<<

Suppression and Repression in Discrepant Self-Other Ratings: Relations with Control and Cardiovascular Reactivity

Karina W. Davidson Dalhousie University

ABSTRACT Individual differences in self-other disagreement may lap phe- nomena that have been notoriously difficult to assess. For example, subjects who believe they possess a trait while their acquaintances disagree may be exhibiting suppression. Further, subjects who deny a trait while acquaintances believe it is present may be displaying repression. In the first study, both sub- jects and their closest friends rated the subject's hostility level. Suppressors and repressors were expected to exhibit enhanced thought control, and indeed these individuals were more able not to think about white bears when instructed to do so than individuals for whom there was high hostility agreement. However, this was also true for those with low hostility agreement. Only suppressors demonstrated blood-pressure hyperreactivity to a hostility-provoking task as expected; this finding was replicated in a second study employing a different, multi-item measure of hostility, as well as a marker of low Agreeableness.

There are matiy reasons why we might disagree with the way others characterize us. According to Kenny's (1991) model, we might not be known by them sufficiently well (acquaintance), we might have seen

This research was supported by grants from the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Nova Scotia and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. I would like to thank Kim Calderwood, Melissa Cutler, and Tracy Mason for their assistance in conducting this research, and William F. Chaplin and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. Requests for reprints should be sent to Karina Davidson, Department of , Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H 3J5.

Journal of Personality 61:4, December 1993. Copyright © 1993 by Duke University Press. CCC 0022-3506/93/$!.50 670 Davidson ourselves in situations that differ from the ones they have observed (overlap), we might not display the trait hehavior consistently (consis- tency), we may mean something different by our trait characterization than they do by theirs (shared meaning), or, finally, we might not have discussed with them our view of ourselves (communication). Indeed, some of these variables clearly affect self-other ratings (Funder, 1987; Funder & Colvin, 1988; Paunonen, 1989). Yet even when someone knows us very well, has seen us in numerous situations, has observed us acting consistently, defines traits similarly to us, and knows what we believe about ourselves, there are still circumstances or traits for which we fail to share others' assessments of ourselves. What might cause such a discrepancy? This article discusses two possibilities—suppression and repression. Following a brief review of the conceptualization of and the measurement difficulties inherent in these two constructs, the potential usefulness of self-other disagree- ments for assessing these tendencies will be presented. Next, traits for which self-other disagreement should predict substantive outcomes will be introduced. Finally, two hypotheses about these defensive tenden- cies as measured by self-other hostility disagreement will be presented. First, those for whom there is disagreement about hostility should be better able to actively suppress . Second, only suppressors should show increased blood-pressure reactivity to an interpersonally stressful task.

The Meaning and Measurement ol Suppression and Repression Methods for identifying and distinguishing repression and suppression have both plagued and intrigued researchers and theorists for a long time (Erdelyi & Goldberg, 1979; Holmes. 1974; Horowitz, 1975; Laza- rus, 1983; McGranahan. 1940). Although there is still much contro- versy over the distinction between these two constructs (Singer, 1990), Vaillant (1990, 1992) has suggested operational definitions that are becoming increasingly accepted. Repression, according to Vaillant, in- volves the "unconsciously motivated or unawareness of ex- ternal events or of internal impulses, feelings, thoughts, or wishes. Although the repressed is not recognized consciously, its effects re- main" (Vaillant, 1992, p. 276). In contrast, suppression is the "con- scious or semi-conscious decision to postpone (but not to avoid) paying attention to a conscious impulse, feeling, or confiict" (p. 277). Thus, SeK-Other Disagreement 671

