Likelihood Ratios: a Simple and Flexible Statistic for Empirical Psychologists

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Likelihood Ratios: a Simple and Flexible Statistic for Empirical Psychologists Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 791-806 Likelihood ratios: A simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists SCOTT GLOVER Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania and PETER DIXON University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Empirical studies in psychology typically employ null hypothesis significance testing to draw statis- tical inferences. We propose that likelihood ratios are a more straightforward alternative to this ap- proach. Likelihood ratios provide a measure of the fit of two competing models; the statistic repre- sents a direct comparison of the relative likelihood of the data, given the best fit of the two models. Likelihood ratios offer an intuitive, easily interpretable statistic that allows the researcher great flexi- bility in framing empirical arguments. In support of this position, we report the results of a survey of empirical articles in psychology, in which the common uses of statistics by empirical psychologists is examined. From the results of this survey, we show that likelihood ratios are able to serve all the im- portant statistical needs of researchers in empirical psychology in a format that is more straightforward and easier to interpret than traditional inferential statistics. A likelihood ratio statistic reflects the relative likeli- LIKELIHOOD RATIOS hood of the data, given two competing models. Likelihood ratios provide an intuitive approach to summarizing the A likelihood ratio can be thought of as a comparison of evidence provided by an experiment. Because they de- two statistical models of the observed data. Each model scribe evidence, rather than embody a decision, they can provides a probability density for the observations and a easily be adapted to the various goals for which inferential set of unknown parameters that can be estimated from the statistics might be used. In contrast, the logic of null hy- data. In a broad range of common situations, the density pothesis significance testing is often at odds with the goals is the multivariate normal distribution, and the param- of the researcher, and that approach, despite its common eters are the means in the various conditions, together usage, is generally ill suited for the varied purposes to with the error variance. The two models differ in terms which it is put. of the constraints on the condition means. For example, We develop our thesis in three sections. In the first sec- a model in which two condition means differ might be tion, we provide an introduction to the use of likelihood ra- compared with one in which the means are identical. The tios as model comparisons and describe how they relate to match of each model to the observations can be indexed more traditional statistics. In the second section, we report by calculating the likelihood of the data, given the best the results of a small survey in which we identify some of estimates of the model parameters: The more likely the the common goals of significance testing in empirical psy- data are, the better the match. In this case, the best pa- chology. In the third section, we describe how likelihood rameter estimates are those that maximize the likelihood ratios can be used to achieve each of these goals more di- of the data, which are termed maximum-likelihood esti- rectly than traditional significance tests. mates. The ratio of two such likelihoods is the maximum likelihood ratio; it provides an index of the relative match of the two models to the observed data. Formally, the likelihood ratio can be written as The present work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engi- ˆ f ()X |q2 neering Research Council of Canada through a fellowship to the first l = , (1) author and a grant to the second author. The authors thank Michael Lee, f X |qˆ Geoff Loftus, and an anonymous reviewer for their insightful comments ()1 on previous versions of this article. Correspondence concerning this ar- where f is the probability density, X is the vector of ob- ticle should be addressed to S. Glover, Department of Psychology, ˆ ˆ Royal Holloway University of London, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, servations, andq1 andq 2 are the vectors of parameter esti- England (e-mail: [email protected]). mates that maximize the likelihood under the two models. 