Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Colour vision in marine organisms

1 2 3

Justin Marshall , Karen L Carleton and Thomas Cronin

Colour vision in the marine environment is on average simpler In this brief review we examine some of the recent

than in terrestrial environments with simple or no colour vision advances in our understanding of colour vision in the ocean

through monochromacy or dichromacy. Monochromacy is across both invertebrates and vertebrates and place this in

found in marine mammals and elasmobranchs, including context of previous hypotheses. Because the ocean is hard

whales and sharks, but not some rays. Conversely, there is also to work within and because its contrasting niches may

a greater diversity of colour vision in the ocean than on land, deliver highly speciose assemblages, there are still

examples being the polyspectral stomatopods and the many many knowledge gaps. Some of our discussion extends

colour vision solutions found among reef fish. Recent advances from what we know about colour vision systems in fresh-

in sequencing reveal more opsin (visual pigment) types than water fish as few marine species are well described.

functionally useful at any one time. This diversity arises through

opsin duplication and conversion. Such mechanisms allow Spectral sensitivities and colour vision

pick-and-mix adaptation that tunes colour vision on a variety of Nearly always, a sense of colour requires the comparison

very short non-evolutionary timescales. At least some of the of excitations between photoreceptor cells with different

diversity in marine colour vision is best explained as spectral sensitivities. At its simplest two such cells, each

unconventional colour vision or as neutral drift. containing visual pigments with peak sensitivity in dif-

Addresses ferent spectral regions, will detect the same colour signal

1

Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, differently, so relative excitation ratios can be compared

Queensland 4070, Australia

2 to form a dichromatic colour-vision system. Visual pig-

Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD

ments are constructed from an opsin protein with cen-

20742, USA

3 trally bound chromophore, often 11-cis retinal [1]. Their

Department of Biological Sciences, University of Maryland Baltimore

County, Baltimore, MD 21250, USA peak sensitivity, or l-max, depends both on variability in

specific opsin amino acids and on the particular chromo-

Corresponding author: Marshall, Justin ([email protected]) 

phore used [2 ].

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 Because both visual pigment absorption spectra and

object reflection spectra are broad, within the 300–

This review comes from a themed issue on Molecular biology of

sensation 750 nm range of light over which visual pigments operate,



three or four visual pigments are usually sufficient [3,4 ]

Edited by David Julius and John Carlson

(Figure 1). Marine generally have two visual

pigments and so are dichromats for reasons examined

in the next section. Some, such as the cephalopods, many

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.02.002 crustaceans and other invertebrates, remain colour-blind

0959-4388/# 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. monochromats [5,6]. Surface-dwelling fish are capable of

trichromacy or even tetrachromatic colour vision, and

stomatopod crustaceans have evolved twelve spectral

sensitivities, apparently processing spectral information

in a unique way.

Introduction Like their terrestrial counterparts, nocturnal and crepus-

Colour vision in the marine environment operates be- cular marine animals generally forgo colour vision alto-

tween two extremes. For some organisms, it is less gether, evolving a single spectral sensitivity for optimal



developed, with animals able to survive as colour-blind light absorption [2 ,7]. While some nocturnal animals

monochromats or at most dichromats. A few possess three possess colour vision on land, in the ocean it is more

or four spectral channels, potentially but not always, common for the opposite to be true. Many diurnal ani-

allowing them to venture into trichromacy or tetrachro- mals, including the cephalopods and many crustaceans,

macy, depending on how information is combined sub- have elaborated polarisation vision more than colour.

retinally. However, the ocean also boasts animals with Their single spectral sensitivity peaks around 500 nm,

complex colour vision such as the stomatopod crusta- an optimum for polarisation vision for several reasons

ceans with up to twelve spectral channels. Several outside the scope of this review [8,9].

drivers contribute to this complexity including the

light environment, phylogeny and behaviour relative In order to absorb as much light as possible, the matching

to colour. of photoreceptor sensitivity to bioluminescent emissions

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 www.sciencedirect.com

Colour vision in marine organisms Marshall, Carleton and Cronin 87

Figure 1

Stomatopod Reef fish Human

Crab

Cuttlefish

300 400 500 600 700

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Comparative colour vision complexity. Black rectangles marks peak spectral sensitivities relative to the spectrum below (in nanometers (nm), UV

300–400 nm and human visible 400–700 nm). Stomatopods have 12 colour channels, humans three (trichromacy), some reef fish are also

trichromats but sample shorter wavelengths than humans and are very variable (Figure 3), crustaceans such as some crabs shrimp or species are

dichromats or, like the cephalopods are colour blind with a single spectral sensitivity around 500 nm.

