The Learning Environment for Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior Among the !Kung

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Learning Environment for Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior Among the !Kung University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Anthropology Faculty Publications Anthropology, Department of May 1978 The Learning Environment for Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior among the !Kung Patricia Draper University of Nebraska, Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/anthropologyfacpub Part of the Anthropology Commons Draper, Patricia, "The Learning Environment for Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior among the !Kung" (1978). Anthropology Faculty Publications. 12. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/anthropologyfacpub/12 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Anthropology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Anthropology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Learning Non-Aggression: The Experience of Non-Literate Societies, Ashley Mon- tagu, ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 1978. Pp. 31–53. Copyright © 1978 Ashley Montagu. Used by permission. [Chapter 3] The Learning Environment for Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior among the !Kung Patricia Draper The !Kung, a hunter-gatherer people of the Kalahari Desert, are of in- terest to this collection of writings on the teaching of non-aggression for a variety of reasons. They have been described as a “harmless people” by Thomas (1958) in a book-length account of the social life and cultural val- ues of !Kung who lived in South West Africa. An opposite characterization of !Kung emerges from an unpublished study by Richard Lee. This study, based on interviews and examination of genealogical records collected in the field, reports on incidents of homicide among !Kung. The murder rate, according to Lee, is rather frequent for a people purported to be harmless and unaggressive. Still other writers, dealing more generally with factors common to hunter-gathering and band-level peoples (Service, 1966; Hoe- bel, 1954, 1958; Lee and DeVore, eds., 1968) have pointed out that hunt- ing and gathering groups possess few formal mechanisms for dealing with social conflict. Such peoples typically rely on informal mechanisms of social control such as gossip, ridicule, sorcery, shunning, ostracism, and public de- bating which lead to the formation of consensus. In discussing the problem of social control in such societies, anthropologists show how conflicts are resolved circuitously and in a lengthy fashion. They point out that the aim 31 32 Patricia Draper in Learning Non-Aggression (1978) Learning Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior among the !Kung 33 of conflict resolution is not to place blame or necessarily to punish an of- different starting point in the ethnographic analysis. There are many exam- fender, but rather to restore amicable relations among individuals. ples of the “central cultural theme” approach. A few of the best known are The !Kung, therefore, are a provocative case study; a controversy exists Malinowski’s portrayal of reciprocity and exchange among the Trobriand as to whether they are harmless or, in fact, murderous. In addition, since Islanders (Malinowski, 1920, 1922), Chagnon’s treatment of aggression the !Kung are by now well studied, one can use specific information about and ritualized conflict in both film and written documents (Asch and Ch- !Kung behavior to examine general propositions about social control and agnon, 1970; Chagnon, 1977), and, of course, Ruth Benedict’s Patterns of interpersonal conflict in hunting and gathering societies generally. Culture (Benedict, 1934). This paper will address the question of interpersonal conflict and ag- The opposite approach, in which an anthropologist describes a culture gression among !Kung, with particular emphasis on the learning environ- by topic, treating different cultural systems in turn (e. g., religion, econom- ment of children and how it relates to the learning of aggressive behavior. ics, socialization, and so forth), is currently out of fashion. For all the mer- Of concern here will be parental attitudes toward children’s behavior and its of this more exhaustive and balanced strategy, it can render the rough, techniques for dealing with conflict. In the !Kung case there are many fac- crannied texture of social life into a flat, but admittedly easy to absorb, tors such as settlement pattern, economy, and the value of sharing which porridge. are less obviously related to values about child-rearing but which have sub- In writing an essay on aggression in !Kung life, one encounters some of stantial impact on the social and emotional climate in which children are the problems outlined above. Aggression, conflict, and violence—none of reared. This paper will describe, therefore, not only some aspects of child these are culturally elaborated preoccupations. Nor could one argue that a socialization that bear on the teaching of non-aggression, but also these central cultural theme is concerned with an opposite set of values—the en- other dimensions of social organization which are relevant to the ability of forcement of peace and the suppression of aggression. From this point of !Kung to discourage interpersonal aggression and to encourage group co- view, values about interpersonal aggression do not qualify as an especially operation. I will leave a discussion of the issue, “Are the !Kung unaggres- auspicious position from which to view the cultural terrain. Nevertheless, sive, aggressive, harmless, or murderous?” to the concluding section of this the !Kung are a people who devalue aggression; they have explicit values paper. against assaulting, losing control, and seeking to intimidate another person Before proceeding directly to the topic, there are a few caveats which by sheer