Information Flow and Organization of Stingless Bee Foraging Jacobus Biesmeijer, E

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Information Flow and Organization of Stingless Bee Foraging Jacobus Biesmeijer, E Information flow and organization of stingless bee foraging Jacobus Biesmeijer, E. Judith Slaa To cite this version: Jacobus Biesmeijer, E. Judith Slaa. Information flow and organization of stingless bee foraging. Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2004, 35 (2), pp.143-157. 10.1051/apido:2004003. hal-00891878 HAL Id: hal-00891878 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00891878 Submitted on 1 Jan 2004 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie 35 (2004) 143–157 © INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2004 143 DOI: 10.1051/apido:2004003 Review article Information flow and organization of stingless bee foraging Jacobus C. BIESMEIJER*, E. Judith SLAA Neurobiology and Behavior, Seeley G. Mudd Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca NY 14850, USA (Received 1st August 2003; revised 16 October 2003; accepted 15 December 2003) Abstract – Stingless bees (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponini) live in populous permanent colonies and face the same problem as other foraging social insects: how to coordinate the worker’s actions and respond to the spatio-temporal uncertainties of food availability in their habitat. Here we review the (social) information used by individual foragers and how organized collective foraging emerges from the individual actions. We also address intra- and interspecific competition for food and the impact of the African honey bee on stingless bee collective foraging. Melipona / Trigona / self-organization / communication / foraging / Africanized honeybee / insect society 1. INTRODUCTION mellifera L. The largest (Michener, 2000) group of social bees, the stingless bees (Api- Large social insect colonies lack central- dae, Meliponini) has received much less atten- ized control yet function as coherent wholes tion, probably because they inhabit the tropical and flexibly adjust their activities in response forest, which traditionally has received less to environmental challenges. How such a sys- attention from scientists. Only a small fraction tem is organized has become apparent through of the more than 400 species has been studied painstakingly observing the behavior of indi- in any detail, but already a wide variety of vidually marked individuals (mostly in bees; nesting and foraging habits has been discov- e.g. Seeley, 1995) or largely anonymous ered. In this paper we review information use worker groups (mostly in ants; e.g. various by individual foragers and how this relates to chapters in Detrain et al., 1999a). Observation organized foraging at colony level. First we of marked individuals allows one to assess describe the various sources of information the roles of previous experience and social that foragers may use in individual decision- information on individual decision-making, making, then we address the behavioral forag- whereas observation of worker groups focuses ing strategies that stingless bees display, and on colony-level processes such as allocation of how these lead to single colony and multi spe- workers over tasks and collective defense and cies foraging patterns observed in the field. foraging. Only through connecting these two Finally we analyze the role of different forag- levels, the mechanisms of individual behavior ing strategies in stingless bee coexistence and and the patterns that emerge at colony level, the influence of the recent invasion of the Afri- can a thorough understanding of the social can honey bee in the Americas on stingless bee physiology of insect societies be obtained. collective foraging. Much of our knowledge of social bees Note that although we aim at reviewing stems from studies performed on a single information on all stingless bees, most studies ‘atypical’ species, the western honey bee Apis deal with Neotropical species, some with * Corresponding author: [email protected]; [email protected] 144 J.C. Biesmeijer, E.J. Slaa Asian species, and very few with African Memory is another major source of intrinsic species. Much study has been devoted to information and is used to decide when to start recruitment mechanisms of stingless bees, foraging, where to