791 Copyright 2004 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 792 GLOVER AND DIXON Figure 1. Comparison of linear (middle panel) and quadratic (right panel) model fits of a theoretical data set. Error bars indicate the standard deviations of the observations under the best-fitting model; for clar- ity, only six are shown. Viewing statistical inference as model comparison is a described by the correlation between the predicted and the well-established perspective. Judd and McClelland (1989), observed values, and the value of R2 for each model is in- for example, organized an entire introductory textbook dicated in the figure. The standard deviation shown by the around this approach. Furthermore, the use of likelihood error bars is an index of the residual variance that is not ex- ratios in statistical inference is common (e.g., Edwards, plained by the model and is proportional to ÷(1ϪR2). The 1972; Royall, 1997), and the role of likelihood in model error bars are shown on the curve to indicate that the esti- comparison is well established (e.g., Akaike, 1973; mate depends on which model is being fit (cf. Estes, 1997). Schwartz, 1978). Furthermore, the likelihood ratio plays a It appears from Figure 1 that the data are more likely pivotal role in most approaches to hypothesis testing. In given the best-fitting quadratic model on the right than Bayesian hypothesis testing, the posterior odds of two hy- given the best-fitting linear model. This is reflected in potheses are related to the products of the likelihood ratio the smaller deviations from the predicted values and the and the prior odds (e.g., Sivia, 1996). In the decision- larger value of R2. As a consequence, 1ϪR2 and the es- theoretic approach advocated by Neyman and Pearson timate of the standard error are smaller for the quadratic (1928, 1933), any suitable decision rule can be viewed as model than for the linear model. In fact, the likelihood is a decision based on the value of the likelihood ratio. related to the inverse of the standard deviation. The ratio Fisher (1955) advocated the use of the log likelihood ratio of the likelihoods thus indexes the relative quality of the as an index of the evidence against a null hypothesis. In two fits, and with normally distributed data, one can all of these approaches, the likelihood ratio represents the write the likelihood ratio as evidence provided by the data with respect to two mod- nn n els. Although the form of the likelihood ratio varies to Ê 1 s2 ˆ 2 Ê s2 ˆ 2 Ê1- R2 ˆ 2 l = 2 = 1 = 1 , (2) some extent in these different approaches, they all have a Á 2 ˜ Á 2 ˜ Á 2 ˜ Ë 1 s ¯ Ë s ¯ Ë1- R ¯ common conceptual basis. 1 2 2 Consider the hypothetical data shown in Figure 1. The where s1 and s2 are the two estimates of the standard de- 2 2 data points in each panel represent the effects of an inde- viation, n is the number of observations, and R1 and R2 pendent variable X on a dependent variable Y. The straight describe the quality of the fits of the two models. (A line in the middle panel indicates the best fitting linear proof is provided in Appendix A.) In this example, the R2 model of the 21 observations, whereas the line in the right values are .689 and .837, and the value of the likelihood panel shows the fit of a more complicated model that in- ratio is 862.6. In simple terms, this means that the data cludes a quadratic component. The fit of the models can be are 862.6 times as likely to occur if the second model LIKELIHOOD RATIOS 793 (and its best-fitting parameter values) is true than if the searcher is indifferent a priori. However, a likelihood first model (and its best-fitting parameter values) is true. ratio of that magnitude could not be regarded as very The 1ϪR2 terms correspond to residual variation that strong evidence one way or the other and should not be is not explained by each model. Thus, an equivalent de- persuasive if there were other reasons to prefer Model 1. scription of the likelihood is A comparable but somewhat different index can be de- n rived using principles of Bayesian inference. The alter- Ê Model 1 unexplained variationˆ 2 native index is referred to as the BIC (Schwartz, 1978), l = Á ˜ . (3) Ë Model 2 unexplained variation¯ and is defined as This conceptual formula provides a straightforward ap- BIC = -2ln(lkn )+ ln( ), (7) proach to calculating likelihood ratios for many kinds of where n is the sample size. Using this criterion, Model 2 model comparisons. In particular, when the models in should be preferred over Model 1 when l ϭ Q l is question are linear, the requisite information about un- B B greater than 1, where Q is explained variation can typically be found in a factorial B analysis of variance (ANOVA) table. ln(n ) 2 QkkB = []exp()12- . (8) Correcting for Model Complexity In this case, lB can be viewed as an estimate of the Bayes- Although arguments based on this form of the likeli- ian posterior odds of the two models, assuming uninfor- hood ratio are sometimes possible, there is an obvious mative priors. problem apparent in this example: Because the quadratic Pitt, Myung, and Zhang (2002) have argued convinc- model includes an extra parameter, it will always fit the ingly that the number of parameters as captured by these data better than the linear model, regardless of what re- indices is only one aspect of model flexibility and that sults are obtained.