two critical distinctions exist between these two definitions: Repres- sors are not aware of the feeling or impulse, whereas suppressors are; repressors should exhibit effects of the teehng or impulse, whereas suppressors should not. How can one measure such phenomena? Many (predominantly un- successful) attempts have been made, both through observational and self-report techniques (Cramer, 1991; Holmes, 1990). Relying exclu- sively on direct self-report measures provides at best a weak basis for making a distinction between those who do not possess the trait and repressors who cannot tell us they possess it. Similarly, exclusive re- liance upon observation or other-report measures cannot conclusively distinguish between those who accurately do not exhibit a trait and suppressors who choose not to exhibit it. Rather, as noted by Vail- lant (1992), it is the inconsistency between informed biography (other- report) and autobiography (self-report) that alerts us to the possible existence of suppression and repression. To obtain reliable observer ratings of defensive tendencies, Vaillant (1977) trained his coders to examine self-report and interview data for marked discrepancies. That is, defenses were hypothesized to exist when autobiographical data ne- glected information reported from significant others (repression), or when autobiographical data focused predominantly upon information neglected in biographical reports (suppression). Indeed, measured in this way, both of these, as well as other defensive tendencies, have been found to be remarkably stable (G, E. Vaillant & C. O. Vaillant, 1990). Weinberger (1990) has suggested that three conditions must be pres- ent to conclude that defensive processes exist. First, sources of infor- mation about internal traits or feelings must disagree with each other. In this way, there is discrepant information that either must be recon- ciled or defended against. For example, believing oneself to be calm when one has sweaty palms leads either to the revision of one's opinion about one's calmness, or to a (motivated) unawareness of any discom- fort. Clearly, if no disconfirming evidence exists—as when one believes oneself to be anxious, and has sweaty palms—then there is no reason to defend against any information. Second, the disconfirming information must be potentially available to the individual. Specific and interpretations of behaviors are arguably not easily accessible (Erdelyi, 1990; Nisbett & Ross, 1980), but we have many reasons to suspect that negative emotions are readily available to the subject when others can infer them (Buck, 1985, 1988). For example, young children can accu- rately identify internal distressing emotions, but often become less Danridson accurate as socialization progresses (Malatesta & Haviland, 1982). Fur- ther, inaccuracy varies by the appropriateness or social sanctions placed on certain emotions; emotions considered inappropriate for the child to experience (such as anger in girls) show increased inaccuracy relative to emotions such as happiness (Brody, 1985). Thus, we have develop- mental evidence that the expression or of affect is internally available, and is increasingly lost as inhibitory controls are learned. Third, a trait or emotion must be in some way aversive, so that it would warrant or conscious inhibition. Of course, not all aversive information is defended against—only that which con- tradicts consciously held beliefs (Weinberger, 1990). Thus, repressors may be motivated to maintain self-perceptions of little subjective ex- perience of negative traits or emotions if their self-concept does not include these traits. Similarly, suppressors may be motivated to inhibit the expression of a trait or emotion only if they believe it to reflect negatively upon themselves. Thus, traits or emotions with negative connotations may potentially foster defensive tendencies. Perhaps self-other trait disagreements can aid in identifying suppres- sive and repressive individuals. Although self-other personality agree- ments are at times substantial (Funder & Colvin, 1988; Watson, 1989), there may still be subsets of individuals within a sample for whom disagreements exist. Two subsets of individuals might be expected to demonstrate low self-other trait agreement, albeit for differing defensive tendencies; where the discrepancy is caused by low self-rating and high other rating, repression may be at work, and where the discrepancy is a result of high self-rating and low other rating, suppression may be occur- ring. If suppression and repression can cause self-other disagreements, then specific hypotheses regarding these constructs should be testable when they are measured by self-other discrepancies. Two hypotheses about these defensive tendencies will be presented, and then data from two studies will be presented that support these operationalizations. It is likely that some specific traits will be more appropriate than others for tapping defensive tendencies. As mentioned above, negative emotions exhibit the three conditions necessary to allow defensive- ness: Negative emotions can demonstrate discrepancies between subjec- tive experience and physiological response or behavioral manifestation; they are considered readily accessible to individuals; and, finally, they are potentially threatening to the individual's self-concept. Although a number of researchers have focused on (Davis, 1990; Wein- berger, 1990), for a number of reasons I will examine discrepancies Self-Other Disagreement 673 in hostility report. First, hostility suppression and repression have been the subject of conjecture for some time (Siegel, 1992). Second, spe- cific hypotheses about the suppression of hostility have been forwarded (Houston, 1992). The role that hostility may play in the development of health prob- lems such as coronary heart disease (CHD) has been debated for some time. Unfortunately, empirical results have not yet resolved this issue. For example, high scores on the Cook and Medley Hostility scale (HO; Cook & Medley. 1954) have been found to be both related and unrelated to incidences of CHD (Leon, Finn, Murray, & Bailey, 1988; Shekelle, Gale, Ostfeld, & Paul, 1983), associated and unassociated with blood- pressure reactivity (Hardy & Smith, 1988; Smith & Houston, 1987), and predictive and nonpredictive of total mortality (Barefoot, Dahl- strom, & Williams, 1983; Hearn, Murray, & Luepker, 1989). What might account for these contradictions? Perhaps more than the simple self-report of hostility is required to fully explain the link between hostility and health. Indeed, it has been suggested that those who are most hostile may be likely to distort their self-reported hostility level (Barefoot, 1992). Investigators have proposed that suppressing or not expressing experienced hostility may be deleterious to one's health (Davies, 1971; Greer & Morris, 1975; Harburg et al., 1973; Houston, 1992; McGinn, Harburg, Julius, & McLeod, 1964; Smith, 1992). Thus, a person who experiences but does not express hostility may be at risk for future health problems such as CHD. Briefly, suppressed hostility has been thought to cause hypertension and CHD (Harburg et al., 1973; Siegel, 1992) by the fol- lowing process. First, an individual avoids or controls thoughts and feelings of hostility during a hostility-provoking situation, but incurs short- and long-term costs for this effort. In the short term, individu- als who inhibit thoughts and feelings have been shown to display ex- aggerated autonomic functioning (Pennebaker, Hughes, & O'Heeron, 1987) and cardiovascular functioning (Harburg et al., 1973). Short- term exaggerated cardiovascular reactivity, in turn, has been associated in some prospective studies with long-term vulnerabilities to hyperten- sion and other related coronary artery disease outcomes (Julius, Weder, & Hinderliter, 1986). However, previous attempts to measure the sup- pression of hostility have been fraught with difficulties, and have often had to rely upon self-reported measures of such tendencies (Cottington, Matthews, Talbott, & KuUer, 1986; MacDougall, Dembroski, Dims- dale, & Hackett, 1985; Siege!, 1985). Some studies have confirmed Davidson the hostility suppression hypothesis employing self-report data (Mills, Schneider, & Dimsdale, 1989), whereas others have not (Smith & Houston, 1987). Consequently, self-other hostility disagreement may provide an alternative to self-report alone as a measure of suppressed hostility, and may be invaluable in testing this substantive hypothesis. Hostility suppressors are hypothesized to have increased cardiovascular reactivity during the hostility-provoking task when compared to others for whom there was self-other hostility agreement; both studies reported below examined the interactive efFects of self-rated and other-rated hos- tility on cardiovascular reactivity during a hostility-provoking labora- tory task, the Structured Interview (Chesney, Eagleston, & Rosenman, 1980). Although repressors are also thought to be at increased CHD risk, the term is often used interchangeably with suppressors to refer to any individual who does not express hostility (Siegel, 1992). How- ever, by following Vaillant's (1990) definitions, I have suggested that only suppressors do not exhibit hostility, whereas repressors exhibit it, but cannot report experiencing it. Thus, if the hostility suppression hy- pothesis is correct, only suppressors should demonstrate hyperreactivity to a bostility-provoking task. Although suppression and repression may manifest themselves very differently, they both may require a superior ability to selectively avoid potentially threatening information. Repression may first involve moti- vated nonlearning, then motivated nonrehearsal, and finally motivated nonretrieval (Shevrin, 1990). In fact, both Shevrin (1990) and Bower (1990) believe that retrieval failures, or the last stage in this process, constitute the largest component of "motivated forgetting," or repres- sion. Suppression, in contrast, also involves the active avoidance of threatening material, but when retrieval of this information is neces- sary or required, the information is available (Vaillant, 1992). Thus, in both cases active avoidance of some material is thought to exist, and differences in conscious control between these two constructs occur predominantly during the retrieval process. To test whether suppres- sors and repressors are more efficient than others at avoiding thoughts when they are instructed that it is desirable to do so, subjects underwent the White Bear paradigm proposed by Wegner, Schneider, Carter, and White (1987), in which they were instructed to talk and think about anything at all except white bears. Although thoughts of white bears are in general neutral rather than threatening, this paradigm has been employed to assess individual differences in general suppression ability, and has been shown to predict successful suppression of more threaten- Self-Other Disagreement 675 ing material {Wegner, Schneider. Knutson, & McMahon, 1991). Low counts of white bear thoughts then constituted a measure of ability, and subjects for whom there was hostility disagree- ment (suppressors and repressors) were expected to do better on this task than self-other agreeing subjects.

