Analysis of Alternative Keyboards Using Learning Curves Allison M

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Analysis of Alternative Keyboards Using Learning Curves Allison M View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Digital Repository @ Iowa State University Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engineering Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engineering Publications 2009 Analysis of Alternative Keyboards Using Learning Curves Allison M. Anderson North Carolina State University Gary Mirka Iowa State University, [email protected] Sharon M.B. Jones North Carolina State University David B. Kaber North Carolina State University Follow this and additional works at: http://lib.dr.iastate.edu/imse_pubs Part of the Ergonomics Commons, Industrial Engineering Commons, and the Systems Engineering Commons The ompc lete bibliographic information for this item can be found at http://lib.dr.iastate.edu/ imse_pubs/159. For information on how to cite this item, please visit http://lib.dr.iastate.edu/ howtocite.html. This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engineering at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engineering Publications by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Analysis of Alternative Keyboards using Learning Curves Allison M. Anderson, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, North Carolina, Gary A. Mirka, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa, and Sharon M. B. Joines and David B. Kaber, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, North Carolina ABSTRACT Objective: Quantify learning percentages for alternative keyboards (chord, contoured split, Dvorak, and split fixed-angle) and understand how physical, cognitive, and perceptual demand affect learning. Background: Alternative keyboards have been shown to offer ergonomic benefits over the conventional, single-plane QWERTY keyboard design, but productivity-related challenges may hinder their widespread acceptance. Method: Sixteen participants repeatedly typed a standard text passage using each alternative keyboard. Completion times were collected and subsequent learning percentages were calculated. Participants were asked to subjectively rate the physical, cognitive and perceptual demands of each keyboard and these values were then related to the calculated learning percentages. Results: Learning percentage calculations revealed the percentage for the split fixed-angle keyboard (90.4%) to be significantly different (p<0.05) from the learning percentages for the other three keyboards (chord: 77.3%, contour split: 76.9%, Dvorak: 79.1%). The average task completion time for the conventional QWERTY keyboard was 40 seconds, and the average times for the 5th trial on the chord, contoured split, Dvorak and split fixed-angle keyboards were 346, 69, 181 and 42 seconds, respectively. Conclusions: Productivity decrements can be quickly regained for the split fixed-angle and contour split keyboard but will take considerably longer for Dvorak and chord keyboards. The split fixed-angle keyboard involved physical learning while the others involved some combination of physical and cognitive learning, a result supported by the subjective responses. Application: Understanding the changes in task performance time that come with learning can provide additional information for a cost-benefit analysis when considering the implementation of ergonomic interventions. KEY WORDS: alternative keyboards, learning curve theory, ergonomic interventions, DVORAK, chord keyboards, split keyboards, musculoskeletal disorders POST-PRINT (manuscript after accepted but prior to publication) of the following published manuscript: Anderson A, GA Mirka, SMB Joines, and DB Kaber (2009) “Analysis of Alternative Keyboards Using Learning Curves”, Human Factors 51: 35-45, (DOI:10.1177/0018720808329844). 1 INTRODUCTION The basic, single-plane QWERTY keyboard has long been the conventional keyboard used in the office environment. Concerns related to the prevalence of work-related musculoskeletal disorders of the upper-extremity in computer users (e.g. Gerr et al., 2002; Gerr, Monteilh, & Marcus, 2006) have prompted designers to develop alternative keyboard designs that 1) reduce ulnar deviations of the wrists (e.g. split keyboards), 2) reduce finger motion (e.g. contoured split keyboards and chord style keyboards), and 3) make better use of the dominant fingers (2nd and 3rd digits) by altering the position of the letters on the keyboard (e.g. Dvorak keyboard). One impediment to the widespread acceptance of these alternative keyboards is the perception of a reduction in typing productivity over the short term, the long term, and/or permanently. A number of studies that have attempted to quantify this productivity impact and how, in the short term, it changes as a person becomes more proficient with the new keyboard (e.g. Smith & Cronin, 1993; Chen, Burastero, Tittiranonda, Hollerbach, Shih, & Denhoy, 1994; Gerard, Jones, & Wang, 1994; Swanson, Galinsky, Cole, Pan, & Sauter, 1997; Zecevic, Miller, & Harburn, 2000; Fagarasanu, Kumar, & Narayan, 2005). Zecevic et al. (2000) evaluated the performance of 16 experienced computer users with a split fixed-angle keyboard and showed that after 10 hours of use, participant’s typing speed was at about 90% of their speed with the conventional QWERTY design. In a study of a contoured split keyboard (Figure 1-top right), Gerard et al. (1994) showed that six professional typists were able to type with 72% of baseline QWERTY speed after 115 minutes of use. The split fixed-angle keyboard and contoured split keyboards described in the previous paragraph have different physical geometries than the conventional QWERTY keyboard, but still maintain the QWERTY layout of the keys. Two other keyboards