The Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition Edited by Allison B

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition Edited by Allison B Cambridge University Press 978-1-108-42674-9 — The Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition Edited by Allison B. Kaufman, Josep Call, James C. Kaufman Index More Information Index aboutness. See intentionality angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) acoustic communication. See auditory/ numerosity judgments in, 561, 564 acoustic communication quantity discrimination in, 582–584 actor-receiver interactions. See cooperative animacy perception behaviours detection of, 295–310 adaptive responses dynamic cues for, 300–304 in animal communication, 6 face and eye-like patterns in, 296–300 in knowledge/ignorance situations, 238–239 fur as cue for, 299 to belief situations, 239–240 neural bases of detection, 304–310 to mental states, 232, 241–246 static cues, 295–300 to witnessed and unwittnessed events, 239 static cues for, 296–300 affiliative behaviours/interactions animal communication copying behaviour and, 539 definition, 38 dog-human tactile contact, 92 evolutionary causes in, 6–12 for captive animal welfare, 462 innovation in research, 640 for reconciliation, 259 linguistic model of, 100 in elephants, 366, 371 animal culture in Norway rats, 345 birdsong as example of, 516–517 updating relationship information by, 258 factors in, 544–547 affordance learning, 450, 537 in chimpanzees, 543–547 African forest elephant (Loxodonta cyclotis), animal motion detection, 300 362–364 animal personality traits, 713 African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), animals in captivity, studies on, 701–702 693–702 Anna’s hummingbirds (Calypte anna), African savanna elephant (Loxodonta 19 africana), 362 anomalies in choice patterns, 771 alarm signals, 8 ant queen pheromone, 47 in cooperative relationships, 9 antennal boxing, 45 intentionality in use of, 252 anticipation effect phermone effects on discrimination in ants, false-belief situations, 240 49 for animate beings, 237 representational interpretation of, 250–251 anti-predator responses/strategies threat learning in, 507 call-referent relations in blackbirds, 107 alliances, 257 context-specific signal production, 102 allomothering in elephants, 366 cooperation in, 275 alternate gender roles, 279 in fish, 158–159, 494–496, 584 altruistic interactions, 274 learning and retention of, 148 American crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos), motion cues and, 309 mobbing behaviour in, 507 response to eye-like patterns, 297 amodal completion, 58, 60 shoal discimination in fish, 581 amygdala, face detection, 306 social learning in, 507, 519–520 Analogue Magnitude System (AMS), 562–563, to face-like stimuli, 297 569, 571 warning signals, 8 792 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-1-108-42674-9 — The Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition Edited by Allison B. Kaufman, Josep Call, James C. Kaufman Index More Information Index 793 ants arginine vasotocin, 284 acoustic communication in, 42 Asian elephant (Elephas maximus), 362 alarm phermone effects in, 49 Asian elephant (Elephas maximus), social begging behaviour in, 44 structure in, 364 chemical communication, 46–50 asocial learning, 454, 456 cuticular hydrocarbons in, 48–49 Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua), 141 foraging phermones in, 49 Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar), memory heterospecific adoption in, 42 retention in, 147 inter-specific differences in, 37 Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis), morphological differences in, 37 387 navigation in, 40 attitudinal reciprocity, 355 olfactory communication, 46 auditory/acoustic communication, 370 olfactory learning in, 47 acoustic cues in elephants, 370 queen phermone in, 47 auditory communication in wolves and recruiting and teaching in, 45 dogs, 74–76 recruitment in, 42 begging, 43 ritualized agonistic behaviours, 45 cues in threat and predator detection, societal living of, 36 370–371 stridulation in, 42 in ants, 42 tactile communication in, 43–46 mimicry in Maculinea butterfly larvae, 42 tandem running in, 45 wild-superb fairy-wens, 11 teaching in, 451 autonoetic consciousnes. See also visual cue use in, 40 phenomenological experience worker-laid egg regulation, 47 autonoetic consciousness/awareness ants (Atta sexdens), 42 evidence for, 128 ants (Camponotus), 37, 49 in episodic memory, 119, 128 ants (Camponotus senex), 41 in what-where-when paradigm, 119 ants (Cardiocondyla), 42 memory recollection, 118 ants (Cataglyphis), 40 recollective experiences and, 118 ants (Crematogaster scutellaris), 40 re-experience of past efents, 117 ants (Dinoponera gigantea), 37 AVPR1A gene, self-recognition ability, 428 ants (Formica fusca), 47 axis alignment of movement, 300 ants (Gigantiops destructor) visual cue use in, 40 baboons ants (Gnamptogenys stritula), 44 fission-fusion dynamics