Study 1 METHOD Subjects A total of 101 undergraduates (38 males and 63 females) were recruited for participation in the study. Subjects were informed that they would complete personality questionnaires, engage in a cognitive task, and be asked a number of questions about their previous work, superficial habits, and school his- tory. Finally, each subject was asked for the name, the phone number, and permission to speak to their closest, same-sexed, nonromantic friend.

Procedure Subjects were required to have refrained from drinking caffeine, smoking, or exercising for 3 hours prior to testing. Upon reporting to the laboratory, subjects rated themselves for hostility on a 5-point scale from not at all hostile (1) to very hostile (5), and then completed the Edwards Social Desirability Scale (ESDS; Edwards. 1957), which consists of 39 items and employs a true-false format. These items are derived from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) to assess a social desirability reponse set, defined as "the tendency of subjects to attribute to themselves, in self-description, person- ality statements with socially desirable scale values and to reject those socially undesirable scale values" (p. vi). Using the White Bear paradigm (Wegner et al., 1987), each subject then verbalized into a tape recorder everything that went through their while alone for a warm-up period of 5 minutes. A researcher next reentered the room and instructed each subject to repeat this exercise while attempting not to think about white bears. However, in the event that they should think of a white bear, they were instructed to tap the microphone of the tape recorder with the pencil provided. The subject was once more left alone in the room for 5 minutes to complete this task. Audiotapes were then coded for number of taps during the second 5-minute period. Finally, subjects entered a third room, were seated in a comfortable chair, and had the blood-pressure recording apparatus attached to their nondominant arms. Because initial blood-pressure readings may be unstable and unpredic- Davidson tive of later readings (Carey et al., 1976), subjects had three initial (unused) blood-pressure measures taken to acclimatize subjects to the blood-pressure cuff. Thereafter subjects rested for 10 to 15 minutes. At the end of this adap- tation period, a fourth blood-pressure reading was taken and this served as the resting (baseline) measure. Third and fourth readings were highly correlated (r = .91), suggesting that acclimatization had occurred. Systolic blood pres- sure (SBP) was recorded with an automated blood-pressure monitor (Spacelabs Model 90202). A l2-minute Structured Interview was administered that evokes a stressful response in tbe subjects (Chesney et al., 1980). Subjects were informed that they would be asked a number of questions about their previous work and school history. As the interviewer asks questions about competitiveness and work relationships, the pace is quickened, until the subject is barely given enough time to respond to one question before the next is posed. Additionally, at a certain point in the interview, the interviewer increasingly appears un- interested in the subject. The interview is thus designed to behaviorally assess the subject's response to a number of standardized hostile interactions. Blood- pressure measures were taken at the end of the interview (arousal phase) and 6 minutes after its completion, to ensure that subjects had returned to base- line levels of SBP. After the final resting period of 6 minutes, subjects were thanked for their participation and presented with the purpose of the study. Closest friends were then telephoned, informed about the reason for the call and the prior consent of the subject, and assured of confidentiality. They were then asked their age and gender, and then to rate the subject on a 5-point scale from not at all (I) to very much (5) for (a) hostility; and (b) level of closeness felt toward the subject.

RESULTS Descriptive Data Subjects were on average 22 years old (SD - 3 years), as were their closest friends (SD = 4 years). Subjects and friends had known each other on average for 5 years (SD = 2 years), and subjects rated friends to be very close (M = 4.6 on a 5-poitit scale, SD = .6). Similarly, friends rated subjects also to be very close (M = 4.5, SD = .6). The friend rating of hostility (M = 2.3, 5D = 1.2) was independent of the self-rating of hostility (M = 2.1, 5D = 1.1, r = .06), and no sig- nificant difference was found between the mean level of these ratings. Further, the means and standard deviations of males and females sepa- rately were similar, and no significant sex differences were noted in any of the analyses. Thus, all analyses combined males and females. Self-Other Disagreement 677

Thought Control Self-other disagreeing subjects were expected to demonstrate superior thought control when compared to subjects with self-other hostility agreement; hence, an interaction was predicted between self- and other hostility ratings, of a specific form. To test for the effect on thought control of the predicted interaction between the self- and friend rating of hostility, a hierarchical multiple was conducted in which the number of unwanted white bear thoughts was employed as the de- pendent measure. Both (centered) self- and friend ratings were entered simultaneously with the cross-product of the two ratings. Neither the self-rating of hostility, f {1, 97) - 1.9. b = .27, nor the friend rating, F(l,97)= .5,b= .14, was significantly related to white bear thoughts. The cross product between these variables added significantly (4% of variance) to the prediction of white bear thoughts when subjects were instructed to purposely avoid such thoughts, F(l,91) = 4.\, b = .34. Those who are able to inhibit either awareness or expression of hos- tility were expected to have fewer white bear thoughts than those indi- viduals for whom there was agreement about their hostility level. To ensure that the significant interaction was of the form hypothesized, the interaction is displayed by three simple regression lines; one standard deviation above, one standard deviation below, and one at the mean for friends' ratings (see Figure !). Although the regression lines are in the predicted direction (white bear thoughts decrease as disagreements increase), clearly the most striking finding is that as both friend and self- ratings of hostility increase, so too do unwanted white bear thoughts. These data suggest that subjects with self-friend disagreements were better able to avoid thoughts when instructed to do so, but low hostility agreement subjects were also able to avoid these thoughts.

