<<

View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE

provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector

Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Patient Education and Counseling

jo urnal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pateducou

Communication Study

How the doc should (not) talk: When breaking bad news with negations

§

influences patients’ immediate responses and medical adherence intentions

a, a b,c

Christian Burgers *, Camiel J. Beukeboom , Lisa Sparks

a

Department of Communication Studies, VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands

b

Health and Risk Communication, Schmid College of Science, Chapman University, Orange, CA, USA

c

Chao Family Comprehensive Cancer Center/NCI Designated, Public Health/Medicine, University of California, Irvine, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Objective: We investigate the role of specific formulations in a doctor’s bad news delivery. We on

Received 3 April 2012

the effects of negations and message framing on patients’ immediate responses to the message and the

Received in revised form 16 July 2012

doctor, and long-term consequences including quality of life and medical adherence intentions.

Accepted 9 August 2012

Methods: Two lab experiments with 2 (language use: negations vs. affirmations) 2 (framing: positive

vs. negative) between-subjects designs. After reading a transcription (experiment 1) or seeing a film clip

Keywords:

(experiment 2), participants rated their evaluation of the message and the doctor, expected quality of

Breaking bad news

life, and medical adherence intentions.

Negations

Results: Positively framed bad news with negations score more negative on these dependent variables

Framing

than positively framed affirmations (both experiments). For negatively framed negations, these results

Doctor–patient interactions

Medical adherence are reversed (experiment 2). Furthermore, the evaluations of the message (experiment 1) and the doctor

(both experiments) mediate the interaction of framing and language use on medical adherence

intentions.

Conclusions: Small linguistic variations (i.e., negations vs. affirmations) in breaking bad news can have a

significant impact on the health message, doctor evaluation and medical adherence intentions.

Practice implications: Doctors should refrain from using negations to break positively framed news, and

employ negations when breaking negatively framed news.

ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. Open access under the Elsevier OA license.

1. Introduction provider training as effective as possible, it is important to

understand the factors that determine patients’ positive or negative

Next to being excellent health experts, doctors need to be good psychosocial responses.

communicators. Doctors deliver bad news to patients thousands of Yet, little is known about which specific elements make doctor–

times during their professional careers [1]. For patients, receiving patient communication effective. Most studies on the required

bad news is stressful in itself [2], but when doctors deliver the news formulation and style of doctor–patient interaction are descriptive,

poorly, additional stress may be induced with negative effects on which means that they describe current practices in breaking bad

patients’ health [2]. Furthermore, good doctor–patient communica- news (for overviews, see [11,12]). In fact, less than 2% of studies

tion can predict medical adherence [3–6]. Thus, breaking bad news focusing on doctor–patient interaction explicitly address how

to patients in an appropriate and effective way is a crucial task that doctors should formulate the information in such a way as to

bears important consequences [7]. Fortunately, many studies increase patient satisfaction [12]. The rare studies that did address

demonstrate that alerting and training doctors in these sensitive this issue adopted a general perspective, and showed that doctors’

communication issues greatly improves doctor–patient interaction general communication styles (e.g., comforting or empowering

[8–10] and subsequent patient satisfaction [7]. In order to make styles) influence the effectiveness of doctor–patient conversations

[7,13–15].

In the present study, we take a micro perspective by focusing on

§

The authors would like to thank Anouk van Berkel, Misha Naumovski, Nikki the actual words chosen by the doctor. Based on prior research, we

Ouborg, Bram Schothorst and Sanne van der Velde for their help with the data argue that – next to the general communication styles employed

collection for experiment 2.

(e.g., direct/indirect, comforting, empowering [7]) – specific words

* Corresponding author at: VU University Amsterdam, Department of Commu-

may also be important predictors of the effectiveness of doctor–

nication Studies, De Boelelaan 1081, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Tel.:

patient interaction. Breaking bad news to patients and their family

+31 20 5987889.

E-mail address: [email protected] (C. Burgers). members entails a highly sensitive communicative exchange in

0738-3991 ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. Open access under the Elsevier OA license. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2012.08.008

268 C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273

which small differences in word choices can make a big difference We expect that a positively framed message yield more positive

[7,16–18]. After all, doctors need to balance their words carefully. responses than a negatively framed message, but that these effects

They need to be truthful, but at the same time, they want to are moderated by the language used in the message (negations vs.

preserve the hopes of the patient and mitigate the information. In affirmations). When negations are used to break news that is

this balancing act, subtle differences in word choice may influence framed as relatively good (e.g., ‘‘not bad’’), patients are expected to

a patient’s evaluation of the conversation and the doctor, which in more negatively evaluate the message (H1a), the doctor (H2a) and

turn have potential long-term effects on medical adherence [19]. their expected quality of life (H3a) and to have lower medical