in, 325 ants (Lasius niger), 47 metacognitive abilities in, 762 ants (Mesoponera caffraria), 44 mirror-image stimulation in, 417 ants (Messor), 38 third party reconciliation in, 259 ants (Monomorium pharaonis), 37 Back-headed caiques (Pionites ants (Myrmica rubra), 42, 44 melanocephalus), 696 ants (Mystrium camillae), 37 basic-level categories, 622, 628 ants (Neoponera apicalis), 41 bees, social learning in, 454 ants (Neoponera goeldii), 42, 45 begging behaviour, 43 ants (Odontomachus), 37 behaviour reading, 241, 244–245 ants (Temnothorax), 45 embodied cognition and, 247 anurans. See frogs Gestalt-like perception in, 246 apes. See also primates intentionality in, 249 attention getting behaviours in, 235 prediction from behavioural cues, 241 empty gaze habituation in, 234 behavioural criteria, 460–461 eye contact and mutual gaze orientation in, for episodic memory in animals, 119 235 in comparative psychology and human gestural communication in, 252 studies, 132 intended meanings of gestures, 254 behavioural ecology, 174 intraspecific and interspecific gesture use, behavioural silence, 159 253 behavioural traditions recall memory in, 215–216 maintenance of, 457 aposematism. See warning coloration network based diffusion analysis of, 457 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-1-108-42674-9 — The Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition Edited by Allison B. Kaufman, Josep Call, James C. Kaufman Index More Information 794 Index behavioural traditions (cont.) blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) role of innovation in, 460 breeding decisions with cross fostering, 508 social network spread of, 506 foraging preferences with cross-fostering, 506 belief situations, adaptations to, 239–240, 248 blue-fronted Amazon parrot (Amazona beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas) aestival), 701 creativity and play in, 711 bluestreak cleaner wrasse (Labroides social learning in, 548 dimidiatus) Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata), vocal cooperation and punishment, 282 learning in, 22 bonobos biases in choices, biological vs. economic framing effects in, 777 currency, 779 future thinking in, 126 binding hypothesis, 194 metacognition in, 760 binding of episodes, 194 mirror-use in, 422–423 biological currency maximization in animals, risk-averse behaviour in, 776 779 timing in gestural communication, 23 biological fitness, 772 bonobos (Pan paniscus) biological inheritance hypothesis, 20 gestural sequences in, 16 biological motion detection logical inferences by, 779 biological motion detect and, 309 bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). See in humans, 308 also dolphins in monkeys, 308 cooperative hunting in, 275 triggering of antipredator and fear creativity and social learning in, 711 responses, 309 jointly coordinated cooperative behaviour, biologically rational behaviours, 779 392–393 birds, 504–521 pointing and gazing cue compremehsion, anti-predator responses/strategies, 507 398–399 breeding decisions and social learning, social awareness and joint attention, 393–396 507 social cognition, 384–385 brood parasitism defense for nestlings, 510 social learning, 548 conspecific alarm calls and fear response in, bowerbirds (Family Ptilonorhynchidae), 507 667–670 cue-based learning biases, 515 bowerbirds (Family Ptilonorhynchidae), framing effects in, 776 problem solving tasks in, 677–680 imitation in social learning, 508–509 brain development inferences from population to sample, 69–70 areas linked to SDM network, 284 learned foraging behaviours in, 506 eye placement and cerebral lateralization, learned mobbing behaviour in, 507 608 learned repetoires in, 505 for perceptual completion, 59 migrations pathways and social learning, group size in social relationships, 255 508 in parrots, 58 mimicry of heterospecifics in birds, 505 morphology in learning and memory, 140 mobbing behaviour in, 507 neural mechanisms in imprinting, 305 observational learning/conditioning in, 506 propensity for innovation and, 641–642 social learning and migratory tendencies, self-recognition ability and, 428 508 social intelligence and size, 230 sympatric species alarm calls and flight, 507 spatial representation in, 203–204 teaching in, 511 subcortical visual pathway, 310 black iguanas (Ctenosaura similis), eye-like breeding aggregations, 274 patterns effects in, 297 brood parasitism blackbirds (Turdus merula) incubation calling and, 511 learned mobbing behaviour in, 507 parental cost reduction by, 11 blind cavefish (Astyanax fasciatus) social learning and, 507 comparative studies, 159 brook trout (Salvelinus fontinalis), spatial blind cavefish (Phreatichthys andruzzii) learning in, 157 numerosity disctimination in, 589 brown thrashers (Toxostoma rufum), learned blue crab (Callinectes sapidus) repetoires in, 505 eye and eye-like pattern effects in, 298 bullet ants (Paraponera clavata), 36 © in this web service
Recommended publications
  • Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers in an Ultimatum Game Keith Jensen Et Al
    REPORTS sensitive to unfairness and punish proposers who Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers make inequitable offers by rejecting those offers at a cost to themselves, and knowing this, pro- in an Ultimatum Game posers make strategic offers that are less likely to be refused. Keith Jensen,* Josep Call, Michael Tomasello The ultimatum game has been used in dozens, possibly hundreds of studies, including 11 12 Traditional models of economic decision-making assume that people are self-interested rational various human cultures ( ) and children ( ). maximizers. Empirical research has demonstrated, however, that people will take into account Testing the ultimatum game on other species the interests of others and are sensitive to norms of cooperation and fairness. In one of the most would be an important contribution to the de- robust tests of this finding, the ultimatum game, individuals will reject a proposed division of a bate on the evolution and possible uniqueness of 6 monetary windfall, at a cost to themselves, if they perceive it as unfair. Here we show that in an human cooperation ( ). Chimpanzees are our ultimatum game, humans’ closest living relatives, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), are rational closest extant relatives and engage in cooperative maximizers and are not sensitive to fairness. These results support the hypothesis that other- behavior such as group hunting, coalitionary 13 regarding preferences and aversion to inequitable outcomes, which play key roles in human social aggression, and territorial patrols ( ). Further- organization, distinguish us from our closest living relatives. more, in experiments they have been shown to coordinate their behavior (14) and to provide help (15, 16).
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins of Grammar in Self-Modelling I Am What I Am: the Origins of Grammar in Self-Modelling
    Martin Edwardes I Am What I Am: the origins of grammar in self-modelling I Am What I Am: The origins of grammar in self-modelling Language is often identified as a defining difference between humans and nonhumans; but is it a feature with its own evolutionary explanation, or is it an adjunct of some other trait, evolved for reasons only indirectly related to language? Despite Noam Chomsky‟s view that language evolved for thinking1, it is generally believed that language is a product of socialisation, the need for individuals to exchange social information. And, if we look at the social information exchanged, we see forms of modelling that are absent from signalling systems used by other animals. First, there is modelling of the relationships between others: the simple sentence Alf likes Beth requires the receiver not just to recognise a single referent or context, but to bring two conceptualised individuals together and establish a relationship between them. Second, there is modelling of events in the past and future: where other animal signalling is dominated by current events, these play only a small part in human discourse. Third, there is modelling of what-if: we can discuss not just future probabilities and past certainties, but events that are unlikely to, or did not, happen. Fourth, there is modelling of the intentions of others; and fifth, there is modelling of our own self. The question of selfhood has long been recognised as vital to understand what makes us human. Even before Darwin‟s theory of evolution by descent, Wilhelm von Humboldt stated that: … the customary demands of mankind are satisfactorily met by forces of nature and by the mechanical continuation of human activity.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding and Sharing Intentions: the Origins of Cultural Cognition
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28, 000–000 Printed in the United States of America Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract: We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities requires not only especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural learning, but also a unique motivation to share psychological states with oth- ers and unique forms of cognitive representation for doing so. The result of participating in these activities is species-unique forms of cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the establishment of social institutions. In support of this proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and some children with autism) understand the basics of intentional action, but they still do not participate in activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared intentionality). Human children’s skills of shared intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: (1) the general ape line of understanding others as animate, goal-directed, and intentional agents; and (2) a species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and activities with other persons.
