Linguistics Module 23: Problems with Phonemic Analysis

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Linguistics Module 23: Problems with Phonemic Analysis Linguistics Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology Problems with Phonemic Analysis Principal Investigator Prof. Pramod Pandey Centre for Linguistics, SLL&CS, Jawaharlal Nehru University, New Delhi-110067 Paper Coordinator Prof. Pramod Pandey Module ID & Name Lings_P2_M23; Problems with Phonemic Analysis Content Writer Pramod Pandey Email id & phone [email protected]; 011-26741258, -9810979446 Reviewer Editor Module 23: Problems with Phonemic Analysis Objectives: • To familiarize students with the problems in phonemic analysis • To provide a background to the motivation for the development of generative phonology Contents: 23.1 Introduction 23.2 Insufficiency of the notions of phonetic substance and significant contrast 23.3 Monosystematicity 23.4 Problems in Phonemicization 23.5 Principle of Biuniqueness 23.6 Morphophonemics and separation of levels 23.7 Summary 1 23.1 Introduction In the present module, we take a close look at the various problems in the structural phonology view of phonemic analysis. We discuss the difficulties lying in the strict use of the principles of analysis and the classical concept of the phoneme, the basic assumptions about the separation of levels and the goals of phonological analysis. These issues are taken up in the successive sections 23.2- 23.6. Section 23.7 sums up the main points of the discussion. Figure 23-1: A problem http://blog.regehr.org/wp-content/uploads/2010/09/bug.jpg 23.2 Insufficiency of the notions of phonetic substance and significant contrast The two basic notions that inform the principles of phonemic analysis are phonetic similarity and significant contrast. Let us consider the notion of significant contrast first. 2 According to the principle of contrastive distribution (see Module 19), two sounds, X and Y, are phonemes if they occur in identical environments and contrast in meaning when substituted, for example, the sounds [s] and [ʃ] as in the words so and show. The notion of significant contrast is not workable on the grounds of the distribution of the sounds in question. One of the aspects of distribution that is problematic is when environments are not identical. For example, in English, it is well-known that the [h] and [ŋ] never occur in identical environments- [h] always occurs in the onset position in a syllable, most commonly at the beginning of words and [ŋ] always occurs in the coda position in a syllable, that is, at the end of words or before a consonant. They are thus in mutually exclusive environments: 23/1 a. him [hɪm], hat [hæt], behave [bɪˈheɪv] b. sing [sɪŋ], banker [bæŋ.kə], England [ɪŋ.glənd] There is no way in which it can be shown that [h] and [ŋ] contrast in meaning. On the contrary, they always occur in mutually exclusive environments. What is the conclusion to be drawn in their case then? We can go by the criterion of phonetic similarity. [h] and [ŋ] are phonetically so different from each other that they cannot be expected to be allophones or variants of one phoneme. Another example where distribution of sounds cannot be fully relied upon is that of the allophones of a set of sounds. Consider the sets of allophones [t s z] and [č š ž] in Congo, that we looked at in Module 20. We arrived at the conclusion that [t] and [č] are allophones of one phoneme, [s] and [š] are allophones of another phoneme, and [z] and [ž] are allophones of yet another phoneme. The distributions of the allophones are reproduced in 23/2. 23/2 a. [t] [s] [z] # __ o (1) e __ o (3) # __ e (5, 11) e __ u (13) a __ u (7) e __ u (14) 3 n __ e (9) n __ w (13) b. [č] [ š] [ž] # __ i (2, 8) o __ i (4) # __ i (6) n __ i (10) a __ i (12) We shall use the IPA symbols [ʧ ʃ ʒ] for [č š ž] respectively. Notice that on the basis of the distribution of the two sets of allophones, we conclude that [t] and [ʧ], because they occur in complementary distribution, are allophones of the same phoneme /t/. However, [t] also occurs in complementary distribution with [ʃ] and with [ʒ]. And the same could be said about [s]/ [ʧ], [s]/ [ʒ] and about [z] /[ʧ], /[z]/[ʃ]. What prevents us from arriving at the conclusion that the latter are not allophones of one another is not the criterion of distribution, but of phonetic similarity, as was observed in Module 18 (section18.4.) However, the notion of phonetic similarity is not yet a well-defined criterion, but a working notion for phonologists. In the case of the phones in [t], [ʧ] and [s], [t] is just as much phonetically similar to [ʧ] as is [s]. The following data show that this indeed is so. a. /ʧ/ > /s/ In Assamese and many other Indo-Aryan dialects in the north-east India [ʧ] has changed to [s], e.g. Hindi [ʧa:r] ‘four’, Assamese [sar] ‘four’. The change has taken place in some Dravidian languages, too, e.g. Irula, Kota and Tamil. b. /s/ > /ʧ/ In the Tibeto-Burman languages, Lepcha and Limbu, /s/ changes to [s]. c. [ʧ] ~ [s] in the Dravidian language Ave, [ʧ] and [s] are in free variation. The point that emerges is that there is no clear way in which the notion of phonetic similarity can be defined. 