A Neurobiological Association of Revenge Propensity During Intergroup Conflict 3 4 Xiaochun Hana, Michele J

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Neurobiological Association of Revenge Propensity During Intergroup Conflict 3 4 Xiaochun Hana, Michele J 1 2 A neurobiological association of revenge propensity during intergroup conflict 3 4 Xiaochun Hana, Michele J. Gelfandb, Bing Wuc, Ting Zhanga, Wenxin Lid, Tianyu Gaoa, 5 Chenyu Panga, Taoyu Wua, Yuqing Zhoua, Shuai Zhouc, Xinhuai Wuc,1, Shihui Hana,1 6 7 aSchool of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain 8 Research, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, 9 China 10 bDepartment of Psychology, University of Maryland, USA c 11 Department of Radiology, The 7th Medical Center of PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China 12 dPeking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China 13 14 15 16 Address correspondence to: 17 Shihui Han Ph.D. Prof. 18 School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences 19 Peking University 20 52 Haidian Road, Beijing 100080, China 21 Phone: (86) 10-6275-9138 22 Email: [email protected] 23 or 24 Xinhuai Wu. M.D. 25 Department of Radiology 26 PLA Army General Hospital 27 5 Nanmencang, Beijing, 100070, China 28 Phone: (86) 10-8400-8368 29 Email: [email protected] 30 1 31 Abstract 32 Revenge during intergroup conflict is a human universal, but its neurobiological 33 underpinnings remain unclear. We address this by integrating functional MRI and 34 measurements of endogenous oxytocin in participants who view an ingroup and an outgroup 35 member's suffering that is caused mutually (Revenge group) or respectively by a computer 36 (Control group). We show that intergroup conflict encountered by the Revenge group is 37 associated with an increased level of oxytocin in saliva compared to in the Control group. 38 Furthermore, the medial prefrontal activity in response to ingroup pain in the Revenge but not 39 Control group mediates the association between endogenous oxytocin and the propensity to 40 give painful electric shocks to outgroup members regardless of whether they were directly 41 involved in the conflict. Our findings highlight an important neurobiological correlate of 42 revenge propensity which may be implicated in conflict contagion across individuals in the 43 context of intergroup conflict. 44 2 45 Revenge, which refers to taking actions of harming someone in retaliation for an 46 injury (Elshout et al., 2015; Jackson et al., 2019), is a global phenomenon and a causal factor 47 in many homicides and transgenerational conflicts (Kopsaj, 2016; Jackson et al., 2019). 48 While revenge is an aggressive act, not all aggressive acts represent vengeance. For example, 49 unsolicited acts of aggression, like deviance, incivility, and bullying, would not count as 50 revenge (Raver and Barling, 2008; Jackson et al., 2019). Revenge often occurs between 51 families or clans when an outgroup member brings harm to an ingroup member which, in 52 turn, induces retaliation upon outgroup members (Ericksen and Horton, 1992). According to 53 early social psychological theories (Allport et al., 1954; Brewer, 1999), a desire to help the 54 ingroup (“ingroup love”) and/or an aggressive motivation to hurt the outgroup (“outgroup 55 hate”) may drive participation in intergroup conflict by taking revenge. Recent behavioral 56 research using economic games indeed suggests that ingroup love plays a key role in driving 57 economic punishment toward outgroup (Halevy et al., 2008; de Dreu, 2010; De Dreu et al., 58 2010; Halevy et al., 2012). Yet despite the severe social consequences of revenge, its 59 neurobiological underpinnings remain unclear. Building upon previous findings (Halevy et al., 60 2008; de Dreu, 2010; De Dreu et al., 2010; Halevy et al., 2012), we suggest that there may 61 be a neurobiological mechanism that links perceived ingroup pain caused by an outgroup and 62 the propensity to seek revenge upon an outgroup during intergroup conflict. The present work 63 specifically examined the hormonal (i.e., oxytocin) and neural responses to ingroup suffering 64 caused by an outgroup that predict revenge propensity against outgroups. 