    [Show full text]
  • Editor-In-Chief Professor Brian Charlesworth Handling Editors
    RSBL_9_2_Sample_cover_Mock-up.qxd 27/2/13 4:13 PM Page 2 Editor-in-Chief Professor Brian Charlesworth GUIDANCE FOR AUTHORS Handling Editors Selection criteria Categories Professor Nigel Bennett The criteria for selection are scientific excellence, originality and Papers published in Biology Letters will be categorised by subject, Dr Joan Herbers interest across disciplines within biology. Papers are assessed by the which include Editorial Board for suitability before full peer-review. Their • animal behaviour • marine biology Dr Paul Sniegowski recommendations are passed to the Editor. The Editor is responsible Publishing Editor for all editorial decisions and he makes these decisions based on the • biomechanics • molecular evolution Charlotte Wray reports received from the referees and the Editorial Board. • community ecology • neurobiology • conservation biology • palaeontology Editorial Board Publishing format • pathogen biology Biology Letters are published regularly online and in bimonthly print • evolutionary biology Brian Charlesworth, Editor-in-Chief Alex Kacelnik School of Biological Sciences, University of Edinburgh Department of Zoology, Oxford University issues. Along with all Royal Society journals, we are committed to • phylogeny Nigel Bennett, Handling Editor Laurent Keller • evolutionary developmental Department of Zoology & Entomology, University of Pretoria Department of Ecology and Evolution, University of Lausanne biology Joan Herbers, Handling Editor Tom Little archiving and providing perpetual access. Although the printed • physiology Department of Evolution, Ecology and Organismal Biology, Ohio State University Institute of Evolutionary Biology, University of Edinburgh Paul Sniegowski, Handling Editor Martine Maan version of Biology Letters is limited to 2500 words, there is the • genome biology Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania Behavioural Biology, University of Groningen • population ecology Daniel Mennill facility for Electronic Supplementary Material (ESM).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Flexibility in Primates: Causes and Consequences DEVELOPMENTS in PRIMATOLOGY: PROGRESS and PROSPECTS
    Behavioral Flexibility in Primates: Causes and Consequences DEVELOPMENTS IN PRIMATOLOGY: PROGRESS AND PROSPECTS Series Editor: Russell H. Tuttle University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois This peer-reviewed book series will meld the facts of organic diversity with the continuity of the evolutionary process. The volumes in this series will exemplify the diversity of theoretical perspectives and methodological approaches currently employed by primatologists and physical anthropologists. Specific coverage includes: primate behavior in natural habitats and captive settings; primate ecology and conservation; functional morphology and developmental biology of primates; primate systematics; genetic and phenotypic differences among living primates; and paleoprimatology. ALL APES GREAT AND SMALL VOLUME I: AFRICAN APES Edited by Biruté M.F. Galdikas, Nancy Erickson Briggs, Lori K. Sheeran, Gary L. Shapiro and Jane Goodall THE GUENONS: DIVERSITY AND ADAPTATION IN AFRICAN MONKEYS Edited by Mary E. Glenn and Marina Cords ANIMAL MINDS, HUMAN BODIES By W.A. Hillix and Duane Rumbaugh COMPARATIVE VERTEBRATE COGNITION Edited by Lesley J. Rogers and Gisela Kaplan ANTHROPOID ORIGINS: NEW VISIONS Edited by Callum F. Ross and Richard F. Kay MODERN MORPHOMETRICS IN PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY Edited by Dennis E. Slice BEHAVIORAL FLEXIBILITY IN PRIMATES: CAUSES AND CONSEQUENCES By Clara B. Jones Behavioral Flexibility in Primates: Causes and Consequences CLARA B. JONES Fayetteville State University Fayetteville, North Carolina Theoretical Primatology Project Fayetteville, North Carolina Community Conservation, Inc. Gays Mills, Wisconsin Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Jones, Clara B. Behavioral flexibility in primates : causes and consequences / Clara B. Jones. p. cm.—(Developments in primatology) Includes bibliographical references (p. ). ISBN 0-387-23297-4 1. Primates—Behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Primate Societies
    The Evolution of Primate Societies The Evolution of Primate Societies Edited by John C. Mitani, Josep Call, Peter M. Kappeler, Ryne A. Palombit, and Joan B. Silk The University of Chicago Press Chicago and London John C. Mitani is the James N. Spuhler Collegiate Professor of Anthropology at the University of Michigan. Josep Call is a senior scientist and director of the Wolfgang Kohler Primate Research Centre at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Peter M. Kappeler is head of the Department of Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology/Anthropology at the University of Gottingen. Ryne A. Palombit is associate professor of anthropology at Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey. Joan B. Silk is professor in the Department of Anthropology and the Institute for Society and Genetics at the University of California, Los Angeles. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago 60637 The University of Chicago Press, Ltd., London © 2012 by The University of Chicago All rights reserved. Published 2012. Printed in the United States of America 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 1 2 3 4 5 ISBN- 13: 978-0-226-53171-7 (cloth) ISBN- 13: 978-0-226-53172-4 (paper) ISBN- 10: 0-226-53171-6 (cloth) ISBN- 10: 0-226-53172-4 (paper) Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data The evolution of primate societies / edited by John C. Mitani, Josep Call, Peter M. Kappeler, Ryne A. Palombit, and Joan B. Silk. pages ; cm Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13: 978-0-226-53171-7 (cloth : alkaline paper) ISBN-10: 0-226-53171-6 (cloth : alkaline paper) ISBN-13: 978-0-226-53172-4 (paperback : alkaline paper) ISBN-10: 0-226-53172-4 (paperback : alkaline paper) 1.