4 To buttress the point further, there are also cases of allophones having remote phonetic similarity. For instance, • In the Tibeto-Burman language Chokri, spoken in Nagaland and Manipur, the post-alveolar approximant [ɹ] freely varies with the voiced velar fricative [ɣ]. As the labels for the sounds suggest, they are similar in a much more remote way than, say, [ʧ] and [s]. • The glottal stop [ʔ] is an allophone of a whole array of consonants in different languages- / p t d k ɡ h/. From the Austro-Asiatic family, [ʔ] is an allophone of /t, k/ in Juang, /ɡ/ in Mundari and /h/ in Korku. It is thus difficult to say precisely when two sounds can be said to be phonetically similar and when not. 23.3 Monosystematicity Figure 23-2: A monolithic structure https://encrypted- tbn3.gstatic.com/images?q=tbn:ANd9GcTfYsDISikqjNdehO1mkVFy9uZ0uiTa_cceZv96gYlafzNGilsA The basic assumption in phonemic analysis is that a language being described is monosystematic, that is, a single system. All the phonemes of a language can be arrived at by examining their distribution and the criterion of significant contrast. However, languages are constantly evolving, among other reasons, by the process of 5 language contact and regional variation. As a result, not all the speech sounds can be analysed decidedly as phonemes or allophones. For example, in Hindi, there are certain sounds which have been borrowed from other languages with which it has come into contact. The voiceless uvular plosive [q] is thus found in the pronunciation of words of Perso-Arabic origin, /qissa:/ ‘story’ and /qətl/ ‘murder’ in the speech of some but not all speakers of Standard Hindi. The words are part of the present-day Hindi. In the speech of these Hindi speakers, /q/ and /k/ contrast, as in [kəmər] ‘waist’ and /qəmər/ ‘(the) moon’. Since the sound is found to contrast in the speech of only some speakers, but not all, do we consider it a phoneme? Similar cases are found in borrowings from English. The vowel [ɔ:] is one such case in words such as [kɔle:ʤ], which freely varies with [a:], [ka:le:ʤ] ‘college’. It is common to find differences of the type described above in the phonemic status of sounds across all languages and communities. Phonemic analysis in the structural approach that we have been discussing in the modules (17-22) assumes that a language is a single system of sounds. In fact, it is more common for languages to have multiple systems. A phonemic analysis is thus best conceived of as restricted to a single homogeneous dialect. As has been suggested above, this is not the normal case. As languages evolve, some sounds remain on the periphery of the phonemic system. They can be described as ‘marginal’ phonemes in a system or left out as belonging to another dialect. As marginal phonemes, the practice is to enclose them in parentheses. For example, the vowels of Hindi are listed as /i, u, ə, i:, u:, a:, e:, o:, æ:, ᴔ: (ɔ:)/. The vowel /ɔ:/ is assumed to have restricted distribution in the language. Examples of fricative consonants from two languages- Santali and Punjabi are given below. As you can see, whereas Santali has a single system, Punjabi has a sub-system in it, shown in parentheses. Fricatives Places → 6 Language↓ Dental Alveolar Post- Retroflex Palatal Glottal Alveolar Santali s ʂ h Punjabi (v) s (z) ʃ (x) (ɣ) h Figure 23-3: Monosystematic and polysystematic Fricatives in Santali and Punjabi. 23.4 Problems in Phonemicization The phonemic principle in Structural phonology (see Module 18, section 18.3) views the phoneme as a minimal contrastive unit of sound. The phoneme cannot be reduced further. This conception of the phoneme was held for a long time, with multiple consequences discussed below. Single segments or sequences of segments? The view of the phoneme as a minimal contrastive unit of sound sometimes led to problems of analysis, when it came to diphthongs among vowels and to complex segments among consonants. With regard to diphthongs, the issue that arises is whether to treat them as single vowels or as sequences of vowels. The principles of phonemic analysis are silent on this issue. There are some points of difference between diphthongs as single vowels and vowel sequences that may prove useful in analysis.