65 Previous brain imaging research has revealed neural responses to ingroup/outgroup 66 members' suffering, yet they have been done in contexts that lack the key character of real- 67 life intergroup conflict, i.e., ingroup and outgroup members causing each others' pain. 68 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have identified increased activity in 69 both the empathy network (e.g., the anterior midcingulate (aMCC) and anterior insula (AI)) 3 70 and theory-of-mind network (e.g., the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and temporoparietal 71 junction (TPJ)) in response to ingroup pain (Hein et al., 2010; Cikara et al., 2011; Han, 72 2018). Outgroup pain, on the other hand, is related to enhanced activity in the reward system 73 (e.g., the ventral striatum and nucleus accumbens, Hein et al., 2010; Cikara et al., 2011; Luo 74 et al., 2015). In addition, the mPFC activity in response to perceived pain is associated with 75 decisions to help ingroup members (Hein et al., 2010; Mathur et al., 2010), and the activity 76 in the nucleus accumbens predicts decisions not to help outgroup members (Hein et al., 2010; 77 Luo et al., 2015). These findings highlight ingroup favoritism in brain responses to others' 78 pain as neural underpinnings of ingroup love but leave open a critical question: Are brain 79 responses to ingroup pain inflicted by outgroup members during intergroup conflict 80 associated with subsequent revenge? Specifically, it is unclear whether activities in the 81 empathy and/or theory-of-mind networks in response to perceived ingroup pain are 82 associated with revenge motives during intergroup conflict. If revenge aims to bring suffering 83 to an outgroup to get reward during intergroup conflict, one may expect the involvement of 84 the reward system in decision making related to outgroup punishment (Hein et al., 2010; 85 Cikara et al., 2011; Han, 2018). However, if the goal of revenge is to help ingroup members 86 who suffer from physical harm caused by an outgroup (Lickel et al., 2006), the mPFC, which 87 responds to ingroup pain and is associated with ingroup help (Hein et al., 2010; Mathur et al., 88 2010), may be associated with tendencies to punish the outgroup. 89 Previous fMRI research has also examined neurobiological correlates of punishment 90 decision-making pertaining to those who have violated social norms in economic games 91 (Seymour et al., 2007; Krueger and Hoffman, 2016). Punishment decisions to prevent social 92 norm violations have been associated with increased activities in both the empathy and 93 theory-of-mind networks including the aMCC, AI, and mPFC (Krueger and Hoffman, 2016). 94 Yet these studies focused on brain activities related to punishment decisions rather than 4 95 neurobiological mechanisms that link perceived ingroup suffering to propensity to seek 96 revenge upon outgroups. Punishment decisions during previously studied economic games 97 were likely motivated by prevention of social norm violations rather than by perceived 98 physical harm to ingroup members caused by outgroup members, which characterizes most 99 of revenge behavior in real-life situations. 100 Finally, at the hormone level, recent research reported increased levels of urinary 101 oxytocin (OT) ù a nine amino acid peptide synthesized in hypothalamic cells ù in 102 Chimpanzees immediately before and during border patrols and intergroup encounters 103 (Samuni et al., 2017). Likewise, intranasal administration of OT (vs. placebo) in humans 104 enhanced empathic neural responses to ingroup pain (Sheng et al., 2013) and individuals’ 105 contributions to ingroup payoffs (De Dreu et al., 2010; de Dreu, 2010). OT administration 106 also promotes motivation to sacrifice outgroup targets (De Dreu et al., 2011) and facilitates 107 within-group coordination for successful outgroup attack during economic games (Zhang et 108 al., 2019). These findings shed light on a functional role of the