    [Show full text]
  • NICOLE THOMPSON GONZALEZ, Ph.D
    NICOLE THOMPSON GONZALEZ, Ph.D. Phone: +1 347 406 3283, Email: [email protected], Albuquerque, NM 87106 Twitter: @NicoleAlineSci, website: http://www.nicolethompsongonzalez.com/ Education & Training . Degree/Position University Field Year Post doctoral fellow University of New Anthropology 2018 – present Mexico Project: Oxidative stress and aging in wild chimpanzees Ph.D. Columbia University, Ecology and Evolution 2018 New York, NY Dissertation: Social ties over the life cycle in blue monkeys M.Res. University of Primate Behavior and Biology 2012 Roehampton, London, Thesis: Social integration and stress in juvenile olive UK baboons. B.A. Columbia University, Evolutionary Anthropology 2008 New York, NY Thesis: The co-evolution of human language and morality. Peer-reviewed publications Thompson Gonzalez, N.A., Thompson, M.E., Machanda, Z.P., Otali, E., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W. (in review). Urinary markers of oxidative stress respond to infection and late-life in wild chimpanzees. Thompson, M.E., Machanda, Z.P., Fox, S.A., Sabbi, K.H., Otali, E., Thompson, N.A., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W. (in press). "Evaluating the Impact of Physical Frailty During Aging in Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Phil Trans B. Thompson, N.A., Higham, J.P., Heistermann, M., Vogel, E., Cords, M., 2020. Energy balance but not competitive environment corresponds with allostatic load during development in an Old World monkey. Horm. Behav. 119, 104664. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104664 Thompson, N.A., Cords, M., 2019. Early life maternal sociality predicts juvenile sociality in blue monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 0(0), e23039. doi: 10.1002/ajp.23039 Thompson, N.A., 2019.
    [Show full text]
  • There Is No Special Problem of Mindreading in Nonhuman Animals Author(S): Marta Halina Source: Philosophy of Science, Vol
    There Is No Special Problem of Mindreading in Nonhuman Animals Author(s): Marta Halina Source: Philosophy of Science, Vol. 82, No. 3 (July 2015), pp. 473-490 Published by: The University of Chicago Press on behalf of the Philosophy of Science Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/681627 . Accessed: 02/11/2015 12:12 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press and Philosophy of Science Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Philosophy of Science. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Mon, 2 Nov 2015 12:12:29 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions There Is No Special Problem of Mindreading in Nonhuman Animals Marta Halina*y There is currently a consensus among comparative psychologists that nonhuman ani- mals are capable of some forms of mindreading. Several philosophers and psycholo- gists have criticized this consensus, however, arguing that there is a “logical problem” with the experimental approach used to test for mindreading in nonhuman animals. I argue that the logical problem is no more than a version of the general skeptical problem known as the theoretician’s dilemma.
    [Show full text]
  • Detailed Description
    Cultural Learning, Imitation, and Articraft Understanding: A Comparative Perspective CEU Summer University Nádor u. 9., Budapest, Hungary 1051 Tel.: (36 1) 327 3069, 327 3811 Fax: (36 1) 327 3124 E-mail: [email protected] Website: http://www.ceu.hu/sun Cognitive Neuroscience Cultural Learning, Imitation, and Articraft Understanding: A Comparative Perspective July 4 - 15, 2005 Course Directors: Josep Call, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology György Gergely, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Developmental Research Institute for Psychological Research, Budapest Faculty: Paul Bloom, Yale University, Department of Psychology Malinda Carpenter, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Gergely Csibra, Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychology, Birkbeck College Tim German, University of California, Department of Psychology Ádám Miklósi, ELTE, Department of Ethology Johannes Roessler, University of Warwick, Department of Philosophy József Topál, ELTE, Department of Ethology INTRODUCTION AND AIMS The summer course will provide a state-of-the-art cutting-edge scientific and research-oriented training for post-doctoral young researchers and highly promising pre-doctoral students on a currently central and heavily investigated interdisciplinary research topic that integrates several different, but partially overlapping fields of knowledge and scientific inquiry. These include human anthropology, social theories of culture, evolutionary and developmental psychology, the psychology of social cognition, cognitive development, and comparative and ethological studies of human and animal cultures. The course will be taught by a faculty consisting of internationally acknowledged leading experts of these fields from a variety of European countries (England, Germany, Hungary) as well as from the United States. Human culture versus population-specific behavioral traditions of non-human animal communities Is culture specific to humans? Well, yes and no, depending on one’s definition of what ‘culture’ is.
