Reappraising the Clinical Relevance of Attentional Bias in Addiction. Addictive Behaviors: 2015, 44(44-50), 44-50

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Reappraising the Clinical Relevance of Attentional Bias in Addiction. Addictive Behaviors: 2015, 44(44-50), 44-50 Christiansen, P., Schoenmakers, T.M., Field, M. Less than meets the eye: reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction. Addictive Behaviors: 2015, 44(44-50), 44-50 Postprint 1.0 Version Journal website http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0306460314003372 Pubmed link http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=25453782 DOI 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.005 This is a NIVEL certified Post Print, more info at http://www.nivel.eu Less than meets the eye: Reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction A, B, , C, D A, B PAUL CHRISTIANSEN , TIM M. SCHOENMAKERS , MATT FIELD a Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom b UK Centre for Tobacco and Alcohol Studies (UKCTAS), United Kingdom c IVO Addiction Research Institute, Rotterdam, The Netherlands d Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands ABSTRACT Recent years have seen an explosion of interest in attentional bias in addiction, particularly its clinical relevance. Specifically, numerous articles claimed to demonstrate either that (1) attentional bias measured in treatment settings could predict subsequent relapse to substance use, or (2) direct modification of attentional bias reduced substance use and improved treatment outcomes. In this paper, we critically evaluate empirical studies that investigated these issues. We show that the evidence regarding both of these claims is decidedly mixed, and that many of the studies that appear to yield positive findings have serious methodological and statistical limitations. We contend that the available literature suggests that attentional bias for drug cues fluctuates within individuals because it is an output of the underlying motivational state at that moment in time, but there is no convincing evidence that it exerts a causal influence on substance use. Future research should make use of experience sampling methodology to characterise the clinical significance of fluctuations in attentional bias over time. 1. Introduction Motivationally-relevant cues attract and hold the attention: the hungry dieter will notice cakes in the canteen, the anxious person will detect threatening shapes in the shadows, and the depressed person will focus on the negative comments amongst the positive. Attentional bias (AB) towards substance-related cues is also seen in people who frequently use those substances, including individuals with substance use disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Literature documenting the This is a NIVEL certified Post Print, more info at http://www.nivel.eu Christiansen, P., Schoenmakers, T.M., Field, M. Less than meets the eye: reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction. Addictive Behaviors: 2015, 44(44-50), 44-50 presence of AB in substance users has been extensively reviewed elsewhere (Cox et al., 2006, Field and Cox, 2008 and Wiers et al., 2014) and this evidence is consistent with theoretical models which posit that AB plays a causal role in the onset, maintenance and recurrence (relapse) of substance use disorders after a period of abstinence (Cox et al., 2006, Franken, 2003, Field and Cox, 2008 and Wiers et al., 2014). Three recent narrative reviews (Cox et al., 2014, Field et al., 2014 and Marhe et al., 2014) reviewed the clinical relevance of AB in substance use disorders and reached quite different conclusions despite considering many of the same studies. In the current review we offer a comprehensive, detailed, and critical review of studies that investigated two key issues: the predictive validity of AB as a predictor of relapse to substance use after treatment, and the efficacy of attentional bias modification (ABM) as an intervention to prevent relapse or reduce substance use among those with substance use disorders. In contrast to the other recent reviews, which aimed to provide a broad overview of the literature, our goal here is to critically dissect the methods used and results that were reported in previous studies. Our conclusions are considerably more pessimistic than those offered in other reviews, and by the authors of much of the original research. The important theoretical and clinical implications of our review will be highlighted throughout this paper. 