have been proposed that do not employ the QWERTY layout. The Dvorak keyboard uses a more purposeful layout of the letters of the alphabet with which the right-hand does 56% of the typing (versus 43% on the QWERTY keyboard). Furthermore, about 70% of typing is performed on the home row thereby minimizing the movements of the fingers. Dvorak claimed that his keyboard layout could improve typing speed by 35% on typewriters (Dvorak, 1943). Subsequent authors have questioned these estimates (Alden, Daniels, & Kanarick, 1972; Norman & Fisher, 1982). The chord keyboard, likewise, does not use the QWERTY layout. On a chord keyboard, each letter has a specific key combination (single key or multiple keys) that is simultaneously pressed, similar to chords in music, to produce a given character (or word). Productivity of the chord keyboard approach has been explored in several studies (e.g. Gopher & Raij, 1988; Kroemer, 1992; McMulkin & Kroemer, 1994). In a study of 10 participants from a university, population Kroemer (1992) was able to show that participants were able to memorize 59 chords for a two-handed ternary chord keyboard (eight keys, one for each finger) within about 3 hours. In a subsequent performance analysis, participants showed a significant increase in typing speed (from 36 to 70 characters per minute (cpm)) after about 7 hours of typing with the chord system. Gopher and Raij (1988) tested 15 participants with no typing experience and placed them into one of three groups: QWERTY, one-handed chord keyboard, or two-handed chord keyboard (the two-handed chord was two separate one-hand chord keyboards, each could stand on their own). After 35 one-hour training sessions, chord users could type 160 cpm while QWERTY users could only type about 105 cpm. With 2 significant training, the chord keyboard appeared to yield higher productivity than the QWERTY keyboard for novices. While the above studies provide some data on change in productivity as a function of training time, none have used learning curve analysis to provide standardized measures for comparison across studies. Learning curve theory provides a structured, mathematical approach to predicting how task time decreases with repetition. It can be used to predict how long it will take for a person to reach a given level of performance on a task. As tasks become more complex, learning time increases. The standard equations used to find the learning percentage for a task is (Wright, 1936): N Yx=KX or (1) N = log (Yx/K) / log (X) (2) and Learning Percentage = 100*2N (3) Where: Yx: production time for Xth unit in sequence K: time required for first unit X: number of production units N: the slope of the line describing change in completion time (y) as a function of repetition number (x) in log-log space Accurate predictions of future productivity are important in industry, and therefore learning percentage values for many industrial tasks have been quantified in the literature (Konz and Johnson, 2000). Researchers have further explored these relationships and provided estimates for learning percentages based on the nature of the task being learned. Cognitive learning percentages have been shown to be around 70%, while more physical, motor learning percentages are about 90%, and tasks involving both physical and cognitive learning are somewhere in between (Dar-El, Ayas, & Gilad, 1995). More dramatic changes in time to complete a task in the early trials of the task are represented by a lower learning percentage (steep descent in the learning curve), while more moderate changes in these times are represented by a higher learning percentage (shallow descent in the learning curve.) Interestingly, while the research noted above has quantified changes in productivity
Recommended publications
  • A Universal Amazigh Keyboard for Latin Script and Tifinagh
    LES RESSOURCES LANGAGIERES : CONSTRUCTION ET EXPLOITATION A universal Amazigh keyboard for Latin script and Tifinagh Paul Anderson [email protected] 1. Introduction Systems of Amazigh text encoding and corresponding keyboard layouts have tended to be narrowly aimed at specific user communities, because of differences in phonology and orthography across Amazigh language variants1. Keyboard layouts for language variants have therefore lacked orthographic features found in other regions. This restricted focus impedes users' experimentation with the writing of other Amazigh regional variants and converged literary forms where they differ in orthographic features or in script. So far there has been no way to type more than a handful of Amazigh variants intuitively on any one layout even within one script. This fragmented development has meant that keyboard driver implementations have often lagged behind advances in technology, and have usually failed to take into account general keyboard layout design, ergonomy and typing speed, and solutions from other Amazigh regions or non-Amazigh languages. Some users even preferred to improvise key definitions based on their own understanding, which often resulted in mistaken use of lookalike letters and diacritics. Keyboard layouts have also failed to provide for Amazigh minority populations around the world, and have considered the multilingual context of Amazigh language use only locally. Several scripts are commonly used to write Amazigh variants, and even within a script there are different orthographies in use. Some orthographies are formal 1 I use the term 'language variant' since distinguishing 'dialect' and 'language' is not necessary here. ~ 165 ~ LES RESSOURCES LANGAGIERES : CONSTRUCTION ET EXPLOITATION standards. In others, some features are obsolete but still in use, some features are still disputed, and some features are regional usages or personal initiatives, or are required only for writing more phonetically.