force of personality. Furthermore, on a daily basis and over months should be laid before the reader. These concern the problems which an an- of fieldwork one finds that overt physical acts by one person against an- thropologist faces in presenting and interpreting information. other are extremely rare. In two years I personally observed three instances When anthropologists describe the exotic peoples with whom they in which people lost control and exchanged blows: two twelve-year-old girls have lived and studied, they often organize their analyses around themes who wrestled and fought with fists; two women who scratched and kicked or preoccupations which are themselves central to the people’s cultural life. each other over a man (the husband of one of the women); and two men This procedure can be a convenient and culturally sensitive vehicle for ex- who violently shoved each other back and forth, shouted and separated to position; the pivotal institution or set of values becomes the basis for show- gather weapons, only to be dissuaded by other people from their respec- ing connections among superficially discrete and independent cultural pro- tive camps. In a fourth case I saw two women who had fought the night be- cesses. A disadvantage of this approach is that it necessarily underplays fore. Lorna Marshall, an anthropologist with much experience among the other customs which could legitimately achieve equal prominence given a !Kung, makes a similar report: 34 Patricia Draper in Learning Non-Aggression (1978) Learning Aggression and Anti-Social Behavior among the !Kung 35 During seventeen and a half months of fieldwork with the Nyae system in what is now southern Angola, Botswana, and South West Africa, Nyae !Kung . , I personally saw only four flare-ups of discord and until recent times they subsisted by hunting and gathering. The great and heard about three others which occurred in neighboring majority of !Kung-speaking people of today have abandoned their tradi- bands during that period. All were resolved before they became tional foraging life style and are living in sedentary and semisquatter status serious quarrels. [Marshall, 1976, pp. 311-12] in or near villages of Bantu pastoralists and European ranchers. A minor- If the !Kung succeed in avoiding direct physical confrontation in most ity of !Kung, amounting to a few thousand people, are still living by tradi- instances, they clearly experience the same emotions which, in other societ- tional foraging techniques, and these are the people who are described in ies, would lead more quickly to hostile acts. The !Kung harbor hatreds, jeal- this paper. ousies, resentments, suspicions—the full panoply of negative emotions. In The few remaining groups of still mobile !Kung subsist on wild veg- fact, their oral traditions are remarkably violent and fratricidal for a people etable foods and game meat. They are seminomadic, moving their camps who, on the surface, maintain the appearance of simple communal harmony at irregular intervals, from a few days to several weeks. They live in small (Biesele, 1972 a, 1972b, 1975, 1976). The difference between the !Kung groups, averaging about thirty-five people, but these bands vary from as and other peoples is that the circumstances of their life are such that they few as fifteen to as many as sixty-five persons. The factors that affect group must dampen their passions to manageable levels or, that failing, separate size are chiefly season and the availability of water. During the rainy season themselves from the people whose society they cannot tolerate. Interest- (October to March), group censuses are lower, owing to the fact that water ingly, the !Kung themselves take on an edgy irascibility when their life style and bush foods are widely available in most regions of the desert. Smaller changes away from that which one sees in small mobile groups living in the numbers of people in the form of two and three family groups spread out bush and depending on foraging and hunting for subsistence (Lee, 1972a). over the bush. As the dry season nears and the temporary watering places The discussion in this paper will concern a group of about 120 bush-liv- dry up, the people begin to regroup and fall back on the remaining water ing !Kung whom I studied in 1968 and 1969.
Recommended publications
  • Iciumv LEADERS in ANIMAL BEHAVIOR the Second Generation
    UO1BJ3U3D iciumv LEADERS IN ANIMAL BEHAVIOR The Second Generation Edited by Lee C. Drickamer Northern Arizona University Donald A. Dewsbury University of Florida CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS 13 Myths, monkeys, and motherhood: a compromising life SARAH BLAFFER HRDY Definition of an Anthropologist: "(Someone) who studies human nature in all its diversity." Carmelo Lison-Tolosana (1966) Maternal effects (1946-64) From a young age, I was interested in why humans do what they do. With little exposure to science, certainly no inkling that there might be people in the world who studied other animals in order to better understand our species, I decided to become a novelist. Born in Leaders in Animal Behavior: The Setond Generation, ed. L. C. Drickamcr & D. A. Dewsbury. Published by Cambridge University Press. CO Cambridge University Press 2010. 344 Sarah Bluffer Hrdy Texas in 1946, right at the start of the postwar baby boom, I was the third of five children - Speedway. Prevailin four daughters and finally the long-awaited son. My father's father, R. L. Blaffer, had come segregation, and pi to Texas from Hamburg via New Orleans in 1901 at the time oil was discovered at interested in the e1 Spindletop. He recognized that fortunes would be made in the oil business. He married inheritance, female Sarah Campbell from Lampasas, whose father was in that business. I was named for her, the women's moven Sarah Campbell Blaffer II. My mother's father's ancestors, the Hardins, French Huguenots Reared by a suo from Tennessee, arrived earlier, in 1825, before Texas was even a state.
