Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on October 1, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Commentary Parsing storage from retrieval in experimentally induced Louis D. Matzel1,3 and Ralph R. Miller2 1Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey 08854, USA; 2Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Binghamton, Binghamton, New York 13902, USA

By 1970, a new framework had begun to emerge with which to evoke fear owing to its association with foot shock) is dependent understand both storage and memory loss. During this on plasticity within the dorsal , and that this plas- period, a number of independent laboratories had demonstrated ticity requires the participation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) that both experimentally induced and naturally occurring in- receptors. However, it has also been established that this re- stances of ‘‘memory failure’’ could be reversed by exposing quirement for NMDA receptors subsides after the first training amnestic subjects to isolated aspects of the training conditions trial, such that NMDA receptor antagonists (such as 2-amino-5- upon which the forgotten memory had been based (Campbell and phosphonopentanoic acid; AP5) block on the first con- Jaynes 1966; Lewis et al. 1968; Miller and Springer 1972). While ditioning event, but not on subsequent exposures to the context these data suggested a nominally novel interpretation of memory paired with shock (Sanders and Fanselow 2003). Hardt et al. ‘‘loss’’ (better described as a ‘‘lapse’’ in these cases), they were, in exploit this observation for the present purpose. They argue that some respect, a reassertion of what had long been known: if inducing experimental amnesia (through the post-training Seemingly forgotten are often recovered. Colloquially, administration of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin; what is forgotten is not necessarily gone. To experience this effect ANI) impairs memory storage after the first training event, then outside of the laboratory, one need only look up a long ‘‘forgotten’’ no residual memory will persist into the second training event, phone number. The phone number will be instantly recognizable and consequently an NMDA receptor antagonist (e.g., AP5) should and easily relearned, indicative of the intact storage of a memory effectively block learning on the second event, as though prior that seems, superficially, to have been ‘‘lost.’’ learning had never occurred. In contrast, if ANI disrupts retrieval Empirical observations from the 1960s led many to conclude of the memory effectively stored after the first training event, then that experimentally induced amnesia, as well as naturally occur- the memory of that event will remain intact into the second ring memory loss, might reflect instances of retrieval rather than training event, and therefore AP5 should be ineffective in blocking storage failure (Lewis 1969; Spear 1978). Despite the compelling learning during the second event; that is, learning in the presence nature of the experimental results and the fundamental signifi- of AP5 during the second training would suggest that a memory of cance of the theory that they encouraged, available data have left the first training event had survived the amnesic effects of ANI. unresolved this long-standing controversy, and this conundrum When these predictions were tested, Hardt et al. find that AP5 may have limited the acceptance of ‘‘retrieval theory’’ by the larger impairs learning of the second training event despite ANI having field of memory researchers. In their recent paper, Hardt et al. been infused into the dorsal hippocampus after the first training (2009) correctly identify this enduring stalemate, arguing that event. This led them to conclude that ANI-induced experimental prevailing methodologies have left unanswered the most basic of amnesia impairs memory via its impact on the storage, not questions, i.e., does experimental amnesia reflect a memory loss or retrieval, of the previously coded memory. a retrieval failure? As Hardt et al. note, if an instance of memory Despite the virtues of their approach, the results of Hardt failure seems immune to recovery, it cannot be said with certainty et al. are not immune from alternative interpretations, and in fact, whether the memory is actually absent or simply that the retrieval introduce a new of complications akin to those that have blockade is too strong to be overcome with the applied technique plagued prior efforts to parse storage failures from retrieval failures (e.g., a specific ‘‘retrieval cue’’). On the other hand, the recovery of in experimental amnesia. First is the issue raised by the assump- a seemingly lost memory could simply reflect an intact, but tion that contextual fear memories reside exclusively in the dorsal subthreshold (or degraded), memory being nudged across some hippocampus (dHPC). While it is clear that the dHPC plays previously unreached threshold for activation. Hardt et al. argue a critical role in the mediation of contextually mediated learning, that a true test of the opposing views would require a test in which available data suggest that the HPC is not the sole storage site of retrieval and storage interpretations of memory failure make nominally context-based memories (Good and Honey 1991; Good opposite predictions regarding a tangible result (i.e., a result that et al. 2007). Of course it is also possible that learned fear responses is not dependent on the observation of a null effect). It is that may depend intimately on the integrity of the HPC, while actual approach that Hardt et al. (2009) take in their recent paper in storage (or some component of the stored memory) may reside in Learning & Memory. other brain structures (Berger et al. 1976, 1980; Talk et al. 2002). It Hart et al.’s approach to this problem is a welcome point of is indisputable that exactly these types of considerations have departure from prior efforts, and brings forth a strategy and complicated any definitive statement regarding the function of techniques that were not available when this vein of research the HPC in memory storage. If one accepts even the remote was originally struck. It has been previously observed that con- possibility that contextual fear memories reside (even to a func- textual fear conditioning (in which a context, or place, comes to tionally trivial degree) in some brain areas in addition to the dHPC, then the dHPC-limited ANI injections administered by Hardt et al. will have left those residual memory traces intact. Were 3Corresponding author. E-mail [email protected]; fax (732) 445-2263. such the case, then those residual memories would have been Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.1478709. available to the animal on Hardt et al.’s second training event, and