oxytocinergic system in 109 decision making related to outgroup punishment. However, there has been little direct 110 evidence for modulations of endogenous OT in humans during intergroup conflict (but see 111 Levy et al., 2016). In addition, neural architectures that mediate endogenous OT and revenge 112 propensity during intergroup conflict have been largely unexplored. Among the brain regions 113 in which activities are sensitive to ingroup pain, the mPFC contains OT-sensitive neurons 114 (Ninan, 2011). The mPFC, cingulate, and insula express OT receptors (Gimpl and Fahrenholz, 115 2001; MacDonald and MacDonald, 2010; Boccia et al., 2013) and mPFC/aMCC activities 116 are modulated by administered OT (Sabihi et al., 2014; Eckstein et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017; 117 Wang et al., 2017). However, to date, whether the neural systems involved in empathy or 118 theory-of-mind link endogenous OT to revenge propensity during intergroup conflict has yet 119 to be examined. 5 120 A key challenge to empirically address these issues is the need for an experimental 121 paradigm of intergroup conflict that can be used in a neuroimaging laboratory setting to 122 measure neurobiological responses to perceived ingroup physical pain caused by an outgroup 123 and revenge propensity to bring physical harm to the outgroup. Another challenge for 124 empirical research on the neurobiological association of revenge propensity during intergroup 125 conflict is to disentangle the effect of the key component of revenge (i.e., to punish outgroup 126 members for their harming to one's ingroup) from other concomitant but nonessential factors, 127 including perceived group identity (Kahn et al., 2017), negative evaluation of the outgroup 128 (Schiller et al., 2014), and decreased empathy for outgroup pain (Hein et al., 2010; Cikara et 129 al., 2011; Han, 2018). These factors themselves may lead to negative treatment of outgroup 130 members based on ingroup biases in social behavior that occur even in the absence of 131 intergroup conflict. It is therefore necessary to examine neurobiological responses in two 132 conditions in which ingroup biases in emotions, attitudes, and behavior are matched but the 133 motive to punish the outgroup is different.
Recommended publications
  • Bad Girls: Agency, Revenge, and Redemption in Contemporary Drama
    e Interdisciplinary Journal of Popular Culture and Pedagogy Bad Girls: Agency, Revenge, and Redemption in Contemporary Drama Courtney Watson, Ph.D. Radford University Roanoke, Virginia, United States [email protected] ABSTRACT Cultural movements including #TimesUp and #MeToo have contributed momentum to the demand for and development of smart, justified female criminal characters in contemporary television drama. These women are representations of shifting power dynamics, and they possess agency as they channel their desires and fury into success, redemption, and revenge. Building on works including Gillian Flynn’s Gone Girl and Netflix’s Orange is the New Black, dramas produced since 2016—including The Handmaid’s Tale, Ozark, and Killing Eve—have featured the rise of women who use rule-breaking, rebellion, and crime to enact positive change. Keywords: #TimesUp, #MeToo, crime, television, drama, power, Margaret Atwood, revenge, Gone Girl, Orange is the New Black, The Handmaid’s Tale, Ozark, Killing Eve Dialogue: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Popular Culture and Pedagogy 37 Watson From the recent popularity of the anti-heroine in novels and films like Gone Girl to the treatment of complicit women and crime-as-rebellion in Hulu’s adaptation of The Handmaid’s Tale to the cultural watershed moments of the #TimesUp and #MeToo movements, there has been a groundswell of support for women seeking justice both within and outside the law. Behavior that once may have been dismissed as madness or instability—Beyoncé laughing wildly while swinging a baseball bat in her revenge-fantasy music video “Hold Up” in the wake of Jay-Z’s indiscretions comes to mind—can be examined with new understanding.