2. MEASUREMENT ISSUES AND LITERATURE SEARCH STRATEGY Although a number of measures of AB for substance cues have been described, the addiction Stroop task and the visual probe task are the most frequently used and to our knowledge these are the only two measures that have been used to investigate the issues that are central to this review paper. Full descriptions of both measures and discussions of their strengths, weaknesses and underlying psychological processes can be found elsewhere (Field and Cox, 2008 and Field, Duka, Tyler and Schoenmakers, 2009). Importantly, both tasks rely on measurements of manual reaction time to make inferences about attentional deployment. In the addiction Stroop task, participants are instructed to name the colour in which substance-related and matched neutral words are printed, whilst ignoring the content of those words. Slower colour-naming times for substance-related words are interpreted to indicate that those words captured the attention (Cox et al., 2006), but it should be noted that slower colour naming could also reflect a non-specific freezing response to stimuli that are perceived as threatening (Algom et al., 2004 and Greenaway et al., 2012), cognitive avoidance of stimuli that are perceived as threatening (De Ruiter & Brosschot, 1994), or individual differences in general inhibitory control ability (Crunelle, Veltman, Booij, van Emmerik–van Oortmerssen, & van den Brink, 2012). In the visual probe task, a pair of words or images is presented side-by-side on a computer screen before being removed and replaced by a visual probe that appears on one side of the screen. Participants must respond to this probe as quickly as possible and AB is inferred if they are faster to respond to probes that replace substance-related words or images compared to probes that replace neutral images. Both of these tasks are characterised by unacceptably low internal reliability, particularly the visual probe task for which internal reliability may be close to zero (Ataya et al., 2012). Although there is debate about the reliability of both measures— it is undoubtedly poor, but may not be bad as Ataya et al. have suggested based on a This is a NIVEL certified Post Print, more info at http://www.nivel.eu Christiansen, P., Schoenmakers, T.M., Field, M. Less than meets the eye: reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction. Addictive Behaviors: 2015, 44(44-50), 44-50 re-analysis of only their own data (see Field & Christiansen, 2012)—this issue is important for understanding the predictive validity of the tasks, and we return to it later in the paper. This review is concerned with relapse, specifically the relationship between AB and relapse to substance use after a period of abstinence, and the effects of ABM on relapse in patients who are trying to abstain. Different researchers have used different definitions and indicators of relapse, and there is no universally accepted ‘gold standard’ measure. One could argue that it should be treated as a dichotomous variable (within a pre-defined follow-up period), but given that it is normal for patients to have multiple relapses before they eventually achieve abstinence, this measure may not adequately capture the nature of recovery from substance use disorders. Alternative measures, such as the duration of abstinence/latency to relapse have been reported in some studies and arguably this is more appropriate from both a clinical perspective (because it yields a more detailed description of progress) and a methodological one, because it has greater statistical power. Another important point is that some patients have a goal of reducing their substance use rather than abstaining altogether, in which case indices of the quantity or frequency of substance use may be more informative than crudely categorising participants as relapsed or abstinent. In this review, we included peer-reviewed studies that have been published or accepted for publication in peer-reviewed journals. Studies were initially identified on the basis of review articles (Cox et al., 2006, Franken, 2003, Field and Cox, 2008, Wiers et al., 2014, Cox et al., 2014, Field et al., 2014 and Marhe et al., 2014), and based on searches of Scopus and PubMed using the following search terms: ‘Attentional bias OR Stroop’ AND ‘addiction OR alcohol OR tobacco OR nicotine OR smoking OR drug OR cannabis OR marijuana OR cocaine OR heroin OR opiates OR amphetamine’. These searches resulted in 1172 results. The titles and abstracts of these papers were inspected to identify any papers that investigated either (1) the relationship between AB and subsequent substance use or relapse, or (2) the effects of ABM. A total of 28 articles, describing 27 unique studies, were retained for detailed consideration: 16 papers (15 studies) for the first section (prediction of relapse, Table 1) and 12 papers (12 studies) for the second (ABM). We did not conduct a formal meta-analysis or report effect sizes because the variation in predictor and outcome measures used in different studies would have rendered statistical comparisons between studies meaningless. In any case, our primary aim was to critically discuss the methods used and interpretation of data obtained in previous studies, something that
Recommended publications
  • A Task-Based Taxonomy of Cognitive Biases for Information Visualization