Recommended publications
  • WHAT DID FISHER MEAN by an ESTIMATE? 3 Ideas but Is in Conflict with His Ideology of Statistical Inference
    Submitted to the Annals of Applied Probability WHAT DID FISHER MEAN BY AN ESTIMATE? By Esa Uusipaikka∗ University of Turku Fisher’s Method of Maximum Likelihood is shown to be a proce- dure for the construction of likelihood intervals or regions, instead of a procedure of point estimation. Based on Fisher’s articles and books it is justified that by estimation Fisher meant the construction of likelihood intervals or regions from appropriate likelihood function and that an estimate is a statistic, that is, a function from a sample space to a parameter space such that the likelihood function obtained from the sampling distribution of the statistic at the observed value of the statistic is used to construct likelihood intervals or regions. Thus Problem of Estimation is how to choose the ’best’ estimate. Fisher’s solution for the problem of estimation is Maximum Likeli- hood Estimate (MLE). Fisher’s Theory of Statistical Estimation is a chain of ideas used to justify MLE as the solution of the problem of estimation. The construction of confidence intervals by the delta method from the asymptotic normal distribution of MLE is based on Fisher’s ideas, but is against his ’logic of statistical inference’. Instead the construc- tion of confidence intervals from the profile likelihood function of a given interest function of the parameter vector is considered as a solution more in line with Fisher’s ’ideology’. A new method of cal- culation of profile likelihood-based confidence intervals for general smooth interest functions in general statistical models is considered. 1. Introduction. ’Collected Papers of R.A.
    [Show full text]
  • Statistical Theory
    Statistical Theory Prof. Gesine Reinert November 23, 2009 Aim: To review and extend the main ideas in Statistical Inference, both from a frequentist viewpoint and from a Bayesian viewpoint. This course serves not only as background to other courses, but also it will provide a basis for developing novel inference methods when faced with a new situation which includes uncertainty. Inference here includes estimating parameters and testing hypotheses. Overview • Part 1: Frequentist Statistics { Chapter 1: Likelihood, sufficiency and ancillarity. The Factoriza- tion Theorem. Exponential family models. { Chapter 2: Point estimation. When is an estimator a good estima- tor? Covering bias and variance, information, efficiency. Methods of estimation: Maximum likelihood estimation, nuisance parame- ters and profile likelihood; method of moments estimation. Bias and variance approximations via the delta method. { Chapter 3: Hypothesis testing. Pure significance tests, signifi- cance level. Simple hypotheses, Neyman-Pearson Lemma. Tests for composite hypotheses. Sample size calculation. Uniformly most powerful tests, Wald tests, score tests, generalised likelihood ratio tests. Multiple tests, combining independent tests. { Chapter 4: Interval estimation. Confidence sets and their con- nection with hypothesis tests. Approximate confidence intervals. Prediction sets. { Chapter 5: Asymptotic theory. Consistency. Asymptotic nor- mality of maximum likelihood estimates, score tests. Chi-square approximation for generalised likelihood ratio tests. Likelihood confidence regions. Pseudo-likelihood tests. • Part 2: Bayesian Statistics { Chapter 6: Background. Interpretations of probability; the Bayesian paradigm: prior distribution, posterior distribution, predictive distribution, credible intervals. Nuisance parameters are easy. 1 { Chapter 7: Bayesian models. Sufficiency, exchangeability. De Finetti's Theorem and its intepretation in Bayesian statistics. { Chapter 8: Prior distributions. Conjugate priors.
