Affective Computing, Emotional Development, and Autism

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Affective Computing, Emotional Development, and Autism OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jul 17 2014, NEWGEN CHAPTER Afective Computing, Emotional 39 Development, and Autism and Abstract Key Words: Introduction the adult he or she will become. Interventions based Afective Computing and Child in afective computing that help children develop Development optimally have the potential to beneft society in Children’s development is a fertile application the long term. Troughout, whenever appropriate, of afective computing. Te nonverbal emotional we discuss how the reviewed studies of detection communication of children and infants may be less and modeling of emotions have contributed to our impacted by social display rules than the commu- understanding of emotional development in chil- nication of older individuals, thus ofering a rich dren with ASD. environment for the automated detection and mod- eling of emotion. Substantively, early dyadic inter- Afective Computing and the Development action between infants and parents ofers a model of Autism Spectrum Disorders for understanding the underpinnings of nonverbal Disordered development can provide insights communication throughout the lifespan. Tese into typical development. Tis chapter discusses interactions, for example, may lay the basis for the the detection and modeling of emotion—and the development of turn-taking and mutual smiling application of interventions grounded in afective that are fundamental to later nonverbal communi- computing—in children with autism spectrum dis- cation (Messinger, Ruvolo, Ekas, & Fogel, 2010). orders (ASDs) and their high-risk siblings. Autism At the same time, the child’s development afects spectrum disorders are pervasive disorders of social 516 book.indb 516 7/18/2014 1:21:49 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jul 17 2014, NEWGEN communication and impact a broad range of non- opportunities for advancement in afective comput- verbal (as well as verbal) interactive skills (American ing eforts with children. Psychiatric Association, 2000). Because the symp- toms of these developmental disorders emerge Automated Measurement of Emotional before 3 years of age, ASDs provide a window Behavior into early disturbances of nonverbal social inter- Automated Facial Measurement action. In addition, the younger siblings of chil- Te face is central to the communication of emo- dren with an ASD—high-risk siblings—can ofer tion from infancy through old age. However, man- a prospective view of the development of ASDs ual measurement of facial expression is laborious and related symptoms. Approximately one-ffth and resource-intensive (Cohn & Kanade, 2007). of these ASD siblings will develop an ASD and As a consequence, much more is known about the another ffth will exhibit ASD-related symptoms perception of facial expressions than of the produc- by 3 years of age that are below the threshold for a tion of facial expressions. Software-based automated clinical diagnosis (Boelte & Poustka, 2003; Bolton, measurement ofers the possibility of efcient, Pickles, Murphy, & Rutter, 1998; Constantino objective portraits of facial expression and emotion et al., 2006; Messinger et al., 2013; Murphy et al., communication. Here, we describe a methodologi- 2000; Ozonof et al., 2011; Szatmari et al., 2000; cal framework for the automated measurement of Wassink, Brzustowicz, Bartlett, & Szatmari., facial expression in infants and their parents during 2004). Automated measurement and model- early interaction. ing often focuses on high-risk siblings to provide A growing body of research on infant–parent objective data on the development of ASD-related interaction uses automated measurement based on symptoms. the facial action coding system (FACS) (Ekman & Friesen, 1992; Ekman, Friesen, & Hager, 2002) Chapter Overview and its application to infants (BabyFACS) (Oster, In a developmental context, afective comput- 2006). FACS is a comprehensive manual system for ing involves the use of computer software to detect recording anatomically based appearance changes behavioral signs of emotions and model emotional in the form of facial action units (AUs; Lucey, functioning and communication and the construc- Ashraf, & Cohn, 2007). To better understand the tion of software and hardware agents that interact dynamics of expression and emotional communica- with children. Te chapter begins with a review of tion, the strength of key AUs is measured using an automated measurement of facial action and the intensity metric that specifes whether a facial action application of those measures to better understand its present and, if present, its strength from mini- early emotion expression. Emotional communi- mal to maximal using FACS criteria (Mahoor et al., cation is complex, and the chapter then reviews 2008). Objective measurement of facial expression time-series and machine-learning approaches to intensity allows for time-series modeling of interac- modeling emotional communication in early inter- tive infuence. action, which includes comparisons between typi- A commonly used automated measurement cally developing children and children with ASDs. pipeline combines active appearance and shape Next, we review automated approaches to emotion models (AASMs) and support vector machines detection—and to the identifcation of ASDs— (SVMs) (Messinger et al., 2012). Active appear- from children’s vocalizations, and we discuss eforts ance and shape models are used to detect and track to model the vocal signal using graph-based and facial movement (see Figure 39.1). Te shape com- time-series approaches. Te fnal measurement ponent of the AASM unites the two-dimensional section reviews new approaches to the collection representations