A first aspect in message formulation is the framing used to adherence intentions (H4a), compared to when affirmations are

deliver the diagnosis, which can be framed to emphasize either the used (e.g., ‘‘good’’). In , when breaking news that is framed

positive or the negative outcomes of a diagnosis. The relative as relatively bad and more mitigated language is thus appropriate,

strengths of positive and negative framing of factually equivalent we expect these effects to be reversed (H1-4b). We expect the

information has been studied with regard to the communication strongest effects of language use in positively framed messages,

involved in health-related decisions, which has its conceptual because in these cases negations (e.g., ‘‘you will not die’’) imply the

roots grounded in prospect theory (see [20–22]). These studies negative inference that the doctor has a more negative expectation

show that individuals react differentially to information presented than the actual message conveys.

in different frames. In the health domain, various studies show Furthermore, we expect the immediate responses to the

occurrences of framing of health-related information [23–25] and message to be related to the more long-term outcomes. That is,

their effects on patient perceptions [26–31]. With respect to given the link between patients’ impressions of the doctor–patient

breaking bad news, a framing differences may refer to emphasizing relationship and medical adherence [19], we expect that the long-

either the positive or negative aspects of a given diagnosis: given term effects of framing and language use on medical adherence

the range of possible diagnoses, is the particular diagnosis intentions are mediated by the immediate evaluation of the

presented as relatively good (positive frame) or relatively bad message (H5a) and the doctor (H5b).

(negative frame)?

An aspect that may moderate a framing effect [32] in the

2. Methods

balancing act of breaking bad news is the actual words that doctors

choose to formulate their message. Empirical evidence suggests

2.1. Design and sample for experiment 1

that doctors tend to mitigate their words when they have to deliver

relatively bad news compared to relatively good news [13]. One

A total of 100 US respondents participated in experiment 1,

verbal strategy that doctors can use to mitigate information and to

which had a 2 (framing of diagnosis: good vs. bad) 2 (language

be polite is using negations (e.g., ‘‘this news is not good’’) rather

use: negations vs. affirmations) between-subjects experimental

than affirmations (e.g., ‘‘this news is bad’’; [33,34]). Indeed, 1

design. Participants were healthy volunteers and recruited in

empirical evidence suggests that doctors frequently use negations

forums on social network sites, such as Facebook, and participated

when making diagnoses [35–38].

in an online experiment by clicking on a link. The average age was

Using negations, however, may come at a cost, because

41.88 years (SD = 12.43). A large majority of participants (78.0%)

negations may provide implicit cues about the expectancies of

was female.

the speaker [39–41]. For example, negations like ‘‘you will not die’’

may implicitly communicate that the doctor expected, or at least

2.2. Design and sample for experiment 2

considered, that the patient would die. Such inferences are not

likely when the doctor says ‘‘you will live’’, because, in contrast to

Experiment 2 was a conceptual replication of experiment 1 in a

the former statement, the latter statement does not implicitly

Dutch sample. A total of 115 Dutch respondents completed the

activate the concept of dying [34,40]. Moreover, from negations

questionnaire. Where a written scenario was employed in

(e.g., not bad), recipients infer that the speaker had an opposite

experiment 1 to present a doctor’s bad news delivery to

prior expectancy [39,40]. Consequently, using negations may

participants, experiment 2 used a film clip. 26 participants

implicitly give the impression that the doctor is insincere and hides

reported that they had experienced technical problems and could

the real message.

not properly see the film clip and were removed from the dataset,

Given that patients are eager to understand the doctor’s real

leaving a total of 89 participants. Their average age was 26.03 years

expectancies about their conditions [42], implicit messages can

(SD = 11.86). A large majority of participants (71.9%) was female.

have a strong impact on psychosocial responses. We expect that in

a positive frame, negations (e.g., ‘‘you will not die’’) yield more

2.3. Stimulus materials for experiment 1

dissatisfied participants, compared to affirmations (e.g., ‘‘you will

live’’). That is, in a positive frame, negations mitigate the good

Participants were presented with a written excerpt of a bad

news, and imply that the doctor may actually have an opposite

news conversation in which a patient was diagnosed with a

negative expectancy. In a negative frame, in contrast, negations

disease. The conversation was about the relatively unknown

(e.g., ‘‘you will not live’’) may yield more positive responses, as

Bekhterev’s disease to prevent previous knowledge about the

compared to affirmations (e.g., ‘‘you will die’’), because in these

disease from influencing results. Indeed, 99% of participants

messages, negations functionally mitigate the negative message

reported they had never heard of this disease prior to their

and implicitly activate positive inferences.

participation (one participant did not complete this question).