forage, and which flower to which is reviewed by Nieh in this issue. visit. This allows bees to inspect known food sources after an interruption (time-place learn- ing; Freitas and Raw, 2000; Biesmeijer and 2. INFORMATION SOURCES Ermers, 1999; Breed et al., 2002), show flower FOR FORAGERS constancy within patches and over consecutive foraging bouts (Inoue et al., 1985; Biesmeijer An individual bee makes its behavioral and Tóth, 1998; Slaa et al., 1998, 2003a), and decisions based on information from a variety discover new patches of learned food plants of sources. We distinguish five major private (Biesmeijer, unpublished data) without need- and social information sources: the individual ing information from nest mates. As a group itself (spontaneous preferences and memory), experienced foragers probably inspect all prof- the nest environment (colony state and nest itable food sources from the previous day after mates), nest mates in the field, conspecific an overnight interruption. Together they form non-nest mates in the field, and heterospecifics the ‘joint memory’ of the colony (Thierry in the field. These extrinsic information et al., 1996) and provide the up-to-date infor- sources provide a continuous flow of informa- mation that is the basis of efficient and flexible tion to the forager who integrates it with its colony foraging. intrinsic information to make behavioral deci- Other sources of intrinsic information sions. We summarize the evidence for infor- include development and hormones. In bum- mation use from each of these sources and its ble bees larval feeding determines the size effect on the receiver (Tab. I). In this review of the adult bee, which in turn determines we will not address other environmental the bee’s tendency to forage (Spaethe and sources of information used by foragers, Weidenmüller, 2002) and her visual acuity and e.g. climatic conditions (Lutz, 1931; Kleinert- olfactory perception (Spaethe et al., 2001). Giovannini, 1982; Heard and Hendrikz, 1993), Juvenile hormone levels are involved in the features of flowers, flower patches and other switch from nurse bee to forager in honey bees food sources, or the presence of predators (e.g. Robinson, 1987). None of these factors (e.g. Craig et al., 1996). has received much attention in stingless bee research. 2.1. Intrinsic information 2.2. Information from nest environment Influence of genes on stingless bee behav- ior is virtually unknown. However, it is safe to Stingless bee colonies consist of several assume that stingless bees show spontaneous hundreds to tens of thousands of individuals, preferences for some foraging parameters, e.g. one or few queens, some virgin queens and flower color and odor, and differ in learning males. Virgin queens and males do not partic- capacity (Pessotti and Lé’Sénéchal, 1981; ipate in foraging [but may visit flowers during Biesmeijer and Slaa, unpubl. data) and other their escapades (Kerr, 1994)] and workers col- aspects of foraging (e.g. scouting tendency, lect all food. Therefore, information exchange food type selection), as do honey bees. Ranger among the workers is key to colony foraging and O’Donnell (1999) demonstrated a genetic efficiency and indirectly to colony growth and effect on pollen and nectar collecting among reproductive success. Social organization is Partamona bilineata foragers (= P. orizabaen- probably largely based on self-organization sis according to Pedro and Camargo, 2003). and individuals use rules of thumb based on The acquisition of information must also differ information obtained from the nest structures among the species as a result of species-spe- and nest mates to make decisions. cific differences in their sensory organs (olfac- Although the nest environment is structured tory disk number: Johnson and Howard, 1987; by activities of the colony’s members, it also slight differences in photoreceptors: Menzel, structures individual behavior. This process is 1990), but has rarely been studied. called stigmergy (Grassé, 1959) and plays an Information flow and stingless bee foraging 145 Table I. Information sources used by stingless bee foragers. Note that not all bee species use all information sources equally (see text for further explanation). Although most species forage for floral nectar and pollen, the information sources also apply to other resources used by stingless bees, such as fruits, meat, resin, mud and water. Information source Information type Receiver Effect References 1. Intrinsic sources Genes Innate preferences All bees Food type selection 1-4 through gene expression (nectar/pollen, flower color, odor etc.), learning capacity, perception, and possibly scouting tendency Brain Learned information All bees through memory A. Time of foraging Start inspection activity 5-7 B. Place of food source Inspection of known 8-10 food source C. Food type Flower constancy (odor, 11-15 color), quality preference (e.g. nectar concentration) Development and Preference mediated by ?? Not studied