    [Show full text]
  • Estimation Methods in Multilevel Regression
    Newsom Psy 526/626 Multilevel Regression, Spring 2019 1 Multilevel Regression Estimation Methods for Continuous Dependent Variables General Concepts of Maximum Likelihood Estimation The most commonly used estimation methods for multilevel regression are maximum likelihood-based. Maximum likelihood estimation (ML) is a method developed by R.A.Fisher (1950) for finding the best estimate of a population parameter from sample data (see Eliason,1993, for an accessible introduction). In statistical terms, the method maximizes the joint probability density function (pdf) with respect to some distribution. With independent observations, the joint probability of the distribution is a product function of the individual probabilities of events, so ML finds the likelihood of the collection of observations from the sample. In other words, it computes the estimate of the population parameter value that is the optimal fit to the observed data. ML has a number of preferred statistical properties, including asymptotic consistency (approaches the parameter value with increasing sample size), efficiency (lower variance than other estimators), and parameterization invariance (estimates do not change when measurements or parameters are transformed in allowable ways). Distributional assumptions are necessary, however, and there are potential biases in significance tests when using ML. ML can be seen as a more general method that encompasses ordinary least squares (OLS), where sample estimates of the population mean and regression parameters are equivalent for the two methods under regular conditions. ML is applied more broadly across statistical applications, including categorical data analysis, logistic regression, and structural equation modeling. Iterative Process For more complex problems, ML is an iterative process [for multilevel regression, usually Expectation- Maximization (EM) or iterative generalized least squares (IGLS) is used] in which initial (or “starting”) values are used first.
    [Show full text]
  • Akaike's Information Criterion
    ESCI 340 Biostatistical Analysis Model Selection with Information Theory "Far better an approximate answer to the right question, which is often vague, than an exact answer to the wrong question, which can always be made precise." − John W. Tukey, (1962), "The future of data analysis." Annals of Mathematical Statistics 33, 1-67. 1 Problems with Statistical Hypothesis Testing 1.1 Indirect approach: − effort to reject null hypothesis (H0) believed to be false a priori (statistical hypotheses are not the same as scientific hypotheses) 1.2 Cannot accommodate multiple hypotheses (e.g., Chamberlin 1890) 1.3 Significance level (α) is arbitrary − will obtain "significant" result if n large enough 1.4 Tendency to focus on P-values rather than magnitude of effects 2 Practical Alternative: Direct Evaluation of Multiple Hypotheses 2.1 General Approach: 2.1.1 Develop multiple hypotheses to answer research question. 2.1.2 Translate each hypothesis into a model. 2.1.3 Fit each model to the data (using least squares, maximum likelihood, etc.). (fitting model ≅ estimating parameters) 2.1.4 Evaluate each model using information criterion (e.g., AIC). 2.1.5 Select model that performs best, and determine its likelihood. 2.2 Model Selection Criterion 2.2.1 Akaike Information Criterion (AIC): relates information theory to maximum likelihood ˆ AIC = −2loge[L(θ | data)]+ 2K θˆ = estimated model parameters ˆ loge[L(θ | data)] = log-likelihood, maximized over all θ K = number of parameters in model Select model that minimizes AIC. 2.2.2 Modification for complex models (K large relative to sample size, n): 2K(K +1) AIC = −2log [L(θˆ | data)]+ 2K + c e n − K −1 use AICc when n/K < 40.