Following recent calls for more research using experimental

The excerpt consisted of seventeen sentences divided over four

methods to demonstrate causal relations in the field of doctor–

paragraphs. The total number of words differed between 186 and

patient interaction [13,43], we conducted two lab experiments,

192 in the four conditions. The first paragraph introduced the topic

using a US and a Dutch sample. In our experiments, we

of the conversation. In the second paragraph, the diagnosis was

investigated the inferences that potential patients draw in cases

given to the patient. The third paragraph dealt with the patient’s

in which a doctor uses negations rather than affirmations when

breaking bad news, using either a positive or negative frame. We 1

One participant did not complete the questionnaire seriously, as this person

measure evaluation of the message and the doctor, expected

paraphrased the conversation in an improper way. This person was removed from

quality of life with the disease and medical adherence intentions. the data.

C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273 269

Table 1

Examples of experimental manipulation.

Negative framing Positive framing

I know that you probably have many questions right now. For now, it is important I know that you probably have many questions right now. For now,

to know that Bekhterev’s disease is a genetic disease. Most patients find it difficult/do it is important to know that Bekhterev’s disease is a genetic disease.

not find it easy to live with this disease. But I would like to prescribe you with a specific Most patients find it easy/do not find it difficult to live with this

drug. Still . . . with these drugs, your quality of life is likely to deteriorate/not likely to disease. But I would like to prescribe you with a specific drug. With

improve over the next few weeks. these drugs, your quality of life is likely to improve/not likely to

deteriorate over the next few weeks.

Manipulation of language use (affirmations vs. negations) in bold.

prognosis and the doctor’s advice to take specific medications. In considerate, compassionate, and respectful (a = .93). Next, parti-

the fourth paragraph, the doctor gave the patient the option of cipants rated their expected quality of life living with the disease,

obtaining further advice about the condition and ended the and the quality of life they thought the doctor expected (two items,

conversation. a = .93). Finally, medical adherence intentions were tapped by three

In the first to third paragraphs, we manipulated the framing of items asking whether following the doctor’s advice was wise, a

the diagnosis (negative vs. positive) and language use (affirmations good idea, and if participants would actually try to follow the

vs. negations). The diagnosis was either framed as relatively recommendations (a = .95). Since all variables proved reliable, we

positive (giving the range of possible diagnoses, the particular calculated a mean overall score for each variable.

diagnosis was relatively good) or negative (the particular diagnosis

was relatively bad). In these frames either negations (e.g., not bad, 2.6. Instrumentation for experiment 2

not good) or affirmations (good and bad) were used. Table 1 shows

our manipulations in the third paragraph of the conversation. In The questions used in experiment 1 were translated into

total, the manipulation was applied to four of the seventeen Dutch. Reliability for the measures evaluation of the message

sentences. The fourth paragraph was equal across conditions. (a = .69), evaluation of the doctor (a = .89), expected quality of life

(a = .69) and medical adherence intentions (a = .89) was again

2.4. Stimulus materials for experiment 2 satisfactory and we calculated a mean overall score for each

variable.

For experiment 2, the materials from experiment 1 were

translated into Dutch. In translation, the total number of words 3. Results

differed between 167 and 173 between the four conditions.

Furthermore, in order to simulate a more realistic doctor–patient Next to a main effect of framing, we expected an interaction

interaction, we created film clips in which a male actor posed as a effect of framing and language use on the evaluation of the

doctor and read the text directly to camera. To participants message, the evaluation of the doctor, the expected quality of life

watching the film clip, this made it seem as if the doctor directly and medical adherence intentions (H1-4). For both experiments,

addressed them. More participants (15.7%) indicated that they we conducted 2 (framing: positive vs. negative) 2 (language use:

were familiar with Bekhterev’s disease and were able to correctly affirmations vs. negations) multivariate analyses of variance

identify at least one major symptom associated with the disease. (MANOVA) with evaluation of the message and the doctor,

expected quality of life and medical adherence intentions as

2.5. Instrumentation for experiment 1 dependent variables. Tables 2 and 3 show the means and standard

deviations of these variables per condition for the two experi-

We measured all dependent variables with multiple-item, 7- ments.

point Likert scales, ranging from 1 = completely disagree to

7 = completely agree. First, participants were asked to evaluate 3.1. Experiment 1: main effects

the message and the doctor. To measure the evaluation of the

message, participants indicated how much they agreed that the The MANOVA showed a main effect of framing on psychosocial

message was informative, clear, understandable and took away responses to the breaking of bad news (Wilks’ l = .26, F(4,

2

hope (reverse-coded; a = .70). For evaluation of the doctor, 93) = 64.86, p < .001, h p ¼ :74). Subsequent univariate analyses

participants indicated their overall impression of the doctor and revealed that framing had a significant effects on the evaluation of

2

the degree to which they thought the doctor was nice, polite, the doctor (F(1, 96) = 12.30, p < .01, h p ¼ :11) and the expected

Table 2

Experiment 1 (USA): mean scores (and standard deviations) of the evaluation of the message and the doctor, the expected quality of life, and medical adherence intentions, in

the framing of the diagnosis (negative vs. positive) and language use (affirmations vs. negations) conditions.