    [Show full text]
  • The Pathologising Effect of TV Revengendas
    University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Law, Humanities and the Arts - Papers Faculty of Arts, Social Sciences & Humanities 1-1-2015 The bitter taste of payback: the pathologising effect of TV revengendas Cassandra E. Sharp University of Wollongong, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://ro.uow.edu.au/lhapapers Part of the Arts and Humanities Commons, and the Law Commons Recommended Citation Sharp, Cassandra E., "The bitter taste of payback: the pathologising effect of TV revengendas" (2015). Faculty of Law, Humanities and the Arts - Papers. 2422. https://ro.uow.edu.au/lhapapers/2422 Research Online is the open access institutional repository for the University of Wollongong. For further information contact the UOW Library: [email protected] The bitter taste of payback: the pathologising effect of TV revengendas Abstract The thirst for vengeance is a timeless subject in popular entertainment. One need only think of Old Testament scripture; Shakespeare's Hamlet; Quentin Tarantino's Kill Bill or the TV series Revenge, and we immediately conjure up images of a protagonist striving to seek justice to avenge a heinous wrong committed against them. These texts, and others like it, speak to that which is ingrained in our human spirit about not only holding others responsible for their actions, but also about retaliation as payback. This article seeks to problematise the way the popular revenge narrative effectively constructs the vendetta as a guilty pleasure through which the audience can vicariously gain satisfaction, while at the same time perpetuates law's rhetoric that personal desires for vengeance are to be repressed and denied.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rise of Controversial Content in Film
    The Climb of Controversial Film Content by Ashley Haygood Submitted to the Department of Communication Studies in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Masters of Arts in Communication at Liberty University May 2007 Film Content ii Abstract This study looks at the change in controversial content in films during the 20th century. Original films made prior to 1968 and their remakes produced after were compared in the content areas of profanity, nudity, sexual content, alcohol and drug use, and violence. The advent of television, post-war effects and a proposed “Hollywood elite” are discussed as possible causes for the increase in controversial content. Commentary from industry professionals on the change in content is presented, along with an overview of American culture and the history of the film industry. Key words: film content, controversial content, film history, Hollywood, film industry, film remakes i. Film Content iii Acknowledgements I would like to thank my family for their unwavering support during the last three years. Without their help and encouragement, I would not have made it through this program. I would also like to thank the professors of the Communications Department from whom I have learned skills and information that I will take with me into a life-long career in communications. Lastly, I would like to thank my wonderful Thesis committee, especially Dr. Kelly who has shown me great patience during this process. I have only grown as a scholar from this experience. ii. Film Content iv Table of Contents ii. Abstract iii. Acknowledgements I. Introduction ……………………………………………………………………1 II. Review of the Literature……………………………………………………….8 a.
    [Show full text]
  • Jastics Delays' Rsveüge
    Jastics Delays’ Rsveüge- . ■ C il 1X1 a i - . : . "Г / с;. Justice Delays Revenge- The Spanish Tragedy and Revenge Tradition A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Letters and the Institute of Economics and Social Sciences of Bilkent University in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in English Language and Literature ta «4^^ by Erhan Kukner September, 1991 PR. Л.65'4 ' S63 è- 3079 We certify that we have read this thesis and that in our combined opinion it is fully adequate, in scope and in quality, as a thesis for the degree of Master of Arts. Asst.Prof.Dr. Hamit Çalışkan (A d v i s o r ) L y ■'· Prof.Dr. Bülent Bozkurt (Committee Member) ' ^ 1 Dr. Laurence A. Raw Approved for the Institute of Economics and Social Sciences 1 1 Abstract Justice Delays Revenge- The Spanish Tragedy and Revenge Tradition Erhan Kükner M.A. In English Literature Advisor: Asst.Prof-Dr. Hamit Çalışkan September, 1991 The Spanish Tragedv. one of the best examples of English Renaissance drama, contributed towards the establishment of the revenge tragedy genre, which gained popularity in the years to come. Kyd in this play not only indicates that when the law is unjust, man will resort to revenge; but also demonstrates that a citizen should obey the ruler and regard revenge as a revolt against the state. Tl-is play tells the story of Hieronimo, who expects the murderer^ of his son to be punished. However, Hieronimo gradually discovers that the institutions of justice are useless and therefore takes revenge.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Revenge Pornography: Public Perceptions of Revenge Pornography And
    1 Understanding Revenge Pornography: Public Perceptions of Revenge Pornography and Victim Blaming Bothamley, S. Tully, R.J. (2017) Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research Abstract Purpose: The disclosure of private images with the intent of causing distress is often described as ‘revenge pornography’. In the UK, this newly legislated crime has received a high level of media attention following several high profile cases, however there is a paucity of research in this area. Methods: 168 adults (UK general public) completed an online survey using a vignette approach. Views of the influence of perpetrator-victim relationship length and reason for termination were considered alongside perception of an offence, the necessity of police intervention, what extent revenge pornography creates psychological harm in victims, and victim blaming. Findings: Perpetrator-victim relationship length and reason for relationship breakdown did not influence perceptions of victim blame. Participants believed that the situation described in the vignettes was likely to be an offence, and that police intervention is somewhat necessary. Participants believed that the scenario was ‘very likely’ to create fear, and ‘moderately likely’ to create psychological/mental harm in victims. In line with the literature relating to stalking and sexual assault, men blamed the victim significantly more than women. Furthermore, women rated police intervention significantly more necessary than men. Implications: The public are recognising that revenge pornography is an offence, with consequences being fear and psychological harm, showing an awareness of the impact on victims. However, there are sex differences in the perceptions of revenge pornography and victim blame and this could be addressed by raising awareness of this crime.