    [Show full text]
  • Typing Tutor
    Typing Tutor Design Documentation Courtney Adams Jonathan Corretjer December 16, 2016 USU 3710 Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Scope 2 3 Design Overview 2 3.1 Requirements . .2 3.2 Dependencies . .2 3.3 Theory of Operation . .3 4 Design Details 5 4.1 Hardware Design . .5 4.2 Software Design . .5 4.2.1 LCD Keyboard Display . .5 4.2.2 Key Stroke Input . .7 4.2.3 Menu Layout . .8 4.2.4 Level and Test Design . .9 4.2.5 Correctness Logic . 11 4.2.6 Buzzer Implementation . 11 4.2.7 Calculations . 12 5 Testing 12 5.0.1 SSI Output to LCD . 12 5.0.2 Keyboard Data Input . 13 5.0.3 Buzzer Frequency . 14 5.0.4 Buzzer Length . 15 5.0.5 Trial Testing . 15 5.0.6 Calculations . 16 6 Conclusion 16 7 Appendix 18 7.1 Code.................................................. 18 7.1.1 LCD.h . 19 7.1.2 LCD.c . 20 7.1.3 main.c . 22 7.1.4 TypingLib.h . 24 7.1.5 TypingMenu.h . 27 7.1.6 TypingMenu.c . 28 7.1.7 Keys.h . 31 1 7.1.8 Keys.c . 32 7.1.9 Speaker.h . 37 7.1.10 Speaker.c . 38 2 1 Introduction The goal of the Typing Tutor is to enable users to practice and develop their typing skills using an interactive interface that encourages improvement. Composed of various levels, the Typing Tutor provides a challenging medium for both beginners and experienced, alike. At any time during the tutoring process, users can take a typing test to measure their progress in speed and accuracy.
    [Show full text]
  • Summary Double Your Typing Speed
    Summary Double Your Typing Speed.............................................................1 Stenography Benefits......................................................................1 Speed...........................................................................................................................................1 Fluency Of Thought....................................................................................................................2 Ergonomy....................................................................................................................................3 Mobile/Wearable Computing and Augmented Reality...............................................................3 Memorable Customizable Macros..............................................................................................4 Stenography Is Cool, But................................................................5 Open Source Stenography: Who Is Using It...................................5 Then I Saw The Light.....................................................................6 My Contributions............................................................................7 Stenography Is The Way.................................................................8 Machine stenography costs too much.........................................................................................8 Proprietary steno software might be better..................................................................................9 Learning takes too much
    [Show full text]
  • Keyboard Layouts: Lessons from the Meꞌphaa and Sochiapam Chinantec Designs
    From the files of Hugh Paterson III https://hughandbecky.us/Hugh-CV Keyboard layouts: Lessons from the Meꞌphaa and Sochiapam Chinantec designs Hugh Paterson III SIL International and the University of North Dakota [email protected] 15 December 2020 Version: Post-Print Preface In contrast to the publication of the original chapter, this post-print includes the references which were cited in the text, directly after the main text. Several style sheet changes have also been made: The main font has been changed, several small spelling corrections have been fixed, some diacritics are shown with ◌ rather than appearing bare, a table of ‘languages mentioned’ in the chapter, a list of abbreviations used, and in some places titles and names have been italicized as is commonly done in some publishing styles. The in-text citations for items mentioned as “in same volume” as the original chapter have now been added as full refernces. The book was first released in 2014, but the printed date/copyright date, as is common in many publishing venues, indicates the year following—in this case 2015. Even though author affiliation changed since publication, affiliation has been left as it was at the time of original authorship. Original publication Paterson III, Hugh J. 2015. Keyboard layouts: Lessons from the Meꞌphaa and Sochiapam Chinan- tec designs. In Mari C. Jones (ed.), Endangered Languages and New Technologies, 49–66. Cam- bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107279063.006. 1 Introduction Codification represents a major challenge for writers of endangered languages. Newtech- nologies render the process of typing on a keyboard more accessible and less expensive than at any previous point in time.