of the movement of 66 vertices of electrophysiological data (electrodermal activa- (Baker, Matthews, & Schneider, 2004; Cohn & tion [EDA]), focusing on eforts in children with Kanade, 2007). Mouth opening can be measured ASD. Finally, we review translational applications as the vertical distance between the upper and of afective computing in two areas that have shown lower lips in the shape component of the AASM. promise in helping children with ASD develop Te appearance component of the AASM con- skills in the areas of emotional development and tains the grayscale values for each pixel contained social communication: embodied conversational in the modeled face. Appearance is the grayscale agents (ECAs) and robotics. Te chapter ends texture within the region defned by the mesh. In with a critical discussion of accomplishments and the research reported here, nonlinear manifold Messinger, Duvivier, Warren, Mahoor, Baker, Warlaumont, Ruvolo 517 book.indb 517 7/18/2014 1:21:49 PM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jul 17 2014, NEWGEN learning (Belkin & Niyogi, 2003) was used to Emotion Measurement via reduce the dimensionality of the appearance and Continuous Ratings shape data to produce a set of variables that are Here, we describe a method for collecting con- used to train SVMs. Support vector machines tinuous ratings of emotion constructs in time are machine learning classifers that were used to that can be modeled in their own right and used determine whether the AU in question was pres- to validate automated measurements of emo- ent and, if present, its intensity level. To make tional behavior. In the automated facial expres- this assignment, a one-against-one classifcation sion measurement, expert manual measurement strategy was used (each intensity level was pitted of facial actions’ levels of cross-system (automated against each of the others) (Chang & Lin, 2001; vs. manual) reliability are typically comparable to Mahoor et al., 2008). standard interobserver (manual vs. manual) reliabil- ity. However, intersystem agreement speaks to the validity of the automated measurements but not to the emotional meaning of the underlying behaviors. One approach to validating automated measure- ments of the face as indices of emotion intensity are Input video with tracking continuous ratings made by third-party observers (http:// measurement.psy.miami.edu/). Continuous emotion measurement is similar to the afect rating dial in which participants in an emotional experience can provide a continuous report on their own afective state (Gottman & Levenson, 1985; Levenson & Gottman, 1983; Ruef & Levenson, 2007). In the research described here, however, continuous ratings were made by Afne observes who moved a joystick to indicate the warp afective valence they perceived in an interacting infant or parent. Te ratings of multiple indepen- dent observers were united into a mean index of perceived emotional valence (Waldinger, Schulz, Hauser, Allen, & Crowell, 2004). Continuous nonexpert ratings have strong face validity because they refect a precise, easily interpretable descrip- tion of a construct such as positive (“joy, happi- ness, and pleasure”) or negative emotion (“anger, Feature sadness, and distress”). extraction (SIFT) Applying Automated and Other Measurement to Early Emotion Expression THE CASE OF SMILING Automated measurement of the intensity of smiling has yielded insights into early positive emotion. Although infant smiles occur frequently in social interactions and appear to index positive AU 12 (0.00) emotion, adult smiles occur in a range of contexts, AU not all of which are associated with positive emo- detection tion. Tis has led some investigators to propose that a particular type of smiling, Duchenne smiling, is uniquely associated with the expression of positive Fig. 39.1 Facial measurement.
Recommended publications
  • I'm Sad You're Sad: Emotional Contagion In
    I’m Sad You’re Sad: Emotional Contagion in CMC Jeffrey T. Hancock,1,2 Kailyn Gee,2 Kevin Ciaccio2 & Jennifer Mae-Hwah Lin1 1Information Science 2Department of Communication Cornell University jth34, kg95, kac83, jml232 @cornell.edu ABSTRACT necessarily undermined in CMC. For example, in one An enduring assumption about computer-mediated study, participants were asked to act nicely or meanly to a communication is that it undermines emotional communication partner during a get to know you type task understanding. The present study examined emotional that took place either FtF or in CMC [11]. The results communication in CMC by inducing negative affect in one revealed that the partners could discern the likable and condition and neutral affect in another. The results revealed dislikable participants as accurately in the CMC condition that 1) participants experiencing negative affect produced as in the FtF condition. In a second study that more directly fewer words, used more sad terms, and exchanged examined emotional expression, participants acted either messages at a slower rate, 2) their partners were able to happy or sad while interacting with a partner through detect their partners emotional state, and 3) emotional Instant Messenger [7]. After the interaction, partners were contagion took place, in which partners interacting with easily able to determine whether their partner seemed happy participants in the negative affect condition had or sad. The study also provided some indications of how significantly less positive affect than partners in the control emotion is communicated through text: participants acting condition. These data support a relational view of CMC. sad used fewer words, agreed less with their partner, used Author Keywords less punctuation, and responded less quickly than Computer-mediated communication, Emotion, Affect participants acting happy (see also work by Gill et al, [4]).
