Cytoplasmic Polyadenylation Element Binding Protein (CPEB): a Prion-Like Protein As a Regulator of Local Protein Synthesis and Synaptic Plasticity
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
A Germline-Specific Isoform of Eif4e (IFE-1) Is Required for Efficient Translation of Stored Mrnas and Maturation of Both Oocytes and Sperm
Research Article 1529 A germline-specific isoform of eIF4E (IFE-1) is required for efficient translation of stored mRNAs and maturation of both oocytes and sperm Melissa A. Henderson1, Elizabeth Cronland1, Steve Dunkelbarger2, Vince Contreras1, Susan Strome2 and Brett D. Keiper1,* 1Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Brody School of Medicine at East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27834, USA 2Department of Molecular Cell and Developmental Biology, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA 95064, USA *Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]) Accepted 26 January 2009 Journal of Cell Science 122, 1529-1539 Published by The Company of Biologists 2009 doi:10.1242/jcs.046771 Summary Fertility and embryonic viability are measures of efficient germ CED-4/Apaf-1, and accumulated as multinucleate cells unable cell growth and development. During oogenesis and to mature to spermatids. A modest defect in oocyte development spermatogenesis, new proteins are required for both mitotic was also observed. Oocytes progressed normally through mitosis expansion and differentiation. Qualitative and quantitative and meiosis, but subsequent production of competent oocytes changes in protein synthesis occur by translational control of became limiting, even in the presence of wild-type sperm. mRNAs, mediated in part by eIF4E, which binds the mRNAs Combined gametogenesis defects decreased worm fertility by 5Ј cap. IFE-1 is one of five eIF4E isoforms identified in 80% at 20°C; ife-1 worms were completely sterile at 25°C. C. elegans. IFE-1 is expressed primarily in the germ line and Thus, IFE-1 plays independent roles in late oogenesis and associates with P granules, large mRNPs that store mRNAs. -
Measuring and Modeling the Trajectory of Visual Spatial Attention
Psychological Review Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 109, No. 2, 260–305 0033-295X/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0033-295X.109.2.260 Measuring and Modeling the Trajectory of Visual Spatial Attention Shui-I Shih George Sperling University of Southampton University of California, Irvine In a novel choice attention-gating paradigm, observers monitor a stream of 3 ϫ 3 letter arrays until a tonal cue directs them to report 1 row. Analyses of the particular arrays from which reported letters are chosen and of the joint probabilities of reporting pairs of letters are used to derive a theory of attention dynamics. An attention window opens 0.15 s following a cue to attend to a location, remains open (minimally) 0.2 s, and admits information simultaneously from all the newly attended locations. The window dynamics are independent of the distance moved. The theory accounts for about 90% of the variance from the over 400 data points obtained from each of the observers in the 3 experiments reported here. With minor elaborations, it applies to all the principal paradigms used to study the dynamics of visual spatial attention. We explored a method of measuring the trajectory of spatial strong test of the possibility of equivalent attention trajectories in attention that is analogous to measuring the trajectory of subatomic different experimental paradigms requires that all paradigms be particles in a Glaser bubble chamber (Gray & Isaacs, 1975). In the tested with the same observers and with similar stimulus materials. bubble chamber, a three-dimensional space is filled with a super- Therefore, in addition to the main experiment, which measured heated liquid. -
Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception . -
Direct Effects of Heat Stress During Meiotic Maturation on Bovine Oocyte and Cumulus RNA
University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 12-2009 Direct Effects of Heat Stress During Meiotic Maturation on Bovine Oocyte and Cumulus RNA Rebecca R. Payton University of Tennessee - Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss Part of the Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Payton, Rebecca R., "Direct Effects of Heat Stress During Meiotic Maturation on Bovine Oocyte and Cumulus RNA. " PhD diss., University of Tennessee, 2009. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss/628 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a dissertation written by Rebecca R. Payton entitled "Direct Effects of Heat Stress During Meiotic Maturation on Bovine Oocyte and Cumulus RNA." I have examined the final electronic copy of this dissertation for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, with a major in Animal Science. J. Lannett Edwards, Major Professor We have read this dissertation and recommend its acceptance: Cheryl Kojima, Arnold Saxton, F. Neal Schrick, Neal Stewart Accepted for the Council: Carolyn -
