Abri Du Maras, France)
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Quaternary Science Reviews 82 (2013) 23e40 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Quaternary Science Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/quascirev Impossible Neanderthals? Making string, throwing projectiles and catching small game during Marine Isotope Stage 4 (Abri du Maras, France) Bruce L. Hardy a,*, Marie-Hélène Moncel b, Camille Daujeard b, Paul Fernandes c, Philippe Béarez d, Emmanuel Desclaux e, Maria Gema Chacon Navarro b,f, Simon Puaud b, Rosalia Gallotti g a Dept. of Anthropology, Kenyon College, Gambier, OH 43022, USA b Département de Préhistoire, UMR 7194, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France c Paléotime, Villars de Lans, France d Département d’écologie et gestion de la biodiversité, UMR 7209, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France e Laboratoire de Préhistoire du Lazaret, Nice, France f IPHES (Institut Català de Paleoecologia Humana i Evolució Social), Tarragona, Spain g Université Bordeaux 1, UMR 5199 PACEA-PPP, Talence, France article info abstract Article history: Neanderthal behavior is often described in one of two contradictory ways: 1) Neanderthals were Received 10 June 2013 behaviorally inflexible and specialized in large game hunting or 2) Neanderthals exhibited a wide range Received in revised form of behaviors and exploited a wide range of resources including plants and small, fast game. Using stone 26 September 2013 tool residue analysis with supporting information from zooarchaeology, we provide evidence that at the Accepted 27 September 2013 Abri du Maras, Ardèche, France, Neanderthals were behaviorally flexible at the beginning of MIS 4. Here, Available online 26 October 2013 Neanderthals exploited a wide range of resources including large mammals, fish, ducks, raptors, rabbits, mushrooms, plants, and wood. Twisted fibers on stone tools provide evidence of making string or Keywords: Residue analysis cordage. Using a variety of lines of evidence, we show the presence of stone projectile tips, possibly used Stone tools in complex projectile technology. This evidence shows a level of behavioral variability that is often de- Subsistence nied to Neanderthals. Furthermore, it sheds light on perishable materials and resources that are not often String recovered which should be considered more fully in reconstructions of Neanderthal behavior. Zooarchaeology Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Laroulandie, 2012; Finlayson et al., 2012), a wide and varied diet (Henry et al., 2011; Blasco and Fernández Peris, 2012; Cochard et al., The arguments over Neanderthal behaviors and capabilities 2012; Salazar-García et al., 2013), and even maritime navigation continue unabated. Recently, two competing threads have emerged (Ferentinos et al., 2012). This recognition of behavioral variability within this debate. One emphasizes the relative inflexibility of through space and time argues for adaptation of Neanderthal Neanderthals from a cognitive (e.g. Wynn and Coolidge, 2004), groups to local conditions (Clark, 2002; Hardy, 2010). behavioral (e.g. Fa et al., 2013) and technological perspective (e.g. In both cases, much research effort is devoted to reconstructing Stiner and Kuhn, 2009). This line of argument often generally Neanderthal subsistence. Influenced heavily by stable isotope speaks of “Neanderthal” capabilities or behaviors as if this group of analysis, Neanderthals are most often portrayed as top-level car- hominins always did the same things no matter the temporal or nivores who derive the vast majority of their food from large ecological circumstances (Brown et al., 2011). The other emphasizes terrestrial herbivores (Balter and Simon, 2006; Bocherens, 2009; an increasing recognition of the variability of Neanderthal behavior Richards and Trinkaus, 2009). However, the relatively small num- and the elucidation of previously unrecognized behaviors including ber of Neanderthals sampled for isotope studies so far are from personal ornamentation (Peresani et al., 2011; Morin and northern, interior areas of their range and should not be taken as indicative of the entire population (Pearson, 2007; Brown et al., 2011). Furthermore, analyses of this hypothetical high protein * Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1 740 427 5887. diet have suggested that it is unrealistic to support life in the long E-mail address: [email protected] (B.L. Hardy). run (Hardy, 2010; Hockett, 2012). 