Are Place Cells Just Memory Cells? Memory Compression Leads to Spatial Tuning and History Dependence

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Are Place Cells Just Memory Cells? Memory Compression Leads to Spatial Tuning and History Dependence bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/624239; this version posted August 31, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. Are place cells just memory cells? Memory compression leads to spatial tuning and history dependence Marcus K. Bennaa,b,c,1,2 and Stefano Fusia,b,d,1,2 aCenter for Theoretical Neuroscience, Columbia University; bMortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Columbia University; cNeurobiology Section, Division of Biological Sciences, University of California, San Diego; dKavli Institute for Brain Sciences, Columbia University This manuscript was compiled on August 31, 2020 The observation of place cells has suggested that the hippocampus to what appears to be noise. Some of the components of plays a special role in encoding spatial information. However, place the variability probably depend on the variables that are cell responses are modulated by several non-spatial variables, and represented at the current time, but some others might depend reported to be rather unstable. Here we propose a memory model also on the recent history, or in other words, they might be of the hippocampus that provides a novel interpretation of place affected by the storage of recent memories. cells consistent with these observations. We hypothesize that the The model of the hippocampus we propose not only pre- hippocampus is a memory device that takes advantage of the corre- dicts that place cells should exhibit history effects, but also lations between sensory experiences to generate compressed rep- that their spatial tuning properties simply reflect an efficient resentations of the episodes that are stored in memory. A simple strategy for storing correlated patterns. Much of the theoreti- neural network model that can efficiently compress information nat- cal work on the memory capacity of neural networks is based urally produces place cells that are similar to those observed in ex- on the assumption that the patterns representing memories are periments. It predicts that the activity of these cells is variable and random and uncorrelated (see e.g. (14–18)). This assumption that the fluctuations of the place fields encode information about the is not unreasonable for long-term storage, despite the fact recent history of sensory experiences. Place cells may simply be a that most of our sensory experiences are highly correlated. consequence of a memory compression process implemented in the Indeed, to efficiently store correlated episodes, it is desirable hippocampus. to preprocess the new memories and compress them before they are placed in long-term memory. Ideally, one would Hippocampus | Place Cells | Memory Models | Compression | Correlated want to extract the uncorrelated (and hence incompressible) Activity Patterns components of the new input patterns and store only those. However, this form of preprocessing and compression, which Introduction would lead to uncorrelated patterns of synaptic modifications, has not been explicitly modeled in previous theoretical studies Several studies show that neurons in the hippocampus encode of memory capacity. the position of the animal in its environment and as a con- We hypothesize that this preprocessing of the incoming sequence they have been named "place cells" (see e.g. (1)). stream of potential episodic memories is to some extent car- Here we propose a novel interpretation of the observation of ried out in the hippocampus, which in itself has long been place cells by suggesting that their response properties ac- tually reflect a process of memory compression in which the hippocampus plays a fundamental role. Our interpretation Significance Statement contributes to the reconciliation between two dominant, but apparently different points of view: one involving the hip- Numerous studies on humans revealed the importance of the pocampus in spatial cognitive maps and navigation, versus hippocampus in memory formation. The rodent literature in- another one that considers the hippocampus playing a broad stead focused on the spatial representations that are observed role in episodic and declarative memory (see e.g. (2, 3)). in navigation experiments. Here we propose a simple model The first view is supported by the observation of place cells, of the hippocampus that reconciles the main findings of the some of which exhibit responses that are easily interpretable as human and rodent studies. The model assumes that the hip- the cells tend to fire only when the animal is in one particular pocampus is a memory system that generates compressed location (single field place cells). However, it is becoming representations of sensory experiences using previously ac- clear that in many brain areas, including the hippocampus quired knowledge about the statistics of the world. These expe- and entorhinal cortex, neural responses are very diverse (4–8), riences can then be memorized more efficiently. The sensory highly variable in time (9, 10) and modulated by multiple experiences during the exploration of an environment, when variables (8, 11–13). Place cells might respond at single or compressed by the hippocampus, lead naturally to spatial rep- multiple locations, and multiple visitations of the same location resentations similar to those observed in rodent studies and to typically elicit different responses. Part of this diversity can be the emergence of place cells. explained by assuming that each neuron responds non-linearly to a different combination of multiple external or internal No