    [Show full text]
  • QUESTION 20 Despair We Next Have to Consider the Vices Opposed to Hope
    QUESTION 20 Despair We next have to consider the vices opposed to hope: first, despair (desperatio) (question 20) and, second, presumption (praesumptio) (question 21). On the first topic there are four questions: (1) Is despair a sin? (2) Can despair exist without unbelief? (3) Is despair the greatest of sins? (4) Does despair arise from listlessness (acedia)? Article 1 Is despair a sin? It seems that despair is not a sin (desperatio not sit peccatum): Objection 1: As is clear from Augustine in De Libero Arbitrio, every sin involves turning toward some changeable good, combined with turning away from the unchangeable good. But despair does not involve turning toward any changeable good. Therefore, it is not a sin. Objection 2: What arises from a good root does not seem to be a sin, since, as Matthew 7:18 says, “A good tree cannot bring produce bad fruits.” But despair seems to proceed from a good root, viz., fear of God, or horror at the magnitude of one’s sins. Therefore, despair is not a sin. Objection 3: If despair were a sin, then, in the case of the damned, their despairing would be a sin. But this is not imputed to them as a sin (non imputatur eis ad culpam); instead, it is imputed to their being damned. Therefore, it is not imputed as a sin to those who are in this life, either (neque viatoribus imputatur ad culpam). And so despair is not a sin. But contrary to this: That through which men are induced to sin seems to be not only a sin, but a principle of sins.
    [Show full text]
  • University Basic Needs Insecurity: a National #Realcollege Survey Report
    APRIL 2019 College and University Basic Needs Insecurity: A National #RealCollege Survey Report AUTHORS: Sara Goldrick-Rab, Christine Baker-Smith, Vanessa Coca, Elizabeth Looker and Tiffani Williams Executive Summary NEARLY 86,000 STUDENTS PARTICIPATED. THE RESULTS The #RealCollege survey is the nation’s largest annual INDICATE: assessment of basic needs security among college students. The survey, created by the Hope Center • 45% of respondents were food for College, Community, and Justice (Hope Center), insecure in the prior 30 days specifically evaluates access to affordable food and housing. This report describes the results of the • 56% of respondents were #RealCollege survey administered in the fall of 2018 at housing insecure in the previous year 123 two- and four-year institutions across the United States. • 17% of respondents were homeless in the previous year Rates of basic needs insecurity are higher for students attending two-year colleges compared to those attending four-year colleges. Rates of basic needs insecurity are higher for marginalized students, including African Americans, students identifying as LGBTQ, and students who are independent from The Hope Center thanks the their parents or guardians for financial aid purposes. Lumina Foundation, the Jewish Students who have served in the military, former foster Foundation for Education of youth, and students who were formerly convicted of a crime are all at greater risk of basic needs insecurity. Women, the City University Working during college is not associated with a lower of New York, the Chicago risk of basic needs insecurity, and neither is receiving City Colleges, the Institute for the federal Pell Grant; the latter is in fact associated with higher rates of basic needs insecurity.