    [Show full text]
  • Ethno-Racial Attitudes and Social Inequality Editor's Proof
    Editor's Proof Ethno-Racial Attitudes and Social Inequality 22 Frank L. Samson and Lawrence D. Bobo 1 Introduction (1997)). Within psychology we have seen an ex- 22 plosion of work on implicit attitudes or uncon- 23 2 Sociologists ordinarily assume that social struc- scious racism that more than ever centers atten- 24 3 ture drives the content of individual level values, tion on the internal psychological functioning of 25 4 attitudes, beliefs, and ultimately, behavior. In the individual. We argue here that, in general, a 26 5 some classic models this posture reaches a point committed social psychological posture that ex- 27 6 of essentially denying the sociological relevance amines both how societal level factors and pro- 28 7 of any micro-level processes. In contrast, psy- cesses shape individual experiences and outlooks 29 8 chologists (and to a degree, economists) operate and how the distribution of individual attitudes, 30 9 with theoretical models that give primacy to in- beliefs, and values, in turn, influence others and 31 10 dividual level perception, cognition, motivation, the larger social environment provides the fullest 32 11 and choice. Within the domain of studies of ethno- leverage on understanding the dynamics of race. 33 12 racial relations, each of these positions has mod- Specifically we argue in this chapter that ethno-ra- 34 13 ern advocates. From the sociologically determin- cial attitudes, beliefs, and identities play a funda- 35 14 istic vantage point Edna Bonacich trumpets the mental constitutive role in the experience, re-pro- 36 15 “‘deeper’ level of reality” exposed by class ana- duction, and process of change in larger societal 37 16 lytics (1980, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Explaining the Exceptional Behaviour of the Portuguese Church Hierarchy in Morality Politics
    Shunning Direct Intervention: Explaining the Exceptional Behaviour of the Portuguese church Hierarchy in Morality Politics by Madalena Meyer Resende (FCSH-UNL and IPRI-UNL) and Anja Hennig (European University Viadrina) Abstract Why are the Catholic churches in most European countries politically active in relevant morality policy issues while the Portuguese hierarchy has remained reserved during mobilizing debates such as abortion and same-sex marriage, whose laws’ recent changes go against Catholic beliefs? The explanation could be institutional, as the fairly recent Portuguese transition to democracy dramatically changed the role attributed to the church by the former regimes. However, in Spain – whose case is similar to Portugal in matters of timing and political conditions – the hierarchy’s behaviour is different. This begs the question: what elements explain the exceptionality of the Portuguese case? This article shows that the Portuguese case illustrates an element usually not emphasized in the literature: the ideological inclination of the church elites. The article thus concludes that institutional access is a necessary, but not a sufficient, condition for the church to directly intervene in morality policy processes. A church may have access to influence political decision makers but, for ideological reasons, may be unwilling to use it. Keywords: Portugal, morality policy, Catholic church, Vatican Council II, abortion, gay-marriage, ideology, historical institutionalism Introduction the church from the policy-making arena. As we The recent debate in the Portuguese parliament will show, up until 2013, the Portuguese hierar- (July 2015) about restricting the 2007 liberal- chy showed great restraint during the process of ized abortion law in Portugal revealed a novum moral-political liberalization.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology
    3GC01 06/09/2015 12:40:42 Page 3 Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2015). The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Second edition. Volume 1: Foundations. (pp. 3-87). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. CHAPTER 1 The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology JOHN TOOBY and LEDA COSMIDES THE EMERGENCE OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY: WHAT IS AT STAKE? HE THEORY OF evolution by natural selection has revolutionary implications for understanding the design of the human mind and brain, as Darwin himself was Tthe first to recognize (Darwin, 1859). Indeed, a principled understanding of the network of causation that built the functional architecture of the human species offers the possibility of transforming the study of humanity into a natural science capable of precision and rapid progress. Yet, more than a century and a half after The Origin of Species was published, many of the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences continue to be grounded on assumptions that evolutionarily informed researchers know to be false; the rest have only in the past few decades set to work on the radical reformulations of their disciplines necessary to make them consistent with findings in the evolutionary sciences, information theory, computer science, physics, the neuro- sciences, molecular and cellular biology, genetics, behavioral ecology, hunter-gatherer studies, biological anthropology, primatology, and so on (Pinker, 1997, 2002; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). Evolutionary psychology is the long-forestalled scientific attempt to assemble out of the disjointed, fragmentary, and mutually contradictory human disciplines a single, logically integrated research framework for the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences—a framework that not only incorporates the evolu- tionary sciences and information theory on a full and equal basis, but that systemati- cally works out all the revisions in existing belief and research practice that such a synthesis requires (Tooby & Cosmides, 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • An Interview with Sherwood Washburn