conflict of interest declared. variables (mixed selectivity; see e.g. (5, 6)). The variability 1 M.K.B. and S.F. contributed equally to this work. might be due to the fact that some of these variables are 2To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] and not being monitored in the experiment, and hence contribute [email protected] 1–13 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/624239; this version posted August 31, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. considered as a memory storage system, albeit with a longest Results time constant that is shorter than that of cortex (19, 20). New sensory episodes are often similar to old ones and hence their Storing correlated patterns efficiently. Most of the patterns neural representations are likely to be correlated to those of sen- we would like to store in memory are likely to be highly cor- sory experiences that are already in memory. Storing directly related, as our experiences are often similar to each other. the neural representations of these sensory episodes would be Storing correlated patterns in artificial neural networks typi- inefficient because they are redundant. However, since the cally requires a synaptic learning rule that is more complicated hippocampus is highly structured and contains distinct regions than simple Hebbian plasticity to avoid a bias towards the that might implement different stages of processing, it can memory components shared my multiple memories. Simple transform and compress incoming sensory information with extensions such as the perceptron learning rule can already respect to other memories already stored there. An important deal with many forms of correlations. However, storing cor- role of the hippocampus in compressing memories has already related patterns in their original format is rarely the optimal been suggested in (21) (see also the Discussion). strategy and often it is possible to greatly increase the memory capacity by constructing compressed neural representations We have built a concrete neural network model that imple- that explicitly take into account the correlations. This form ments this compression process in a manner broadly consistent of preprocessing can be illustrated with a simple example in with the known neuroanatomy. The model consists of a three- which the patterns to be memorized are organized as in an layer network in which the first layer represents the sensory in- ultrametric tree (see e.g. (24) and Figure 1a). To generate puts (which would already have been processed by the sensory these correlated patterns one starts from p uncorrelated ran- areas corresponding to different modalities). The feed-forward dom patterns (the ancestors at the top of the tree). In these synapses that connect it to the second layer are continuously patterns each neuron is either active or inactive with equal modified to create a compressed, sparse representation of the probability, as in the case of the Hopfield model (14). One can inputs. This layer implements a form of statistical learning, as then generate k descendants for each ancestor by resampling the compressed representations are based on the statistics of randomly with a certain probability 1 − γ the activation state recent sensory experiences (and possibly input from cortical of each of the neurons in the patterns representing the an- long-term memory in the case of familiar environments, as cestors. The parameter k is called the branching ratio of the discussed below). A third layer is used to store the memories, tree. The total number of descendants is p k, and these are the i.e., specific episodes. This architecture and the computational patterns that we intend to store in memory. The descendants principles are similar to those proposed in (22) (see also the that come from the same ancestor will be
Recommended publications
  • Position–Theta-Phase Model of Hippocampal Place Cell Activity Applied to Quantification of Running Speed Modulation of Firing Rate
    Position–theta-phase model of hippocampal place cell activity applied to quantification of running speed modulation of firing rate Kathryn McClaina,b,1, David Tingleyb, David J. Heegera,c,1, and György Buzsákia,b,1 aCenter for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003; bNeuroscience Institute, New York University, New York, NY 10016; and cDepartment of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003 Edited by Terrence J. Sejnowski, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, and approved November 18, 2019 (received for review July 24, 2019) Spiking activity of place cells in the hippocampus encodes the Another challenge in understanding this system is the dynamic animal’s position as it moves through an environment. Within a interaction between the rate code and phase code. As these codes cell’s place field, both the firing rate and the phase of spiking in combine, different formats of information are conveyed simulta- the local theta oscillation contain spatial information. We propose a neously in place cell spiking. The interaction can produce unin- – position theta-phase (PTP) model that captures the simultaneous tuitive, although entirely predictable, results in traditional analyses expression of the firing-rate code and theta-phase code in place cell of place cell activity. These practical challenges have hindered the spiking. This model parametrically characterizes place fields to com- pare across cells, time, and conditions; generates realistic place cell effort to explain variability in hippocampal firing rates. A com- simulation data; and conceptualizes a framework for principled hy- putational tool is needed that accounts for the well-established pothesis testing to identify additional features of place cell activity.