Negative framing Positive framing

Affirmations Negations Affirmations Negations

(e.g., bad) (e.g., not good) (e.g., good) (e.g., not bad)

a a x y

Evaluation of the message 3.28 (1.46) 3.78 (1.14) 4.29 (1.27) 3.44 (1.42)

a a x y

Evaluation of the doctor 2.95 (1.24) 3.34 (1.13) 4.44 (1.12) 3.65 (1.52)

a a x y

Expected quality of life 1.65 (.56) 2.07 (.76) 5.35 (1.02) 4.78 (1.40)

a a x x

Medical adherence intentions 4.26 (1.70) 4.52 (1.48) 5.17 (1.43) 4.59 (1.58)

Note: 7-point Likert scales: higher numbers indicate more positive evaluations, higher expected quality of life and higher intention to follow the doctor’s advice. Means of

variables with different subscript within negative (a, b) and positive (x, y) framing conditions are significantly different according to pairwise comparisons with a certainty of p < .05.

270 C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273

Table 3

Experiment 2 (Netherlands): mean scores (and standard deviations) of the evaluation of the message and the doctor, the expected quality of life, and medical adherence

intentions, in the framing of the diagnosis (negative vs. positive) and language use (affirmations vs. negations) conditions.

Negative framing Positive framing

Affirmations Negations Affirmations Negations

(e.g., bad) (e.g., not good) (e.g., good) (e.g., not bad)

a a x1 y1

Evaluation of the message 4.30 (1.18) 4.28 (1.29) 3.73 (1.19) 2.97 (1.20)

a b x y

Evaluation of the doctor 3.08 (1.22) 3.77 (1.01) 4.21 (1.33) 3.31 (1.19)

a a x x

Expected quality of life 3.33 (1.32) 2.67 (1.26) 4.58 (1.46) 3.89 (1.73)

a b x y

Medical adherence intentions 4.93 (1.57) 5.79 (1.08) 5.55 (1.23) 4.70 (1.59)

Note: 7-point Likert scales: higher numbers indicate more positive evaluations, higher expected quality of life and higher intention to follow the doctor’s advice. Means of

variables with different subscript within negative (a, b) and positive (x, y) framing conditions are significantly different according to pairwise comparisons with a certainty of

p < .05. x1,y1: this specific pairwise comparison reached significance at p < .05, even though the interaction term was non-significant.

2

quality of life (F(1, 96) = 261.01, p < .001, h p ¼ :73), but not on the 3.3. Experiment 1: interaction effects

evaluation of the message (F(1, 96) = 1.60, p = .21) or medical

adherence intentions (F(1, 96) = 2.47, p = .12). Participants receiv- In line with hypotheses 1–4, we observed an interaction

ing a positively framed message were more positive about the between framing and language use on psychosocial outcomes

2

doctor (M = 4.04, SD = 1.38) and the expected quality of life (Wilks’ l = .90, F(4, 93) = 2.67, p < .05, h p ¼ :10). Subsequent

(M = 5.07, SD = 1.24) than participants receiving a negatively univariate analyses revealed that the interaction was significant

framed message (evaluation of the doctor: M = 3.20, SD = 1.17; for the evaluation of the message (F(1, 96) = 6.56, p < .05,

2

expected quality of life: M = 1.92, SD = .72). No main effect of h p ¼ :06), the evaluation of the doctor (F(1, 96) = 5.39, p < .05,

2

language use was observed (Wilks’ l = .99, F(4, 93) < 1). h p ¼ :05) and the expected quality of life (F(1, 96) = 6.22, p < .05,

2

h p ¼ :06). The interaction for medical adherence intentions was

3.2. Experiment 2: main effects non-significant (F(1, 96) = 1.82, p = .18).

Because our hypotheses predict distinctive effects within frames,

Similar to experiment 1, the MANOVA showed a main effect of we conducted pairwise comparisons for the significant interaction

framing on the psychosocial responses to the breaking of bad news effects. These revealed that, for negatively framed messages, no

2

(Wilks’ l = .69, F(4, 82) = 9.30, p < .001, h p ¼ :31). Subsequent effects of language use were observed on the evaluation of the

univariate analyses revealed that framing had significant effects on message (p = .12) and the doctor (p = .27). The expected quality of

2

evaluation of the message (F(1, 85) = 12.84, p < .01, h p ¼ :13) and life was marginally higher (p = .081) when negations were used