16:670–671 Ó 2009 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 670 Learning & Memory ISSN 1072-0502/09; www.learnmem.org Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on October 1, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Storage and retrieval

consequently, should have been immune to the AP5 treatment. administration of ANI would be avoided, in the present case, Why then were Hardt et al.’s animals vulnerable to the localized brain-wide inhibition of protein synthesis (and its impact on ill- effects of AP5 during the second training event (i.e., after the in- defined secondary storage sites) might be advantageous. Such duction of ANI-induced amnesia for the first training event)? One a strategy does not, however, mitigate the incomplete action of can only speculate, but given the complex, if not conflicting, as- ANI as an inhibitor of new protein synthesis. To this end, sumptions that must be met to accept Hardt et al.’s interpretation electroconvulsive shock (ECS) has often been employed for the of these results, it is worth considering a more parsimonious specific purpose of promoting widespread and ostensibly ‘‘com- alternative to their account. It is reasonable to ask an as yet plete’’ amnesia (Barnes et al. 1994). Because it does not differen- unanswered question: Does the vulnerability of the second tiate brain structures and induces a complex disruption of synaptic training event to AP5 depend on the memory of the first training transmission regardless of transmitter system or receptor subtype, trial having been lost, or on the failure of the memory of the first ECS would normally be shunned as a tool for modern neurosci- training event to be retrieved? If the vulnerability of the second entific inquires. Exactly because of these properties, ECS as an training event to AP5 depends on the retrievability of the original amnestic agent might be preferable to the localized and site- memory, then Hardt et al.’s observations would be entirely limited approach favored by Hardt et al. explicable in terms of ANI having induced a retrieval failure. In summary, Hardt et al. have made a serious effort to address Hardt et al.’s preferred interpretation of these effects presupposes an issue that has heretofore proven intractable, and further work that the basis for ANI-induced amnesia is known, when in fact in this vein will surely provide a more complete and better these very experiments were intended to determine the basis of of the nature of experimental amnesia. In the this effect. These complications are precisely analogous to the one end, it is entirely possible that amnestic treatments might under asserted by Hardt et al. to have plagued previous efforts to some (or even many) conditions disrupt the storage of recently distinguish storage from retrieval interpretations of experimen- acquired memories. Under other circumstances, it is possible that tally induced amnesia. seemingly lost memories may reflect a failure of the retrieval Related to the above issue is the effectiveness of ANI itself. process. Perhaps, more important is the very nature of this debate. When employed as an amnestic agent, the impact of ANI on A question that might better be asked is under what circumstances protein synthesis inhibition is often reported as an indicator of the do each of these interacting influences prevail, and what might be effectiveness of the drug. Such an analysis is absent from the the functional consequences for memory processing? report by Hardt et al. (It is noted that the procedures used by these authors are well established, and their previously demonstrated expertise in these areas instill complete confidence in the effec- References tiveness of their ANI treatment.) However, a brief review of the Barnes CA, Jung MW, McNaughton BL, Korol DL, Andreasson K, Worley PF. previous literature suggests that, independent of the route of 1994. LTP saturation and spatial learning disruption: Effects of task J Neurosci administration, concentrations of ANI comparable to those em- variables and saturation levels. 