Hostility Suppression and Blood-Pressuie HyperreactivitY Next, suppressors were expected to exhibit increased blood-press ure reactivity when placed in a hostility-provoking situation. A hierarchi- cal regression was performed, and this time the arousal phase SBP served as the dependent measure. First, the centered baseline SBP was entered as a covariate, as biood-pressure reactivity was of interest here, f(l, 98) = 169.9, and this covariate accounted for 63% of the vari- ance. Again, centered self- and friend hostility ratings were entered simultaneously with their cross product on the second step. Self-rating 678 Davidson

Low Friend Rating = Mean Friend Rating = 5.0- High Friend Rating

4.0-

3.0-

2.0-

1.0- 1.0 2.1 3.2 Low Self Rating High Sen Rating

Figure 1 White Bear Thought Differences for Varying Self- and Friend Hostilifv Ratings Note. Self- and friend hostility ratings are ± I standard deviation. of hostility was significant, F(l, 95) = 3.8, b = 1.9, although the friend rating was not, f(l, 95) - 1.7, b = -.97. The cross product of the two ratings was significant, accounting for 3% of the variance in blood-pressure reactivity, F(\, 95) = 7.2, b = -2.2. Finally, the cross products between self- and friend ratings of hostility and baseline SBP were entered in a third step, to check the assumption of homogeneity SeU-Othei Disagreement 679

Low Friend Rating Mean Friend Rating 150- High Friend Rating

145-

140-

135-

130- 1.0 2.1 3.2 Low Self Rating High Self Rating

Figure 2 Reactive SBP Levels lor Varying Sell- and Other Hostility Ratings Note. Self- and friend hostility ratings are ± 1 standard deviation. of regression coefficients (J. Cohen & P. Cohen, 1983); this assumption was met. Once again, to ensure that the interaction conformed to the hypothe- sized form, the interaction is depicted as three simple regression lines (see Figure 2). It appears that only suppressors showed the expected cardiovascular hyperreactivity to an interpersonally stressful task, as had been hypothesized. Davidson Although these results are promising, there are two concerns about the present design that may limit the interpretation of these results. First, the lack of even a minimal relation between hostility ratings may be of concern, as others have found higher relations for similar single items (e.g., "has hostility towards others," r = .33; Funder, 1980). There are a few methodological reasons that might explain the indepen- dence of the self- and friend rating of hostility. There could have been a restriction of range for either rating; however, an examination of scat- terplots, the standard deviations, and the range of ratings for friends and subjects suggested that no such restriction occurred. Also, it could be that social desirability is suppressing (by which I mean statistically, and not psychodynamically) the relation between the two ratings. As might be expected, social desirability was significantly related to the self-rating of hostility (r = .23, p < .05), but not to the friend rating (r = .13). Importantly, partialling out the ESDS score from the self- rating of hostility did not substantially change its relation to the friend rating (r = .03). Finally, curvilinearity could have compromised the relation between these two variables; again an examination of scatter- plots did not reveal such a pattern. The second limitation to this study is that only a single item was used to assess hostility; thus the unreliability inherent in single-item measures may have compromised these findings. To both replicate and extend the results reported for the first study, a second study was conducted in which the Cook and Medley HO scale was completed by friends and subjects, rather than the single hostility item. It has been shown that hostility measures (including the HO scale) combine to varying degrees iow Agreeableness and high Neuroticism— two of the five enduring factors of personality^whereas others only tap low Agreeableness (Smith & Williams, 1992). Perhaps hostility mea- sures such as the HO scale vary in the strength of their prediction of CHD as they vary in their blend of these two major personality traits. Low Agreeableness alone has been proposed as the critical coronary- prone component of hostility (Costa, McCrae, & Dembroski, 1989), whereas Neuroticism is considered a nuisance factor within hostility (Costa & McCrae, 1987). For this reason, a marker of low Agreeable- ness (or high Antagonism) and Neuroticism from a five-factor person- ahty scale (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1992) was also completed by subjects and friends. If speculations about the relation between hostility, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism are correct, then low Agreeableness should act similarly to hostility, but Neuroticism should not. Self-Ottxer Disagreement 681

Study 2 METHOD All procedures were identical to those of Study 1, with the following two ex- ceptions. The White Bear paradigm was not included in this study, and in place of the single hostility item, the Cook and Medley HO scale and the low Agree- ableness (or high Antagonism) and Neuroticism scales (John et al., 1992) were substituted. The HO scale is a 50-item true-false list of sentences extracted from the MMPI that was originally designed to differentiate unpopular teachers from the more popular ones (Cook & Medley, 1954). Many have now noted the multi- dimensionality of the HO items (Costa, Zonderman, McCrae, & Williams, 1986) and Barefoot and his colleagues have offered conceptually derived sub- scales that demonstrate differential predictive power for CHD survival (Bare- foot, Dodge. Peterson, Dahlstrom, & Williams. 1989); the 27 items that tap hostile emotions, thoughts, and behaviors—as opposed to social avoidance, paranoia, or unknown^and that predict CHD survival were employed here. The Agreeableness subscale of the Big Five Inventory (Version 54) is a 9-item marker of Agreeableness that employs a 5-point Likert scale. Since hy- potheses have concerned low Agreeableness or high Antagonism, scores from subjects and friends were reversed, and renamed Antagonism scores, to ease interpretation. The internal consistency estimate is .81 for this scale, and it correlates .82 with its equivalent on the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae. 1985). Similarly, the Neuroticism subscale contains eight items, has an internal consistency estimate of .86. and correlates .82 with its NEO-PI counterpart (John et al., 1992).