    [Show full text]
  • QWERTY: the Effects of Typing on Web Search Behavior
    QWERTY: The Effects of Typing on Web Search Behavior Kevin Ong Kalervo Järvelin RMIT University University of Tampere [email protected] [email protected] Mark Sanderson Falk Scholer RMIT University RMIT University [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT amount of relevant information for an underlying information need Typing is a common form of query input for search engines and to be resolved. In IIR, the focus is on the interaction between a hu- other information retrieval systems; we therefore investigate the man and a search system. The interaction can take place over three relationship between typing behavior and search interactions. The modalities, such as search by using/viewing images, speaking [17] search process is interactive and typically requires entering one or or traditionally by typing into a search box. In this paper we focus more queries, and assessing both summaries from Search Engine Re- on typing. sult Pages and the underlying documents, to ultimately satisfy some Nowadays, searchers expect rapid responses from search en- information need. Under the Search Economic Theory (SET) model gines [15]. However, the amount of useful information gathered of interactive information retrieval, differences in query costs will during the search process is also dependent on how well searchers result in search behavior changes. We investigate how differences are able to translate their search inputs (such as typing), into useful in query inputs themselves may relate to Search Economic Theory information output [2]. IIR is a collaborative effort between the by conducting a lab-based experiment to observe how text entries searcher and the system to answer an information need.
    [Show full text]
  • Belgian AZERTY Keyboard
    Belgians, Flemings: Demand your Swiss Keyboard! Serge Y. Stroobandt Copyright 2013–2015, licensed under Creative Commons BY-NC-SA Belgian AZERTY keyboard Three variants: French, Belgian and Tamazight (Berber). The AZERTY layout is used in France, Belgium and some African countries. It differs from the QWERTY layout in this: • The location of the [A] and [Q] keys are interchanged. • The location of the [Z] and [W] keys are interchanged. • [M] is moved to the right of [L] (where [; :] is on a US keyboard), • The digits [0] to [9] are on the same keys, but to be typed the shift key must be pressed. The unshifted positions are used for accented characters, • [Caps lock] is replaced by [Shift lock] , thus affecting non-letter keys as well. However, there is an ongoing evolution towards a [Caps lock] key instead of a [Shift lock] . It is used by most French speakers based in Europe, though France and Bel- gium each have their own national variations on the layout. Luxemburg and the French-speaking part of Switzerland use the Swiss QWERTZ keyboard. Most of the residents of Quebec, the mainly French-speaking province of Canada, use a QWERTY keyboard that has been adapted to the French lan- guage, although the government of Quebec and the Canadian federal gov- ernment stipulate and use the Multilingual Standard keyboard CAN/CSA Z243.200-92.[1][2][3] Het is de meest gebruikelijke indeling in België en in Frankrijk. Het toetsen- bord is ontworpen om gemakkelijk Franstalige teksten te kunnen invoeren, doch wordt niet alleen in Franstalige gebieden gebruikt: in heel België, waaron- der het Nederlandstalige Vlaanderen, is AZERTY de norm.