    [Show full text]
  • THE EVOLUTION of COMMUNICATIVE CREATIVITY from Fixed Signals to Contextual Flexibility 7-10 July 2005
    12th Altenberg Workshop in Theoretical Biology 2005 THE EVOLUTION OF COMMUNICATIVE CREATIVITY From Fixed Signals to Contextual Flexibility 7-10 July 2005 organized by D. Kimbrough Oller and Ulrike Griebel Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research Altenberg, Austria The topic The study of flexibility in communication systems in a wide variety of animals is offering new perspectives on the special forces that may have favored the evolution of language, with its omnipresent and seemingly unlimited flexibility. A variety of birds, marine mammals, New World monkeys, and even certain invertebrates show notable capabilities to break free from the fixed signaling patterns that have been so much the focus of description in classical ethology. A useful summary of the selection forces that favor steps toward contextual flexibility can now be provided, a summary that places in perspective speculations about the forces that may have guided hominid evolution toward spoken language. Research in human infant vocal development also provides invaluable clues about the first steps of vocal communication that may have been taken by ancient hominids as they became differentiated from the primate background. In particular, human infants, during the first half-year of life, develop capabilities to produce vocalizations with a remarkable degree of contextual flexibility, apparently surpassing all the other primates and perhaps all other mammals. Since spoken language requires vocal contextual flexibility in all its aspects and functions, it appears that an extremely early step in hominid evolution, establishing a necessary foundation for later evolution of language, was the emergence of contextually flexible vocalization. Philosophical work on communication and its origins along with modeling and simulation have vastly increased our view of possible sources and routes in the emergence of language.
    [Show full text]
  • Another Look at the Universal Grammar Hypothesis: Commentary on Evans 2014 Adele E
    200 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 92, NUMBER 1 (2016) quickly becomes evident that this is not a genuine tour de force, à la Pinker 1994. No, TLM is not the antidote to the long cognitivist nightmare that is The language instinct . In fact, by the end of the book, I fear it not only misses the point, but if adopted to any degree within the field, will also mislead young researchers and students of language into thinking that the debate is between E’s brand of linguistics on the one hand, and a bunch of raving, delusional, irrational bullies on the other. Ultimately, then, TLM will do little to advance the debate on linguistic nativism, although it might very well energize those already in E’s camp. REFERENCES Bickerton, Derek . 1981. The roots of language . Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma. Bickerton, Derek . 1984. The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7.173–88. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00044149 . Crain, Stephen, and Mineharu Nakayama. 1987. Structure dependence in grammar formation. Language 63.522–43. DOI: 10.2307/415004 . Evans, Vyvyan. 2014 . The language myth: Why language is not an instinct . Cambridge: Cambridge Uni - versity Press. Pinker , Steven . 1994. The language instinct . New York: William Morrow & Company. Department of Linguistics University of Hawai‘i at Manoa [[email protected]] Another look at the universal grammar hypothesis: Commentary on Evans 2014 Adele E. Goldberg Princeton University * It is important to recognize that The language myth (TLM ) is not a research monograph, but is instead aimed at a popular audience, and therefore it should be judged in this light.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding and Sharing Intentions: the Origins of Cultural Cognition
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28, 675–735 Printed in the United States of America Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] http://www.eva.mpg.de/psycho/ Abstract: We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities requires not only especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural learning, but also a unique motivation to share psychological states with oth- ers and unique forms of cognitive representation for doing so. The result of participating in these activities is species-unique forms of cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the establishment of social institutions. In support of this proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and some children with autism) understand the basics of intentional action, but they still do not participate in activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared intentionality). Human children’s skills of shared intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: (1) the general ape line of understanding others as animate, goal-directed, and intentional agents; and (2) a species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and activities with other persons.
    [Show full text]