CPEB3 Inhibits Translation of Mrna Targets by Localizing Them to P Bodies
CPEB3 inhibits translation of mRNA targets by localizing them to P bodies Lenzie Forda,b,c,1, Emi Linga,d,1, Eric R. Kandela,b,c,e,2, and Luana Fioritia,f,2 aDepartment of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027; bMortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027; cHoward Hughes Medical Institute, Chevy Chase, MD 20815; dDepartment of Genetics, Harvard Medical School, Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, MA 02142; eKavli Institute for Brain Science, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027; and fDulbecco Telethon Institute, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, 20156 Milan, Italy Contributed by Eric R. Kandel, June 28, 2019 (sent for review September 20, 2018; reviewed by Cristina M. Alberini and Sathyanarayanan V. Puthanveettil) Protein synthesis is crucial for the maintenance of long-term of CPEB3. Soluble CPEB3 inhibits target mRNA translation while memory-related synaptic plasticity. The cytoplasmic polyadenyla- oligomeric, partially insoluble CPEB3 promotes the translation of tion element-binding protein 3 (CPEB3) regulates the translation of target mRNA (4). several mRNAs important for long-term synaptic plasticity in the As neurons are polarized structures, we presume that mRNAs hippocampus. In previous studies, we found that the oligomeri- involved in the maintenance of long-term memory will be under zation and activity of CPEB3 are controlled by small ubiquitin-like strict spatial control. Indeed, intracellular transport of mRNA modifier (SUMO)ylation. In the basal state, CPEB3 is SUMOylated; and local translation play a key role in neuronal physiology. it is soluble and acts as a repressor of translation. -
The CPEB3 Ribozyme Modulates Hippocampal-Dependent Memory 3 4 Authors 1 2 2 2† 3 4 5 Claire C
bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.23.426448; this version posted May 5, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 1 2 The CPEB3 ribozyme modulates hippocampal-dependent memory 3 4 Authors 1 2 2 2† 3 4 5 Claire C. Chen , Joseph Han , Carlene A. Chinn , Xiang Li , Mehran Nikan , Marie Myszka , Liqi 5 2† 2* 1,4,6* 6 Tong , Timothy W. Bredy , Marcelo A. Wood , Andrej Lupták 7 Affiliations 1 8 Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, 9 United States. 2 10 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 11 University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 3 12 Ionis Pharmaceuticals, 2855 Gazelle Court, Carlsbad, CA 92010, USA. 4 13 Department of Chemistry, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 5 14 Institute for Memory Impairments and Neurological Disorders, University of California–Irvine, 15 Irvine, California 92697, United States. 6 16 Department of Molecular Biology and Biochemistry, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, 17 California 92697, United States 18 *Correspondence to: Andrej Lupták. Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of 19 California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. [email protected]. Marcelo A. Wood. 20 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 21 University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. -
The CPEB3 Ribozyme Modulates Hippocampal-Dependent Memory
bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.23.426448; this version posted January 24, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. The CPEB3 ribozyme modulates hippocampal-dependent memory Claire C. Chen1, Joseph Han2, Carlene A. Chinn2, Xiang Li2†, Mehran Nikan3, Marie Myszka4, Liqi Tong5, Timothy W. Bredy2†, Marcelo A. Wood2*, Andrej Lupták1,4,6* 1 Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 2 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 3 Ionis Pharmaceuticals, 2855 Gazelle Court, Carlsbad, CA 92010, USA. 4 Department of Chemistry, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 5 Institute for Memory Impairments and Neurological Disorders, University of California– Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. 6 Department of Molecular Biology and Biochemistry, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States *Correspondence to: Andrej Lupták. Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. [email protected]. Marcelo A. Wood. Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, California 92697, United States. [email protected]. † Present address: Cognitive Neuroepigenetics Laboratory, Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia. Keywords: ribozyme, splicing, self-scission, polyadenylation, local translation 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.23.426448; this version posted January 24, 2021. -