RESULTS Descriptive Data Ninety-two subjects (31 males, 61 females) participated in the study. Subjects were on average 22 years old {SD = 4 years), and their friends were on average 23 years old (SD ^ 6 years). Subjects and friends had known each other for AVi years (SD = 3 years), and both subjects and friends stated they were very close to each other (M = 4.3 on a 5-point scale, 5D = .7 for subjects; M - 4.2, 5D - .5 for friends). The means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, and Cronbaeh alphas for both the friends' and subjects' personality scores can be found in Table 1. The friend rating of hostility as measured by the HO scale was again independent of the self-rating of hostility (r ^ .08), and this time a significant difference was found between the mean level of these ratings 682 Davidson

Table 1 Sample Statistics and Intercorrelations among Variables in Study 2

1 2 3 4 5 6 M SD 1. HO scale (.72) .45 .33 .08 .45 .11 11.0 3.9 2. Antagonism (.80) .34 .12 .25 .22 10.4 5.5 3. Neuroticism (.79) -.12 .09 .13 23.5 5.7 4. Friend HO scale (.90) .64 .59 13.4 6.9 5. Friend Antagonism (.91) .54 13.8 6.5 6. Friend Neuroticism (.79) 20.5 3.9 Note. N ^ 92 (61 females, 31 males); HO scale - subject's Cook-Medley Hostility Scale (revised) score; Antagonism - subject's Antagonism score; Neuroticism - sub- ject's Neuroticism score; Friend HO scale = HO scale by friend for subject; Friend An- tagonism = Antagonism score by friend for subject; Friend Neuroticism - Neurolicism score by friend for subject. Cronbaeh alphas are shown on the diagonal; correlations in bold are significant at the .05 level, two-tailed test.

(t = 3,0,p< .01).Consistent with Other findings, the self-friend agree- ment correlation for Antagonism was significant (r = .25), although the Neuroticism agreement correlation (r — . 13) was somewhat below that found by others (Watson, 1989). Intercorrelations among the HO scale and markers of Antagonism and Neuroticism were as expected (Smith & Williams, 1992).

Suppression and Blood-Piessuie Hyperreactivity A hierarchical regression was performed, and again the arousal-phase SBP served as the dependent measure. First, the centered baseline SBP was entered as a covariate, F{\, 90) ^ 99.6, and this covariate accounted for 53% of the variance. Again, centered self- and friend hostility ratings and their cross product were entered simultaneously on the second step. This time neither the self-rating of hostility, F(\, 87) = 2.2, b = .35, nor the friend rating of hostility, F( I, 87) = 2.9, b = —.22, was significant. Once again, the cross product of the two ratings was significant and accounted for 3% of the variance in blood- pressure reactivity, F(l, 87) = 5.0, b = —.08. Finally, the assumption of homogeneity of regression coefficients (J. Cohen & P. Cohen, 1983) Self-Other Disagreement 683 was met. This interaction again conformed to the hypothesized form, and repUcated that shown for Study 1 in Figure 2. As in Study 1 and as hypothesized, only suppressors showed the expected cardiovascular hyperreactivity to an interpersonally stressful task. Finally, two further multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine first Antagonism and then Neuroticism. First, centered self- and friend Antagonism scores were substituted for the self- and friend HO scores as described above. SBP reactivity scores once more served as the dependent measure. The baseline SBP covariate on the first step accounted for 53% of the variance, as would be expected. When self- and friend Antagonism scores and their cross product were entered on the second step, the self-rating of Antagonism was not significant, F{{, 87) = .1, but friend Antagonism was, f(l, 87) - 5.7. The cross product accounted for a nearly significant amount of variance in blood-pressure reactivity (2%), F(l, 87) = 3.6, b = — .05, and again replicated that shown for Study 1 in Figure 2. For the final regression analysis, centered self- and friend Neuroti- cism scores were substituted for the HO scores, and SBP reactivity was the dependent measure. Although baseline SBP again accounted for 53% of the variance on the first step, neither self- nor friend Neu- roticism scores nor their interaction were significant. As suggested by Costa et al. (1989), low Agreeableness or Antagonism appears to be the component within hostility that predicts cardiovascular reactivity, whereas high Neuroticism does not.

DISCUSSION Researchers often consider consensus or self-other agreement in terms of convergent validity, and thus treat the self-other correlation as a validity coefficient. For example, in discussing the results of the Nor- man and Goldberg (1966) study, Watson (1989) points out that well- acquainted peers demonstrated better validity (the averaged other ratings correlated highly with the self-ratings) than less-acquainted peers, and so concluded that acquaintanceship may have compromised validity in some of the previous studies in the area. Others have suggested that behavioral prediction is a criterion of interest in self-other trait agree- ment (Funder, 1987), and that interpersonal behavior is the criterion of interest to personality (Block, 1977). Each criterion for personality traits leads to a different interpreta- tion of self-other disagreements, and thus different possible sources of 684 Davidson