    A Task-based Taxonomy of Cognitive Biases for Information Visualization Evanthia Dimara, Steven Franconeri, Catherine Plaisant, Anastasia Bezerianos, and Pierre Dragicevic Three kinds of limitations The Computer The Display 2 Three kinds of limitations The Computer The Display The Human 3 Three kinds of limitations: humans • Human vision ️ has limitations • Human reasoning 易 has limitations The Human 4 ️Perceptual bias Magnitude estimation 5 ️Perceptual bias Magnitude estimation Color perception 6 易 Cognitive bias Behaviors when humans consistently behave irrationally Pohl’s criteria distilled: • Are predictable and consistent • People are unaware they’re doing them • Are not misunderstandings 7 Ambiguity effect, Anchoring or focalism, Anthropocentric thinking, Anthropomorphism or personification, Attentional bias, Attribute substitution, Automation bias, Availability heuristic, Availability cascade, Backfire effect, Bandwagon effect, Base rate fallacy or Base rate neglect, Belief bias, Ben Franklin effect, Berkson's paradox, Bias blind spot, Choice-supportive bias, Clustering illusion, Compassion fade, Confirmation bias, Congruence bias, Conjunction fallacy, Conservatism (belief revision), Continued influence effect, Contrast effect, Courtesy bias, Curse of knowledge, Declinism, Decoy effect, Default effect, Denomination effect, Disposition effect, Distinction bias, Dread aversion, Dunning–Kruger effect, Duration neglect, Empathy gap, End-of-history illusion, Endowment effect, Exaggerated expectation, Experimenter's or expectation bias,
    [Show full text]
  • The Lightbulb Paradox: Evidence from Two Randomized Experiments
    NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES THE LIGHTBULB PARADOX: EVIDENCE FROM TWO RANDOMIZED EXPERIMENTS Hunt Allcott Dmitry Taubinsky Working Paper 19713 http://www.nber.org/papers/w19713 NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 December 2013 We are grateful to Raj Chetty, Lucas Davis, Stefano DellaVigna, Mushfiq Mubarak, Sendhil Mullainathan, Emmanuel Saez, Josh Schwartzstein, and other colleagues, as well as seminar audiences at the ASSA Annual Meeting, the Behavioral Economics Annual Meeting, Berkeley, the European Summer Symposium for Economic Theory, Harvard, the NBER Public Economics Meetings, Resources for the Future, Stanford, the Stanford Institute for Theoretical Economics, UCLA, the University of California Energy Institute, and the University of Chicago for constructive feedback. We thank our research assistants - Jeremiah Hair, Nina Yang, and Tiffany Yee - as well as management at the partner company, for their work on the in-store experiment. Thanks to Stefan Subias, Benjamin DiPaola, and Poom Nukulkij at GfK for their work on the TESS experiment. We are grateful to the National Science Foundation and the Sloan Foundation for financial support. Files containing code for replication, the in-store experiment protocol, and audio for the TESS experiment can be downloaded from Hunt Allcott's website. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research. NBER working papers are circulated for discussion and comment purposes. They have not been peer- reviewed or been subject to the review by the NBER Board of Directors that accompanies official NBER publications. © 2013 by Hunt Allcott and Dmitry Taubinsky.
    [Show full text]
  • The Threat of Appearing Prejudiced and Race-Based Attentional Biases Jennifer A
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Threat of Appearing Prejudiced and Race-Based Attentional Biases Jennifer A. Richeson and Sophie Trawalter Department of Psychology and C2S: The Center on Social Disparities and Health at the Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University ABSTRACT—The current work tested whether external heightened anxiety in anticipation of, as well as during, those motivation to respond without prejudice toward Blacks is interracial interactions that they are unable to avoid (Plant, associated with biased patterns of selective attention that 2004; Plant & Devine, 1998). Research by Amodio has found, reflect a threat response to Black individuals. In a dot- furthermore, that such high-EM individuals automatically probe attentional bias paradigm, White participants with evaluate Black targets more negatively than White targets, as low and high external motivation to respond without revealed by potentiated startle eye-blink amplitudes (Amodio, prejudice toward Blacks (i.e., low-EM and high-EM in- Harmon-Jones, & Devine, 2003), unlike people who are inter- dividuals, respectively) were presented with pairs of White nally motivated to respond in nonprejudiced ways toward and Black male faces that bore either neutral or happy Blacks. Taken together, this work suggests that exposure to facial expressions; on each trial, the faces were displayed Blacks automatically triggers negative affective reactions, in- for either 30 ms or 450 ms. The findings were consistent cluding heightened anxiety, in high-EM individuals. with those of previous research on threat and attention: Although Amodio’s work has begun to explore some of the High-EM participants revealed an attentional bias toward component processes that underlie differences between high- and neutral Black faces presented for 30 ms, but an attentional low-EM individuals’ affective and stereotypical evaluations of bias away from neutral Black faces presented for 450 ms.