    UC Berkeley Anthropology Faculty Publications Title An Interview with Sherwood Washburn Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1xp1w64r Journal Current Anthropology, 33(4) Authors Washburn, Sherwood L. Devore, Irven Publication Date 1992 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Reports An Interview with much more fun. In retrospect, I think that my family was amazingly generous in what they allowed me to do. Sherwood Washburn' The first skeleton I had anything to do with was a porcupine skeleton which my brother and I found in the Catskill Mountains. It had dried out and become just IRVEN DEVORE bones and a few quills. We gave it to the Harvard Mu­ Cambridge, Mass., U.S.A. t5 II 86 seum of Comparative Zoology. Dr. Henshaw was the director at the time. In retrospect, I believe my father 10: We thought we might start with your precollege must have called him before we arrived, because when years, and I urge you to go back to as early a period as we came in with this absolutely wretched bunch of bro­ you want to. ken porcupine bones, he welcomed us as if we were giv­ ing Harvard a great, valuable specimen. He treated us SW: Of course/ I was always interested in zoology­ like adults even though my brother and I were respec­ mammal skeletOns, birds, and behavior. I kept a great tively about eight and six at the time. This experience horned owl for some years when I was young, as well as of respect from an admired adult was very important crows and hawks.
    [Show full text]
  • Harassment, Bullying, and Hazing Policy
    HARASSMENT, BULLYING, AND HAZING POLICY It is the policy of the District to provide a safe and nurturing educational environment for all of its students. This policy protects all students from bullying/cyberbullying/harassment/sexual harassment and other aggressive behaviors regardless of the subject matter or motivation for such impermissible behavior. Bullying/cyberbullying/harassment/sexual harassment or other aggressive behavior toward a student, whether by other students, staff, or third parties, including Board members, parents, guests, contractors, vendors, and volunteers, is strictly prohibited. This prohibition includes written, physical, verbal, and psychological abuse, including hazing, gestures, comments, threats, or actions to a student, which cause or threaten to cause bodily harm, reasonable fear for personal safety or personal degradation. Demonstration of appropriate behavior, treating others with civility and respect, and refusing to tolerate harassment or bullying/cyberbullying is expected of administrators, faculty, staff, and volunteers to provide positive examples for student behavior. This policy applies to all "at school" activities in the District, including activities on school property, in a school vehicle, and those occurring off school property if the student or employee is at any school sponsored, school-approved or school-related activity or function, such as field trips or athletic events where students are under the school's control, or where an employee is engaged in school business. Misconduct occurring outside of school may also be disciplined if it interferes with the school environment. Notification: ​ Notice of this policy will be annually circulated within the District and discussed with students, as well as incorporated into teacher, student, and parent/guardian handbooks.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Psychology
    SN-05(153-196) 3/4/02 2:12 PM Page 153 CHAPTER 5 Evolutionary psychology esearchers committed to an evolutionary perspective Ron humanity were initially united in the face of wide- spread hostility to human sociobiology. However, in the 1980s, as the number of investigators using evolution to study human behaviour increased, subgroups began to emerge with different opinions on how best to proceed. One such subgroup was dominated by academic psycho- logists searching for the evolved psychological mechanisms that they envisaged underpinned any universal mental and behavioural characteristics of humanity. While the intellec- tual roots of some of these practitioners could be traced to human sociobiology, or to the study of animal behaviour, the majority were fresh recruits who sought to differentiate themselves from human sociobiology, and restyled them- selves as Darwinian or evolutionary psychologists. For Leda Cosmides and John Tooby, two of the pioneers of this new discipline, evolutionary psychology owed little intellectual debt to Edward Wilson but did draw inspiration from the writings of Bill Hamilton, Robert Trivers, and George Williams. Tooby, a Harvard-trained anthropologist who had worked closely with Irven DeVore, and Cosmides, a psychologist also from Harvard, were brought by Donald Symons to Santa Barbara where they founded the first Center for Research in Evolutionary Psychology. SN-05(153-196) 3/4/02 2:12 PM Page 154 154 SENSE AND NONSENSE The ‘Santa Barbara school’1 were concerned that human sociobiologists and behavioural ecologists had neglected psychological adaptations: In the rush to apply evolutionary insights to a science of human behavior, many researchers have made a conceptual ‘wrong turn’, leaving a gap in the evolutionary approach that has limited its effectiveness.