Recommended publications
  • Finding Phonological Rules
    14 February 2013 Aaron J. Dinkin Linguistics 1 [email protected] Lecture 8 Phonology: finding phonological rules Given a data from a language, how do you identify phonological rules? —how do you determine if two sounds are allophones of the same phoneme, and/or what rule determines the relationship between the allophones? Basic process: 1) Look at the environments in which the sounds appear 2) Look for natural classes in the environments, and 3) Determine if the sounds are in complementary distribution 4) If so, find which sound occurs in the elsewhere condition if any 5) State a rule which accounts for the distribution you see Example: Consider a dialect of English with two allophones of /aI/, [aI] and [ʌI]. What rule accounts for their distribution? Words containing [aI] Words containing [ʌI] tribe hype dive white rise life oblige bike mime ice fine fly First, we just list the environments in which [aI] and [ʌI] appear in the data: [aI] [ʌI] ɹ__b h__p d__v w__t ɹ__z l__f l__dʒ b__k m__m #__s f__n l__# Next, we look for natural classes among the environments: [ʌI] is always followed by a voiceless consonant. Do we have complementary distribution? Yes: ; [aI] is never pre-voiceless. [aI] is followed by both voiced consonants and the word boundary: not a natural class. Thus [aI] occurs “elsewhere”; it’s the underlying form. So we write the rule: /aI/ becomes [ʌI] before voiceless sounds aɪ ʌɪ / __[–vce] Example: Tojolabal uses both ejective [k’] and regular [k]. Are they allophones of the same phoneme, or distinct phonemes? Sample
    [Show full text]
  • Download This PDF File
    Sinan Bayraktaroğlu Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies Vol.7, No.1, April 2011 A Model of Classification of Phonemic and Phonetic Negative Transfer: The case of Turkish –English Interlanguage with Pedagogical Applications Sinan Bayraktaroğlu [email protected] Abstract This article introduces a model of classification of phonemic and phonetic negative- transfer based on an empirical study of Turkish-English Interlanguage. The model sets out a hierarchy of difficulties, starting from the most crucial phonemic features affecting “intelligibility”, down to other distributional, phonetic, and allophonic features which need to be acquired if a “near-native” level of phonological competence is aimed at. Unlike previous theoretical studies of predictions of classification of phonemic and phonetic L1 interference (Moulton 1962a 1962b; Wiik 1965), this model is based on an empirical study of the recorded materials of Turkish-English IL speakers transcribed allophonically using the IPA Alphabet and diacritics. For different categories of observed systematic negative- transfer and their avoidance of getting “fossilized” in the IL process, remedial exercises are recommended for the teaching and learning BBC Pronunciation. In conclusıon, few methodological phonetic techniques, approaches, and specifications are put forward for their use in designing the curriculum and syllabus content of teaching L2 pronunciation. Key Words: Interlanguage, Language transfer, Negative transfer, Intelligibility, Fossilızation, Allophonic transcription, Phonological competence, Common European Framework (“CEF”) Özet Bu makale, Türkçe-İngilizce Aradili üzerine yapılan deneysel bir çalışmadaki sesbilgisel ve sesbirimsel nitelikli olumsuz dil aktarımlarının sınıflandırılmasını öneren bir modeli tanıtmaktadır. Birinci dilden kaynaklanan bu olumsuz dil aktarımları önem ve önceliklerine göre derecelendirilmektedir. Bunlar Aradili konuşmada “anlaşılabilirliği” etkileyen en önemli sesbirimsel özelliklerden başlamaktadır.