    [Show full text]
  • Attentional Bias and Subjective Risk in Hypochondriacal Concern. Polly Beth Hitchcock Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses Graduate School 1993 Attentional Bias and Subjective Risk in Hypochondriacal Concern. Polly Beth Hitchcock Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses Recommended Citation Hitchcock, Polly Beth, "Attentional Bias and Subjective Risk in Hypochondriacal Concern." (1993). LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses. 5641. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/5641 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Reasoning and Parent-Based Reasoning in Normal Children
    Morren, M., Muris, P., Kindt, M. Emotional reasoning and parent-based reasoning in normal children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development: 35, 2004, nr. 1, p. 3-20 Postprint Version 1.0 Journal website http://www.springerlink.com/content/105587/ Pubmed link http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dop t=Abstract&list_uids=15626322&query_hl=45&itool=pubmed_docsum DOI 10.1023/B:CHUD.0000039317.50547.e3 Address correspondence to M. Morren, Department of Medical, Clinical, and Experimental Psychology, Maastricht University, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands; e-mail: [email protected]. Emotional Reasoning and Parent-based Reasoning in Normal Children MATTIJN MORREN, MSC; PETER MURIS, PHD; MEREL KINDT, PHD DEPARTMENT OF MEDICAL, CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, MAASTRICHT UNIVERSITY, THE NETHERLANDS ABSTRACT: A previous study by Muris, Merckelbach, and Van Spauwen1 demonstrated that children display emotional reasoning irrespective of their anxiety levels. That is, when estimating whether a situation is dangerous, children not only rely on objective danger information but also on their own anxiety-response. The present study further examined emotional reasoning in children aged 7–13 years (N =508). In addition, it was investigated whether children also show parent-based reasoning, which can be defined as the tendency to rely on anxiety-responses that can be observed in parents. Children completed self-report questionnaires of anxiety, depression, and emotional and parent-based reasoning. Evidence was found for both emotional and parent-based reasoning effects. More specifically, children’s danger ratings were not only affected by objective danger information, but also by anxiety-response information in both objective danger and safety stories.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Effects of State and Trait Anxiety on Selective Attention To
    International Journal of Psychological Research ISSN: 2011-2084 [email protected] Universidad de San Buenaventura Colombia Ortega Marín, Jeniffer; Jiménez Solanilla, Karim; Acosta Barreto, Rocio Effects of state and trait anxiety on selective attention to threatening stimuli in a non-clinical sample of school children International Journal of Psychological Research, vol. 8, núm. 1, 2015, pp. 75-89 Universidad de San Buenaventura Medellín, Colombia Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=299033832007 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative INT.J.PSYCHOL.RES. 2015; 8 (1): 75-90 Effects of state and trait anxiety on selective attention to threatening stimuli in a non- clinical sample of school children Efectos de la ansiedad estado-rasgo sobre la atención selectiva a estímulos amenazantes en una muestra no clínica de niños escolarizados R e s e a r c h a a a, Jeniffer Ortega Marín , Karim Jiménez Solanilla , and Rocio Acosta Barreto * a Faculty of Psychology, Universidad San Buenaventura,Bogotá, Colombia. ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Attentional biases, consisting of a preferential processing of threatening stimuli, have been found in anxious adults as predicted by several cognitive models. However, Article history: studies with non-clinical samples of children have provided mixed results. Therefore, Received: 30-07-2014 the aim of this research was to determine the effects of state and trait anxiety on the Revised: 28-08-2014 selective attention towards threatening stimuli in a non-clinical sample of school Accepted: 10-12-2014 children (age: 8 to 13, n = 110) using the dot-probe task.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: a Systematic Mapping Study
    Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: A Systematic Mapping Study Rahul Mohanani, Iflaah Salman, Burak Turhan, Member, IEEE, Pilar Rodriguez and Paul Ralph Abstract—One source of software project challenges and failures is the systematic errors introduced by human cognitive biases. Although extensively explored in cognitive psychology, investigations concerning cognitive biases have only recently gained popularity in software engineering research. This paper therefore systematically maps, aggregates and synthesizes the literature on cognitive biases in software engineering to generate a comprehensive body of knowledge, understand state of the art research and provide guidelines for future research and practise. Focusing on bias antecedents, effects and mitigation techniques, we identified 65 articles (published between 1990 and 2016), which investigate 37 cognitive biases. Despite strong and increasing interest, the results reveal a scarcity of research on mitigation techniques and poor theoretical foundations in understanding and interpreting cognitive biases. Although bias-related research has generated many new insights in the software engineering community, specific bias mitigation techniques are still needed for software professionals to overcome the deleterious effects of cognitive biases on their work. Index Terms—Antecedents of cognitive bias. cognitive bias. debiasing, effects of cognitive bias. software engineering, systematic mapping. 1 INTRODUCTION OGNITIVE biases are systematic deviations from op- knowledge. No analogous review of SE research exists. The timal reasoning [1], [2]. In other words, they are re- purpose of this study is therefore as follows: curring errors in thinking, or patterns of bad judgment Purpose: to review, summarize and synthesize the current observable in different people and contexts. A well-known state of software engineering research involving cognitive example is confirmation bias—the tendency to pay more at- biases.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • Attentional Bias Components in Anxiety and Depression: a Systematic Review