Verbal Learning Burbank Seminar Rooms Session 2B -Mathematical
Mathematical Psychology Meetings Stern Sail - Stanford Wednesday, August 23; 10-12 aojis, - BfoHadsy Lounges Session la - .'Neural nets Burbank Seminar Room:: Session lb - Verbal learning 1-3 pt-m. - Holiaday Lounge: Session 2s - Measurement and Sealing X Burbank Seminar Rooms Session 2b Mathematical Analysis of - Learning Data 3530-5 P«m. gfolladay Lounges Session 3: Symposium on Geometric Representation - of Psychological Data p«*nu - Reception at the Stanford Faculty Club Sburaday., August 29s 10-12 a^nu - felladay Lounge; Session 4a - Paychophyiiiesi Burbaok Seminar Ro®,n Sesuion Ub Concept learning and Social - Psychology Burbank Seminar Room Kb* 2'. Session Ue "" Choice Behavior 1-3 P»m*» - $3urbank Seminar Rooms Session _?a - Measurement and Scaling XI 1-5 P-n« - Holiaday Lounges Session Sb - Symposium on Models of Memory > Mathematical Psychology Meetings Stanford University August 23 and 29, 1968 PROGRAM Wednesday, August 28 10-12 a.m. Session la : Neural Nets Earl Hunt, University of Washington, A performance model for memory tasks based on a physiological model of retrieval. Robert J. Baron, Clarkson College of Technology, An associative memory system. Floyd Ratliff, Bruce Knight, and Norma Graham, The Rockefeller University, On tuning and amplification by lateral inhibition in a neural network. Naomi Weisstein, Loyola University, A Rashevsky-Landahl neural net for simulation of metacontrast. George Sperling, Bell Telephone Laboratories, Inc., Energy models of binocular vision. 10-12 a.m. Session lb : Verbal Learning John Brelsford, Yale University, A finite Integer analysis of recall and recognition performance. William H. Batchelder, University of Illinois, A mathematical framework for item interrelationships during learning. -
RNA-Binding Profiles of Drosophila CPEB Proteins Orb and Orb2
RNA-binding profiles of Drosophila CPEB proteins Orb and Orb2 Barbara Krystyna Stepiena,1,2, Cornelia Oppitza,3, Daniel Gerlacha,3,4, Ugur Dagb, Maria Novatchkovaa, Sebastian Krüttnera,5, Alexander Starka, and Krystyna Kelemana,b,1 aThe Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, 1030 Vienna, Austria; and bHoward Hughes Medical Institute, Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, VA 20147 Edited by Eric C. Lai, Sloan-Kettering Institute, New York, NY 10065, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Kathryn V. Anderson September 26, 2016 (received for review March 8, 2016) Localized protein translation is critical in many biological contexts, zinc finger (Znf) region (12). Most CPEB proteins exist in mul- particularly in highly polarized cells, such as neurons, to regulate tiple isoforms (11). Orb2 has two variants, Orb2A and Orb2B (12), gene expression in a spatiotemporal manner. The cytoplasmic which differ in the composition of the N terminus preceding the polyadenylation element-binding (CPEB) family of RNA-binding poly-Q and share a common RBD (12). The poly-Q is required proteins has emerged as a key regulator of mRNA transport and exclusively for LTM, whereas the RBD is required for both de- local translation required for early embryonic development, synap- velopment and LTM (20), and its mutations are lethal (12, 13). tic plasticity, and long-term memory (LTM). Drosophila Orb and Moreover, the RBD of Orb2 can be functionally replaced by the Orb2 are single members of the CPEB1 and CPEB2 subfamilies of RBD of mouse CPEB2 (mCPEB2) but not by that of Orb or the CPEB proteins, respectively. At present, the identity of the mCPEB1, suggesting the conservation of target specificity within mRNA targets they regulate is not fully known, and the binding but not between the CPEB subfamilies, at least in regard to specificity of the CPEB2 subfamily is a matter of debate. -
The Role of CPEB Family Proteins in the Nervous System Function in the Norm and Pathology Eugene Kozlov1 , Yulii V
Kozlov et al. Cell Biosci (2021) 11:64 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13578-021-00577-6 Cell & Bioscience REVIEW Open Access The role of CPEB family proteins in the nervous system function in the norm and pathology Eugene Kozlov1 , Yulii V. Shidlovskii1,2 , Rudolf Gilmutdinov1 , Paul Schedl1,3 and Mariya Zhukova1* Abstract Posttranscriptional gene regulation includes mRNA transport, localization, translation, and regulation of mRNA stabil- ity. CPEB (cytoplasmic polyadenylation element binding) family proteins bind to specifc sites within the 3′-untrans- lated region and mediate poly- and deadenylation of transcripts, activating or repressing protein synthesis. As part of ribonucleoprotein complexes, the CPEB proteins participate in mRNA transport and localization to diferent sub- cellular compartments. The CPEB proteins are evolutionarily conserved