    [Show full text]
  • A Probabilistic Model of Phonological Relationships from Contrast to Allophony
    A Probabilistic Model of Phonological Relationships from Contrast to Allophony Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Kathleen Currie Hall Graduate Program in Linguistics The Ohio State University 2009 Dissertation Committe: Elizabeth Hume, Advisor Mary Beckman Chris Brew Cynthia Clopper Abstract This dissertation proposes a model of phonological relationships, the Probabilistic Phonological Relationship Model (PPRM), that quantifies how predictably distributed two sounds in a relationship are. It builds on a core premise of traditional phonological analysis, that the ability to define phonological relationships such as contrast and allophony is crucial to the determination of phonological patterns in language. The PPRM starts with one of the long-standing tools for determining phonological relationships, the notion of predictability of distribution. Building on insights from probability and information theory, the model provides a way of calculating the precise degree to which two sounds are predictably distributed, rather than maintaining the traditional binary distinction between “predictable” and “not predictable.” It includes a measure of the probability of each member of a pair in each environment they occur in, the uncertainty (entropy) of the choice between the members of the pair in each environment, and the overall uncertainty of choice between the members of the pair in a language. These numbers provide a way
    [Show full text]
  • Manual for Linguistics I Linguistics I: Phonetics & Phonology 1º Year Instituto Superior De Lenguas Universidad Nacional De Asunción
    Manual for Linguistics I Linguistics I: Phonetics & Phonology 1º Year Instituto Superior de Lenguas Universidad Nacional de Asunción Phonetics & Phonology 1º Year Facultad de Lenguas Vivas Universidad Evangélica del Paraguay Applied Phonetics 1º Year Instituto Superior de Educación Ministerio de Educación y Cultura Susan Spezzini - Christian Cristoful Scappini 2013 [email protected] 2 “…and the whole earth was of one language and of one speech” Genesis 11:01 … …en aquel tiempo todos hablaban un mismo idioma …ɚ ...erat autem terra labii unius et sermonum eorundem ...naquele tempo toda a humanidade falava uma só língua ...Καὶ ἦν πᾶσα ἡ γῆ χεῖλος ἕν, καὶ φωνὴ μία πᾶσιν …Toute la terre avait une seule langue et les mêmes mots …온 땅의 구음이 하나이요 언어가 하나이었더라 …ora tutta la terra parlava la stessa lingua e usava le stesse parole ...nan konmansman, tout moun sou latè te pale yon sèl lang, yonn te konprann lòt …واحدة ول غة واحدا ل سان ا ك لها اﻻر ض وك ان ت ... 那時,天下人的口音言語,都是一樣。 ...hele Menneskeheden havde eet Tungemål og samme Sprog ...und die ganze Erde hatte ein und dieselbe Sprache und ein und dieselben Wörter … Ekuqalekeni uThixo wadala amazulu nehlabathi. אחדים׃ ודברים אחת שפה כל־הארץ ויהי 3 LESSON 1. INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS ................................................................................................................................. 1 LESSON 3. WHAT IS LANGUAGE? .................................................................................................................................................. 3 HUMAN/ANIMAL
    [Show full text]
  • The Possible Patterns of Sounds Are Often Called Their Distribution
    Theoretical Phonetics Theoretical Phonetics has the following branches: 1. Articulatory, 2. Acoustic, 3. Auditory / Perceptual, 4. Functional /Phonological Phone Definition: It is the smallest identifiable unit found in a stream of speech that is able to be transcribed with an IPA symbol. Phoneme Definition: A phoneme is the smallest contrastive unit in the sound system of a language. Discussion: Phonologists have differing views of the phoneme. Following are the two major views considered here: In the American structuralist tradition, a phoneme is defined according to its allophones and environments. In the Generative tradition, a phoneme is defined as a set of distinctive features. Sounds interact in connected speech If you are saying a word that ends in /s/ or /z/ and this is following a word that begins with /ʃ / or /j/ the final /s/ tends to turn into /ʃ/ whereas the final /z/ tends to turn into /ʒ/. Sounds interact in connected speech This should be clearer in an example, given by Roach (2009, 111). In connected speech ‘this shoe’ might end up being pronounced whereas ‘those years’ might sound When changes like this happen it is as if one sound has become more similar, or has assimilated to another. The phenomenon is called assimilation. Accommodation Accommodation is the process of mutual influence of consonants and vowels. The modification in the articulation of a consonant under the influence of an adjacent vowel is called accommodation. In accommodation the accommodated sound does not change its main phonemic features and is pronounced slightly modified under the influence of a neighboring sound.
