The Grammatical Properties of Mass Nouns] an Aphasia Case Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Grammatical Properties of Mass Nouns] an Aphasia Case Study Neuropsycholo`ia\ Vol[ 24\ No[ 4\ pp[ 558Ð564\ 0886 Þ 0886 Elsevier Science Ltd[ All rights reserved Pergamon PII] S9917Ð2821"85#99013Ð7 Printed in Great Britain \ 9917Ð2821:86 ,06[99¦9[99 The grammatical properties of mass nouns] An aphasia case study CARLO SEMENZA\$ SARA MONDINI$ and MARINELLA CAPPELLETTI$ Dipartimento di Psicologia\ Universita di Trieste\ Triest\ Italy^ $Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale\ Universita di Padova\ Padova\ Italy "Received 29 April 0885^ accepted 15 October 0885# Abstract*A patient "F[A[# is described who\ as a consequence of brain damage\ shows an isolated de_cit concerning the use\ across a series of tasks\ of the grammatical properties of mass:non!countable nouns[ Her use of grammar is otherwise perfect[ This behaviour dissociates from that of other patients who have severe grammatical di.culties\ but do not show any impairment in the mass nouns tasks that F[A[ fails[ This case is thought to demonstrate how speci_c grammatical rules\ that are said to be stored at the lemma level of lexical retrieval\ are indeed independently represented and accessible[ Þ 0886 Elsevier Science Ltd[ Key Words] non!countable nouns^ lemma[ Introduction two major classes of nouns\ i[e[ common nouns "the other being proper nouns#[ More precisely\ there are features In a speaker|s mental lexicon each item is viewed\ accord! that separate common nouns into count nouns and mass ing to recent psycholinguistic models ð1\ 05Ł\ as a listing of nouns along syntactic lines "just as other features dis! features specifying "a# the item|s meaning\ "b# its syntactic tinguish common nouns from proper nouns#[ properties\ "c# its morphology and "d# its phonology[ The Contemporary studies of aphasia have described sev! _rst two sets of features constitute what is called\ in a eral subtle de_cits consisting of dissociations of functions widely followed tradition started by Kempen and that seem to support theoretically grounded distinctions[ Huijbers ð04Ł\ the {lemma| component of each lexical The emerging picture is one characterized by a system in entry[ This component is thus distinguished from the so! which di}erent components of language processing are called {lexeme| component which speci_es the mor! individually dissociated[ For example\ Caramazza and phophonological features[ In particular a lemma|s syn! Miceli ð6Ł described a patient for whom the dissociability tactic information ð05Ł speci_es the item|s syntactic of de_cit of thematic role assignment "a lemma level category\ its assignment of grammatical function and a function# from de_cits of morphological and function set of diacritic features or parameters[ Thus\ for a given word processing mechanisms could be documented word\ a lemma component may contain\ for example\ the unambigously[ The authors maintained that these data information that the word belongs to the category of postulate a thematic role assignment mechanism that verbs and not to the category of nouns or to any other operates independently of other lexical and grammatical grammatical category[ The information that a given word processing mechanisms[ Studies of this kind appear to may require a subject\ a direct object or a verbal comp! support the idea that the syntactic speci_cation of a word lement is also contained in the lemma component and so may be stored more independently than the lemma level are the tense\ the person\ the number\ etc[\ and even theory seems to admit[ the pitch accent[ The particular lemma level features of In the present study a patient is described\ who shows interest in the present investigation concern one of the how it is possible to be able to retrieve a certain category of words "in this case mass nouns# and yet to be unable to take into account\ in both production and reception\ Address for correspondence and requests for reprints] Carlo Semenza\ Dipartimento di Psicologia\ Universita di Trieste\ via the syntactic information that distinguishes that category dell|Universita 6\ 23012 Trieste\ Italy^ fax] 28"39#201161^ e! from other categories[ It will be argued that this dis! mail] SemenzaÝuniv[trieste[it[ sociation may be attributed to a selective loss of speci_c 558 569 C[ Semenza et al[:An aphasia case study grammatical rules that are