0277-3791/$ e see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.quascirev.2013.09.028 24 B.L. Hardy et al. / Quaternary Science Reviews 82 (2013) 23e40 Fig. 1. Location of the site of the Abri du Maras. Increasingly, evidence is emerging at some sites that Neander- Here, we present evidence for behavioral variability and thals exploited a wider range of smaller, faster prey including birds complexity among Neanderthals at the beginning of Marine Isotope (Blasco and Fernández Peris, 2009, 2012), rabbits (Blasco and Stage 4 (MIS 4) at the Abri du Maras located above the Ardèche Fernández Peris, 2009, 2012; Cochard et al., 2012), and fish (e.g. River in southern France. Using residue analysis of stone tools with Le Gall, 1990, 2000; Roselló-Izquierdo and Morales-Muñiz, 2005; supporting evidence from zooarchaeology, we show that Nean- Hardy and Moncel, 2011) as well as a detailed knowledge and use derthals at the Abri du Maras had a detailed knowledge of their of plant foods (Hardy and Moncel, 2011; Henry et al., 2011; Salazar- surrounding environment, captured fast and agile prey (rabbits, García et al., 2013). This evidence goes against the widespread fish and birds), exploited a range of plant species, and used com- argument that the hunting of fast and agile prey such as birds, posite technology such as hafted stone points and the manufacture leporids, and fish as well as the exploitation of plant foods are of string and cordage. Overall, we present evidence which dem- defining features of “modern” behavior which only occurred sys- onstrates that Neanderthals at the Abri du Maras were far from tematically in the Upper Paleolithic (Stiner et al., 1999, 2000; Klein, inefficient foragers. 2001; Richards et al., 2001, 2005; Drucker and Bocherens, 2004; Klein et al., 2004; Balter and Simon, 2006; Richards, 2009). 2. Site background Recently, Fa et al. (2013) have gone as far as to suggest that Nean- derthals’ inability to switch to rabbit as prey factored into their The site of the Abri du Maras is located in a small valley less than extinction. This returns us to a picture of Neanderthals as inefficient 1 km from the Ardèche River, a tributary of the Rhône River and foragers incapable of adapting to changing conditions (Klein and close to the Rhône Valley (Fig. 1). Its elevation is 170 m a.s.l. and Cruz-Uribe, 2000; Klein et al., 2004). Such a view suggests that 70 m above the Ardèche River. First excavated in the 1950’s and Neanderthals would have gone extinct well before they did 1960’s, new excavations have taken place since 2006 in order to (Sorensen and Leonard, 2001; White, 2006). obtain more data on the oldest human occupations and open a Other researchers offer a different view where some limited large excavated area. This site was famous in the past for a Middle behavioral change takes place with Neanderthals but only post Paleolithic (MP) deposit with a Levallois laminar debitage (level 1) 50 kya (Langley et al., 2008; Stiner and Kuhn, 2009). In this sce- and covering seven distinct levels (levels 8e2) with MP assem- nario, behavioral complexity, as reflected by composite technology blages (Combier, 1967; Moncel et al., 1994). Little is known of the and evidence for symbolic thought, appears with some “late Ne- bottom of the sequence due to reduced excavation during early anderthals”, most notably with the Chatelperronian (d’Errico et al., fieldwork. 1998, 2003). However, this is often presented as being too little, too Geological study attests that the cave roof collapsed over time late for Neanderthals. and the youngest occupations were settled under a shelter (Debard, B.L. Hardy et al. / Quaternary Science Reviews 82 (2013) 23e40 25 1988). The oldest occupations, however, took place under a large 3. Lithic technology cave roof and are located in front of the shelter as it is nowadays. In 2006, new excavations yielded two thick and distinct deposits in a Layer 4 has been excavated over more than 40 m2 and has test pit located in front of the site. We were not able to distinguish yielded more than 3600 artifacts (40 artifacts/m3). The vertical the different lenses observed in the past and preferred instead to distribution indicates two concentrations of artifacts. These den- name large phases of sedimentation (Fig. 2). The oldest one was sities could represent two large phases of occupation of the site for named layer 5 (ancient levels 8e6), an organic brown level, layer 4. No significant difference was observed between these two covering the limestone substratum. The second one is layer 4 phases and the material has been studied as a single unit. Most are (ancient levels 5 and 4), with homogeneous eolian silts (loess) with flint flakes, blades, bladelets and points between 15 and 90 mm few small blocks of limestone, recovered in three levels (layers 3, 2, long. The flaking is mainly of Levallois-type (Fig. 3). Two large and 1), similar to the previous excavations and contains the groups of products may be distinguished: 1) small and middle-size youngest occupations (Moncel et al., 2010). Rich lithic and faunal flakes, flakes, bladelets and Levallois points made on the site on remains and fire places characterize our level 4 which has been core-flakes, 2) long flakes, blades and points, most of them Leval- excavated over more than 40 m2. Two sub-levels are visible with a lois, made outside and brought to the site. Most of the series is not higher density of archaeological remains (4.1 and 4.2). They are retouched (less than 5% are scrapers, denticulates, and points).