error. A construct validity perspective opens up four major areas to be explored for understanding self-other disagreements. These four areas do not appear as method error or nuisance variables, but can be viewed as variables that are themselves intriguing. First, there may be "other" variables (e.g., shared meaning or unique impressions)—some quality or trait unique to the second rater that distorts their rating ability. Sec- ond, relationship variables may affect agreement; the self and other may not have been exposed to behavior from the same situations (overlap), may not have known each other for a long time (acquaintanceship), or may not have known each other well. Third, there are trait variables to consider; some traits are socially desirable, others are easily ob- servable, still others are concretely operationalized. Finally, there may be subject variables, or characteristics unique to the individual, that may impact upon self-other agreement. This research sought to explore the last variable—individual differences in subject characteristics—that might cause self-other disagreements. These two studies have demonstrated that suppressors have SBP hyperreactivity to a stressful interpersonal task when compared to all others. This result was found with a single-item measure of hostility, a traditional measure of hostility, and a marker of Antagonism, the suspected critical component of hostility. These results are consistent with the long-held hypothesis that suppressed hostility has an impact on cardiovascular functioning (Harburgetal., 1973; Siegel, 1992). Why do suppressors hyperreact to hostility-provoking tasks? Dis- charge theory (Jones, 1950) holds that emotional expression and auto- nomic arousal are inversely related because of their common role in tension reduction. Although this theory clearly implies within-subject covariation, data typically employed as support for this theory demon- strate between-subject covariation; individuals who exhibit little emo- tional expression (internalizers) show enhanced autonomic arousal, whereas individuals who express emotion freely (extemalizers) show no abnormal internal or physiological arousal (Buck, 1979). Cacioppo et al. (1992) have suggested that this theory can be conceptualized as involving differential gains in two separate systems. Extemalizers may display a large gain in the expressive system and only a small gain in a physiological system in reaction to emotional stimuli. By con- trast, internalizers show small gains in expressiveness and large gains in their physiological system. This conceptualization does not necessarily lead to a hydraulic, causal relation between emotion and physiology; it merely suggests that stable large gains in one system may relate to Self-Other Disagreement 685 smaller gains in another system. In the current context, suppressors (those who do not express emotion freely, but instead internalize these emotions) should show enhanced autonomic arousal, such as cardio- vascular reactivity. All others studied here either express their emotions (although for the repressors not necessarily knowingly) or do not ex- perience the emotion and thus do not express it. Thus, only suppressors should show a larger gain in the cardiovascular system when compared to others, and this is the pattern of results found here. Alternatively, some type of prolonged thought intrusion or "rebound" (Wegner et al., 1987) may have occurred in suppressors to cause their heightened SBP reactivity. However, we know that both repressors and suppressors are able to successfully avoid thoughts when instructed to do so, such as was found with the white bear task. Thus, one would think that both might be equally vulnerable to later thought intrusion or rebound, and so both should demonstrate SBP reactivity. But the critical difference that distinguishes suppressors and repressors is the expression of these unwanted thoughts or feelings. Suppressors are able both to avoid unwanted thoughts and their expression, whereas repres- sors are successful at the avoidance of a thought, but not its expression. Thus, there is reason to believe that suppressors and repressors differ not in their thought avoidance abilities, but in their expression avoid- ance abilities. That is, by purposefully avoiding the expression of hostile feelings during the Structured Interview, suppressors may have con- tinued to feel hostile at the time of the SBP reactivity reading (after the hostile situation was over). Repressors, on the other hand, although able to avoid thoughts such as white bears, may be less able to avoid the expression of certain feelings, such that they may have less emotion rebound when the Structured Interview is over. Such conjectures could be tested in two ways. First, process mea- sures of hostile feelings/thoughts and the expression of these feelings could be added to the Structured Interview. During the interview sup- pressors and repressors should not necessarily differ in their number of hostile thoughts, but should differ dramatically in the expression of these thoughts. Second, the White Bear paradigm could again be em- ployed, but tbis time a third period of free expression of white bear thoughts could be added to test the differential rebound hypothesis out- lined above. Further, suppression and then expression of less neutral material, such as negative emotions, could be substituted for the white bear stimulus. Self-other disagreements also predicted individual differences in Davidson avoidance of unwanted thoughts. Although suppressors and repressors both demonstrated below-average white bear thoughts, so too did the low hostility self-friend agreement individuals. Further investigations of this finding, including a multi-item measure of hostility and sub- stituting unwanted affect-laden thoughts for white bear thoughts, are warranted. Perhaps variables other than subject variables account for the current findings. If this were the case, however, an alternative variable would need to cause self-friend disagreement, individual differences in thought suppression, and SBP reactivity. As an example, some individuals may have stricter standards (or lower criteria) for labeling themselves as hostile, and this would clearly affect the degree of self-friend hos- tility disagreement. Thus, the individuals labeled as suppressors here could have obtained the rating disagreement because a certain subset of individuals have more self-critical standards than others. However, for such a factor to account for the present findings, stricter hostility self- standards would also have to cause hyperreactivity and efficient thought control. Is this plausible? Although debates about the for engaging in a defensive process have raged for some time, Weinberger (1990) persuasively argues that individuals are motivated to be self-defensive when infor- mation contradicts strongly held beliefs about the self. initiated this line of thought by suggesting that defense "proceeds from self- respect of the ego. The very impressions, experiences, impulses and desires that one man indulges . . . will be rejected with the utmost in- dignation by another . . ." (cited in Weinberger, 1990, p. 343). Thus, even those within the psychodynamic tradition believe that differential self-standards (overactive superegos, in psychoanalytic parlance) cause suppression and repression, and thus could cause blood-pressure hyper- reactivity and the ability to avoid unwanted thoughts. The differential self-standard hypothesis can clearly be empirically tested: Suppressors should overestimate hostility levels in standardized stimuli, or should rate hostile material as relatively more hostile when told to attribute the material to themselves versus others. However, conceptual controversy will remain over the substantive versus methodological interpretation of self-other disagreements. This study examined only the trait hostility, because it was this trait that was thought to cause increased SBP reactivity. It would be useful to investigate other traits, such as anxiety, and properties of a trait, such as personal aversiveness, that lead to both self-friend disagreements Self-Other Disagreement 687 and substantive outcomes. Future studies could also examine the limits of a relationship variable, such as intimacy, to see if this variable af- fects the relation between self-friend disagreement and SBP reactivity. For example, one would expect that the disagreement of more intimate partners would better predict SBP reactivity than the disagreements of less intimate ones. The main finding of this article is that self-other disagreements can be useful for measuring other constructs and testing hypotheses about these constructs. Although two subject variables have been the focus of this line of research, trait, other, and relationship variables that would both cause self-other disagreements and are recalcitrant to operationalization may now be explored.