    [Show full text]
  • Ekman, Emotional Expression, and the Art of Empirical Epiphany
    JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY Journal of Research in Personality 38 (2004) 37–44 www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp Ekman, emotional expression, and the art of empirical epiphany Dacher Keltner* Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, 3319 Tolman, 94720 Berkeley, CA, USA Introduction In the mid and late 1960s, Paul Ekman offered a variety of bold assertions, some seemingly more radical today than others (Ekman, 1984, 1992, 1993). Emotions are expressed in a limited number of particular facial expressions. These expressions are universal and evolved. Facial expressions of emotion are remarkably brief, typically lasting 1 to 5 s. And germane to the interests of the present article, these brief facial expressions of emotion reveal a great deal about peopleÕs lives. In the present article I will present evidence that supports this last notion ad- vanced by Ekman, that brief expressions of emotion reveal important things about the individualÕs life course. To do so I first theorize about how individual differences in emotion shape the life context. With this reasoning as backdrop, I then review four kinds of evidence that indicate that facial expression is revealing of the life that the individual has led and is likely to continue leading. Individual differences in emotion and the shaping of the life context People, as a function of their personality or psychological disorder, create the sit- uations in which they act (e.g., Buss, 1987). Individuals selectively attend to certain features of complex situations, thus endowing contexts with idiosyncratic meaning. Individuals evoke responses in others, thus shaping the shared, social meaning of the situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Literature Review: Is the Emotional Expression of Contempt
    Literature Review: Is the Emotional Expression of Contempt Recognized Universally or Culturally? Julianna Phoukhao University of California, Merced Keywords: Contempt, Facial Expression, Forced Choice, Freely Labeling, Universal, Cultural, Emotion. 1 Abstract The universal facial expression of contempt is often described as one lip corner raised and tightened. This literature reviews whether or not this expression is recognized universally. After examining theories and methods, low agreement of this expression recognized as contempt was found across cultures. Evidence so far is not sufficient enough to support the unilateral lip corner as an universal expression for contempt. The expression and recognition of contempt is highly dependent on culture and context. 2 Literature Review Contempt is an emotion described as annoyance (Alvarado, 1996), ignoring or belittling a person (Fischer & Roseman, 2007), and involving another person’s negative actions and feelings of superiority (Wagner, 2000). This emotion is usually initiated when one perceives the inability to change or correct another person’s behavior. The social function of contempt is to end a relationship through rejection and exclusion (Fischer & Roseman, 2007). The concept of emotions serving functions dates back to Darwin’s days (1876) when he proposed the physiological component and facial expressions of basic emotions as innate because they serve towards a goal in one’s environment. He used his observations of evolution as evidence of the universality of emotions. Ekman (1971) challenged Darwin’s theory by conducting his own research in a remote village of Papua New Guinea. Ekman discovered emotions as innate and recognizable through facial expression. His research has also suggested the universality of a contempt expression (Ekman, 1988).
    [Show full text]
  • Joy, Distress, Hope, and Fear in Reinforcement Learning (Extended Abstract)
    Joy, Distress, Hope, and Fear in Reinforcement Learning (Extended Abstract) Elmer Jacobs Joost Broekens Catholijn Jonker Interactive Intelligence, Interactive Intelligence, Interactive Intelligence, TU Delft TU Delft TU Delft Delft, The Netherlands Delft, The Netherlands Delft, The Netherlands [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT orbitofrontal cortex, reward representation, and (subjective) In this paper we present a mapping between joy, distress, affective value (see [14]). hope and fear, and Reinforcement Learning primitives. Joy While most research on computational modeling of emo- / distress is a signal that is derived from the RL update sig- tion is based on cognitive appraisal theory [9], our work is nal, while hope/fear is derived from the utility of the current different in that we aim to show a direct mapping between state. Agent-based simulation experiments replicate psycho- RL primitives and emotions, and assess the validity by repli- logical and behavioral dynamics of emotion including: joy cating psychological findings on emotion dynamics, the lat- and distress reactions that develop prior to hope and fear; ter being an essential difference with [5]. We believe that fear extinction; habituation of joy; and, task randomness before affectively labelling a particular RL-based signal, it that increases the intensity of joy and distress. This work is essential to investigate if that signal behaves according to distinguishes itself by assessing the dynamics of emotion in what is known in psychology and behavioral science. The an adaptive agent framework - coupling it to the literature extent to which a signal replicates emotion-related dynamics on habituation, development, and extinction.