    [Show full text]
  • Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning During
    Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg1 1Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems Center for Adaptive Systems, and Center of Excellence for Learning in Education, Science and Technology Boston University 677 Beacon St Boston, Massachusetts 02215 [email protected] Telephone: 617-353-7858/7 Fax: 617-353-7755 http://www.cns.bu.edu/~steve July 29, 2008 Revised: November 24, 2008 CAS/CNS-TR-08-003 Hippocampus, in press Running head: Beta Oscillations during Hippocampal Place Cell Learning Corresponding author: Stephen Grossberg, [email protected] Keywords: grid cells, category learning, spatial navigation, adaptive resonance theory, entorhinal cortex 1 Abstract The functional role of synchronous oscillations in various brain processes has attracted a lot of experimental interest. Berke et al. (2008) reported beta oscillations during the learning of hippocampal place cell receptive fields in novel environments. Such place cell selectivity can develop within seconds to minutes, and can remain stable for months. Paradoxically, beta power was very low during the first lap of exploration, grew to full strength as a mouse traversed a lap for the second and third times, and became low again after the first two minutes of exploration. Beta oscillation power also correlated with the rate at which place cells became spatially selective, and not with theta oscillations. These beta oscillation properties are explained by a neural model of how place cell receptive fields may be learned and stably remembered as spatially selective categories due to feedback interactions between entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. This explanation allows the learning of place cell receptive fields to be understood as a variation of category learning processes that take place in many brain systems, and challenges hippocampal models in which beta oscillations and place cell stability cannot be explained.
    [Show full text]
  • May-Britt Moser Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
    Grid Cells, Place Cells and Memory Nobel Lecture, 7 December 2014 by May-Britt Moser Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway. n 7 December 2014 I gave the most prestigious lecture I have given in O my life—the Nobel Prize Lecture in Medicine or Physiology. Afer lectures by my former mentor John O’Keefe and my close colleague of more than 30 years, Edvard Moser, the audience was still completely engaged, wonderful and responsive. I was so excited to walk out on the stage, and proud to present new and exciting data from our lab. Te title of my talk was: “Grid cells, place cells and memory.” Te long-term vision of my lab is to understand how higher cognitive func- tions are generated by neural activity. At frst glance, this seems like an over- ambitious goal. President Barack Obama expressed our current lack of knowl- edge about the workings of the brain when he announced the Brain Initiative last year. He said: “As humans, we can identify galaxies light years away; we can study particles smaller than an atom. But we still haven’t unlocked the mystery of the three pounds of matter that sits between our ears.” Will these mysteries remain secrets forever, or can we unlock them? What did Obama say when he was elected President? “Yes, we can!” To illustrate that the impossible is possible, I started my lecture by showing a movie with a cute mouse that struggled to bring a biscuit over an edge and home to its nest. Te biscuit was almost bigger than the mouse itself.
    [Show full text]
  • Specific Changes in Hippocampal Spatial Codes Predict Spatial
    Behavioural Brain Research 169 (2006) 168–175 Short communication Specific changes in hippocampal spatial codes predict spatial working memory performance Corey B. Puryear, Michael King, Sheri J.Y. Mizumori ∗ Department of Psychology, Box 351525, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, United States Received 7 July 2005; received in revised form 8 October 2005; accepted 18 December 2005 Available online 2 February 2006 Abstract This study examined the relationship between hippocampal place fields and spatial working memory. Place cells were recorded while rats solved a spatial working memory task in light and dark testing conditions. Rats made significantly more errors when tested in darkness, and although place fields changed in multiple ways in darkness, only changes in place field specificity predicted the degree of impaired spatial memory. This finding suggests that more spatially distinct place fields may contribute to hippocampal-dependent mnemonic functions. © 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Place cells; Place fields; Behavior; Spatial learning; Single units Hippocampal (HPC) damage in several animal species environment that caused place fields to be out of register rel- impairs the ability to learn tasks that depend on the use of ative to their standard configuration also resulted in a decrease allocentric spatial information [1,5,19]. Furthermore, numerous in rats’ performance on a continuous alternation task [10].In electrophysiological studies have demonstrated that the firing contrast, other manipulations that cause a robust reorganization rates of HPC pyramidal cells (place cells) are strongly modu- of place fields do not always affect rats’ performance of HPC- lated by the spatial location (place field) of the rat within the dependent spatial tasks [2,8].