Recommended publications
  • Classification of the Apidae (Hymenoptera)
    Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Mi Bee Lab 9-21-1990 Classification of the Apidae (Hymenoptera) Charles D. Michener University of Kansas Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.usu.edu/bee_lab_mi Part of the Entomology Commons Recommended Citation Michener, Charles D., "Classification of the Apidae (Hymenoptera)" (1990). Mi. Paper 153. https://digitalcommons.usu.edu/bee_lab_mi/153 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Bee Lab at DigitalCommons@USU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Mi by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@USU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 4 WWvyvlrWryrXvW-WvWrW^^ I • • •_ ••^«_«).•>.• •.*.« THE UNIVERSITY OF KANSAS SCIENC5;^ULLETIN LIBRARY Vol. 54, No. 4, pp. 75-164 Sept. 21,1990 OCT 23 1990 HARVARD Classification of the Apidae^ (Hymenoptera) BY Charles D. Michener'^ Appendix: Trigona genalis Friese, a Hitherto Unplaced New Guinea Species BY Charles D. Michener and Shoichi F. Sakagami'^ CONTENTS Abstract 76 Introduction 76 Terminology and Materials 77 Analysis of Relationships among Apid Subfamilies 79 Key to the Subfamilies of Apidae 84 Subfamily Meliponinae 84 Description, 84; Larva, 85; Nest, 85; Social Behavior, 85; Distribution, 85 Relationships among Meliponine Genera 85 History, 85; Analysis, 86; Biogeography, 96; Behavior, 97; Labial palpi, 99; Wing venation, 99; Male genitalia, 102; Poison glands, 103; Chromosome numbers, 103; Convergence, 104; Classificatory questions, 104 Fossil Meliponinae 105 Meliponorytes,
    [Show full text]
  • Stingless Bee Nesting Biology David W
    Stingless bee nesting biology David W. Roubik To cite this version: David W. Roubik. Stingless bee nesting biology. Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2006, 37 (2), pp.124-143. hal-00892207 HAL Id: hal-00892207 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00892207 Submitted on 1 Jan 2006 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie 37 (2006) 124–143 124 c INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2006 DOI: 10.1051/apido:2006026 Review article Stingless bee nesting biology* David W. Ra,b a Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Apartado 0843-03092, Balboa, Ancón, Panamá, República de Panamá b Unit 0948, APO AA 34002-0948, USA Received 2 October 2005 – Revised 29 November 2005 – Accepted 23 December 2005 Abstract – Stingless bees diverged since the Cretaceous, have 50 times more species than Apis,andare both distinctive and diverse. Nesting is capitulated by 30 variables but most do not define clades. Both architectural features and behavior decrease vulnerability, and large genera vary in nest habit, architecture and defense. Natural stingless bee colony density is 15 to 1500 km−2. Symbionts include mycophagic mites, collembolans, leiodid beetles, mutualist coccids, molds, and ricinuleid arachnids.
    [Show full text]
  • Copyright by Megan O'connell 2021
    Copyright by Megan O’Connell 2021 The Dissertation Committee for Megan O’Connell certifies that this is the approved version of the following Dissertation: PLANT-POLLINATOR INTERACTIONS IN THE FACE OF GLOBAL CHANGE Committee: Shalene Jha, Supervisor Stanley Roux Lawrence Gilbert Alexander Wild Thomas Juenger PLANT-POLLINATOR INTERACTIONS IN THE FACE OF GLOBAL CHANGE by Megan O’Connell Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Texas at Austin May 2021 Dedication I dedicate this dissertation to anyone who is curious about pursuing the sciences but does not believe they can. To anyone who thinks they are not smart enough or feels they do not have the resources and support to pursue field work, research, and graduate studies. To