    [Show full text]
  • Plausibility Functions and Exact Frequentist Inference
    Plausibility functions and exact frequentist inference Ryan Martin Department of Mathematics, Statistics, and Computer Science University of Illinois at Chicago [email protected] July 6, 2018 Abstract In the frequentist program, inferential methods with exact control on error rates are a primary focus. The standard approach, however, is to rely on asymptotic ap- proximations, which may not be suitable. This paper presents a general framework for the construction of exact frequentist procedures based on plausibility functions. It is shown that the plausibility function-based tests and confidence regions have the desired frequentist properties in finite samples—no large-sample justification needed. An extension of the proposed method is also given for problems involving nuisance parameters. Examples demonstrate that the plausibility function-based method is both exact and efficient in a wide variety of problems. Keywords and phrases: Bootstrap; confidence region; hypothesis test; likeli- hood; Monte Carlo; p-value; profile likelihood. 1 Introduction In the Neyman–Pearson program, construction of tests or confidence regions having con- trol over frequentist error rates is an important problem. But, despite its importance, there seems to be no general strategy for constructing exact inferential methods. When an exact pivotal quantity is not available, the usual strategy is to select some summary statistic and derive a procedure based on the statistic’s asymptotic sampling distribu- tion. First-order methods, such as confidence regions based on asymptotic normality arXiv:1203.6665v4 [math.ST] 12 Mar 2014 of the maximum likelihood estimator, are known to be inaccurate in certain problems. Procedures with higher-order accuracy are also available (e.g., Brazzale et al.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Maximum Likelihood in R
    1 INTRODUCTION 1 An introduction to Maximum Likelihood in R Stephen P. Ellner ([email protected]) Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Cornell University Last compile: June 3, 2010 1 Introduction Maximum likelihood as a general approach to estimation and inference was created by R. A. Fisher between 1912 and 1922, starting with a paper written as a third-year undergraduate. Then, and for many years, it was more of theoretical than practical interest. Now, the ability to do nonlinear optimization on the computer has made likelihood methods practical and very popular. Let’s start with the probability density function for one observation x from normal random variable with mean µ and variance σ2, (x µ)2 1 − − f(x µ, σ)= e 2σ2 . (1) | √2πσ For a set of n replicate independent observations x1,x2, ,xn the joint density is ··· n f(x1,x2, ,xn µ, σ)= f(xi µ, σ) (2) ··· | i=1 | Y We interpret this as follows: given the values of µ and σ, equation (2) tells us the relative probability of different possible values of the observations. Maximum likelihood turns this around by defining the likelihood function n (µ, σ x1,x2, ,xn)= f(xi µ, σ) (3) L | ··· i=1 | Y The right-hand side is the same as (2) but the interpretation is different: given the observations, the function tells us the relative likelihood of different possible values of the parameters for L the process that generated the data. Note: the likelihood function is not a probability, and it does not specifying the relative probability of different parameter values.
    [Show full text]
  • SPATIAL and TEMPORAL MODELLING of WATER ACIDITY in TURKEY LAKES WATERSHED Spatial and Temporal Modelling of Water Acidity in Turkey Lakes Watershed
    SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL MODELLING OF WATER ACIDITY IN TURKEY LAKES WATERSHED Spatial and Temporal Modelling of Water Acidity in Turkey Lakes Watershed By Jing Lin, M.A. A Project Submitted to the School of Graduate Studies in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science McMaster University ©Copyright by Jing Lin, May, 2005 MASTER OF SCIENCE (2005) McMaster University (Statistics) Hamilton, Ontario TITLE: Spatial and Temporal Modelling for Thrkey Lakes Watershed Acidity AUTHOR: Jing Lin , M.A. (Shandong University, P. R. China) SUPERVISOR: Professor Abdel H. El-Shaarawi NUMBER OF PAGES: x, 74 ll Abstract Acid rain continues to be a major environmental problem. Canada has been monitoring indicators of acid rain in various ecosystems since the 1970s. This project focuses on the analysis of a selected subset of data generated by the Turkey Lakes Watershed (TLW) monitoring program from 1980 to 1997. TLW consists of a series of connected lakes where 6 monitoring stations are strategically located to measure the input from an upper stream lake into a down stream lake. Segment regression models with AR(1) errors and unknown point of change are used to summarize the data. Relative likelihood based methods are applied to estimate the point of change. For pH, all the regression parameters except autocorrelation have been found to change significantly between the model segments. This was not the case for so~- where a single model was found to be adequate. In addition pH has been found to have a moderate increasing trend and pronounced seasonality while so~- showed a dramatic decreasing trend but little seasonality.