    Attentional Bias Components in Anxiety and Depression: A Systematic Review Denise Rogersa, Edel Murphya, Sarah Jane Windersa & Ciara M. Greenea aSchool of Psychology, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland Corresponding author: Associate Professor Ciara M. Greene Email: [email protected] Postal address: School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland Tel: +353 1 7168334 1 Abstract Attentional biases have been identified as a transdiagnostic cognitive process that may underlie a range of psychological disorders. They comprise three measurable and observable components; facilitation (rapid detection of concern-related stimuli), difficulty in disengagement (slower attentional shifting away from concern-related stimuli) and attentional avoidance (allocation of attention away from concern-related stimuli). Attentional biases to negative stimuli are common in anxiety and depression; however, their shared (i.e. transdiagnostic) and distinct components have not yet been systematically investigated. Literature searches were conducted on the PsychINFO, Embase, PubMed and Web of Science databases, yielding 560 articles after duplicates were removed. Articles were subject to abstract and full-text screening against study eligibility criteria. Twenty-five articles were included in the extraction phase. Data regarding population, experimental paradigm and attentional bias components were extracted. Results are suggestive of facilitation as a transdiagnostic attentional process across anxiety, depression and those with co- occurring anxiety and depression. There was strong evidence of avoidance in depression, with weaker evidence in anxiety, while delayed disengagement was observed in anxiety, but not in depression. Critical gaps in the literature were also identified. These findings provide support for the transdisagnostic nature of attentional biases, shedding light on the commonalities observed across diagnostic categories.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of State and Trait Anxiety on Selective Attention to Threatening
    INT.J.PSYCHOL.RES. 2015; 8 (1): 75-90 Effects of state and trait anxiety on selective attention to threatening stimuli in a non- clinical sample of school children Efectos de la ansiedad estado-rasgo sobre la atención selectiva a estímulos amenazantes en una muestra no clínica de niños escolarizados R e s e a r c h a a a, Jeniffer Ortega Marín , Karim Jiménez Solanilla , and Rocio Acosta Barreto * a Faculty of Psychology, Universidad San Buenaventura,Bogotá, Colombia. ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Attentional biases, consisting of a preferential processing of threatening stimuli, have been found in anxious adults as predicted by several cognitive models. However, Article history: studies with non-clinical samples of children have provided mixed results. Therefore, Received: 30-07-2014 the aim of this research was to determine the effects of state and trait anxiety on the Revised: 28-08-2014 selective attention towards threatening stimuli in a non-clinical sample of school Accepted: 10-12-2014 children (age: 8 to 13, n = 110) using the dot-probe task. This study did not reveal an effect of trait anxiety on selective attention towards threatening stimuli. However, a Key words: significant difference was found between participants with low state anxiety and high attentional bias, dot- state anxiety. Nevertheless, the effect size was small. Specifically, participants with low probe task, trait state anxiety showed a bias towards threatening stimuli. Overall, the findings of this anxiety, state anxiety, research with a non-clinical sample of school children suggest that attentional biases non-clinical sample, towards threatening information, which has been repeatedly found in anxious adults, and schoolchildren.
    [Show full text]
  • Attentional Bias and Its Temporal Dynamics Among War Veterans Suffering from Chronic Pain: Investigating the Contribution of Post-Traumatic Stress Symptoms
    Attentional bias and its temporal dynamics among war veterans suffering from chronic pain: investigating the contribution of post-traumatic stress symptoms Mahdi Mazidi a,b, Kelsey Vig c,Seyran Ranjbar d,Mohammad-Reza Ebrahimi e, Ali Khatibi f* a Centre for the Advancement of Research on Emotion, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia b Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran c Department of Psychology, University of Regina, Regina, SK, Canada d Psychology Department, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran e Military Psychiatry Center, 505 Psychiatric hospital, AJA University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran f Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada * Corresponding Author: Ali Khatibi, [email protected], address: NW141, Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, Canada, Phone: +1-514-398-5564 Funding sources: This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Conflict of interests: Authors declare no conflict of interest regarding the current submission 1 Abstract Background: Cognitive models propose that attentional dysregulation, including an attentional bias towards threat, is one of the factors through which chronic pain and post- traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) maintain and exacerbate one another. The current investigation assessed the attentional bias for painful facial expressions and its relationship with PTSS, using both traditional and variability-based attentional bias measures, among veterans with chronic pain and PTSS and controls. Method: Fifty-four veterans with chronic pain and 30 age/education-matched controls participated in this investigation. Participants completed a self-report measure of PTSS and a modified version of the dot-probe task with painful, happy, and neutral facial expressions.
    [Show full text]