Collective Rhythm As an Emergent Property During Human Social Coordination
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Collective rhythm as an emergent property during human social coordination Arodi Farrera1, Gabriel Ramos-Fernández1,2 1Mathematical Modeling of Social Systems Department, Institute for Research on Applied Mathematics and Systems, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico. 2Corresponding author: [email protected] Abstract: The literature on social interactions has shown that participants coordinate not only at the behavioral but also at the physiological and neural levels, and that this coordination gives a temporal structure to the individual and to the social dynamics. However, it has not been fully explored whether such temporal patterns emerge during interpersonal coordination beyond dyads, whether this phenomenon arises from complex cognitive mechanisms or from relatively simple rules of behavior, or the sociocultural processes that underlie this phenomenon. We review the evidence for the existence of group-level rhythmic patterns that result from social interactions and argue that, by imposing a temporal structure at the individual and interaction levels, interpersonal coordination in groups leads to temporal regularities that cannot be predicted from the individual periodicities: a collective rhythm. Moreover, we use this interpretation of the literature to discuss how taking into account the sociocultural niche in which individuals develop can help explain the seemingly divergent results that have been reported on the social influences and consequences of interpersonal coordination. We make recommendations on further research to test these arguments and their relationship to the feeling of belonging and assimilation experienced during group dynamics. Keywords: Interpersonal coordination, Collective rhythm, Emergence, Spontaneous mimicry, Synchronization. 1. Introduction: During social activities, participants not only coordinate at the behavioral but also at the physiological and neural levels [Hoehl et al., 2020]. Moving in and out of this type of social interaction organizes the biological rhythms of the individual [Feldman, 2012] and gives a temporal structure to the collective dynamics. In humans, these coordination dynamics have been associated with prosocial behaviors [Gordon et al., 2020] and, more indirectly, with the feeling of belonging and assimilation to the group that people experience when participating in collective activities such as rituals [Mogan et al., 2017]. However, it has not been fully explored whether the rhythmic patterns emerging during interpersonal coordination beyond dyads arise from complex cognitive mechanisms or from relatively simple rules of behavior, or what can this phenomenon inform us about the social processes that participants face [see Boyer and Ramos-Fernandez, 2018]. Our aim in this contribution is to review the evidence for the existence of group-level rhythmic patterns that result from social interactions and their relationship with social processes. We start by reviewing two mechanisms that have been frequently used to explain patterns, both in time and form, of interpersonal coordination: spontaneous mimicry and synchrony [Bernieri and Rosenthal, 1991]. Next, we propose a framework on how both mechanisms may contribute to the temporal organization of social interactions at the individual and group levels, and how taking their social impact into account can help explain seemingly divergent results on their functional significance in the literature. The final section integrates the evidence to explore whether coordinated activities could lead to the emergence of a collective rhythm, its relationship to the group feeling of belonging and connection, and if such phenomena could be integrated into the constructed human niche. 1.1 Collective rhythm: The organization of behavioral events and its development over time build up the temporal structure of behavior [e.g. Ravignani et al., 2014]. In a single individual, complex behaviors are temporally structured by layers of multimodal signals [Pouw and Dixon, 2020] nested on different time scales [Abney et al. 2021]. For example, several occurrences of body movements and utterance activity produced at short scales (e.g. typing) can be grouped into larger time scales, in turn delimited by moments of no activity (e.g. writing a chapter of a manuscript in short bursts). Likewise, in even larger time scales, such ensembles of behavioral activity can be arranged in clusters depending on the constraints and contingencies of the various stages of the task at hand (e.g. planning, writing, evaluation). This type of temporal pattern (i.e. the duration and timing of events) of any series of behavioral or physiological activity builds the rhythm of the individual [Ravignani and Norton, 2017]. When several participants are involved, the temporal structure of the interaction contains additional layers of timing patterns [Figure 1: chorus behavior, sensu Ravignani et al., 2014]. For instance, during a conversation, behavioral activity is organized by turns taken by each participant, delimited by gaps. On larger timescales (e.g. complete conversation) the temporal pattern recovered may differ whether we consider the rhythm of one participant at a time, or the rhythm of all participants at the same time. It is this latter that would conform the social rhythm of a given group of individuals (Figure 1: the collective rhythm). Figure 1. Schematic representation of temporal rhythmic patterns. Visual examples of the onset time of behavioral events produced by two individuals are represented in red and orange. Examples in each category depict a series of behavioral activity over time (left to right). Coupled simultaneous patterns represent multiple individuals coordinating independently of joint plans, e.g. people clapping together. Coupled alternating patterns describe cases in which individuals coordinate with others due to planned joint action, like people coordinating turns during a conversation. Finally, uncoupled patterns are rhythms produced by individuals independently, like people dancing to different songs with headphones on. Collective rhythm shown in black is the temporal organization at large timescales of all the participants’ behavior. Notice that two coupled rhythmic patterns, with different individual rhythms (couple simultaneous and coupled alternating patterns) may exhibit the same collective rhythm. Figure adapted from Ravignani et al., 2014. Defined as such, we can recover the collective rhythm of a group of individuals who independently generate behavioral activity (Figure 1: uncoupled pattern), for example, pedestrians passing a footbridge. Additionally, this rhythmic structure can be recovered from groups in which individuals coordinate with others, either due to planned joint action (Figure 1: coupled alternating pattern) or independently of joint plans (Figure 1: coupled simultaneous pattern) [see Knoblich et al., 2011]. Notice that our definition of collective rhythm is distinguished from the collective rhythm in a group of people singing in unison or dancing, specifically because in the second case, the rhythm that is being followed has been previously specified. In the following, we present the evidence that suggests that in chorus behaviors, a collective rhythm may emerge, i.e. temporal regularities arising in groups that cannot be predicted from the individual periodicities, analogous to the emergent wave-like dynamics in a crowded stadium [Couzin, 2018]. Although different emergent forms of social coordination have been reported before [spontaneous coordination: Knoblich et al., 2011; synergy: Fusaroli et al., 2014; self-similarity: Abney et al. 2021], emergent rhythmic patterns have been explored mainly in dyads and on short timescales, which hinders the distinction of the local and global properties of collective behaviors [Bourbousson and Fortes-Bourbousson, 2016]. 2. Mechanisms As a first step in understanding collective rhythmic interaction as an emergent property, in this section we review two mechanisms that, in addition to being frequently related to coordination, have been studied in distantly related species. This perspective complements previous accounts of interpersonal coordination based on processes observed solely in contemporary humans [e.g. behavioral alignment: Rasenberg et al., 2020, interpersonal synergies: Fusaroli and Tylén, 2016, coordination: Clayton et al., 2020] and shows that comparative studies in different model organisms will help disentangle cause and effect of collective emergent phenomena in humans. 2.1. Behavior matching When coordinating with others during social interactions, we observe a tendency to spontaneously adopt the behaviors of interaction partners [Lakin et al., 2003] that can arise from fractions of a second (e.g. finger mimicry) to several seconds (e.g. yawning) after the stimulus [Arnold and Winkielman, 2019; Prochazkova and Kret, 2017]. Spontaneous mimicry is based on motor resonance, where the action of observing others activates neurons that represent that same action in the observer’s motor system [cf. Uithol et al., 2011, on controversies in the interpretation of motor resonance]. This phenomenon is considered to confer the ability to quickly communicate with other members of the group about important aspects of the physical and the social environment (e.g. physiological internal states like arousal due to food availability or fear due to predator presence). For instance, in different species of mammals, mimicry has been found to be associated