    [Show full text]
  • P Values, Hypothesis Testing, and Model Selection: It’S De´Ja` Vu All Over Again1
    FORUM P values, hypothesis testing, and model selection: it’s de´ja` vu all over again1 It was six men of Indostan To learning much inclined, Who went to see the Elephant (Though all of them were blind), That each by observation Might satisfy his mind. ... And so these men of Indostan Disputed loud and long, Each in his own opinion Exceeding stiff and strong, Though each was partly in the right, And all were in the wrong! So, oft in theologic wars The disputants, I ween, Rail on in utter ignorance Of what each other mean, And prate about an Elephant Not one of them has seen! —From The Blind Men and the Elephant: A Hindoo Fable, by John Godfrey Saxe (1872) Even if you didn’t immediately skip over this page (or the entire Forum in this issue of Ecology), you may still be asking yourself, ‘‘Haven’t I seen this before? Do we really need another Forum on P values, hypothesis testing, and model selection?’’ So please bear with us; this elephant is still in the room. We thank Paul Murtaugh for the reminder and the invited commentators for their varying perspectives on the current shape of statistical testing and inference in ecology. Those of us who went through graduate school in the 1970s, 1980s, and 1990s remember attempting to coax another 0.001 out of SAS’s P ¼ 0.051 output (maybe if I just rounded to two decimal places ...), raising a toast to P ¼ 0.0499 (and the invention of floating point processors), or desperately searching the back pages of Sokal and Rohlf for a different test that would cross the finish line and satisfy our dissertation committee.
    [Show full text]
  • Maximum Likelihood Estimation ∗ Contents
    Statistical Inference: Maximum Likelihood Estimation ∗ Konstantin Kashin Spring óþÕ¦ Contents Õ Overview: Maximum Likelihood vs. Bayesian Estimationó ó Introduction to Maximum Likelihood Estimation¢ ó.Õ What is Likelihood and the MLE?..............................................¢ ó.ó Examples of Analytical MLE Derivations..........................................ß ó.ó.Õ MLE Estimation for Sampling from Bernoulli Distribution..........................ß ó.ó.ó MLE Estimation of Mean and Variance for Sampling from Normal Distribution.............. ó.ó.ì Gamma Distribution................................................. ó.ó.¦ Multinomial Distribution..............................................É ó.ó.¢ Uniform Distribution................................................ Õþ ì Properties of MLE: e Basics ÕÕ ì.Õ Functional Invariance of MLE................................................ÕÕ ¦ Fisher Information and Uncertainty of MLE Õó ¦.þ.Õ Example of Calculating Fisher Information: Bernoulli Distribution...................... Õ¦ ¦.Õ e eory of Fisher Information.............................................. Õß ¦.Õ.Õ Derivation of Unit Fisher Information...................................... Õß ¦.Õ.ó Derivation of Sample Fisher Information..................................... ÕÉ ¦.Õ.ì Relationship between Expected and Observed Fisher Information...................... ÕÉ ¦.Õ.¦ Extension to Multiple Parameters......................................... óþ ¢ Proofs of Asymptotic Properties of MLE óÕ ¢.Õ Consistency of MLE.....................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Ways of Our Errors
    The Ways of Our Errors Optimal Data Analysis for Beginners and Experts c Keith Horne Universit y of St.Andrews DRAFT June 5, 2009 Contents I Optimal Data Analysis 1 1 Probability Concepts 2 1.1 Fuzzy Numbers and Dancing Data Points . 2 1.2 Systematic and Statistical Errors { Bias and Variance . 3 1.3 Probability Maps . 3 1.4 Mean, Variance, and Standard Deviation . 5 1.5 Median and Quantiles . 6 1.6 Fuzzy Algebra . 7 1.7 Why Gaussians are Special . 8 1.8 Why χ2 is Special . 8 2 A Menagerie of Probability Maps 10 2.1 Boxcar or Uniform (ranu.for) . 10 2.2 Gaussian or Normal (rang.for) . 11 2.3 Lorentzian or Cauchy (ranl.for) . 13 2.4 Exponential (rane.for) . 14 2.5 Power-Law (ranpl.for) . 15 2.6 Schechter Distribution . 17 2.7 Chi-Square (ranchi2.for) . 18 2.8 Poisson . 18 2.9 2-Dimensional Gaussian . 19 3 Optimal Statistics 20 3.1 Optimal Averaging . 20 3.1.1 weighted average . 22 3.1.2 inverse-variance weights . 22 3.1.3 optavg.for . 22 3.2 Optimal Scaling . 23 3.2.1 The Golden Rule of Data Analysis . 25 3.2.2 optscl.for . 25 3.3 Summary . 26 3.4 Problems . 26 i ii The Ways of Our Errors c Keith Horne DRAFT June 5, 2009 4 Straight Line Fit 28 4.1 1 data point { degenerate parameters . 28 4.2 2 data points { correlated vs orthogonal parameters . 29 4.3 N data points . 30 4.4 fitline.for .
