ATINER's Conference Paper Series LNG2017-2259
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LNG2014-1176 Athens Institute for Education and Research ATINER ATINER's Conference Paper Series LNG2017-2259 The Proof is in the Pronoun: Grammatical and Semantic Gender in Anglo Saxon John M. Ryan Associate Professor of Spanish Linguistics Department of Hispanic Studies University of Northern Colorado USA 1 ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LIT2017-2258 An Introduction to ATINER's Conference Paper Series ATINER started to publish this conference papers series in 2012. It includes only the papers submitted for publication after they were presented at one of the conferences organized by our Institute every year. This paper has been peer reviewed by at least two academic members of ATINER. Dr. Gregory T. Papanikos President Athens Institute for Education and Research This paper should be cited as follows: Ryan, J.M. (2017). “The Proof is in the Pronoun: Grammatical and Semantic Gender in Anglo Saxon”, Athens: ATINER'S Conference Paper Series, No: LNG2017-2259. Athens Institute for Education and Research 8 Valaoritou Street, Kolonaki, 10671 Athens, Greece Tel: + 30 210 3634210 Fax: + 30 210 3634209 Email: [email protected] URL: www.atiner.gr URL Conference Papers Series: www.atiner.gr/papers.htm Printed in Athens, Greece by the Athens Institute for Education and Research. All rights reserved. Reproduction is allowed for non-commercial purposes if the source is fully acknowledged. ISSN: 2241-2891 07/09/2017 2 ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LIT2017-2258 The Proof is in the Pronoun: Grammatical and Semantic Gender in Anglo Saxon John M. Ryan Associate Professor of Spanish Linguistics Department of Hispanic Studies University of Northern Colorado USA Abstract In the literature on gender systems, anaphoric pronouns are commonly given the proverbial backseat to nouns and even nominal modifiers such as adjectives; however, as this paper will point out, in the case of Old English, the pronominal system played a major role in the development of the gender system that we employ today. Following Curzan (2003), who draws on the Helsinki corpus of Old English texts, this paper will demonstrate how the pronominal gender system in Old English, like its Modern counterpart today, might very well serve as a window to the mind of the Old English speaker and what s/he perceived as "natural" gender. I will show how the pronominal gender system of Old English has in actuality evolved very little into Modern English. I will argue that in Old English for nouns there were two very distinct gender systems: 1) grammatical (e.g., wif [neuter] "woman"); and 2) semantic (or "natural") (e.g., modor [feminine] "mother"), that operated simultaneously. For Old English pronouns, however, I suggest a more complex gender system than for nouns, one that: 1) preferred/selected "natural" gender pronouns for human or human-like referents despite the grammatical gender of the referent; and 2) preferred/selected grammatical gender for non-human or inanimate referents throughout the Old English period, ultimately gravitating toward overall preference for natural, or semantic, gender by the early Middle English period. I will term this system pronominal complexity and show how this has major implications on how natural gender in Modern English evolved. Keywords: Diachronic linguistics, Gender, Old English, Pronouns Acknowledgments: The author wishes to thank the University of Northern Colorado for a 2017 Provost Travel Award so that he could present a portion of this paper at the 10th Annual International Conference on Languages and Linguistics in Athens, Greece. 3 ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LIT2017-2258 Introduction Languages that sprout from the Indo-European family tree have descended into modern times with differing arrangements of gender or noun classes1. Some have retained all three Late Proto-Indo-European (LPIE) grammatical genders of masculine, feminine, and neuter. Others, for varying reasons, have lost one of the three. Still other languages, like Modern English (and the reason for this paper), have lost all their historical affiliation with grammatical gender in nouns and assumed a "natural" interpretation of gender as evidenced when they appear with corresponding anaphoric pronouns2, as exemplified in (1) through (3): 1. John (masculine referent) bought his (masculine anaphoric pronoun) car in Florida. 2. Mary (feminine referent) bought her (feminine anaphoric pronoun) car in Philadelphia. 