    [Show full text]
  • A Guide for Nurturing Hope, Resilience and Happiness Pasiàka Style
    I AM A guide for nurturing hope, resilience and happiness Pasi\ ka style. By Philip Siataga Acknowledgements Fakatu’amelie innovation fund Before anything else is said, sincere thanks are due to Manase Lua and Monique Faleafa and the awesome Le Va team, for inspiration, encouragement and support. Without which the I AM approach would not have developed to this point. I would like to thank my friends and colleagues Wiremu Gray Ngai Tahu, Ngati Porou (Mana Facilitation Ltd) and Paddy Pawson (Adventure Therapy) for invaluable support, insightful commentary and precious time spent sharing your hearts and minds. Special acknowledgement is due to Maui Paraki (Humble Enterprises) and to Andrew Smith (About Life ltd) whose lives are inspirational. To Alan and Alice Va’a, who continue to also serve so many in the true spirit of grace and humility – much respect. To my family, with mountains of love and gratitude, I can not thank you enough. And to, Sophia and Talia, wonderful daughters, thank you for making my journey one of enduring happiness. I am blessed with the joy of a fathers pride. Many thanks are due to Fuimaono Karl Pulotu-Endemann and Dr Francis Agnew, who inspire innovation and integrity from those of us privileged to have journeyed with them. I must also acknowledge the Occasional paper on negotiated space and thank Karlo Mila Schaaf and Maui Hudson for their insightful theorising which has boosted my own desire to plant this work in the va. To the fantastic crew at Waipuna Trust, my warmest thanks for your support and your many kindnesses along the way.
    [Show full text]
  • Hope and Anger As Mediators Between Collective Action Frames and Participation in Collective Mobilization: the Case of 15-M
    Journal of Social and Political Psychology jspp.psychopen.eu | 2195-3325 Original Research Reports Hope and Anger as Mediators Between Collective Action Frames and Participation in Collective Mobilization: The Case of 15-M Anna Wlodarczyk* ab, Nekane Basabe c, Darío Páez b, Larraitz Zumeta b [a] Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Católica del Norte, Antofagasta, Chile. [b] Department of Social Psychology and Methodology of Behaviour Sciences, University of the Basque Country, San Sebastián, Spain. [c] Department of Social Psychology and Methodology of Behaviour Sciences, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain. Abstract The study set out to integrate collective action models and emphasize the role of emotions. Whereas the importance of anger is indisputable, relatively little attention has been paid to the role of positive emotions, such as hope, in collective action research. Hence, the aim of the study was to explore the role of hope and anger as drivers of participation and involvement in collective mobilizations. A cross-sectional field study (N = 638) conducted right after the emergence of the 15-M socio-political protest movement in Spain assessed the emotions and beliefs of both demonstrators and those who took no part in the active mobilization. We hypothesized that anger and hope would sequentially mediate the relationship between collective action frames and participation in collective action. Furthermore, to test this premise, we ran two alternative sequential mediation models based on the social identity model of collective action (SIMCA) and the encapsulated model of social identity in collective action (EMSICA), but with emotions as mediators between collective action frames and intensity of participation.
    [Show full text]
  • Multi-Emotion Classification for Song Lyrics
    Multi-Emotion Classification for Song Lyrics Darren Edmonds João Sedoc Donald Bren School of ICS Stern School of Business University of California, Irvine New York University [email protected] [email protected] Abstract and Shamma, 2010; Bobicev and Sokolova, 2017; Takala et al., 2014). Building such a model is espe- Song lyrics convey a multitude of emotions to the listener and powerfully portray the emo- cially challenging in practice as there often exists tional state of the writer or singer. This considerable disagreement regarding the percep- paper examines a variety of modeling ap- tion and interpretation of the emotions of a song or proaches to the multi-emotion classification ambiguity within the song itself (Kim et al., 2010). problem for songs. We introduce the Edmonds There exist a variety of high-quality text datasets Dance dataset, a novel emotion-annotated for emotion classification, from social media lyrics dataset from the reader’s perspective, datasets such as CBET (Shahraki, 2015) and TEC and annotate the dataset of Mihalcea and Strapparava(2012) at the song level. We (Mohammad, 2012) to large dialog corpora such find that models trained on relatively small as the DailyDialog dataset (Li et al., 2017). How- song datasets achieve marginally better perfor- ever, there remains a lack of comparable emotion- mance than BERT (Devlin et al., 2019) fine- annotated song lyric datasets, and existing lyrical tuned on large social media or dialog datasets. datasets are often annotated for valence-arousal af- fect rather than distinct emotions (Çano and Mori- 1 Introduction sio, 2017). Consequently, we introduce the Ed- Text-based sentiment analysis has become increas- monds Dance Dataset1, a novel lyrical dataset that ingly popular in recent years, in part due to its was crowdsourced through Amazon Mechanical numerous applications in fields such as marketing, Turk.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Natural History of Emotions: Darwin's Legacy