thus demonstrated to be inde! enumerated in each single component but may or may pendently stored and processed at the lemma level of not share the feature of countability[ Those that are coun! lexical retrieval[ table "like ~ock# obey the same rules for count nouns\ while for the others "like furniture# the rules for mass nouns apply[ Count and mass nouns^ the notion of countability The distinction between count and mass nouns is thus complex and\ it must be added\ by no means clear!cut[ A very basic distinction\ learned from early childhood\ In many cases\ a given word belonging to one or other is that between compact\ enduring things and the stu}s category is determined by the context] thus Bunt ð2Ł of which they are constituted[ The former are counted\ writes] {{the countÐmass distinction is not really a dis! the latter are measured[ Philosophers "see Pelletier ð10Ł tinction between words\ but a distinction between ways and Burge ð3Ł for a review# have traditionally treated this of using the words||[ For example Pelletier ð10Ł considers distinction with categories like form and matter\ sub! the "supposed# mass noun oatmeal] given an appropriate stance and substrate\ mode and extension\ non!sortal and setting "such as a customer in a diner to the waitress#\ it sortal[ The distinction between count nouns and mass is clear a question such as] {{How many oatmeals are in nouns is the linguistic analogue of these category pairs[ your kitchen<|| has as a clear answer\ {{Three oatmeals||\ The following de_nitions are taken from Macnamara and thus violating the two related criteria of mass nouns Reyes ð06Ł[ A word is assigned to the syntactic category accepting neither pluralization nor numeral quanti_ers[ mass nouns "like water\ calcium\ oatmeal# if the samples A way around these di.culties is to observe that in such to which it is applied are taken as constituting in com! cases there is transfer of meaning\ e[g[ in a _gurative sense bination a larger sample^ they denote monadic entities as in smooth waters[ Or that\ as in the example a co}ee whose boundaries are perceptually inaccessible[ Count "perfectly acceptable in Italian when ordering a cup of nouns are\ instead\ words "like do`\ window\ hammer# co}ee#\ such phrase as {{a cup of || has been deleted from applied to perceptual entities that in combination do not the surface structure[ Thus\ the ultimate answer as to yield another entity of the same kind[ Thus the jux! whether a given name is a count or a mass noun depends taposition of two dogs does not yield a single dog\ upon being able to decide\ for any speci_c use "or alter! whereas singles bodies of water can be combined to yield natively any speci_c {sense| in Frege|s ð09Ł terms#\ whether another single body of water\ dissolving the boundaries it is a count or a mass use "or sense#[ Other di.culties of the two original components[ stem from the fact that the use made of the mass:count These semantic distinctions are mirrored by gram! contrast for grammar is somehow di}erent from the use matical properties that di}erentiate the use of the two that has been made of the same "or analogous# distinction categories ð05Ł[ Indeed it has been claimed\ with exper! in philosophy[ For example\ the grammatical distinction imental support ð01Ł\ that in both children and adults the applies only to nouns\ whereas the philosophical dis! representation of the count:mass distinction is not based tinction is usually held to apply to all monadic entities[ on the semantic notion of objects and substance\ but Furthermore the grammatical distinction seems to apply rather on syntactic properties relating to the role that easily to certain abstract terms] thus knowled`e or count nouns and mass nouns play in syntactic construc! jealousy are considered mass nouns\ whereas plot or tion[ Thus\ in languages like Italian or English "there are quality are count[ Many philosophers\ however\ would minor di}erences between Italian and English that are be reluctant to apply a mass:count distinction to such not relevant here*see Renzi ð11Ł or Chierchia ð8Ł^ mind instances[ also that other languages may di}er completely] in Chi! For the purpose of the present study\ where only nese or Japanese for example it can be argued that all concrete\ central samples of the two classes of nouns will nouns are mass#\ mass nouns\ unlike count nouns] be used in an unambiguous context\ all these di.culties can be safely set aside[ cannot take the plural\ cannot take inde_nite articles\ Case report cannot take quanti_ers that necessarily denumerate like one\ two\[[[[[[another\ many\ each\ several\ etc[ F[A[ was a 62!year!old housewife who had received a They can take\ however\ quanti_ers "like little or much# formal education of 7 years and spoke a rather standard that do not necessarily denumerate[ Instead count nouns\ type of Italian\ which is not common among women of unlike mass nouns] her age in the part of Italy "the province of Vicenza# she came from[ In the past she had helped her husband in a can take the plural\ business that entailed public relations and travelling all can take de_nite and inde_nite articles\ over northern Italy[ She had no record of developmental cannot take quanti_ers that do not denumerate like linguistic abnormalities[ much and little[ In August
Recommended publications
  • Definiteness Determined by Syntax: a Case Study in Tagalog 1 Introduction
    Definiteness determined by syntax: A case study in Tagalog1 James N. Collins Abstract Using Tagalog as a case study, this paper provides an analysis of a cross-linguistically well attested phenomenon, namely, cases in which a bare NP’s syntactic position is linked to its interpretation as definite or indefinite. Previous approaches to this phenomenon, including analyses of Tagalog, appeal to specialized interpretational rules, such as Diesing’s Mapping Hypothesis. I argue that the patterns fall out of general compositional principles so long as type-shifting operators are available to the gram- matical system. I begin by weighing in a long-standing issue for the semantic analysis of Tagalog: the interpretational distinction between genitive and nominative transitive patients. I show that bare NP patients are interpreted as definites if marked with nominative case and as narrow scope indefinites if marked with genitive case. Bare NPs are understood as basically predicative; their quantificational force is determined by their syntactic position. If they are syntactically local to the selecting verb, they are existentially quantified by the verb itself. If they occupy a derived position, such as the subject position, they must type-shift in order to avoid a type-mismatch, generating a definite interpretation. Thus the paper develops a theory of how the position of an NP is linked to its interpretation, as well as providing a compositional treatment of NP-interpretation in a language which lacks definite articles but demonstrates other morphosyntactic strategies for signaling (in)definiteness. 1 Introduction Not every language signals definiteness via articles. Several languages (such as Russian, Kazakh, Korean etc.) lack articles altogether.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2106.08037V1 [Cs.CL] 15 Jun 2021 Alternative Ways the World Could Be
    The Possible, the Plausible, and the Desirable: Event-Based Modality Detection for Language Processing Valentina Pyatkin∗ Shoval Sadde∗ Aynat Rubinstein Bar Ilan University Bar Ilan University Hebrew University of Jerusalem [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Paul Portner Reut Tsarfaty Georgetown University Bar Ilan University [email protected] [email protected] Abstract (1) a. We presented a paper at ACL’19. Modality is the linguistic ability to describe b. We did not present a paper at ACL’20. events with added information such as how de- sirable, plausible, or feasible they are. Modal- The propositional content p =“present a paper at ity is important for many NLP downstream ACL’X” can be easily verified for sentences (1a)- tasks such as the detection of hedging, uncer- (1b) by looking up the proceedings of the confer- tainty, speculation, and more. Previous studies ence to (dis)prove the existence of the relevant pub- that address modality detection in NLP often p restrict modal expressions to a closed syntac- lication. The same proposition is still referred to tic class, and the modal sense labels are vastly in sentences (2a)–(2d), but now in each one, p is different across different studies, lacking an ac- described from a different perspective: cepted standard. Furthermore, these senses are often analyzed independently of the events that (2) a. We aim to present a paper at ACL’21. they modify. This work builds on the theoreti- b. We want to present a paper at ACL’21. cal foundations of the Georgetown Gradable Modal Expressions (GME) work by Rubin- c.