    [Show full text]
  • A Recipe for Success in the 'English World': an Investigation of the Ex
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-2018 A Recipe for Success in the ‘English World’: An Investigation of the Ex-Amish in Mainstream Society Jessica R. Sullivan Western Michigan University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations Part of the Sociology of Culture Commons Recommended Citation Sullivan, Jessica R., "A Recipe for Success in the ‘English World’: An Investigation of the Ex-Amish in Mainstream Society" (2018). Dissertations. 3358. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations/3358 This Dissertation-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. A RECIPE FOR SUCCESS IN THE ‘ENGLISH WORLD’: AN INVESTIGATION OF THE EX-AMISH IN MAINSTREAM SOCIETY by Jessica R. Sullivan A dissertation submitted to the Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Sociology Western Michigan University December 2018 Doctoral Committee: Angela Moe, Ph.D., Chair Whitney DeCamp, Ph.D. Jesse Smith, Ph.D. Cynthia Visscher, Ph.D. Copyright by Jessica R. Sullivan 2018 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS My graduate work and dissertation would not have been possible without the help of my participants and the amazing support and love of those around me. I would like to take a moment to acknowledge their contributions (in no particular order of course). First of all, I would like to thank Angie Moe, my dissertation chair.
    [Show full text]
  • Two Cultures of Punishment
    Stanford Law Review Volume 68 May 2016 ARTICLE Two Cultures of Punishment Joshua Kleinfeld* Abstract. As American criminal punishment has become more severe and European more mild, the two systems of punishment have come to represent different cultural possibilities for the modern West. Implicit in American and European punishment are two visions of wrongdoing and wrongdoers, of the terms of the social contract, and of the foundations of rights. American punishment pictures serious offenders as morally deformed people rather than ordinary people who have committed crimes. Their criminality is thus both immutable and devaluing, a feature of the actor rather than merely the act. The forms of punishment deployed in response do not just exact retribution or exert social control, they expressively deny offenders’ claims to membership in the community and to the moral humanity in virtue of which a human being is rights bearing. European criminal punishment expressively denies that any offense marks the offender as a morally deformed person. Criminality is always mutable and never devaluing, actors are kept at a distance from their acts, and the forms of punishment affirm even the worst offenders’ claims to social membership and rights. These conflicting moral visions are not only implicit or immanent in European and American punishment but likely played a role—albeit not an exclusive role—in causing European and American punishment to diverge. After an enormous mid-century crime wave, two very different groups took hold of America’s politics of crime: moralists who viewed criminals as evil and instrumentalists who viewed criminals as dangerous beings. Different as the two groups were, they agreed on policy: for both, the crime problem was a criminals problem, and the solution was to get rid of criminals.