    [Show full text]
  • Phase Precession of Medial Prefrontal Cortical Activity Relative to the Hippocampal Theta Rhythm
    HIPPOCAMPUS 15:867–873 (2005) Phase Precession of Medial Prefrontal Cortical Activity Relative to the Hippocampal Theta Rhythm Matthew W. Jones and Matthew A. Wilson* ABSTRACT: Theta phase-locking and phase precession are two related spike-timing and the phase of the concurrent theta phenomena reflecting coordination of hippocampal place cell firing cycle: place cell spikes are ‘‘phase-locked’’ to theta with the local, ongoing theta rhythm. The mechanisms and functions of both the phenomena remain unclear, though the robust correlation (Buzsaki and Eidelberg, 1983). Thus, the spikes of a between firing phase and location of the animal has lead to the sugges- given neuron are not distributed randomly across the tion that this phase relationship constitutes a temporal code for spatial theta cycle. Rather, they consistently tend to occur information. Recent work has described theta phase-locking in the rat during a restricted phase of the oscillation. Further- medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), a structure with direct anatomical and more, in CA1, CA3, and the dentate gyrus, phase- functional links to the hippocampus. Here, we describe an initial char- locking to the local theta rhythm is accompanied by acterization of phase precession in the mPFC relative to the CA1 theta rhythm. mPFC phase precession was most robust during behavioral the related phenomenon of phase precession: each epochs known to be associated with enhanced theta-frequency coordi- neuron tends to fire on progressively earlier phases of nation of CA1 and mPFC activities. Precession was coherent across the the concurrent theta cycle as an animal moves through mPFC population, with multiple neurons precessing in parallel as a func- its place field (O’Keefe and Recce, 1993).
    [Show full text]
  • A Survey of Entorhinal Grid Cell Properties
    A Survey of Entorhinal Grid Cell Properties Jochen Kerdels Gabriele Peters October 18, 2018 University of Hagen - Chair of Human-Computer Interaction Universit¨atsstrasse1, 58097 Hagen - Germany Abstract About a decade ago grid cells were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex of rat. Their peculiar firing patterns, which correlate with periodic locations in the environment, led to early hypothesis that grid cells may provide some form of metric for space. Subsequent research has since uncovered a wealth of new insights into the characteristics of grid cells and their neural neighborhood, the parahippocampal-hippocampal region, calling for a revision and refinement of earlier grid cell models. This survey paper aims to provide a comprehensive summary of grid cell research published in the past decade. It focuses on the functional characteristics of grid cells such as the influence of external cues or the alignment to envi- ronmental geometry, but also provides a basic overview of the underlying neural substrate. 1 Introduction The parahippocampal-hippocampal region (PHR-HF) of the mammalian brain hosts a variety of neurons whose activity correlates with a number of allocentric variables. For example, the activity of so-called place cells correlates with specific, mostly individual locations in the environment [50, 51], the activity of head direction cells correlates with the absolute head direction of an animal [66, 65], the activity of grid cells correlates with a regular lattice of allocentric locations [16, 22], the activity of border cells correlates with the proximity of an animal to specific borders in its environment [60, 59], and, finally, the activity of speed cells correlates with the current speed of an animal [32].