    [Show full text]
  • Calculus of Possibilities As a Technique in Linguistic Typology
    Calculus of possibilities as a technique in linguistic typology Igor Mel’uk 1. The problem stated: A unified conceptual system in linguistics A central problem in the relationship between typology and the writing of individual grammars is that of developing a cross-linguistically viable con- ceptual system and a corresponding terminological framework. I will deal with this problem in three consecutive steps: First, I state the problem and sketch a conceptual system that I have put forward for typological explora- tions in morphology (Sections 1 and 2). Second, I propose a detailed illus- tration of this system: a calculus of grammatical voices in natural languages (Section 3). And third, I apply this calculus (that is, the corresponding con- cepts) in two particular case studies: an inflectional category known as an- tipassive and the grammatical voice in French (Sections 4 and 5). In the latter case, the investigation shows that even for a language as well de- scribed as French a rigorously standardized typological framework can force us to answer questions that previous descriptions have failed to re- solve. I start with the following three assumptions: 1) One of the most pressing tasks of today’s linguistics is description of particular languages, the essential core of this work being the writing of grammars and lexicons. A linguist sets out to describe a language as pre- cisely and exhaustively as possible; this includes its semantics, syntax, morphology and phonology plus (within the limits of time and funds avail- able) its lexicon. 2) Such a description is necessarily carried out in terms of some prede- fined concepts – such as lexical unit, semantic actant, syntactic role, voice, case, phoneme, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Respiratory Therapy Pocket Reference
    Pulmonary Physiology Volume Control Pressure Control Pressure Support Respiratory Therapy “AC” Assist Control; AC-VC, ~CMV (controlled mandatory Measure of static lung compliance. If in AC-VC, perform a.k.a. a.k.a. AC-PC; Assist Control Pressure Control; ~CMV-PC a.k.a PS (~BiPAP). Spontaneous: Pressure-present inspiratory pause (when there is no flow, there is no effect ventilation = all modes with RR and fixed Ti) PPlateau of Resistance; Pplat@Palv); or set Pause Time ~0.5s; RR, Pinsp, PEEP, FiO2, Flow Trigger, rise time, I:E (set Pocket Reference RR, Vt, PEEP, FiO2, Flow Trigger, Flow pattern, I:E (either Settings Pinsp, PEEP, FiO2, Flow Trigger, Rise time Target: < 30, Optimal: ~ 25 Settings directly or by inspiratory time Ti) Settings directly or via peak flow, Ti settings) Decreasing Ramp (potentially more physiologic) PIP: Total inspiratory work by vent; Reflects resistance & - Decreasing Ramp (potentially more physiologic) Card design by Respiratory care providers from: Square wave/constant vs Decreasing Ramp (potentially Flow Determined by: 1) PS level, 2) R, Rise Time (­ rise time ® PPeak inspiratory compliance; Normal ~20 cmH20 (@8cc/kg and adult ETT); - Peak Flow determined by 1) Pinsp level, 2) R, 3)Ti (shorter Flow more physiologic) ¯ peak flow and 3.) pt effort Resp failure 30-40 (low VT use); Concern if >40. Flow = more flow), 4) pressure rise time (¯ Rise Time ® ­ Peak v 0.9 Flow), 5) pt effort (­ effort ® ­ peak flow) Pplat-PEEP: tidal stress (lung injury & mortality risk). Target Determined by set RR, Vt, & Flow Pattern (i.e. for any set I:E Determined by patient effort & flow termination (“Esens” – PDriving peak flow, Square (¯ Ti) & Ramp (­ Ti); Normal Ti: 1-1.5s; see below “Breath Termination”) < 15 cmH2O.