    [Show full text]
  • Allophones.Pdf
    Dr. Tarkan KAÇMAZ Within a given language, some sounds are considered to be the same sound, even though they are phonetically distinct. • Same or different? • pool [phul] spool [spul] • phonetically different (aspirated vs. unaspirated) • native speakers perceive the same sound •A phoneme can be pronounced in different ways according to its context. Compare: The difference between /t/ in : tea, eat, writer, eighth, two The difference between /i:/ in: see, seed, seat, seen •Therefore, a phoneme may have more than one realization. •The different realizations of a phoneme are called allophones of that phoneme. The allophone is a variant of a phoneme. Try saying these two words: car and keys What’s different about the initial sound in each word? Phonetically: [kʰα: cʰi:z] (ʰ = aspiration c=palatal stop;) [kʰ] and [cʰ] are allophones of the / k / phoneme. English pʰu:ɫ pool spu:ɫ spool kʰɪɫ kill skɪɫ skill • Complementary distribution • Predictable (no minimal pairs) • [ p ] and [ pʰ ] are allophones of the / p / phoneme Two or more sounds are allophones of the same phoneme if: a) they have a predictable, complementary distribution; b) they do not create a semantic contrast; and c) they are phonetically similar. (E.g. [l] and [ɫ] in English: [l] never occurs before consonants or word-finally, [ɫ] never occurs before vowels) Contrastive distribution: Two sounds are said to be contrastive if replacing one with the other results in a change of meaning. Example: ‘cat’ [khQt] and ‘hat’ [hQt] Complementary distribution: phones appear in differing environments; are allophones of the same phoneme ▪ Example: ‘top’ [thap] and ‘stop’ [stap] Free Variation: phones appear in exactly the same environments; no difference in meaning; are allophones of the same phoneme.
    [Show full text]
  • Minimal Pairs -Complementary & Contrastive Distribution 2 Minute Warmup
    Phonetics Review & Phonology LIN 001Y – A09 WINTER 2017 FRIDAY JANUARY 27, 2017 TA – JEFF MORAN HW 2 & Phonetics: What Do We Need to Review? -Test 1 -Discussion -Logistics -Vowel chart -Phoneme & allophones -Minimal Pairs -Complementary & contrastive distribution 2 Minute Warmup It’s Friday! I’m so happy I want you all to tell the world about it! So… work with a partner to tell the world how happy you are in IPA! (That’s a lot of exclamation marks!) Please transcribe the following phrase into IPA: Happy Friday [hæpi fɹaide] Vowels Vowel Chart & Descriptions • All voiced • No/minimal air obstruction • So we don’t use place & manner of articulation So we describe them with: Front-Central-Back High-Mid-Low Tense vs. Lax Activity Practice saying each vowel sound. Then, for each monophthong and diphthong, say one word that contains that vowel sound. Monophthongs Diphthongs 1. [ɑi] 2. [ɔɪ] 3. [aʊ] American English IPA Consonants & Vowels Transcription activity Transcribe your full name Jeffrey Chapman Moran ʤɛfɹi ʧæpmən məɹæn Phonology: 30 Second Review Phonology is the branch of linguistics that is concerned with how sounds are systematically organized within a language It is distinct from phonetics, which as we have seen, is the study of the acoustic properties of sounds However, in order to understand phonological processes, one must have a good grasp of phonetics. More Basic Concepts Phonemes are abstract mental representations of sounds. Phonemes are marked by slashes ( ex. /b/ ) They reflect speaker knowledge of sounds. Allophones are the physical “realizations” of phonemes. They are the sounds that are actually made by speakers of a language.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Linguistics Study Guide Anna Gartsman & Laura Hughes Northeastern University Senior Honors Project 2006-2007
    Honors Project Cover Page Introductory Linguistics Study Guide By Anna Gartsman and Laura Hughes, Class of 2007 May 2007 Department of Linguistics College of Arts & Sciences Northeastern University Advisor: Janet Randall Project field: Linguistics Colleges/Majors: • Anna Gartsman: o School of Computer Science: Computer Science and Cognitive Psychology o School of Arts & Sciences: Linguistics • Laura Hughes o School of Arts & Sciences: Linguistics and Psychology Abstract: A step-by-step guide to the major areas of introductory-level theoretical linguistics (phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics) with a focus on practical problem-solving. Designed to assist students in low-level linguistics classes and tutors of same by providing