    [Show full text]
  • 11279 Keyboard Comparison
    11279 Keyboard Comparison After spending several weeks typing using the right-handed Dvorak keyboard layout, the coach (in- troduced in the previous problem) started to wonder if it really is any better. He decided to devise a method to verify this. The coach would like to compare typing on the right-handed Dvorak keyboard against using a QWERTY keyboard with two hands and with one hand. A crude, but effective way to compare keyboard layouts is to measure the total distance the typing fingers must travel in order to type a certain passage of text. Can you write a program to help the coach perform this experiment? The distance travelled by a finger will be measured from the centre of its home key to the centreof the target key and back. In order to simplify the computations, we pretend that every key is a perfect square with unit length sides, and that all keys are laid out on a perfect grid (even though in reality they are usually staggered). The diagrams below show the straightened keyboards; see the previous problem for shift-modified diagrams. The QWERTY and right!handed Dvorak keyboard layouts arranged on perfect grids. On the QWERTY keyboard, the home keys are “ASDFJKL;” for two hands and “FGHJ” for one hand. On the Dvorak keyboard, the home keys are “EHTD”. A key is always struck by the finger from the nearest home key. For example, the distances for ‘S’ would be 0, 4, and 2 for the two-hand QWERTY, one-hand QWERTY, and Dvorak keyboards, respectively. Likewise, the distances for ‘C’ would be 2, 2.828427…, and 2, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Per Minute (WPM) Lampung Script Keyboard
    Word Per Minute (WPM) Lampung Script Keyboard Mardiana Fahreza A. Arizal Meizano Ardhi Muhammad Informatics Engineering, Lampung Informatics Engineering, Lampung Informatics Engineering, Lampung University, Bandar Lampung, 35141, University, Bandar Lampung, 35141, University, Bandar Lampung, 35141, Indonesia Indonesia Indonesia [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Martinus Gita Paramita Djausal Mechanical Engineering, Lampung University, International Business, Lampung University, Bandar Lampung, 35141, Indonesia Bandar Lampung, 35141, Indonesia [email protected] [email protected] Abstract— Users of Font Lampung in previous studies Lampung people use unique writing called Lampung have difficulty in typing because the layout of the script or better known as Kaganga (A. Restuningrat Lampung script is complicated to understand. Re- 2017). designing the Lampung alphabet keyboard layout allows Lampung language itself is only used by a small group users to be able to type text in characters more easily. The of Lampung people as well as Lampung script. The use of purpose of this research is to create a Lampung alphabet Script (Aksara) is only used and studied in school, the keyboard layout for more effective typing of the rest is Latin (G. F. Nama and F. Arnoldi 2016). Lampung alphabet. The methods used are UX (User Modern society tends to use computer systems to Experience) that are, requirements gathering, alternative communicate. No representation of Lampung script in the design, prototype, evaluation, and result report computer system. Research has been done before to solve generation. The layout design is created by eliminating the problem, which is research on font Lampung script to the use of the SHIFT key and regrouping parent letters, be used in a computer system (Meizano A.
    [Show full text]
  • Japanese Typing Tutorial (For Mac): コンピュータで日本語
    JPN_Typing_Tutorial_Mac.doc Japanese Typing Tutorial (for Mac): コンピュータで日本語 1. Enable Japanese language features 1) In the Microsoft Office 2004 folder on your hard disk, open the Additional Tools folder, and then open the Microsoft Language Register folder. 2) Drag the Microsoft Word icon on top of the Microsoft Language Register utility. 3) In the Select the language to enable for pop-up menu, click Japanese. Note After running the Microsoft Language Register program, you should select the Japanese keyboard layout provided with the Apple OS, or a third-party keyboard layout of your choice. To do so, open System Preferences, click International, and then click the Input Menu tab. 4) In the menu bar at the top of the screen do you see American flag icon or “A” in the black box? Click the icon, and choose [あ] Hiragana. 2. How to Type ひらがな 2.1 Romaji Typing System When you type Japanese, you have to use Romanization because HIRAGANA or KATAKANA is not listed on your keyboard. But the Romanization you are supposed to use for the word processor is not quite the same as the one you learned in class. You have to interpret Romanization as a mere substitution for the written syllables, HIRAGANA. You first have to consider how the word you want to type is written in HIRAGANA and then transcribe each HIRAGANA character into corresponding romaji. After typing in the Romanization, hit ‘Enter’ key (do not hit ‘Space’ key before hitting ‘Enter’ key, that will convert HIRAGNA into Kanji). Hit space key after each word if you want to have a space.