    [Show full text]
  • Categories of Nouns
    Categories of nouns Nouns describe people, places or things. They also express a range of meanings such as concepts, qualities, organisations, communities, sensations and events. What do they look like? A small proportion of nouns have identifiable ‘noun endings’. These include: Tradition, ability, excellence, significance, factor, rigour. Many plural nouns end in ‘s’, e.g. cats Proper Nouns and Capital Letters Words which begin with capital letters but are not always at the beginning of sentences are often the names of people, places (town, countries, etc.) or institutions. These are called proper nouns. Example Lauren and Jack, Africa, International House A capital letter is also used for days of the week, months of the year, and the names of nationalities, ethnic groups and languages. Example Tuesday, August, Swahili Where do nouns come in sentences? Nouns can: o act as the subject of a verb: Example Cats kill mice o act as the object of a verb: Example Cats kill mice Developed by Learning Advisers 1 o act as the complement of a verb: Example They are men They often end a phrase which begins with an article such as a, an or a quantifier such as either, any, or many. They also often follow adjectives: Adjective Adjective Example A drunk either way a much older elite larger mice Countable and Uncountable Nouns Countable or ‘unit’ nouns [C] have a singular and a plural form, e.g. bookbooks. Uncountable or ‘mass’ nouns [U] have only one form, e.g. furniture. [C] [U] Singular Plural another biscuit three apples not much success The distinction between countable and uncountable is based on whether or not we can count what the noun describes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Interaction Between Telicity and Agentivity: Experimental Evidence from Intransitive Verbs in German and Chinese
    Originally published in: Lingua vol. 200 (2017), pp. 84-106. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2017.08.006 POSTPRINT a,b,* a a a,b Tim Graf , Markus Philipp , Xiaonan Xu , Franziska Kretzschmar , a Beatrice Primus a Institute of German Language and Literature I, University of Cologne, Albertus-Magnus-Platz, 50923 Cologne, Germany b CRC 1252 Prominence in Language, University of Cologne, Germany The interaction between telicity and agentivity: Experimental evidence from intransitive verbs in German and Chinese Abstract: Telicity and agentivity are semantic factors that split intransitive verbs into (at least two) different classes. Clear-cut unergative verbs, which select the auxiliary HAVE, are assumed to be atelic and agent-selecting; unequivocally unaccusative verbs, which select the auxiliary BE, are analyzed as telic and patient-selecting. Thus, agentivity and telicity are assumed to be inversely correlated in split intransitivity. We will present semantic and experimental evidence from German and Mandarin Chinese that casts doubts on this widely held assumption. The focus of our experimental investigation lies on variation with respect to agentivity (specifically motion control, manipulated via animacy), telicity (tested via a locative vs. goal adverbial), and BE/HAVE-selection with semantically flexible intransitive verbs of motion. Our experimental methods are acceptability ratings for German and Chinese (Experiments 1 and 2) and event-related potential (ERP) measures for German (Experiment 3). Our findings contradict the above- mentioned assumption that agentivity and telicity are generally inversely correlated and suggest that for the verbs under study, agentivity and telicity harmonize with each other. Furthermore, the ERP measures reveal that the impact of the interaction under discussion is more pronounced on the verb lexeme than on the auxiliary.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammatical Gender and Linguistic Complexity
    Grammatical gender and linguistic complexity Volume I: General issues and specific studies Edited by Francesca Di Garbo Bruno Olsson Bernhard Wälchli language Studies in Diversity Linguistics 26 science press Studies in Diversity Linguistics Editor: Martin Haspelmath In this series: 1. Handschuh, Corinna. A typology of marked-S languages. 2. Rießler, Michael. Adjective attribution. 3. Klamer, Marian (ed.). The Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology. 4. Berghäll, Liisa. A grammar of Mauwake (Papua New Guinea). 5. Wilbur, Joshua. A grammar of Pite Saami. 6. Dahl, Östen. Grammaticalization in the North: Noun phrase morphosyntax in Scandinavian vernaculars. 7. Schackow, Diana. A grammar of Yakkha. 8. Liljegren, Henrik. A grammar of Palula. 9. Shimelman, Aviva. A grammar of Yauyos Quechua. 10. Rudin, Catherine & Bryan James Gordon (eds.). Advances in the study of Siouan languages and linguistics. 11. Kluge, Angela. A grammar of Papuan Malay. 12. Kieviet, Paulus. A grammar of Rapa Nui. 13. Michaud, Alexis. Tone in Yongning Na: Lexical tones and morphotonology. 14. Enfield, N. J. (ed.). Dependencies in language: On the causal ontology of linguistic systems. 15. Gutman, Ariel. Attributive constructions in North-Eastern Neo-Aramaic. 16. Bisang, Walter & Andrej Malchukov (eds.). Unity and diversity in grammaticalization scenarios. 17. Stenzel, Kristine & Bruna Franchetto (eds.). On this and other worlds: Voices from Amazonia. 18. Paggio, Patrizia and Albert Gatt (eds.). The languages of Malta. 19. Seržant, Ilja A. & Alena Witzlack-Makarevich (eds.). Diachrony of differential argument marking. 20. Hölzl, Andreas. A typology of questions in Northeast Asia and beyond: An ecological perspective. 21. Riesberg, Sonja, Asako Shiohara & Atsuko Utsumi (eds.). Perspectives on information structure in Austronesian languages.