    [Show full text]
  • The Understandings of Revenge Through Discussions with University Students
    The understandings of revenge through discussions with University students Elise McKenna Honours Thesis in Legal Studies Murdoch University, 2016 Declaration: “I declare this thesis is my own account of my research and contains as its main content work which has not been previously submitted for a degree at any tertiary education institution” (Elise May McKenna) Abstract: The goal of this study was to examine how revenge is understood by a sample of university students using a grounded theory approach. Desires and acts of revenge were found to be more prevalent in interpersonal relationships than stranger to stranger. Revenge was also shown to be a behaviour made through a costs benefit analysis to the victim rather than an act of irrationality. In addition to this, the understanding and concepts of revenge by the students were at times to be influenced by how the media had portrayed it to them. Lastly, it was shown that acts of revenge were used in all different groups of victims and types of harms through indirect and direct behaviours. Acknowledgments: This research was initially supported by Dr Courtney Field and I would like to thank him for initiating my interest in the topic of revenge and providing me with his insight. I would also, however, like to express my sincere gratitude to my Supervisor- Associate Professor Guy Hall. Supervision for this thesis has been complicated and when I needed someone to step in he did not hesitate to take on this role. I am very grateful for the time, effort and encouragement Guy has provided me over this year and previous years.
    [Show full text]
  • Revenge Season 4 Episode 3 Recap
    Revenge season 4 episode 3 recap After three seasons of sudsy buildup, the highly anticipated reunion of "Emily Thorne" and David Clarke went down on Revenge this Sunday. Tonight on ABC Revenge returns with an all new Sunday October 12, season 4 episode 3 called "Ashes." On this evening's. And now, Revenge Season 4, Episode 3: “Ashes” The Stowaway burns in Montauk. Jack rushes inside and finds Emily. He gets her out as. type: TV Show; Current Status: In Season; seasons: 4; run date: 09/21/11 In last week's episode, Charlotte tried to burn her sister alive, and in. Abe seeks revenge, but Simcoe will be waiting. genre: Drama; run date: 04/06/14; broadcaster: AMC; seasons: 4; Current Status: In Season. Another day, another pearl- clutching episode of 'Revenge' in which we ate our emotions in the form of lobster rolls. But before we get into. Revenge season 4, episode 3, "Ashes," just finished airing. Read our recap to see what happened with our South Hampton residents this week. Revenge season 4, episode 3 recap: 'Ashes.' David tries to kill Emily, Victoria needs money, Charlotte runs scared, Daniel can't get a job. Season chronology. ← Previous Season 3 · List of Revenge episodes. The fourth and final season of the ABC American television drama series Revenge premiered .. Finally abandoning her persona of Emily, Amanda looks to her future, with this episode looking back over her relationships with those around her, her. Watch Revenge Season 4 Episode 3 Online Recap. Jack arrives at the Stowaway to see it in flames.