3. John bought a car (neuter referent) and lent it (neuter anaphoric pronoun) to Mary. This paper has four goals: 1) to provide a typological framework for gender systems across multiple languages, starting with a comparison among languages overall, to be followed by a study of the Indo-European languages, and finishing specifically with the Germanic languages; 2) to investigate the historical shift from grammatical to natural gender in English, in both nouns and pronouns; 3) to argue for a similarity between the gender systems of Early Proto-Indo-European and Modern English with regards to semantic "transparency"; and 4) to conclude with a summary of findings and to suggest whether certain gender shifts that have happened diachronically across languages have been either semantically or socially motivated, or both. Most importantly, in terms of gender in Old English, this paper will argue how consideration of the pronominal gender system of Old English, like its Modern counterpart today, is necessary to the overall picture of gender and might very well serve as a window to the mind of the Old English speaker and what s/he perceived as "natural" gender. 1 In this paper it is important to distinguish between the terms "gender" and "noun class". Gender distinctions have some basis, either natural or arbitrary, on sex; and 2) Noun class distinctions have no basis whatsoever on sex. As Matasovic (2004) points out, noun class systems across world languages have been found to be based on other such concepts as animacy, shape, etc. This dichotomy allows us to specify systems in languages that possess large numbers of noun classes. 2 Exceptions to this generality are few (e.g., anaphoric "she" referring to ships, machines, and countries, and, as Matasovic points out, "he" or "she" referring to pets, although in my opinion this does not break the natural gender rule because this use does indeed correspond to the natural sex of the particular pet). 4 ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LIT2017-2258 Typology and Work on Gender Systems Typology According to Gender Linguists have always been interested in gender as a grammatical category since the earliest days of inquiry, having first been treated by the Greeks such as Pythagoras3, Aristotle, and Dionysius Thrax. The past thirty years have witnessed more comparative studies of gender systems across languages. Undoubtedly, the two most important and exhaustive of these are the pioneering work of Corbett (1991) and more recently, Matasovic (2004). Corbett’s treatment of gender is a seminal work from a typological standpoint. In his work, Corbett both: 1) defines gender for purposes of typological comparison; and 2) establishes five (5) typological parameters that can be used to compare gender systems across languages. According to Corbett, gender is unlike other grammatical categories such as case or number that can vary according to the particular need of the speaker or affect the meaning of the noun in some way. Corbett defines gender as a unique grammatical category, again unlike case or number, that has to be specified in a person’s lexicon, either by general assignment principles or individually for specific nouns. Corbett further explains that one can spot gender systems in languages where nouns tend to have the following two properties: 1) nominal classification—whereby nouns of a language typically belong to a closed set of noun classes; and 2) agreement—whereby the gender of the noun determines the form that other words take in the sentence, thereby forming some kind of syntactic unit. Corbett goes on to explain that some languages have gender and others do not. He further specifies that for all languages having gender systems there exists a semantic core of nouns for which gender can be predicted solely from meaning. All other nouns of a given language, for which gender cannot be predicted from meaning alone, are considered part of the semantic residue. Languages differ widely as to the extent to which nouns belong to the semantic core or the semantic residue. Some languages, such as the Dravidian languages, have all their nouns exclusively within the semantic core so in every case gender can be predicted by meaning. Other languages for the most part do not work this way, and their nouns are divided in some ratio between the core and residue. According to Corbett, another important concept related to the notion of semantic core and residue is that of gender assignment, whether on a semantic basis for nouns in the core, or formally (morphologically or phonologically) for nouns in the residue. In other words, the gender of nouns in the semantic core can be predicted, as we have already seen, on the basis of meaning, whereas the gender of those in the residue can be predicted to the noun class or declension they belong to. Of course, there are always exceptions where nouns 3 Pythagoras was the first to classify nouns according to notions of gender. 5 ATINER CONFERENCE PAPER SERIES No: LIT2017-2258 may be formally declined according to a specific class and have a gender atypical of that class. Drawing on both Corbett and Matasovic, Table 1 provides five typological parameters to help compare gender systems across languages: Table 1. Typological Parameters for Gender Systems Source: Corbett 1991, Matasovic 2004. These parameters provide some tools that can help to make typological assessments regarding gender about any language. Gender around the World Perhaps the most basic task of a typological review of language gender systems is an overall distinction between languages or language families as to whether those languages have gender or not.