    [Show full text]
  • “Doing Gender” in Doctor-Patient Interactions
    “DOING GENDER” IN DOCTOR-PATIENT INTERACTIONS: GENDER COMPOSITION OF DOCTOR-PATIENT DYADS AND COMMUNICATION PATTERNS A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Kelly MacArthur August, 2008 Thesis written by Kelly MacArthur B.A., Fairleigh Dickinson University 2004 M.A., Kent State University, 2008 Approved by , Advisor Timothy Gallagher , Chair, Department of Sociology Richard Serpe , Dean, College of Arts and Sciences John R.D. Stalvey ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ...................................................................................................................iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ......................................................................................................v 1. INTRODUCTION.................................................................................................................1 2. LITERATURE REVIEW......................................................................................................4 Language and Gender...................................................................................................4 The Turn-Taking Model...............................................................................................7 “Doing Gender” in Doctor-Patient Interactions .........................................................14 The Environment in Which Women Physicians Work...............................................16 Do Women and Men Communicate Differently?.....................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Automatically Generating Psychiatric Case Notes from Digital Transcripts of Doctor-Patient Conversations
    Automatically Generating Psychiatric Case Notes From Digital Transcripts of Doctor-Patient Conversations Nazmul Kazi Indika Kahanda Gianforte School of Computing Gianforte School of Computing Montana State University Montana State University Bozeman, MT, USA Bozeman, MT, USA [email protected] [email protected] Abstract Limited face-to-face time is especially disadvan- tageous for working with mental health patients Electronic health records (EHRs) are notori- where the psychiatrist could easily miss a non- ous for reducing the face-to-face time with verbal cue highly important for the correct diag- patients while increasing the screen-time for clinicians leading to burnout. This is espe- nosis. Moreover, EHR’s usability related prob- cially problematic for psychiatry care in which lems lead to unstructured and incomplete case maintaining consistent eye-contact and non- notes (Kaufman et al., 2016) which are difficult verbal cues are just as important as the spoken to search and access. words. In this ongoing work, we explore the Due to the above-mentioned downsides of feasibility of automatically generating psychi- EHRs, there have been recent attempts for de- atric EHR case notes from digital transcripts veloping novel methods for incorporating vari- of doctor-patient conversation using a two-step approach: (1) predicting semantic topics for ous techniques and technologies such as natu- segments of transcripts using supervised ma- ral language processing (NLP) for improving the chine learning, and (2) generating formal text EHR documentation process. In 2015, American of those segments using natural language pro- Medical Informatics Association reported time- cessing. Through a series of preliminary ex- consuming data entry is one of the major prob- perimental results obtained through a collec- lems in EHRs and recommended to improve EHRs tion of synthetic and real-life transcripts, we by allowing multiple modes of data entry such demonstrate the viability of this approach.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Positive Versus Negative Language Use by a Physician
    THE EFFECTS OF POSITIVE VERSUS NEGATIVE LANGUAGE USE BY A PHYSICIAN To what extent does positive versus negative language used by a physician impact the patient outcomes of patients with nonspecific low back pain? A randomized study using video vignettes. Bachelor Thesis De invloed van positief versus negatief taalgebruik door een huisarts op patiëntuitkomsten. May 15, 2017 RADBOUD UNIVERSITY NIJMEGEN International Business Communication I.A. Stortenbeker M. Balsters Iris Nijemeisland, S4333950 [email protected] 06-23861611 Words: 7063 2 Summary The way in which a physician communicates has a great impact on patients, but little research has been focused on examining which specific words influence patient outcomes. Given the fact that up till 95% of people will experience low back pain at one point during their lives, this research tried to answer to what extent positive versus negative language used by a physician impacts the patient outcomes of patients with nonspecific low back pain. By using two different versions of fragments in a video vignette the researchers tried to measure any differences in analogue patients with regards to kinesiophobia (‘fear of moving’), recovery expectations, intention to therapy compliance and trust in the physician. This experiment did not indicate any present relationship between either a positive or a negative version of language use, and its impact on patient outcomes of people with nonspecific low back pain. No significant results with regard to these variables were found. After making a distinction between participants with a medical occupation and those with non-medical backgrounds, a significant result for the variable ‘empathy towards the patient’ was found.