14: 5793–5806. Berger TW, Alger B, Thompson RF. 1976. Neuronal substrate of classical ployed by Hardt et al. produce less than complete inhibition of conditioning in the hippocampus. Science 192: 483–485. new protein synthesis (typically ranging from 70% to 90%) Berger TW, Laham RI, Thompson RF. 1980. Hippocampal unit-behavior (Milekic et al. 2006; Languille et al. 2008). This incomplete in- correlations during . Brain Res 193: 229–248. Psychol Rev hibition of protein synthesis is unavoidable, as higher concentra- Campbell BA, Jaynes J. 1966. Reinstatement. 73: 478–480. Good M, Honey RC. 1991. Conditioning and contextual retrieval in tions of the drug often promote adverse/nonspecific consequences. hippocampal rats. Behav Neurosci 105: 499–509. We are then left to ask how such an amnestic agent could ever be Good MA, Barnes P, Staal V, McGregor A, Honey RC. 2007. Context- but not complete, and if not, what the consequences might be for in- familiarity-dependent forms of object recognition are impaired Behav Neurosci terpretation of the recovery from experimentally induced amnesia. following excitotoxic hippocampal lesions in rats. 121: 218–223. Hardt et al. have utilized a creative and novel approach in an Hardt O, Wang SH, Nader K. 2009. Storage or retrieval deficit: The yin and effort to resolve an enduring controversy, i.e., to separate the yang of amnesia. Learn Mem 16: 224–230. effects of experimentally induced amnesia on retrieval processes Languille S, Gruest N, Richer P, Hars B. 2008. The temporal dynamics of from its impact on memory storage. Behavioral neuroscience has consolidation and reconsolidation decrease during postnatal development. Learn Mem 15: 434–442. undergone a rapid evolution during the past two decades. Since Lewis DJ. 1969. Sources of experimental amnesia. Psychol Rev 76: this line of inquiry initially began in the 1960s, it is now possible 461–472. to specify (with some precision) neuroanatomical storage sites for Lewis DJ, Misanin JR, Miller RR. 1968. Recovery of memory following Nature process-limited memories. Moreover, we now have technical amnesia. 220: 704–705. Milekic MH, Brown SD, Castellini C, Alberini CM. 2006. Persistent capabilities that permit neuroanatomically specific administration disruption of an established morphine conditioned place preference. (via microinjection) of both amnestic agents and modulators of J Neurosci 26: 3010–3020. neuronal plasticity. While these advances have promoted new Miller RR, Springer AD. 1972. Induced recovery of memory in rats following Physiol Behav knowledge and advances in our discipline, they also introduce electroconvulsive shock. 8: 645–651. Sanders MJ, Fanselow MS. 2003. Pre-training prevents context fear new complications, like those described above. Thus, for the conditioning deficits produced by hippocampal NMDA receptor present purposes, old strategies may be more appropriate. For blockade. Neurobiol Learn Mem 80: 123–129. instance, it was noted above that localized administration of the Spear NE. 1978. The processing of memories: and retention. Erlbaum, amnestic agent ANI is confounded by potentially distributed Hillsdale, NJ. Talk AC, Gandhi CC, Matzel LD. 2002. Hippocampal function during memory storage and the incomplete effectiveness of ANI as an behaviorally silent associative learning: Dissociation of memory storage inhibitor of protein synthesis. While for most purposes, systemic and expression. Hippocampus 12: 648–656.

www.learnmem.org 671 Learning & Memory Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on October 1, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Parsing storage from retrieval in experimentally induced amnesia

Louis D. Matzel and Ralph R. Miller

Learn. Mem. 2009, 16: Access the most recent version at doi:10.1101/lm.1478709

Related Content New approaches to amnesia Karim Nader Learn. Mem. November , 2009 16: 672-675

References This article cites 14 articles, 5 of which can be accessed free at: http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/16/11/670.full.html#ref-list-1

Articles cited in: http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/16/11/670.full.html#related-urls

License

Email Alerting Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the Service top right corner of the article or click here.

Copyright © 2009 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press