and have similar functions in vertebrates and invertebrates. In the nervous system, the CPEB proteins are involved in cell division, neural development, learning, and memory. Here we consider the functional features of these proteins in the nervous system of phylogenetically distant organisms: Drosophila, a well-studied model, and mammals. Disruption of the CPEB proteins functioning is associated with various pathologies, such as autism spectrum disorder and brain cancer. At the same time, CPEB gene regulation can provide for a recovery of the brain function in patients with fragile X syndrome and Huntington’s disease, making the CPEB genes promising targets for gene therapy. Keywords: CPEB, Translation, Prion, Neurogenesis, Long-term memory Background for transport, anchoring and translational regulation Te functioning of the nervous system is based on the including signals for regulating cytoplasmic polyadenyla- ability of neurons to perceive, transmit, and store infor- tion (cytoplasmic polyadenylation elements: CPE) are mation encoded in electrical and chemical signals. -
VITA GEORGE SPERLING, UCI Distinguished Professor
VITA GEORGE SPERLING, UCI Distinguished Professor, Department of Cognitive Sciences, Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, and Institute For Mathematical Behavioral Sciences, University of Cali- fornia, Irvine Business address: Dept. of Cognitive Sciences, SSPA3, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-5100 Tel: 949-824-6879 Fax: -2517 http://aris.ss.uci.edu/HIPLab Email: [email protected] Education: University of Michigan, B.S. Mathematics, 1955. Joint major in mathematics and biophysics. (Fulfilled requirements for summa cum laude.) Award: Gomberg Chemistry Prize and Scholarship, 1953-54. Memberships in collegiate honor societies: Phi Beta Kappa, Phi Eta Sigma, Phi Kappa Phi, Sigma Xi. Columbia University, M.A. Psychology, 1956. Harvard University, Ph.D. Psychology, 1959. Professional Employment: Research Assistant in Biophysics, Brookhaven National Laboratories, Upton, New York, summer, 1955. Research Assistant in Psychology, Harvard University, 1957-59. Member Technical Research Staff, Acoustical and Behavioral Research Center, AT&T Bell Laboratories, Murray Hill, New Jersey: 1958 (summer); full-time, 1959-70; part-time, 1970-86. Instructor in Psychology (part-time), Department of Psychology, Washington Square College, New York University, 1962-63. Visiting Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Duke University, Spring, 1964. Adjunct Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Columbia University, 1964-65. Acting Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, 1967-68. Fellow of the John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation, 1969-70. Aw ard for a theoretical study of perception and short-term memory. Honorary Research Associate, Department of Psychology, University College, University of London, 1969-70. Professor of Psychology and Neural Sciences, Graduate School of Arts and Science, New York University, 1970-1992. -
17. MEMORY in Psychology, Memory Is an Organism's Ability to Store, Retain, and Recall Information. Traditional Studies of Memor
17. MEMORY In psychology, memory is an organism's ability to store, retain, and recall information. Traditional studies of memory began in the fields of philosophy, including techniques of artificially enhancing the memory. The late nineteenth and early twentieth century put memory within the paradigms of cognitive psychology. In recent decades, it has become one of the principal pillars of a branch of science called cognitive neuroscience, an interdisciplinary link between cognitive psychology and neuroscience. Sensory memory Sensory memory corresponds approximately to the initial 200 - 500 milliseconds after an item is perceived. The ability to look at an item, and remember what it looked like with just a second of observation, or memorization, is an example of sensory memory. With very short presentations, participants often report that they seem to "see" more than they can actually report. The first experiments exploring this form of sensory memory were conducted by George Sperling (1960) using the "partial report paradigm." Subjects were presented with a grid of 12 letters, arranged into three rows of 4. After a brief presentation, subjects were then played either a high, medium or low tone, cuing them which of the rows to report. Based on these partial report experiments, Sperling was able to show that the capacity of sensory memory was approximately 12 items, but that it degraded very quickly (within a few hundred milliseconds). Because this form of memory degrades so quickly, participants would see the display, but be unable to report all of the items (12 in the "whole report" procedure) before they decayed. This type of memory cannot be prolonged via rehearsal.