    [Show full text]
  • The Grammatical Category of Modality∗
    The grammatical category of modality∗ Valentine Hacquard University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, United States [email protected] Abstract In many languages, the same words are used to express epistemic and root modality. These modals further tend to interact with tense and aspect in systematic ways, based on their interpretation. Is this pattern accidental, or a consequence of grammar or mean- ing? I address this question by: (i) comparing `grammatical' modals to verbs/adjectives that share meanings with modals, but not the same scope constraints; (ii) examining pat- terns of grammaticalization from `lexical' to `grammatical' modality; (iii) comparing scope interactions in languages where modals are 'polysemous' and in those where they are not. 1 Introduction In many languages, the same modal words are used to express a variety of `root' and `epistemic' meanings. English may, for instance, can express deontic or epistemic possibility. About half of the 200+ languages in [vdAA05] have a single form that is used to express both kinds of modality. Yet, in many other languages, modal markers are unambiguously determined for meaning. In the Kratzerian tradition ([Kra81, Kra91, Kra12]), modals are lexically specified only for force (as existential or universal quantifiers over worlds), and the various meanings a `polysemous'1 modal expresses arise from the modal combining with various modal bases and ordering sources. This account, based on the case of polysemous languages, easily extends to non-polysemous ones: a modal can further lexically specify the kind of modal base and ordering source it allows, restricting its meaning to a single epistemic or root meaning. An alternative account based on non-polysemous cases would provide separate lexical entries for the various modals, and extend to polysemous cases by postulating ambiguity.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:1909.02224V2 [Cs.CL] 9 Sep 2019 Code Gender Bias in Society (Bolukbasi Et Al., 2016; Other Languages, Such As French (FR)
    Examining Gender Bias in Languages with Grammatical Gender Pei Zhou1;2, Weijia Shi1, Jieyu Zhao1, Kuan-Hao Huang1, Muhao Chen1;3, Ryan Cotterell4, Kai-Wei Chang1 1Department of Computer Science, University of California Los Angeles 2Department of Computer Science, University of Southern California 3Department of Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania 4Department of Computer Science, Johns Hopkins University [email protected]; fswj0419,jyzhao, khhuang, [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] Abstract in English word embeddings cannot be directly applied to languages with grammatical gender1, Recent studies have shown that word embed- where all nouns are assigned a gender class and dings exhibit gender bias inherited from the the corresponding dependent articles, adjectives, training corpora. However, most studies to and verbs must agree in gender with the noun (e.g. date have focused on quantifying and mitigat- ing such bias only in English. These analy- in Spanish: la buena enfermera the good female ses cannot be directly extended to languages nurse, el buen enfermero the good male nurse) (Cor- that exhibit morphological agreement on gen- bett, 1991, 2006). This is because most existing ap- der, such as Spanish and French. In this paper, proaches define bias in word embeddings based on we propose new metrics for evaluating gender the projection of a word on a gender direction (e.g. bias in word embeddings of these languages “nurse” in English is biased because its projection and further demonstrate evidence of gender on the gender direction inclines towards female). bias in bilingual embeddings which align these When the grammatical gender exists, such bias def- languages with English.
    [Show full text]