2

expected quality of life (F(1, 85) = 16.14, p < .001, h p ¼ :16), but compared to affirmations. For positively framed messages, however,

not on the evaluation of the doctor (F(1, 85) = 1.72, p = .19) or the evaluation of the message (p < .05) and the doctor (p < .05) and

medical adherence intentions (F < 1). Participants in the condition the expected quality of life (p = .052) were all more negative when

with positive framing were more positive about the expected negations were used compared to affirmations (see Table 2).

quality of life (M = 4.29, SD = 1.59) and more negative about the

message (M = 3.41, SD = 1.24) than participants in the condition 3.4. Experiment 2: interaction effects

with negative framing (evaluation of the message: M = 4.29,

SD = 1.23; expected quality of life: M = 2.96, SD = 1.32). Like in Again, we observed an interaction between framing and

experiment 1, no main effect of language use was observed (Wilks’ language use on psychosocial outcomes (Wilks’ l = .87, F(4,

2

l = .91, F(4, 82) = 1.93, p = .11). 82) = 3.13, p < .01, h p ¼ :13). Subsequent univariate analyses

Table 4

Mediation analysis with the evaluation of the message and the doctor as hypothesized mediators of the interaction of framing and language use on medical adherence

intentions. Main effects of framing and language use are included as covariates.

B SE Normal theory p Bootstrap 95% CI

Experiment 1

Covariate main effect of framing .08 .41 .84

Covariate main effect of language use .11 .35 .76

Interaction framing language use

Total effect (c path) .85 .63 .18

0

Direct effect (c path) .21 .53 .70

*

Total indirect effect (via mediators) 1.05 .43 1.99, .30

*

Evaluation of the message .64 .30 1.32, .13

*

Evaluation of the doctor .42 .22 1.00, .08

2

Model R (p) .39 (<.001)

Experiment 2

Covariate main effect of framing .35 .38 .36

Covariate main effect of language use .59 .35 .09

Interaction framing language use

Total effect (c path) 1.70 .58 <.01

Direct effect (c’path) .83 .52 .11

*

Total indirect effect (via mediators) .87 .38 1.76, .23

Evaluation of the message .24 .21 .81, .05

*

Evaluation of the doctor .63 .28 1.31, .21

2

Model R (p) .37 (<.001)

Note: Mediation with 5000 bootstrap samples.

*

Indirect effect is significant with a certainty of p < .05, because the confidence interval does not include zero.

C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273 271

revealed that the interaction was significant for the evaluation of negations rather than affirmations in a negatively framed message

2

the doctor (F(1, 85) = 9.56, p < .01, h p ¼ :10) and medical (H2b).

2

adherence intentions (F(1, 85) = 8.65, p < .01, h p ¼ :09). In contrast Expected quality of life: Only in experiment 1 did participants

to experiment 1, the interaction for the evaluation of the message rate their expected quality of life lower when the doctor used

(F(1, 85) = 1.98, p = .16) and the expected quality of life were non- negations rather than affirmations in a positively framed message

significant (F < 1). (H3a). In both experiments, we observed no differences between

Pairwise comparisons revealed that, for negatively framed negatively framed messages with affirmations or negations on this

messages, using negations increased the evaluation of the doctor dependent variable.

(p = .056) and medical adherence intentions (p < .05) compared to Medical adherence intentions: In both experiments, negations

affirmations. In contrast, for positively framed messages, using influenced medical adherence intentions [19]. We observed a direct

negations decreased the evaluation of the doctor (p < .05) and effect only in experiment 2: participants reported lower medical

medical adherence intentions (p < .05) compared to using adherence intentions when the doctor used negations rather than

affirmations. Although – in contrast to experiment 1 – the affirmations in a positively framed message (H4a), and this effect

interaction term was non-significant for evaluation of the message, was reversed in negatively framed messages (H4b). Furthermore, we

pairwise comparisons to test H1a replicated the expected found indirect effects of the interaction between framing lan-

significant difference (p < .05, see Table 3). language use on medical adherence intentions via the evaluation of

the message (experiment 1) and the doctor (both experiments, H5).

3.5. Experiment 1: mediation analyses Given that the experiments differed in method (written scenario

vs. film clip), language of experimental materials and cultural

To test whether evaluation of the message and the doctor background of the sample (USA vs. Netherlands), the comparable

mediated the interaction effect of framing and language use on general pattern of results is striking. For both experiments, the

medical adherence intentions (H5), we conducted mediation choice of negations over affirmations is particularly influential in

analyses and estimated indirect effects with 5000 bootstrap positively framed messages. In these cases, negations yield more

samples [44,45]. These analyses revealed significant indirect negative responses compared to affirmations. These findings are

effects of the evaluation of the doctor and the message on medical aligned with the idea that negations (e.g., ‘‘not bad’’) activate

adherence intentions (see Table 4), indicating that the interaction negative associations, and imply that the doctor may actually have

of framing and language use indirectly influences medical an opposite negative expectancy [34,39,40].