anyone who does not see their likeness reflected in the images of scientists they see in the media, text books, and names of authors listed on publications. With training, we all can be scientists, we all can earn PhD’s, we all can pursue our curiosities about the world, measure its patterns, and marvel at its wonders. I dedicate my dissertation to anyone who dreams of being a scientist but is too intimidated to pursue their dream. On your behalf, I promise to actively work make my field a more welcoming, diverse, and inclusive community in all my future endeavors. iv Acknowledgements This dissertation would not have been possible without the tremendously generous support of so many people, but above all my graduate mentor, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Apidae, Meliponini) Thiago Jesus, Giorgio Venturieri, Felipe Contrera
    Time–place learning in the bee Melipona fasciculata (Apidae, Meliponini) Thiago Jesus, Giorgio Venturieri, Felipe Contrera To cite this version: Thiago Jesus, Giorgio Venturieri, Felipe Contrera. Time–place learning in the bee Melipona fasciculata (Apidae, Meliponini). Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2014, 45 (2), pp.257-265. 10.1007/s13592-013- 0245-2. hal-01234722 HAL Id: hal-01234722 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01234722 Submitted on 27 Nov 2015 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie (2014) 45:257–265 Original article * INRA, DIB and Springer-Verlag France, 2013 DOI: 10.1007/s13592-013-0245-2 Time–place learning in the bee Melipona fasciculata (Apidae, Meliponini) 1 2 Thiago Nazareno Conceição Silva de JESUS , Giorgio Cristino VENTURIERI , 1 Felipe Andrés León CONTRERA 1Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Laboratório de Biologia e Ecologia de Abelhas, Universidade Federal do Pará, Rua Augusto Corrêa, No. 1, Campus Básico, Guamá, CEP 66075-110, Belém, Pará, Brazil 2Laboratório de Botânica, Embrapa Amazônia Oriental, Trav. Dr. Enéas Pinheiro s/nº Caixa Postal, 48, CEP 66095-100, Belém, Pará, Brazil Received 31 May 2013 – Revised 10 August 2013 – Accepted 20 September 2013 Abstract – Nectar and pollen are highly sought-after resources by different species of animals, including several stingless bees.
    [Show full text]
  • Observations on the Urban Ecology of the Neotropical Stingless Bee Tetragonisca Angustula (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Meliponini)
    KU ScholarWorks | http://kuscholarworks.ku.edu Please share your stories about how Open Access to this article benefits you. Observations on the urban ecology of the Neotropical stingless bee Tetragonisca angustula (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Meliponini). by Velez-Ruiz, Victor Gonzalez, and Michael Engel 2013 This is the published version of the article, made available with the permission of the publisher. The original published version can be found at the link below. Engel, Michael S. (2013). Observations on the urban ecology of the Neotropical stingless bee Tetragonisca angustula (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Meliponini). Journal of Melittology 15:41647. Published version: https://journals.ku.edu/index.php/melittology/ article/view/4528 Terms of Use: http://www2.ku.edu/~scholar/docs/license.shtml KU ScholarWorks is a service provided by the KU Libraries’ Office of Scholarly Communication & Copyright. Journal of Melittology Bee Biology, Ecology, Evolution, & Systematics The latest buzz in bee biology No. 15, pp. 1–8 29 July 2013 Observations on the urban ecology of the Neotropical stingless bee Tetragonisca angustula (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Meliponini) Rita I. Velez-Ruiz1, Victor H. Gonzalez2,3, & Michael S. Engel3,4 Abstract. Tetragonisca angustula (Latreille) is a small, docile, cavity-nesting stingless bee that is widely distributed in the Neotropical region. This species is particularly abundant in disturbed environments, including human settlements. Between August 2005 and March 2006, we located and followed during eight months 59 nests of this species in Medellín, the second most popu- lated city in Colombia. Herein, we document their foraging behavior, mortality, and incidence of predators and natural enemies. Also, to determine if higher ambient temperature and light intensity in urban environments affect the daily foraging activity of T.