additional explanations, examples and exercises. Introduction to Linguistics Study Guide Anna Gartsman & Laura Hughes Northeastern University Senior Honors Project 2006-2007 Contents Phonetics Syntax The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) Constituency Natural Classes Lexical Categories Features English Phrase Structure & Syntax Trees Ambiguous Sentences Phonology Complex Sentences Minimal Pairs Transformations Phonemes and Allophones Complementary & Contrastive Distribution Semantics Environments Connotation & Denotation Determining the Underlying Phoneme Processes of Semantic Change Rules Theta roles Analyzing Data Sets Tautologies, Contradictions, & Phonological Processes Contingencies Sense & Reference Morphology Presupposition & Entailment Basic Lexical Categories (Noun, Verb,
    [Show full text]
  • MORPHOPHONEMIC ALTERNATION the Smallest Minimal Meaningful Elements in Language Are Known Are Morphemes
    BA Sem II: Linguistics, Paper II (Sounds of Language II) MORPHOPHONEMIC ALTERNATION The smallest minimal meaningful elements in language are known are morphemes. For instance, words like man, cat, bag etc. have a single morpheme each. But words like men, cats, bags and so on consist of two morphemes each: one, the morpheme available as singular forms referring to different entities, and two, the morpheme meaning ‘plural’ signified by the suffix –s. Similarly, the words like play, kill and pick have one morpheme each. But words like played, killed and picked consists of two morphemes each: one, the verb itself and two, the morpheme with the meaning ‘past tense’ in English realized as the suffix –ed. Sometimes, a morpheme could have different phonological shapes depending on the contexts they occur in. For instance, the regular plural morpheme in English is formed by adding an s-ending to the nouns. But this s- ending to the nouns, which is phonetically a voiceless alveolar fricative [s], is pronounced differently as [s], [z] or [az] in different words as given below: caps [kæps] cabs [kæpz] bush [buʃəz] If we look carefully at these words, we observe that it is the final sound segment of the noun that determines these phonological shapes of the plural morpheme. If the nouns ends in voiceless consonants as in cap, bat, sack, booth etc. the plural morpheme will take the shape [s] and if it ends in voiced consonants as in bag, nib, toad it takes the shape [z]. If the noun ends in sibilants as in bus, wage, catch, brush etc., the plural occurs in the form [əz] because it is not possible to pronounce two sibilants distinctly in succession.
    [Show full text]
  • Formal and Empirical Issues in Phonological Typology∗
    Formal and empirical issues in phonological typology∗ Paul Kiparsky Stanford University 1 Lexical representations 1.1 Problems with phonemes Typological generalizations and universals are explicanda for linguistic theory, but they are themselves theory-dependent, for in order to be intelligible and falsifiable they must adhere to some explicit descriptive framework. This mutual dependency comes to a head at the margins of typological space, where reconciling typologies with descriptive frameworks and the analyses dictated by them can involve a labyrinth of choices. I explore a few of the tangled paths through it in the realm of syllable structure and vowel systems. Phonological typology has been based on three distinct levels of representation: phonemic, phonetic, and morphophonemic (underlying, “systematic phonemic”). Most work on segment in- ventories is framed in terms of phonemic systems in the tradition of Trubetzkoy (1929, 1939), Jakobson 1958, and Greenberg (1978). A major resource is the UPSID collection of phonemic systems (Maddieson 1984, 2013, Maddieson & Precoda 1990), which has the virtue of being genet- ically balanced (to the extent possible), carefully vetted, and to some extent normalized to conform to a standard set of analytic principles.1 The same resource has also been used by phoneticians to investigate the typology of speech sound inventories (Schwartz et al. 1997). Proponents of Disper- sion Theory have attempted to model the UPSID vowel systems, even though the theory is strictly speaking about the phonetic realization of phonemes (maximization of perceptual distance and minimization of articulatory effort). A growing body of typology crucially relies on underlying representations (phonemes in the generative phonological sense), such as Dresher 2009 and Casali 2014.