    [Show full text]
  • Profile Likelihood Estimation of the Vulnerability P(X>V) and the Mixing
    Revista Colombiana de Estadística Diciembre 2013, volumen 36, no. 2, pp. 193 a 208 Profile Likelihood Estimation of the Vulnerability P (X>v) and the Mixing Proportion p Parameters in the Gumbel Mixture Model Estimación de verosimilitud perfil de los parámetros de vulnerabilidad P (X>v) y proporción de mezcla p en el modelo Gumbel de mezclas José A. Montoya1;a, Gudelia Figueroa1;b, Nuša Pukšič2;c 1Departamento de Matemáticas, División de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Sonora, Hermosillo, México 2Institute of Metals and Technology, Ljubljana, Slovenia Abstract This paper concerns to the problem of making inferences about the vul- nerability θ = P (X>v) and the mixing proportion p parameters, when the random variable X is distributed as a mixture of two Gumbel distributions and v is a known fixed value. A profile likelihood approach is proposed for the estimation of these parameters. This approach is a powerful though sim- ple method for separately estimating a parameter of interest in the presence of unknown nuisance parameters. Inferences about θ, p or (θ; p) are given in terms of profile likelihood regions and can be easily obtained on a computer. This methodology is illustrated through a real problem where the main pur- pose is to model the size of non-metallic inclusions in steel. Key words: Invariance principle, Likelihood approach, Likelihood region, Mixture of distributions. Resumen En este artículo consideramos el problema de hacer inferencias sobre el parámetro de vulnerabilidad θ = P (X>v) y la proporción de mezcla p cuando X es una variable aleatoria cuya distribución es una mezcla de dos distribuciones Gumbel y v es un valor fijo y conocido.
    [Show full text]
  • Near Observational Equivalence and Persistence in GNP
    ~ear Observational Equivalence and © Persistence by Stephe~ R. BIo~gh ~ear ObservationalE’quwalence Persistence in G~%~P by Stephen R. Blough December 1994 Working Paper No. 94-6 Federal Reserve Bank of Boston Near Observational Equivalence and Persistence in GNP Stephen R. Blough" October 1994 "Research Department, Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, Boston, MA 02106-2076. I am indebted to Alicia Sasser for research assistance, and to Jeff Fuhrer for comments. This paper is a substantially revised and abridged version of Blough (1992b), which was written at Johns Hopkins University. The views in this paper are not necessarily endorsed by the Federal Reserve Bank of BoSton or the Federal Reserve System. The question of whether aggregate output is best described as a trend-stationary (TS) or as a difference-stationary (DS, or un1~ root) process continues to generate a substantial volume of research a dozen years after i~ was first raised by Nelson and Plosser (1982), including a recen~ paper by Rudebusch (1993). Rudebusch argues that "Based on the usual unit root tests, little can be said about the relative likelihood of the specific DS and TS models of real GNP." Rudebusch concludes by emphasizing "the importance of measuring the confidence intervals for estimates of persistence without conditioning on the TS or DS modeI." This paper provides a strong theoretical result on distinguishing TS and DS models, and gives confidence intervals for the GNP impulse response function that do not require such distinction. Theoretically, the paper shows that, in the absence of a prior~ specification restrictions, the classes of unit root and stationary processes are nearly observationally equivalent: no finite data sample can provide information on the TS/DS issue.
    [Show full text]