    [Show full text]
  • Native Bee Flashcards
    Blue-Banded Bee This bee has blue and black stripes on its abdomen and is a solitary bee, which means it likes to travel around and live on its own. Teddy Bear Bee This bee has the name of Teddy Bear Bee because it is a bit chubby and is covered in a brown, fluffy fur! They build burrows in soil to use as their nests. (Image source: https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Big_Ol_Teddy_Bear_Bee.jpg) Carpenter Bee This is a female Carpenter Bee. The males have quite different colouring, they still have a metallic sheen but are more yellowy-green. When building its nest the female bee will cut nest burrows into the flower spikes of grass trees or other soft timbers. Stingless Bee This is a Stingless Bee, which means these bees don’t have a sting. They also produce honey. There are 11 species of Stingless Bees in Australia. Resin Bee Resin Bees get their name because they build their houses out of resin (like the sap from trees). They come in a range of colours, including red, orange and black. Lasioglossum Bees There are over 250 species of Lasioglossum Bees in Australia. The female Lasioglossum Bees can carry large amounts of pollen on their furry bodies and legs. Females usually work alone to create their nests in burrows in the ground, although sometimes a group of females may share a nest. Leafcutter Bee The amazing Leafcutter Bee cuts out a neat circle or oval from a leaf and uses these leaf pieces to weave little cradles for her eggs to rest in inside her burrow nest.
    [Show full text]
  • What Are Stingless Bees, and Why and How to Use Them As Crop Pollinators? - a Review
    JARQ 34(3), 183- 190 (2000) hltp://ss.jircas.afirc.go.jp What are Stingless Bees, and Why and How to Use Them as Crop Pollinators? - a Review - Kazuhiro AMAN01*, Tetsu NEMOTa2 and Tim A. HEARD3 1 Laboratory of Apiculture, National Institute of Animal Industry (Tsukuba, lbaraki, 305-0901 Japan) 2 Laboratory of Neurophysiology, National Institute of Animal Industry (Tsukuba, lbaraki, 305-0901 Japan) 3 Long Pocket Laboratories, CSIRO Entomology (P:M:B 3 Indooroopilly 4068, Australia) Abstract The efficiency of insects as crop pollinators depends on their biological characteristics in relation to the crop and the environment in which they are needed. For glasshouse pollination in Japan, stingless bees are potentially promising pollinators for the following reasons: they are harmless to beekeepers and glasshouse workers, they visit a wide range of crops (polylecly), they are tolerant of high tempera­ tures, they are active throughout the year, they can be transported easily, and they do not pose an envi­ ronmental risk by escaping and invading natural habitats as they would not survive the Japanese winter. There are still, however, some limitations to using stingless bees in such areas, one of which lies in how to improve methods for propagating and maintaining the colonies throughout the year. To address this prob! em we suggest the development of a new type of hive box. Discipline: Crop production I Horticulture Additional key words: beekeeping, meliponiculture 111i11a11gkaba11, T. moorei, T. itama, from Southeast Asia; Introduction Na111101rigo11a testaceicomis, Plebeia dro,ya,,a, T. a11g11s111la, T. barocoloradensis from Brazil, with a view Stingless bees are soc ial bees which lack a func­ to testing them as pollinators of strawberries and pub­ tional st ing.