REFERENCES

Barefoot. J. C. (1992). Developments in the measurement of hostility. InH. S. Fried- man (Ed.). Hostility, and health (pp. 13-32). Washington. DC: American Psychological Association. Barefoot, J. C, Dahlstrom. W. G.. & Williams, R. B. (1983). Hostility. CHD inci- dence, and total mortality: A 25-year follow-up study of 255 physicians. Psychoso- matic Medicine, 45, 59-63. Barefoot. J. C. Dodge. K. A., Peterson. B. L., Dahlstrom, W. G.. & Williams, R. B.. Jr. (1989). The Cook-Medley Hostility scale: Item content and ability to predict survival. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51. 46-57. Block, J. (1977), Advancing the science of personality: Paradigmatic shift or improving the quality of research? In D. Magnusson & N, S. Endler (Eds.). Personality Psy- chology at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 37-63). Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bower. G. H. (1990). Awareness, the unconscious, and repression: An experimental psychologist's perspective, InJ, E, Singer {Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Impli- cations for personality theory, psychopathology. and health (pp. 209-232). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brody. L. R. (1985). Gender differences in emotional development: A review of theories and research. Journal of Personality, 53. 102-149. Buck. R. (1979). Individual differences in nonverbal sending accuracy and electroder- mal responding: The externalizing-intemalizing dimension. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.). Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences (pp. 140-170). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager. Gunn. & Hain. Buck. R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion./*.rvc/io- logicalReview, 92. 389-413. Buck, R, (1988). Nonverbal communication: Spontaneous and symbolic aspects, American Behavioral Scientist, 31. 341-354, Cacioppo. J. T.. Uchino. B, N.. Crites. S. L,. Snydersmith. M. A. Smith. G.. Bern- ston, G. G., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Relationship between facial expressiveness and Davidson

sympathetic activation in emotion: A critical review, with emphasis on modeling underlying mechanisms and individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62. 110-128. Carey, R. M., Reid. R. A.. Ayers. C. R.. Lynch. S. S,, McLain. W. L.. & Vaughan. E, D. (1976). The Charlottesv 11 le blood pressure survey: Value of repeated blood pressure measurements. Journal of American Medical Association, 236. 847-851, Chesney. M. A., Eagleston. J. R.. & Rosenman. R. H, (1980), The Type A Structured Interview: A behavioral assessment in the rough. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 2, 255-272. Cohen, J.. & Cohen. P. (1983). Applied multiple regression and correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nded,). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cook. W. W,, & Medley, D. M. (1954). Proposed Hostility and Pharisaic-Virtue scales for the MMPI. Journal of Applied Psychology. 38, 414-418, Costa, P. T, Jr., & McCrae. R. R, (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Costa. P. T.. Jr.. & McCrae. R, R. (1987). Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: is the bark worse than the bite? Journal of Personality, 55. 299-316. Costa. P T.. Jr.. McCrae. R. R., & Dembroski. T M, (1989), Agreeableness vs. an- tagonism: Explication of a potential risk factor for CHD. inQ, W. Siegmen &T. M. Dembroski {Eds.), In search t^coronary-prone behavior (pp. 41-63). Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Costa. P. T,, Jr.. Zonderman. A. B., McCrae. R. R.. & Williams. R. B.. Jr. (1986). Cynicism and paranoid alienation in the Cook and Medley HO scale. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48. 283-285. Cottington. E. M.. Matthews. K, A,. Talbott. E.. & Kuller. L. H. (1986). Occupational stress, suppressed anger, and hypertension. Psychosomatic Medicine, 48. 249-260. Cramer, P. (1991). The development cf defense mechanisms: Theory, research, and assessment. New York: Springer-Verlag. Davies. M, H. (1971). Is high blood pressure a psychosomatic disorder? A critical review of the evidence. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 24, 239-258, Davis. P. J. (1990). Repression and the inaccessibility of emotional memories. In J. E. Singer (Ed.). Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology, and health (pp. 387-404). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Edwards. A. L. {1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. New York: Holt. Rinehart & Winston. Erdelyi. M. H. (1990), Repression, reconstruction, and defense: History and integra- tion of the psychoanalytic and experimental frameworks. In J. E, Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications for persoruxlity theory, psychopathology, and health (pp. 1-32). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Erdelyi. M. H.. & Goldberg, B. (1979). Let's not sweep repression under the rug: Toward a cognitive psychology of repression. In J, F. Kihlstrom & F, J. Evans (Eds.). Functional disorders of (pp, 355-402). Hiltsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Funder. D. C. (1980). On seeing ourselves as others see us: Self-other agreement and discrepancy in personality ratings. Journal of Personality. 48, 473-493. Funder. D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social mcnt. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75-90, Seli-Other Disagreement 689