    [Show full text]
  • Alphasmart Manager 2
    AlphaSmart Manager 2 User’s Guide AlphaSmart Sales and Technical Support For AlphaSmart sales and technical support contact information, see page 81 or visit the AlphaSmart web site at www.alphasmart.com (United States) www.alphasmart.co.uk (United Kingdom) www.alphasmart.com/international (all other countries) Nothing in this manual may be reproduced in any manner, either wholly or in part, for any purpose whatsoever without written permission from AlphaSmart, Inc. © 2004, AlphaSmart, Inc. All rights reserved. AlphaSmart and SmartApplet are registered trademarks, and AlphaWord is a trademark of AlphaSmart, Inc. Macintosh is a registered trademark of Apple Computer, Inc. Windows is a registered trademark of Microsoft Corporation. Contents Chapter 1 Introduction ....................................................................................1 System requirements........................................................................... 2 AlphaHub considerations ............................................................ 2 Upgrading from AlphaSmart Manager 1.0 ......................................... 3 Installing AlphaSmart Manager.......................................................... 4 Chapter 2 Basics .............................................................................................7 Using AlphaSmart Manager in group settings .................................. 7 Starting AlphaSmart Manager ............................................................ 9 Moving around in AlphaSmart Manager.........................................
    [Show full text]
  • Keyboard Layouts
    Manuel Matuzović @mmatuzo ViennaJS 05/2017 KEYBOARD CONTROLS FOR AN INTERNATIONAL USER BASE Manuel Matuzovic Manuel Matuzović č, ć, đ, dž, š, ž, nj, lj q, w, x Keyboard layouts “A keyboard layout is any specific mechanical, visual, or functional arrangement of the keys, legends, or key-meaning associations (respectively) of a computer, typewriter, or other typographic keyboard.” Wikipedia QWERTY QWERTZ AZERTY JCUKEN Soooo? Events • keydown The keydown event is fired when a key is pressed down. • keyup The keyup event is fired when a key is released. • keypress The keypress event is fired when a key is pressed down Values • keyCode Returns a numerical code identifying the value of the pressed key. • which Returns a numeric code identifying the value of the pressed key; this is usually the same as keyCode. • charCode Returns a Number representing the Unicode reference number of the key; this atribute is used only by the keypress event. event.keyCode window.addEventListener('keydown', navigate); function navigate(e) { ... if (e.keyCode === 68) { moveLeftAndRight(1); } if (e.keyCode === 90) { shootMissile(); } } Issues • Different meaning of properties when handling a key event (keydown or keyup) versus a character event (keypress). • The values of keys in keypress events are different between browsers. • Inconsistent values for keys and events cross-browser • keyCode on key events tries to be international-friendly, but isn’t QWERTZ QWERTY QWERTY Remember AZERTY? UI Events: API • Two new properties: key and code • event.key - printable character or a descriptive string, e.g. z • event.code - physical key, e.g. KeyY • Reference keyboard in the specification Reference keyboard event.keyCode window.addEventListener('keydown', navigate); function navigate(e) { ..
    [Show full text]
  • The Dvorak Keyboard Layout and Possibilities of Its Regional Adaptation
    373 The Dvorak Keyboard Layout and Possibilities of its Regional Adaptation Tomislav NakiC-AlfireviC, Marijan Durek Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, University of Zagreb tomisluv.nakic-alfirevic@,kr. hr: marl:ian.diurek@,fer. hr Abstract. During the last several decades, the 2. The Dvorak layout keyboard has proved to be the most important The Dvorak layout was constructed to computer input device. It was inherited @om the [1][2][3][4] be the optimal layout where "optimal" simply means mechanical typewriter from whom computer "fastest". There are a number of factors that have keyboards inherited a suboptimal key layout. Better some impact on typing speed and comfort: letter solutions have been kept out use by economic of frequencies, key reachability, human hand anatomy, pressure. An arguably optimal layout for the certain regularities found in any text in any English language is the Dvorak layout. This article language, and one or two others. discusses the possibility of applying the same guidelines and ideas that shaped the Dvorak layout on keyboards used in other languages. Some of the questions answered are how good the Dvorak layout is in EngIish. how good is it in other languages and how a language specific version would look like. Keywords. Dvorak, layout, keyboard, left handed, right handed, frequency, regional, national, language Obviously, not all letters occur with the same frequency: vowels are a good example of very 1. Introduction frequent letters. Knowing that it takes a certain amount of time to reach any key on the keyboard A machine with a complicated system of levers and and knowing that some keys can be reached faster weights called the typewriter was invented around than others an optimization problem arises: how 1870.
    [Show full text]