    [Show full text]
  • Philip G. Zimbardo
    PHILIP G. ZIMBARDO An Oral History conducted by Daniel Hartwig STANFORD HISTORICAL SOCIETY ORAL HISTORY PROGRAM Stanford University ©2017 2 Ferne Millen Photography Philip G. Zimbardo, 2015 3 4 Contents Publisher’s Note p. 9 Introduction p. 11 Abstract p. 13 Biography p. 15 Part One—May 9, 2016 p. 19 Ancestors, childhood, and family life in New York Temporary move to California High school classmate Stanley Milgram Milgram’s obedience experiments and career Stanford Prison Experiment Undergraduate education Social activism Part Two—September 1, 2016 p. 67 Graduate study at Yale Teaching at New York University Research on deindividuation Social activism Harlem Summer Project Vietnam War Malcolm X International European Social Psychology Summer Program Part Three—September 1, 2016 p. 97 Columbia University Joining Stanford faculty 5 Stanford Psychology Department Resident faculty at Cedro Teaching methods Psychology & Life Faculty and colleagues 1960s, activism, Vietnam War Social Psychology in Action Birth of the Stanford Prison Experiment Stanford Shyness Project Part Four—December 9, 2016 p. 145 End of the Stanford Prison Experiment Stanford Shyness Clinic Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory Mind control Madness Part Five—December 9, 2016 p. 163 Discontinuity and paranoia Discovering Psychology Teaching psychology at Stanford American Psychological Association 9/11, terrorism Abu Ghraib Retirement The Lucifer Effect Heroic Imagination Project Part Six—March 12, 2017 p. 207 Man Interrupted, A Boy Disconnected Heroic Imagination Project Discovering Psychology 6 Recruitment of minorities and women Mentoring graduate students Applied psychology Program in Human Biology Part Seven—March 12, 2017 p. 245 Music Department, Stanley Getz Activism Psi Chi, the Psychology Honor Society Reflections and accomplishments Curriculum Vitae p.
    [Show full text]
  • It Does Take a Village by Melvin Konner | the New York Review Of
    Font Size: A A A It Does Take a Village DECEMBER 8, 2011 Melvin Konner Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding by Sarah Blaffer Hrdy Belknap Press/ Harvard University Press, 422 pp., $29.95; $19.95 (paper) Anthony Bannister/Gallo Images/Corbis A !Kung grandmother and grandchild, Namibia Sarah Blaffer Hrdy is one of the most original and influential minds in evolutionary anthropology. She first became known for her field study of Hanuman langurs, the sacred monkeys that range widely in the Indian peninsula. They are large and sometimes dangerous, and Hrdy was among that second generation of bold primatologists, just behind Jane Goodall and Diane Fossey, who did original work with primates. Hrdy discovered, among other things, that dominant males in a group are challenged from time to time by roving adventurers who can mate only by defeating them. If defeated, the former leaders slink away, often wounded, while their successors attack and kill all infants under six months old. This brings their mothers back into heat, and the slain infants are supplanted by the new males’ offspring. Females resist this bravely, but to little avail. If such behavior had been limited to langurs, it might have been an anomaly. But thanks in no small part to Hrdy’s leadership, it was also documented in chimpanzees, patas monkeys, lions, and many other species. Competitive infanticide was seen as a dark side of Darwinism, and a confirmation that no part of nature is free from the amoral logic of natural selection. Hrdy went on to write a well-received book on the evolution of females, The Woman That Never Evolved.
    [Show full text]
  • Out of the Playground and Into Senior Living Communities
    Seniors Bullying Seniors: Out of the Playground and into Senior Living Communities Ruben Rivera-Jackman, MNPL, GMHS, PSC Senior Resident Services Manager (206) 574-1187 – [email protected] Ruben’s Experience ®25+ years of experience working in the health, human and social services sector. ®Education: MNPL, GMHS, PSC, and CDP. ® Senior Resident Services Manager, 10 years . 28 mixed population buildings public housing developments serving older-adults and younger adults with disabilities. 8 RSCs . 1,563 households Learning Outcomes 1. Describe the characteristics of bullying behaviors. 2. Learn the five different types of bullies. 3. Understand the psycho-social impacts of bullying behaviors. Learning Outcomes 4. Identify Organizational interventions that may help minimize bullying among older-adults. 5. Pinpoint skills to share with older- adults to avoid them from being bullying targets. Hands-Raised Polling Question Housing Developer, Office Assistant, Owner Administrative Assistant Service Coordinator Facilities Manager, Property Manager Maintenance Personnel Housing Administrator Adult Bullying: Not Just a Playground Issues Self-Reflection Exercise Hands-Raised Polling Question Have you personally ever been bullied? Have you ever witnessed someone else being bullied? If you are a parent, has your child ever shared with you that s/he has been bullied? Have you ever witnessed an older-adult bully another person? Nursery Rhyme Sticks and stones may break my bones, but words will never hurt me. Wikipedia: an English language children’s
    [Show full text]