adherence intentions, via the evaluation of the message and the In negatively framed messages, however, the effects of

doctor. linguistic formulation was less pronounced, although the Dutch

experiment revealed that negations yielded more positive

3.6. Experiment 2: mediation analyses responses compared to affirmations. This is in line with the idea

that negations mitigate bad news (e.g., not good rather than bad)

As in experiment 1, mediation analyses revealed a significant and imply a positive expectation. Why we find this effect for Dutch

indirect effect of the evaluation of the doctor on medical adherence participants only may be explained by cultural differences in

intentions. In contrast to experiment 1, however, we did not doctor–patient interaction [46]. In feminine cultures like the

observe mediation for the evaluation of the message (see Table 4). Netherlands, doctors use more instrumental (task-oriented)

communication, while in masculine cultures (like the USA),

doctors more often use affective communication in which

4. Discussion and conclusion information may be mitigated [46]. Because affective communi-

cation is less common in Dutch doctor–patient interactions,

4.1. Discussion mitigating with negations for negatively framed news may be

more unexpected and thus increase positive psychosocial

Two experiments support the claim that formulation differ- responses. For the American participants, in contrast, an affective

ences in breaking bad news have psychosocial effects on patients. style is more commonly used and thus patients’ default expecta-

Both experiments suggest that positively framed messages have tion of what effective breaking bad news should be. For these

positive effects on patients’ evaluations as compared to negatively participants, an affective style does not lead to more positive

framed messages. More importantly, within these frames, the use responses compared to a more direct approach.

of negations or affirmations makes a difference. Negations tend to It should be noted, however, that differences between experi-

have positive effects in negatively framed messages, presumably ments are not necessarily due to culture, given that the stimuli also

because they mitigate the message’s direct blow. These effects are differed: where participants in experiment 1 read a transcript of

reversed in positively framed messages, probably because they the conversation, participants in study 2 saw a film clip.

make the doctor seem insincere. Findings are generally compara- Participants in experiment 2 may have been more transported

ble across our experiments, but specific differences on separate into the scenario and thus more open to empathetic remarks (i.e.,

variables were observed between experiments. We now discuss negations in negatively framed news) made by the doctor than

results per specific dependent variable. those in experiment 1 [47,48]. Future research comparing and

Evaluation of the message: Participants in both experiments contrasting these factors may provide further insights.

evaluated the positively framed message more negatively when it Some limitations about our study should be noted. We used

contained negations (e.g., ‘‘the news is not bad’’) rather than healthy volunteers as subjects, whose responses may differ from

affirmations (‘‘the news is good’’), which supports H1a. No effects those of real patients, and a relatively unknown disease, which

of language use were observed in negatively framed messages, may lead to different responses than well-known diseases.

disconfirming H1b. Furthermore, even though intentions are one of the strongest

Evaluation of the doctor: Participants in both experiments predictors of behavior [49–51], patients can always act differently

evaluated the doctor more negatively when s/he used negations from their intentions. Future research may monitor the language of

rather than affirmations in a positively framed message, which real doctors while breaking bad news concerning various medical

supports H2a. In experiment 2 (but not in experiment 1), conditions. By connecting specific observations to real patients’

participants evaluated the doctor more positively when s/he used survey responses over multiple waves, more information can be

272 C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273

[2] Street Jr RL, Makoul G, Arora NK, Epstein RM. How does communication heal?

obtained about the relations between doctor behavior and

Pathways linking clinician–patient communication to health outcomes. Pa-

patients’ subsequent satisfaction and medical adherence over

tient Educ Couns 2009;74:295–301.

time. [3] Arbuthnott A, Sharpe D. The effect of physician–patient collaboration on

patient adherence in non-psychiatric medicine. Patient Educ Couns 2009;

In sum, our findings demonstrate that subtle linguistic variations

77:60–7.

in breaking bad news may influence patients’ immediate evaluation

[4] Haskard Zolnierek KB, DiMatteo MR. Physician communication and patient

of the message and the doctor, as well as their long-term views on adherence to treatment: a meta-analysis. Med Care 2009;47:826–34.

[5] Penn C, Watermeyer J, Evans M. Why don’t patients take their drugs?

living with the disease and medical adherence intentions. Further-

The role of communication, context and culture in patient adherence and

more, in both experiments, we observed that medical adherence

the work of the pharmacist in HIV/AIDS. Patient Educ Couns 2011;83:

intentions were indirectly influenced via the measures of message 310–8.

and doctor evaluation. These results replicate earlier findings [19] [6] Schoentaler A, Chaplin WF, Allegrante JP, Fernandez S, Diaz-Gloster M, Tobin

JN, et al. Provider communication effects medication adherence in hyperten-

that found that the quality of the doctor–patient relationship was an

sive African Americans. Patient Educ Couns 2008;75:185–91.

important predictor of medical adherence. This highlights the

[7] Sparks L, Villagran MM, Parker-Raley J, Cunningham CB. A patient-centered

importance of effective delivery of bad news. approach to breaking bad news: communication guidelines for health care

providers. J Appl Commun Res 2007;35:177–96.

