last fifty years, primarily as a result of literacy in English, and the massive influx of Ilokano (and Tagalog) loanwords in the language. These changes have resulted in the addition of at least seven new phonemes in the language, two vowels /e/ and /o/2, and five consonants. There are, in addition to the phonological changes, extensive lexical changes, and a fairly substantial set of morphological and syntactic changes as a result of contact, but these will not be discussed in this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER TWO PHONOLOGY Throughout This Thesis I Employ A
    CHAPTER TWO PHONOLOGY Throughout this thesis I employ a practical orthography, based on similar orthographies for other Kimberley languages, such as Bardi, Ngarinyin, Worora/Worrorra, Gooniyandi, Bunuba, and Miriwoong. 1 Except for two features this orthography is fairly standard for Australian languages. The first feature is the use of "voiced letter symbols" for stops, even though phonetically the stops are most often unvoiced. I regard the choice of symbol as somewhat arbitrary where voicing is non- contrastive, but in this case I am also following a convention established for Northern Kimberley languages by other linguists, and preferred by speakers where they have had input into Kimberley orthographies. They all use the voiced series except in some cases for k,g where k is regarded as the more convenient choice for writing the nasal-stop clusters ngk, nk and/or rnk (KLRC 1999). I use ngg, n.g and rn.g, with the convention of a full stop to separate segments (though I am open to using k, as for the latest Worora and Ngarinyin community orthographies adopted by the Kimberley Language Resource Centre (KLRC 1999). 2 .1 CONSONANTS The consonant phonemes of Wunambal are unremarkable for an Australian language (Dixon 1980: 125-127; 132-150). Manners of articulation include the expected stops, nasals, laterals, rhotics and glides. Voicing is not contrastive in the stops. Stops and nasals distinguish four and five places of articulation. The expected alveolar vs. post- alveolar contrast described for all other Kimberley languages (Capell and Coate 1984; Clendon 1994; Hudson 1978; Kofod 1978, 1996; McGregor 1990, 1993; Rumsey 1982, 2000; Tsunoda 1981) is difficult to establish in Wunambal, particularly in the stops.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACT Title of Dissertation: PHONOLOGICAL
    ABSTRACT Title of dissertation: PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING UNIT TRANSFER: THE IMPACT OF FIRST LANGUAGE SYLLABLE STRUCTURE AND ITS IMPLICATIONS FOR PREFERRED SUBSYLLABIC DIVISION UNITS Shih-wei Chen, Doctor of Philosophy, 2006 Dissertation directed by: Professor Mariam Jean Dreher Department of Curriculum and Instruction This study investigated the potential transfer of first language (L1) phonological processing unit to second language processing. English and Chinese phonology differ mainly in the complexity of their syllable structures. English phonology allows highly complex syllable structures, whereas Chinese has been characterized primarily as a core syllable language, i.e., its syllables typically consist only of a consonant and vowel (CV). This sharp contrast is hypothesized to entail different phonological processing units in the two languages, and to result in, through L1 transfer, the poor phonological awareness often observed in Chinese speakers learning English as a second language (ESL). This hypothesis was tested by examining the performance patterns of Chinese ESL fourth graders on phoneme deletion and phoneme isolation tasks. The results suggest that Chinese ESL children do seem to process an English syllable in terms of an intact core syllable plus its appendices due to L1 transfer. This gives support to a developmental account of subsyllabic division unit preference, which suggests that core syllable is universally preferred in the initial stages of language development, only after which speakers of different languages diverge in their division unit preferences due to linguistic characteristics of their respective L1s. The presence of transfer suggested that Chinese ESL children performed differently on two item types—core-syllable items (requiring segmentation of an element within the core syllable) and non-core-syllable items (requiring segmentation of any appendices from the core syllable).
    [Show full text]
  • Retroflex Processes and Their Phonetic Grounding
    4 Retroflex processes and their phonetic grounding In this chapter, cross-linguistically very common phonological processes involving retroflex segments are discussed. These processes are: retroflexion in a rhotic context and in a back vowel context, de-retroflexion in a front vowel context (and in secondary palatalization) or retraction of the front vowel, retroflexion of velarized or labialized segments, retroflexion of vowels before retroflex segments, non- occurrence of retroflexes word-initially and post-consonantally, and (local and non- local) assimilation of non-retroflex coronals. They are represented in this order in .where t is a cover symbol for a [+anterior] coronal, and ֒ for a retroflex segment ,(1) C is any kind of consonant, V any kind of vowel, and (ω indicates the left boundary of a prosodic category higher than the syllable (phonological word, phrase boundary). The first column gives the inputs, the second the outputs, and the third gives these processes terms that I will continue to use in this and following chapters. (1) input output process a) /rt/ [֒] retroflexion in rhotic context) b) /ut/ [u֒] retroflexion in back vowel context) c) /i֒/ [it] or [᭣֒] deretroflexion or vowel retraction) or [֒] deretroflexion or depalatalization [ܕt] /ܕ֒/ ײ d) /V֒/ [V ֒] retroflexion of adjacent vowel) e) /֒/ (ω[t], C[t] phonotactic restrictions on retroflexes) f) /֒t/ [֒֒] local assimilation of retroflexes) Vt/ [֒V֒] non-local assimilation of retroflexes֒/ Bhat (1973) already described some of the processes in (1), namely the retroflexion of dentals or alveolars by a preceding /r/ (1a), by a preceding back vowel (1b), and a preceding retroflex consonant (1f).
    [Show full text]