    [Show full text]
  • Day 6: Phonology Ling 200: Introduction to Linguistic Thought
    . Outline Today Natural Classes Basic Concepts Day 6: Phonology Ling 200: Introduction to Linguistic Thought . Jonathan North Washington . .. 25 June 2007 Jonathan North Washington Day 6: Phonology . Outline Today Natural Classes Basic Concepts 1 Today .. 2 Natural Classes 3 Basic Concepts .. What is Phonology? Contrastiveness Minimal Pairs Phonemes and Allophones .. Complementary Distribution Jonathan North Washington Day 6: Phonology . Outline Today Natural Classes . Basic Concepts Today Last-minute questions on homework 1, then turn in http://archive.phonetics.ucla.edu/Language/GSW/ gsw.html Hand back quiz 2, discuss Assign homework 2 Phonology Transition from phonetics: natural classes Intro Minimal pairs Distribution (Allophones, etc) Jonathan North Washington Day 6: Phonology . Outline Today Natural Classes . Basic Concepts Natural Classes Definition (Natural Classes) . A group of sounds, which have a particular feature (or combination of features) in common, that are treated as a group by the phonology of a language. Example (Natural Classes) labial consonants: [p, b, m, w, f, v] . .. low vowels: [æ, a] . sonorant consonants (liquids, nasals, glides): [l, ɹ, n, m, ŋ, j, w] (and [ɾ]) obstruent consonants: (non-sonorants): . [p, b, f, v, , , t, d, s, z, ʃ, ʒ, tʃ, dʒ, k, ɡ, ʔ, h] sibilants: (\hushing sounds"): [s, z, ʃ, ʒ] Jonathan North Washington Day 6: Phonology . .. What is Phonology? Outline Contrastiveness Today Minimal Pairs Natural Classes Phonemes and Allophones Basic Concepts . Complementary Distribution Phonology Phonology Definition (Phonology) The study of the organisation of speech sounds in a language How they \function" How they're distributed . (How they pattern cross-linguistically) . Jonathan North Washington Day 6: Phonology .. What is Phonology? Outline Contrastiveness Today Minimal Pairs Natural Classes Phonemes and Allophones Basic Concepts Complementary Distribution .
    [Show full text]
  • The Strategy of Generative Phonology* Arnold M•. Zwicky 1. · Introduction•. the Purpose of This Paper Is to Examine Some P
    The Strategy of Generative Phonology* Arnold M•. Zwicky 1. · Introduction•. _The purpose of this paper is to examine some principles of a.rgl.l.lllentation and verification used by generative phonologists, so as to show areas of agreement and to highlight areas of controversy.1 By 'generative phonology' I refer not necessarily to the analyses in The Sound Pattern of E~glish, but rather to a wider body of practice, of which the Chomsky-Halle approach represents one rather extreme position out of a large class of possible positions. I begin by attempting to clarif;{ the central notion of methodo- logical principle, which is meant to ~e opposed to theoretical, or substantive. principle, Briefly, substantive principles are theoretical requirements, methodological principles are theoretical biases,2 For example, the sequential application of processes vithin derivations is a theoretical principle of generative phonology-; to question this principle is clearly to advocate a new, though related, theory of phonology, in the sense that the tvo systems allow distinct sorts of phenomena as potential human languages .. On the other hand, the greater worth of phonological variants as opposed to distributional restrictions (Principle (E) below) is a principle of good practice. To question this principle is to question its value within a given theoretical structlll"e--but r~versing the bias would not create a new theory (since all languages consistent· with one theory would be consistent with the other). I do not wish to give the impression that there is a clear or a priori distinction bet~een the two sorts of principles. Obviously there is not, The same assumption may figure no~ as a substantive principle~ now as a methodological one; consider in this light the ass1.Unptions that tvo segments that distinguish morphemes are underlyingly distinct, ceteris paribus (Principle {L) below) and that two segments that never distinguish morphemes are not underlyingly distinct,· ceteris paribus (Principle (M) below).
    [Show full text]