[8] Bonvicini KA, Perlin MJ, Bylund CL, Carroll G, Rouse RA, Goldstein MG. Impact

4.2. Conclusion

of communication training on physician expression of empathy in patient

encounters. Patient Educ Couns 2009;75:3–10.

[9] Fallowfield L, Jenkins V, Farewell V, Saul J, Duffy A, Eves R. Efficacy of a Cancer

In two experiments, we investigated the influence of specific

Research UK Communication Skills Training Model for Oncologists: a random-

message formulations on the evaluation of the message, the doctor,

ised controlled trial. Lancet 2002;359:650–6.

expected quality of life, and medical adherence intentions. [10] Schildmann J, Kupfer S, Burchardi N, Vollmann J. Teaching and evaluating

breaking bad news: a pre–post evaluation study of a teaching intervention for

Findings indicate that negations have negative effects on these

medical students and a comparative analysis of different measurement instru-

variables in positively framed messages (e.g., not bad rather than

ments and raters. Patient Educ Couns 2012;86:210–4.

good), while negations in some cases have positive effects in [11] Hagerty RG, Butow PN, Ellis PM, Dimitry S, Tattersall MHN. Communicating

prognosis in cancer care: a systematic review of the literature. Ann Oncol negatively framed messages (e.g., not good). Furthermore,

2005;16:1005–53.

immediate responses to differentially formulated messages as

[12] Paul CL, Clinton-McHarg T, Sanson-Fisher RW, Douglas H, Webb G. Are we

well as doctors delivering messages indirectly influence long-term there yet? The state of the evidence base for guidelines on breaking bad news

medical adherence intentions. Results showed that seemingly to cancer patients. Eur J Cancer 2009;45:2960–6.

[13] Del Vento A, Bavelas J, Healing S, MacLean G, Kirk P. An experimental

harmless linguistic variations in doctors’ bad news delivery can

investigation of the dilemma of delivering bad news. Patient Educ Couns

have significant consequences. We conclude that doctors should 2009;77:443–9.

balance their use of negations carefully when breaking bad news. [14] Schmid Mast M, Kindlimann A, Langewitz W. Recipients’ perspective on

breaking bad news: how you put it really makes a difference. Patient Educ

Furthermore, future research into the effectiveness of doctor–

Couns 2005;58:244–51.

patient interactions may consider looking beyond general com-

[15] Vail L, Sandhu H, Fisher J, Cooke H, Dale J, Barnett M. Hospital consultants

munication styles [7,13–15] and additionally investigate more breaking bad news with simulated patients: an analysis of communication

using the Roter Interaction Analysis System. Patient Educ Couns 2011;83:

subtle differences in linguistic variations.

185–94.

[16] Buckman R, Lipkin M, Sourkes B, Tolle S. Strategies and skills for breaking bad

4.3. Practice implications news. Patient Care 1997;31:61–71.

[17] du Pre´ A, Beck C. ‘‘How can I put this?’’ Exaggerated self-disparagement as

alignment strategy during problematic disclosures by patients to doctors.

Given the negative effects that poor doctor–patient interaction

Qual Health Res 1997;7:487–503.

may have on medical adherence [4–6] and patients’ health in [18] Sparks L, Villagran MM. Patient and provider interaction: a global health

communication perspective. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press; 2010.

general [2], the present results provide important practice

[19] Stavropolou C. Non-adherence to medication and doctor–patient relationship:

implications for improving doctor–patient interaction. While it

evidence from a European survey. Patient Educ Couns 2011;83:7–13.

remains important to train practitioners in adopting an appropri- [20] Kahneman D, Tversky A. ‘‘Prospect theory’’: an analysis of decision under risk.

ate general communication style when breaking bad news, the Econometrica 1979;47:263–91.

[21] Kahneman D, Tversky A. Choices, values and frames. Cambridge, UK: Cam-

present results underscore the importance of specific linguistic

bridge University Press; 2000.

variables such as negations. Awareness of the impact of specific

[22] Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of

words in message formulation may improve doctors’ communica- choice. Science 1981;211:453–8.

[23] Brown P. Naming and framing: the social construction of diagnosis and illness.

tion effectiveness. Our results suggest that subtle formulation

J Health Soc Behav 1995;35:34–52.

differences can even medical adherence. When these results

[24] Jutel A. Framing disease: the example of female hypoactive sexual desire

are replicated in other studies and additional linguistic variables disorder. Soc Sci Med 2010;70:1084–90.