    [Show full text]
  • 33130558.Pdf
    SERIE RECURSOS HIDROBIOLÓGICOS Y PESQUEROS CONTINENTALES DE COLOMBIA VII. MORICHALES Y CANANGUCHALES DE LA ORINOQUIA Y AMAZONIA: COLOMBIA-VENEZUELA Parte I Carlos A. Lasso, Anabel Rial y Valois González-B. (Editores) © Instituto de Investigación de Recursos Impresión Biológicos Alexander von Humboldt. 2013 JAVEGRAF – Fundación Cultural Javeriana de Artes Gráficas. Los textos pueden ser citados total o parcialmente citando la fuente. Impreso en Bogotá, D. C., Colombia, octubre de 2013 - 1.000 ejemplares. SERIE EDITORIAL RECURSOS HIDROBIOLÓGICOS Y PESQUEROS Citación sugerida CONTINENTALES DE COLOMBIA Obra completa: Lasso, C. A., A. Rial y V. Instituto de Investigación de Recursos Biológicos González-B. (Editores). 2013. VII. Morichales Alexander von Humboldt (IAvH). y canangunchales de la Orinoquia y Amazonia: Colombia - Venezuela. Parte I. Serie Editorial Editor: Carlos A. Lasso. Recursos Hidrobiológicos y Pesqueros Continen- tales de Colombia. Instituto de Investigación de Revisión científica: Ángel Fernández y Recursos Biológicos Alexander von Humboldt Fernando Trujillo. (IAvH). Bogotá, D. C., Colombia. 344 pp. Revisión de textos: Carlos A. Lasso y Paula Capítulos o fichas de especies: Isaza, C., Sánchez-Duarte. G. Galeano y R. Bernal. 2013. Manejo actual de Mauritia flexuosa para la producción de Asistencia editorial: Paula Sánchez-Duarte. frutos en el sur de la Amazonia colombiana. Capítulo 13. Pp. 247-276. En: Lasso, C. A., A. Fotos portada: Fernando Trujillo, Iván Mikolji, Rial y V. González-B. (Editores). 2013. VII. Santiago Duque y Carlos A. Lasso. Morichales y canangunchales de la Orinoquia y Amazonia: Colombia - Venezuela. Parte I. Serie Foto contraportada: Carolina Isaza. Editorial Recursos Hidrobiológicos y Pesqueros Continentales de Colombia. Instituto de Foto portada interior: Fernando Trujillo.
    [Show full text]
  • Melliferous Insects and the Uses Assigned to Their Products in The
    Flores et al. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine (2018) 14:27 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-018-0222-y RESEARCH Open Access Melliferous insects and the uses assigned to their products in the northern Yungas of Salta, Argentina Fabio Fernando Flores1, Norma Inés Hilgert2* and Liliana Concepción Lupo1,3 Abstract Background: The order Hymenoptera comprises melliferous insects (bees, wasps and bumblebees); among them, stingless bees comprise a diverse group of eusocial insects present in tropical and subtropical areas. Of a total of approximately 500 species, 400 are found in the Neotropics. On the continent of America, before the introduction of Apis mellifera, these insects represented the main source of honey and wax. In Argentina, ethnobiological investigations had been carried out on this group of insects, principally in the Atlantic Forest and Chaco regions. Out of a total of 33 species, only 14 were recorded for use or breeding. In the Yungas, however, there are no ethnobiological studies analyzing this group of species, although the use of their products is mentioned in different ethnobotanical works. This paper studies the knowledge and uses of melliferous insects by the inhabitants of the village of Baritú and surrounding. Method: Informationonlocation,managementanddutiesassigned(e.g.,preparationandadministration)todealwithbee products like honey, pollen, wax and propolis was obtained through semi-structured interviews. Besides, reference material was collected to identify melliferous insects known and used in the region. Results: Fifteen ethnospecies were identified and grouped locally according to their defensive behavior. The culturally most important species is the stingless bee Plebeia sp. nov.—mansita-, in terms of frequency of citations and diversity of uses, and among those that sting, the honeybee Apis mellifera—extranjera-.