    [Show full text]
  • Judgment, Authority, and the Morality of Justice in Hamlet
    Sugrue 1 Maegan Sugrue Judgment, Authority, and the Morality of Justice in Hamlet Shakespeare’s Hamlet, a complex and moving text, asks difficult questions: Is it right to commit an evil act in order to rid the world of a worse evil? Is it damnation to allow evil to remain because one desires to abstain from committing evil? What is the cost of justice? What knowledge can be trusted when determining when to pursue justice? How does one decide when to act? How does one decide when to hold back? These questions are further complicated by the many variants of the text. Quarto 1 (Q1) from 1603 is notoriously known as the “bad quarto.” Because of the many perceived corruptions that exist in the text, it is thought that Q1 is perhaps a reconstruction of the play from memory by some of the members of Shakespeare’s company. Quarto 2 (Q2) from 1604 was likely prepared from Shakespeare’s own handwritten manuscript, but the folio version from 1623 (F) has been accepted by scholars as the control text because it is likely the most true to how the play would have been performed while Shakespeare was alive and active in the company. Although most scholars agree that both Q2 and F come from Shakespeare himself, the two texts differ in many ways confronting editors with the questions of what to keep and what to cut. Two such instances are in 4.4 and 4.7. At the heart of these scenes specifically lie the following questions: Is justice only attainable at the expense of morals and ethics? If it is only attainable at the expense of morals and ethics, is it still justice? Is it revenge instead? In exploring these questions, one must also consider what knowledge should be trusted in making such important decisions.
    [Show full text]
  • Shakespeare and Poetry Pre-1900: Candidate Style Answers
    AS LEVEL Candidate Style Answers with commentary H072 ENGLISH LITERATURE H072/01 Shakespeare and poetry pre-1900 December 2015 AS LEVEL ENGLISH LITERATURE CANDIDATE STYLE ANSWERS WITH COMMENTARY CONTENTS INTRODUCTION 3 SECTION 1 QUESTION 1(A) 4 QUESTION 1(B) 6 QUESTION 2(A) 8 QUESTION 2(B) 11 QUESTION 3(A) 14 QUESTION 3(B) 17 QUESTION 4(A) 19 QUESTION 4(B) 21 QUESTION 5(A) 23 QUESTION 5(B) 25 QUESTION 6(A) 28 QUESTION 6(B) 32 SECTION 2 QUESTION 7 36 QUESTION 8 39 QUESTION 9 43 QUESTION 10 46 QUESTION 11 48 We’d like to know your view on the resources we produce. By clicking on the ‘Like’ or ‘Dislike’ button you can help us to ensure that our resources work for you. When the email template pops up please add additional comments if you wish and then just click ‘Send’. Thank you. If you do not currently offer this OCR qualification but would like to do so, please complete the Expression of Interest Form which can be found here: www.ocr.org.uk/expression-of-interest OCR Resources: the small print OCR’s resources are provided to support the teaching of OCR specifications, but in no way constitute an endorsed teaching method that is required by the Board and the decision to use them lies with the individual teacher. Whilst every effort is made to ensure the accuracy of the content, OCR cannot be held responsible for any errors or omissions within these resources. © OCR 2015 – This resource may be freely copied and distributed, as long as the OCR logo and this message remain intact and OCR is acknowledged as the originator of this work.