    [Show full text]
  • Modulation of Brain Hyperexcitability: Potential New Therapeutic Approaches in Alzheimer’S Disease
    International Journal of Molecular Sciences Review Modulation of Brain Hyperexcitability: Potential New Therapeutic Approaches in Alzheimer’s Disease Sofia Toniolo 1,2,*, Arjune Sen 3 and Masud Husain 1,2 1 Cognitive Neurology Group, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK; [email protected] 2 Wellcome Trust Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6AE, UK 3 Oxford Epilepsy Research Group, Nuffield Department Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK; [email protected] * Correspondence: sofi[email protected]; Tel.: +44-1865-271310 Received: 29 October 2020; Accepted: 5 December 2020; Published: 7 December 2020 Abstract: People with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have significantly higher rates of subclinical and overt epileptiform activity. In animal models, oligomeric Aβ amyloid is able to induce neuronal hyperexcitability even in the early phases of the disease. Such aberrant activity subsequently leads to downstream accumulation of toxic proteins, and ultimately to further neurodegeneration and neuronal silencing mediated by concomitant tau accumulation. Several neurotransmitters participate in the initial hyperexcitable state, with increased synaptic glutamatergic tone and decreased GABAergic inhibition. These changes appear to activate excitotoxic pathways and, ultimately, cause reduced long-term potentiation, increased long-term depression, and increased GABAergic inhibitory remodelling at the network level. Brain hyperexcitability has therefore been identified as a potential target for therapeutic interventions aimed at enhancing cognition, and, possibly, disease modification in the longer term. Clinical trials are ongoing to evaluate the potential efficacy in targeting hyperexcitability in AD, with levetiracetam showing some encouraging effects.
    [Show full text]
  • Ripple Band Phase Precession of Place Cell Firing During Replay
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.05.438482; this version posted April 6, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. Ripple Band Phase Precession of Place Cell Firing during Replay Daniel Bush*,1,2, Freyja Olafsdottir*,3, Caswell Barry#,4, Neil Burgess#,1,2 1UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Queen Square, London, UK 2UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK 3Donders Institute for Brain Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands 4UCL Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Gower Street, London, UK *,# These authors contributed equally to this work Correspondence: [email protected], [email protected] Summary Phase coding offers several theoretical advantages for information transmission compared to an equivalent rate code. Phase coding is shown by place cells in the rodent hippocampal formation, which fire at progressively earlier phases of the movement related 6-12Hz theta rhythm as their spatial receptive fields are traversed. Importantly, however, phase coding is independent of carrier frequency, and so we asked whether it might also be exhibited by place cells during 150-250Hz ripple band activity, when they are thought to replay information to neocortex. We demonstrate that place cells which fire multiple spikes during candidate replay events do so at progressively earlier ripple phases, and that spikes fired across all replay events exhibit a negative relationship between decoded location within the firing field and ripple phase. These results provide insights into the mechanisms underlying phase coding and place cell replay, as well as the neural code propagated to downstream neurons.
    [Show full text]
  • Path Integration, Continuous Attractors and Grid Cells ‘Path Integration’ in Mammals
    Path integration, continuous attractors and grid cells ‘Path integration’ in mammals Aims Many animals retain a outward path Although there are • Understand the concept of path integration and how it might contribute to sense of the total angle often biases… and distance travelled navigation and place cell firing so that a return path • Discuss the continuous attractor network model of place cell firing can be made, even in return path total darkness • Describe the firing pattern of grid cells in entorhinal cortex and why they might be suited to provide the path integration input to place cells References Zhang (1996) Representation of spatial orientation by the intrinsic dynamics of the head-direction cell The capacity to retain ensemble: a theory. Journal of Neuroscience 16: 2112-2126. this information is Samsonovich and McNaughton (1997) Path integration and cognitive mapping in a continuous attractor limited to around three neural network model. Journal of Neuroscience 17: 5900-5920. passive or five active rotations in hamsters Etienne and Jeffery (2004) Path integration in mammals. Hippocampus 14:180-92. returning to the nest Hafting et al. (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436: 801-806. O’Keefe and Burgess (2005) Dual phase and rate coding in hippocampal place cells: theoretical significance and relationship to entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus 15: 853-866. Jeffery and Burgess (2006) A metric for the cognitive map: found at last? Trends in Cognitive Science 10: 1-3. Etienne, Maurer & Seguinot (1996) Journal of Experimental Biology Place cells and path-integration Place cells and path-integration Path integration appears to orient the place cell representation unless stable Path integration appears to provide Effect of running direction on firing visual orientation cues are present additional information about a boundary location of a stretched field: Rotate rat & Rotate rat Rotate rat & that the rat has recently visited (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Design Principles of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map