    [Show full text]
  • 6. Meliponiculture of Stingless Bees
    6. MELIPONICULTURE OF STINGLESS BEES MELIPONINAE Stingless bees are closely related to the honeybees, bumblebees and orchid bees. Work with stingless bees is called meliponiculture. Stingless bees are amongst the longest evolved bees, and have been found preserved inside pieces of amber 80 million years old. Stingless bees developed before the continents drifted apart from each other. Therefore, they are present in all tropical parts of the world. It is estimated that 400 to 500 different species of stingless bees are known, but new species are identified every year. Approximate numbers of species so far identified are 50 in Africa, 300 species in the Americas, 60 in Asia, 10 in Australia. Four species occur in Madagascar. The different species are diverse: their size ranges from two millimetres (e.g. the tiny sweet bees) to stingless bees slightly bigger than the European honeybee. The number of bees a colony can contain ranges from some few hundred to more than a hundred thousand bees. This differs from species to species. The stingless bees have evolved a wide range of nesting and feeding behaviours that allow them to share habitats and to occur in high densities. Some species of stingless bees are nest parasites. This means that their queens are laying eggs in other bee’s nests. All stingless bees build their nest in a closed structure. The Melipona species of stingless bees and most of the species Trigona usually build their nests in hollow tree trunks or branches, and some in cavities in the ground or empty mice or parrot nests. Other species live in ants or termites’ nests.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Landscapes and Landmarks in Bee Navigation: a Review
    insects Review The Role of Landscapes and Landmarks in Bee Navigation: A Review Bahram Kheradmand * and James C. Nieh Section of Ecology, Behavior, and Evolution, Division of Biological Sciences, UC San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-858-822-5010 Received: 31 August 2019; Accepted: 9 October 2019; Published: 12 October 2019 Abstract: The ability of animals to explore landmarks in their environment is essential to their fitness. Landmarks are widely recognized to play a key role in navigation by providing information in multiple sensory modalities. However, what is a landmark? We propose that animals use a hierarchy of information based upon its utility and salience when an animal is in a given motivational state. Focusing on honeybees, we suggest that foragers choose landmarks based upon their relative uniqueness, conspicuousness, stability, and context. We also propose that it is useful to distinguish between landmarks that provide sensory input that changes (“near”) or does not change (“far”) as the receiver uses these landmarks to navigate. However, we recognize that this distinction occurs on a continuum and is not a clear-cut dichotomy. We review the rich literature on landmarks, focusing on recent studies that have illuminated our understanding of the kinds of information that bees use, how they use it, potential mechanisms, and future research directions. Keywords: landmark detection; salience; visual information; multimodal navigation; Apis mellifera 1. Introduction 1.1. What Are Landmarks? A major and fascinating question in the study of animal cognition is how animals identify, remember, and use landmarks to navigate.
    [Show full text]
  • A Molecular Phylogeny and the Evolution of Nest Architecture and Behavior in Trigona S.S
    Apidologie 39 (2008) Available online at: c INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2008 www.apidologie.org DOI: 10.1051/apido:2007051 Original article A molecular phylogeny and the evolution of nest architecture and behavior in Trigona s.s. (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Meliponini)* Claus Rasmussena,JoãoM.F.Camargob a Department of Entomology, University of Illinois, 320 Morrill Hall, 505 S. Goodwin Ave. Urbana, IL 61801, USA b Departamento de Biologia, Faculdade de Filosofia, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Av. Bandeirantes, 3900, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto-SP, Brazil Received 29 June 2007 – Revised 28 October 2007 – Accepted 1 November 2007 Abstract – Stingless bees exhibit extraordinary variation in nest architecture within and among species. To test for phylogenetic association of behavioral traits for species of the Neotropical stingless bee genus Trigona s.s., a phylogenetic hypothesis was generated by combining sequence data of 24 taxa from one mitochondrial (16S rRNA) and four nuclear gene fragments (long-wavelength rhodopsin copy 1 (opsin), elongation factor-1α copy F2, arginine kinase, and 28S rRNA). Fifteen characteristics of the nest architec- ture were coded and tested for phylogenetic association. Several characters have significant phylogenetic signal, including type of nesting substrate, nest construction material, and hemipterophily, the tending of hemipteroid insects in exchange for sugar excretions. Phylogenetic independent habits encountered in Trig- ona s.s. include coprophily and necrophagy. molecular phylogeny / neotropical / stingless bee / behavior / nest 1. INTRODUCTION largest and most diverse group of social bees worldwide, the external nest entrance and in- Among the social bees there exists a bewil- ternal nest features often exhibit details that dering array of nest architecture and nesting allow for species-specific recognition (e.g., behavior, from the simple underground tun- Michener, 1959; Sakagami, 1982; Camargo nels of halictid bees to the elaborate structures and Pedro, 2003b; Franck et al., 2004).
    [Show full text]