[25] Kwan S. Framing the fat body: contested meanings between government,

other than negations are explored, we recommend that educators

activists, and industry. Sociol Inq 2009;79:25–50.

training doctors in breaking bad news incorporate our results into

[26] Garcia-Retamero R, Galesic M. How to reduce the effect of framing on

their protocols. We hope that, in addition to studying general messages about health. J Gen Intern Med 2010;25:1323–9.

[27] O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The advantages of compliance or the disadvantages of

communication styles, future studies also focus on important and

noncompliance? A meta-analytic review of the relative persuasive effective-

understudied linguistic variables in health contexts.

ness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages. Communication yearbook,

vol. 30. 2006. p. 1–43.

[28] O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-

Disclosure statement

framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: a meta-

analytic review. J Health Commun 2007;12:623–44.

[29] O’Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-

The authors confirm that all patient/personal identifiers have

framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-ana-

been removed or disguised so the patient/person(s) described are

lytic review. J Commun 2009;59:296–316.

not identifiable and cannot be identified through the details of the [30] O’Keefe DJ, Nan X. The relative persuasiveness of gain- and loss-framed

story. messages for promoting vaccination: a meta-analytic review. Health Commun

2012;27:776–83.

[31] Zhao X, Villagran MM, Kreps GL, McHorney C. Gain versus loss framing in

References adherence-promoting communication targeting patients with chronic dis-

eases: the moderating effect of individual time perspective. Health Commun

2012;27:75–85.

[1] Baile WF, Buckman R, Lenzi R, Glober G, Beale EA, Kudelka AP. SPIKES—A six-

[32] Borah P. Conceptual issues in framing theory: a systematic examination of a

step protocol for delivering bad news: application to the patient with cancer.

decade’s literature. J Commun 2011;61:246–63.

Oncologist 2000;5:302–11.

C. Burgers et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 89 (2012) 267–273 273

[33] Fraenkel T, Schul Y. The meaning of negated . Intercult Pragmatics [43] Noar SM, Mehrotra P. Toward a new methodological paradigm for testing

2008;5:517–40. theories of health behavior and health behavior change. Patient Educ Couns

[34] Giora R, Balaban N, Fein O, Alkabets I. Negation as positivity in disguise. In: 2011;82:468–74.

Colston HL, Katz AN, editors. Figurative language comprehension: social and [44] Preacher K, Hayes A. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and

cultural influences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2005. p. 233–58. comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Meth

[35] Chapman WW, Bridewell T, Hanbury P, Cooper GF, Buchanan BG. Evaluation of 2008;40:879–91.

negation phrases in narrative clinical reports. Proc AMIA Symp 2001;105–9. [45] Hayes A. Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new

[36] Dalianis H, Velupillai S. How certain are clinical assessments? Annotating millennium. Commun Monogr 2009;76:408–20.

Swedish clinical text for (un)certainties, speculations and negations. Proc [46] Meeuwesen L, Van den Brink-Muinen A, Hofstede G. Can dimensions of

LREC Symp 2010;3071–5. national culture predict cross-national differences in medical communica-

[37] Harkema H, Dowling JN, Thornblade T, Chapman WW. Context: an algorithm tion? Patient Educ Couns 2009;75:58–66.

for determining negation, experiencer, and temporal status from clinical [47] Green MC, Brock TC. The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public

reports. J Biomed Inform 2009;42:839–51. narratives. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;79:701–21.

[38] Huang Y, Lowe HJ. A novel hybrid approach to automated negation detection [48] Slater MD, Rouner D. Entertainment-education and elaboration likelihood:

in clinical radiology reports. J Am Med Inform Assn 2007;14:304–11. understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. Commun Theory

[39] Beukeboom CJ, Finkenauer C, Wigboldus DHJ. The negation bias: when nega- 2002;12:173–91.

tions signal stereotypic expectancies. J Pers Soc Psychol 2010;99:978–92. [49] Albarracı´n D, Johnson BT, Fishbein M, Muellerleile PA. Theories of reasoned

[40] Schul Y. Alive or not dead: implications for framing on research of negations. action and planned behavior as models of condom use: a meta-analysis.

In: Keren G, editor. Perspectives on framing. New York, NY: Psychology Press; Psychol Bull 2001;127:142–61.

2011. p. 157–76. [50] Kim M-S, Hunter JE. Relationships among attitudes, behavioral intentions,

[41] Verhagen A. Constructions of intersubjectivity: discourse, syntax and cogni- and behavior: a meta-analysis of past research, Part 2. Commun Res 1993;

tion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2007. 20:331–64.

[42] Brown VA, Parker PA, Furber L, Thomas AL. Patient preferences for the delivery [51] Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior

of bad news – the experience of a UK cancer centre. Eur J Cancer Care change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull 2011;20:56–61. 2006;132:249–68.