    Design Principles of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map Kimberly L. Stachenfeld1, Matthew M. Botvinick1, and Samuel J. Gershman2 1Princeton Neuroscience Institute and Department of Psychology, Princeton University 2Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Hippocampal place fields have been shown to reflect behaviorally relevant aspects of space. For instance, place fields tend to be skewed along commonly traveled directions, they cluster around rewarded locations, and they are constrained by the geometric structure of the environment. We hypothesize a set of design principles for the hippocampal cognitive map that explain how place fields represent space in a way that facilitates navigation and reinforcement learning. In particular, we suggest that place fields encode not just information about the current location, but also predictions about future locations under the current transition distribu- tion. Under this model, a variety of place field phenomena arise naturally from the structure of rewards, barriers, and directional biases as reflected in the tran- sition policy. Furthermore, we demonstrate that this representation of space can support efficient reinforcement learning. We also propose that grid cells compute the eigendecomposition of place fields in part because is useful for segmenting an enclosure along natural boundaries. When applied recursively, this segmentation can be used to discover a hierarchical decomposition of space. Thus, grid cells might be involved in computing subgoals for hierarchical reinforcement learning. 1 Introduction A cognitive map, as originally conceived by Tolman [46], is a geometric representation of the en- vironment that can support sophisticated navigational behavior. Tolman was led to this hypothesis by the observation that rats can acquire knowledge about the spatial structure of a maze even in the absence of direct reinforcement (latent learning; [46]).
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Theta Cells in Competitive Burst Synchronization Networks: Reference Frames from Phase Codes
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/211458; this version posted October 30, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. RUNNING TITLE: Spatial Theta Cells Spatial Theta Cells in Competitive Burst Synchronization Networks: Reference Frames from Phase Codes Joseph D. Monaco1∗, Hugh T. Blair2, Kechen Zhang1;3 1Biomedical Engineering Department, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; 2Psychology Department, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA 3Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA ∗Correspondence: Joseph Monaco Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine Biomedical Engineering Department 720 Rutland Ave Traylor 407 Baltimore, MD 21205, USA [email protected] bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/211458; this version posted October 30, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 1 Abstract 2 Spatial cells of the hippocampal formation are embedded in networks of theta cells. The septal 3 theta rhythm (6–10 Hz) organizes the spatial activity of place and grid cells in time, but it remains 4 unclear how spatial reference points organize the temporal activity of theta cells in space. We 5 study spatial theta cells in simulations and single-unit recordings from exploring rats to ask 6 whether temporal phase codes may anchor spatial representations to the outside world.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning an Efficient Place Cell Map from Grid Cells Using Non-Negative Sparse Coding
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.12.248534; this version posted August 13, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. Learning an efficient place cell map from grid cells using non-negative sparse coding Yanbo Lian1,*, Anthony N. Burkitt1,* 1 Department of Biomedical Engineering, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia * fyanbo.lian, [email protected] Abstract Experimental studies of grid cells in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex (MEC) have shown that they are selective to an array of spatial locations in the environment that form a hexagonal grid. However, place cells in the hippocampus are only selective to a single-location of the environment while granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus have multiple discrete firing locations, but lack spatial periodicity. Given the anatomical connection from MEC to the hippocampus, previous feedforward models of grid-to-place have been proposed. Here, we propose a unified learning model that can describe the spatial tuning properties of both hippocampal place cells and dentate gyrus granule cells based on non-negative sparse coding. Sparse coding plays an important role in many cortical areas and is proposed here to have a key role in the navigational system of the brain in the hippocampus.Our results show that the hexagonal patterns of grid cells with various orientations, grid spacings and phases are necessary for model cells to learn a single spatial field that efficiently tile the entire spatial environment.
    [Show full text]