Funder, D. C. & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judgement. Journal of Personatity and Social Psychology, $5. 149-158. Greer, S., & Morris, T. (1975). Psychological attributes of women who develop breast cancer: A controlled study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 19, 147-153. Harburg, E.. Erfurt. J. C, Hauenstein, L. S., Chape, C, Schull, W, J., & Schork, M. A. (1973). Soc io ecological stress, suppressed hostility, skin color, and black- white male blood pressure: Detroit. Psychosomatic Medicine. 35, 276-296. Hardy, J. D., & Smith, T. W. (1988). Cynical hostility and vulnerability to disease: Social support, life stress, and physiological response to conflict. Health Psychology, 7, 447-459. Hearn, M. D., Murray, D. M., & Luepker, R. V. (1989). Hostility, coronary heart dis- ease, and total mortality: A 33-year follow-up study of university students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 12, 105-121. Holmes, D. S. (1974). Investigation of repression: Differential recall of material ex- perimentally or naturally associated with ego threat. Psychological Bulletin, 81. 632-653. Holmes, D. S. (1990). Tbe evidence for repression: An examination of sixty years of research. In J. E. Singer (Ed.). Repression and dissociation: Implications for per- sonality theory, psychopathology. and health (pp. 85-102). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Horowitz, M. (1975). Intrusive and repetitive thoughts after experimental stress. Ar- chives of General Psychiatry, 32. 1457-1463. Houston, B. K. (1992). Personality characteristics, reactivity, and cardiovascular dis- ease. In J. R. Turner, A. Sherwood, & K. C. Light (Eds.). Individual differences in cardiovascular response to stress (pp. 103-123). New York: Plenum. John. O. P.. Donahue, E. M., & Kentle. R. L. (1992). The 'Big Five" inventory- Versions 4a and 54. Unpublished manuscript. Institute of Personality Assessment and Researcb, Berkeley. Jones, H. E. (1950). The study of patterns of emotional expression. In M. Reymert (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 161-168). New York: McGraw-Hill. Julius. S., Weder. A. B., & Hinderliter. A. L. (1986). Does behaviorally induced blood pressure variability lead to hypertension? ln K. A. Matthews, S. M. Weiss, B. Falkner, S. B. Manuck, & R. B. Williams, Jr. (Eds.), Handbook of stress reactivity and cardiovascular disease (pp. 71-82). New York: Wiley. Kenny, D. A. (1991). A general model of consensus and accuracy in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 98, 155-163. Lazanis, R. S. (1983). The costs and benefits of . In S. Breznitz (Ed.), The denial of stress (pp. 1-30). New York: International Universities Press. Leon, G. R., Finn, S. E., Murray, D.. & Bailey, J. M. (1988). Inability to pre- dict cardiovascular disease from hostility scores or MMPI items related to Type A bebavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 56, 597-600. MacDougall, J. M., Dembroski, T. M., Dimsdale, J. E,, & Hackett, T. P. (1985). Components of T^pe A. hostility, and anger-in: Further relationships to angiograpbic findings. Health Psychology, 4, 137-152. Malatesta. C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: Tbe socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Development, 53, 991-1003. Davidson

McGranahan, D. V. (1940). A critical and experimental study of repression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35, 212-225. McGinn, N. F., Harburg, E., Julius, S,, & McLeod, J. M. (1964). Psychological correlates of blood pressure. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 209-219. Mills, R J., Schneider. R. H.. & Dimsdale. J. E. (1989). Anger assessment and reactivity to stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 33. 379-382. Nisbett, R. E.. & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and .shortcomings of sociat judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Norman. W. T.. & Goldberg, L. R. {1966). Raters, ratees, and randomness in person- ality structure. Journal of Personatity and Sociat Psychotogy. 4. 681-691. Paunonen, S. V. (1989). Consensus in personality judgments: Moderating effects of larget-rater acquaintanceship and behavior observability. Journat of Personality and Social Psychotogy. 56. 823-833. Pennebaker. J. W., Hughes, C, & O'Heeron. R. C. (1987). The psychophysiology of confession: Linking inhibitory and psychosomatic processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychotogy, 52, 781-793. Shekelle. R. B.. Gale. M., Ostfeld. A. M., & Paul. O. (1983). Hostility, risk of coronary heart disease, and mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine. 45, 109-114. Shevrin. H. (1990). Subliminal perception and repression. In J. E. Singer (Ed.). ^^• pression and dissociation: Imptications for personatity theory, psychopathology, and heatth (pp. 103-120). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Siegel. J.M. (1985). The measurement of anger as a multidimensional construct. In M. S. Chesney & R. H. Rosenman (Edi.). Anger and hostitity in cardiovascutar and behavioral disorders (pp. 59-81). New York: Hemisphere/McGraw-Hill. Siegel, J. M. (1992). Anger and cardiovascular health. lnH. S. Friedman (Ed.).//OT- titity, coping and health (pp. 49-64). Washington. DC: American Psychological Association. Singer, J. E. (Ed.). (1990). Repression and dissociation: Imptications for personality theory, psychopathology, and heatth. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, Smith, M. A.. & Houston. B. K. (1987). Hostility, anger expression, cardiovascular responsivity. and social support. Biotogical Psychology, 24. 39-48. Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Current status of a psychosomatic hypothesis. Health Psychology, II. 139-150. Smith, T. W.. & Williams, P. G. (1992). Personality and health: Advantages and limitations of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 395-423. Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Boston: Little. Brown. Vaillant, G. E. (1990). Repression in college men followed for half a century. In J. E. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology. and heatth (pp. 259-274). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Vaiilant, G. E. (1992). Ego mechanisms of defense: A guide for clinicians and re- searchers. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Vaillant, G. E., & Vaillant, C. O. (1990). Natural history of male psychological health: XII. A forty-five year study of predictors of successful aging at age 65. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 31-37. Watson. D. (1989). Strangers" ratings ofthe five robust personality factors: Evidence of a surprising convergence with self-report. Journal of Personality and Sociat Psy- chotogy, 57. 120-128. Self-Other Disagreement 691

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J,, Carter, S, R,, & White. T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression, Journat of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13, Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D, J,, Knutson. B,. & McMahon, S. R. (1991). Pollut- ing the stream of : The effect of thought suppression on the mind's emironmenl. Journal of Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 141-152. Weinberger, D. A. (1990), The construct validity of the repressive coping style. In J, E. Singer (Ed,), Repression and dissociation: Imptications for personatity theory, psychopathology, and health (pp. 337-386). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Manuscript received October 3, 1992; revised April 6, t993.