    [Show full text]
  • Villarreal Dissertation 04152016 Final
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Drug Violence, Fear of Crime and the Transformation of Everyday Life in the Mexican Metropolis Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6rc3w8pr Author Villarreal Montemayor, Ana Teresa Publication Date 2016 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Drug Violence, Fear of Crime and the Transformation of Everyday Life in the Mexican Metropolis By Ana Teresa Villarreal Montemayor A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Philosophy in Sociology in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Loic J. Wacquant, Chair Professor Laura J. Enriquez Professor Mara Loveman Professor Teresa Caldeira Professor Javier Auyero Spring 2016 Abstract Drug Violence, Fear of Crime and the Transformation of Everyday Life in the Mexican Metropolis by Ana Teresa Villarreal Montemayor Doctor of Philosophy in Sociology University of California, Berkeley Professor Loïc Wacquant, Chair This dissertation brings sharp social theory, deep history and precise ethnography to illumine the nexus of social and urban structure, human emotions, and power. I draw on Norbert Elias, Emile Durkheim, Marcel Mauss, among other social theorists and historians, to counter dominant views of fear in the social sciences as a sole destroyer of the social fabric with evidence of how and why fear both tears and tightens the social fabric, both destroys and fosters solidarity. Yet with the exception of a few spaces of hope where families of victims of forced disappearances organized to demand justice from the state, this “tightening” of the social fabric did not transcend but rather exacerbated socio-spatial divides.
    [Show full text]
  • Symbolic Revenge in Holocaust Child Survivors Nancy Isserman
    D 7 SYMBOLIC REVENGE IN HOLOCAUST CHILD SURVIVORS Nancy Isserman Introduction The attitudes of Holocaust survivors toward their persecutors present an intriguing subject for exploration. One component of the attitudes expressed is revenge. The concept of revenge is pervasive throughout literature, religious and legal writings, and history. Revenge is also considered a subset of political intolerance. Research on intolerance has been linked to age, education, and religious affiliation, among other demographic factors.1 Political intolerance is associated with low education, older age, rural residence, and fundamentalist reli- gious affiliation.2 My recent qualitative research found a connection between family-of-origin relationships and intolerance.3 Survivors who evidenced positive relationships with their family-of-origin caregivers also demonstrated tolerant attitudes toward the perpetrators of the genocide; conversely, survivors who had troubled relationships with their family-of-origin caregivers expressed intolerance toward the perpetrators.4 The Oxford Dictionaries online define revenge as both concrete and symbolic. The concrete definition incorporates physical behavior: “The action of inflicting hurt or harm on someone for an injury or wrong suffered at their hands.”5 Examples of concrete revenge are found in Holocaust survivor narratives during the later years of the war and immediately afterward. The most famous example of concrete revenge is Abba Kovner, who believed that “the Jews must seek revenge, answering a crime that could not be answered.”6 In the spring of 1945 he declared, “Yes, the War is over, but no, not for the Germans; it is time for the Germans to suffer; the Germans, who killed the Jews, must now pay with their own lives,”7 and quoted Psalm 94 in which God is called on to take revenge on the enemies of Israel.
    [Show full text]
  • Living Small Is the Best Revenge
    Living Small Is the Best Revenge Despite less-than-rhapsodic memories of miniaturized apartment living, the married ar- chitects Mimi Hoang and Eric Bunge designed the winner of the adAPT micro-unit com- petition sponsored by the New York City Housing and Preservation Department and endorsed by Mayor Michael R. Bloomberg. Their entry, “My Micro NY,” features 55 prefabricated apartments, of 360 square feet or less, stacked in a 10-story building on East 27th Street in Kips Bay. The building does not stint on amenities, providing a common terrace, a laundry room and a bike room. The couple’s firm, nArchitects, is based in a Dumbo loft, and they live in a Carroll Gar- dens town house, but for the competition they drew on their cohabitation in a tenement in Lower Manhattan. The mission was to seize an opportunity to render a tight space habitable and, if possi- ble, aesthetically pleasing. “At first blush we weren’t so sure about entering,” Mr. Bunge said, “because we felt people should have more space than this. But that was before we understood that the alternatives out there are far worse. There are people living together in substandard apartments all over the city who would prefer to live alone but can’t afford to. So we said, ‘Wait a minute, let’s make a humane small space where people would want to live.’ ” The architects say the design is flexible and transferable to virtually any neighborhood or borough. “This is a pilot program, and it doesn’t mean micro-units are going to be sprouting everywhere,” Ms.
    [Show full text]