    revista de historia de la psicología © 2009: Publicacions de la Universitat de València 2009,On the vol. natural 30, núm. history 2-3 (junio-septiembre) of emotions 161-168 Valencia (España). ISSN: 0211-0040161 On the natural history of emotions: Darwin’s legacy Paulo Jesus* Universidade Lusófono do Porto Abstract One of the most striking applications of Darwinian principles resides in the evolutionary account of expression of emotions. The main purpose of this paper is to ask some fundamental questions concerning this realm of Darwinian investigations and its implicit or explicit legacy in contem- porary psychology of emotions, which appears inhabited by the essential tension between the primacy of embodiment (from James to Damasio) and that of cognitive mediation (from Peirce to Lazarus). One should ask again: what is an emotion? Is it reducible to a bodily expression or is there any qualitative difference between emotion and expression? Why do emotions belong to the phenomena of life? How do the grammar of life and the grammar of culture interact in emotional expressions? What is the «truth» of Darwin’s principles on expressions and what degree of coherence can be detected when one attempts at integrating the theory of emotions in the larger picture of the evolution of life forms and cultural meaningful phenomena? Darwin is systematically attracted towards normative invariants, that is, nomological relation- ships and functions which organise a dynamic morphogenetic process. Thus, Darwin’s theory of expression of emotions identifies a set of three universal principles that complement each other. These comprise not only the notion of adaptive behaviour, association of learned habits and evolution of instinct, but also that of body structure (specially the web of muscles that define and constrain the landscape of the face), inheritance of movements and independent action of the nervous system.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Stop Lusting? That Lust Had Become a Bigger Problem Than the Problems We Were Trying to Avoid
    Why Stop Lusting? that lust had become a bigger problem than the problems we were trying to avoid. Many of us came to Sexaholics Anonymous (SA) driven to total despair by our The medicine became our poison. Our “solution” became the problem. We were destructive sexual thoughts and behaviors. Within the meeting rooms of SA we out of control. discovered, to our surprise, that lust was the driving force behind our acting out. Sexual Lusting, for us, is like riding a roller coaster. Once started, it is nearly im- Sexaholics lust is an inordinate thought or feeling that drives us to use ourselves, others, or things possible to stop. Therefore, lust must be for self-centered destructive purposes. The stopped where it begins, with the first Anonymous spiritual sickness of lust wants sexual drink. Getting out from under the influ- ence of lust, therefore, would require us to stimulation at that moment instead of what a Higher Power or God of our understanding avoid getting on board in the first place. That meant forsaking the thrill and the is offering us. Later we come to see that lust wants anything other than what is offered us risk-taking. But how could we turn our backs on something that we had allowed each moment. At first it was hard to believe. As we began to accept this fact, we won- to dominate our lives for so many years? How could we succeed now where we had Why dered how we could live without lust. It be- came clear that we had to give it up, yet we failed a thousand times before? doubted that life without lust was possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Hope, Courage, and Resilience in the Lives of Transgender Women of Color
    The Qualitative Report Volume 24 Number 8 Article 11 8-24-2019 Hope, Courage, and Resilience in the Lives of Transgender Women of Color Nadine Ruff Southern Connecticut State University, [email protected] Amy B. Smoyer Southern Connecticut State University, [email protected] Jean Breny Southern Connecticut State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr Part of the Public Health Education and Promotion Commons, Quantitative, Qualitative, Comparative, and Historical Methodologies Commons, Social Statistics Commons, and the Social Work Commons Recommended APA Citation Ruff, N., Smoyer, A. B., & Breny, J. (2019). Hope, Courage, and Resilience in the Lives of Transgender Women of Color. The Qualitative Report, 24(8), 1990-2008. https://doi.org/10.46743/2160-3715/ 2019.3729 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the The Qualitative Report at NSUWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in The Qualitative Report by an authorized administrator of NSUWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Hope, Courage, and Resilience in the Lives of Transgender Women of Color Abstract There is a lack of qualitative and strengths-based knowledge about the lived experience of transgender women of color in the US. To address this research gap, a Photovoice project was undertaken with five transgender women living in a small urban area. Thematic analysis of the participants’ discussion of their photographs identified three major themes: hope, courage, and resilience. Analysis suggests a framework for understanding these women’s lived experiences and the psychosocial tools that they use to negotiate their daily lives and persevere in the face of interpersonal and structural oppression.
    [Show full text]