[10] or residual daylight from the surface has driven many Evaluating colour vision type and evolution

vertebrate and invertebrate marine species to focus sen- A good starting point for determining the performance



sitivity between 450 and 500 nm [4 ,7]. The early visual and limits of colour vision is to measure spectral sensitiv-

ecology literature developed the ‘sensitivity hypothesis’ ities. Such information can be determined by absorption

to explain this adaptation [7,11]. The exact match microspectrophotometry (MSP) of dissociated photo-

depends on the local environment, as estuarine or marine receptors (rods and cones in vertebrates, rhabdoms or

water peak transmission varies from green to blue, and to rhabdomeres in invertebrates [17–19] or spectral ab-

an extent on diurnal activity periods. Therefore, species sorption measurements from expressed visual pigments

in either habitat will possess spectral sensitivities that [20]. The sensitivities either method delivers should

generally sample within the envelope of available light be viewed as a first estimate however. MSP, unless

provided by spatial and temporal variations [7,12].

Why is dichromacy common in many marine Figure 2

animals?

Based on our relatively scant knowledge of marine animal

colour vision, dichromacy emerges as the oceanic favour- Kasmira

Bohar reef

Outer

ite. This perhaps derives from what is thought to be the

Quinquelineatus

basal colour sense in all animals, an ultraviolet (UV) and a Adetii

Carponotatus

green (or a mid-wavelength) sensitivity, used to differen- reef

 Russelli (adult) Middle

tiate UV-rich sky or open space [4 ] from substrate, Malabaricus

reef

which reflects UV poorly. This solution is still found in Sebae

Erythropterus

several invertebrates [13] including many crustaceans [5].

Russelli (juvenile)

It has also been postulated that two-channel colour vision Fulviflamma (juvenile)

evolved in the shallow Cambrian oceans in order to Johnii (adult)

Johnii (juvenile) Estuary Inner

remove luminance flicker via the differential subtractive

 Argentimaculatus

input that two spectral sensitivities allows [4 ,14]. Final-

400 450 500 550 600

ly, since the available spectrum of light narrows with

increasing depth or viewing distance, a pair of spectral Wavelength (nm)

channels may provide all the information needed Current Opinion in Neurobiology

(Figure 2). John Lythgoe and others developed the ‘offset

hypothesis’, a slight variation on this, suggesting that two l-Max positions of cones and rods in 14 species of snapper (Lutjanus)

sensitivities are ideal under water. One is matched to the species from green coastal through clearer blue outer ocean waters

are compared. Rod sensitivities — vertical bars, single cones — filled

background, and one is offset to discriminate objects

 circles, double cones — semicircles. Vertical lines plot the zones of

against that background [4 ,7]. The blue and green

greatest sensitivity for each water type for visual pigments within at

sensitivities of many oceanic predators (fish or marine least 90% of optimum. Note how double cones fall within this

 

mammals) support this hypothesis [15 ,16,17]. range.Adapted from [4 ,12].

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94

88 Molecular biology of sensation

performed systematically, often misses photoreceptor clas- including polarisation vision [8,25] but cross-species

ses, while opsin expression almost invariably overestimates behavioural evidence is lacking.

what the animal actually uses for colour vision.

It is not always clear from a spectral sensitivity count

In a systematic MSP examination of 39 species of Hawai- alone, which receptor channels combine beneath the

ian reef fish, Losey and co-workers determined that retina and coordinate colour-relevant behaviours.

dichromatic or trichromatic colour vision apparently dom- Electrophysiological recording from photoreceptors,

inate in this ecosystem [19]. Analyses of these results interneurons, or brain areas associated with colour vision

[21,22] used the considerable overlap of double and single often detects opponent processing [26]. Barry and Hawry-

cone sensitivities in some species (Figure 3) to suggest shyn recorded extracellularly from retinal ganglion cells

that, as in birds [7], double cones may function as a and optic tectum in the Hawaiian ,

luminance channel and that only the single cones partici- duperrey, and found at least two cone mechanisms that

pate in colour vision (for definitions of cone types, see appear well suited to their colour tasks [27]. Gruber and



[23]). This hypothesis is supported by the fact that among colleagues (1975) [28] and Bedore et al. [29 ] used elec-

several marine fish, double cone sensitivities match light troretinogram (ERG) recordings to demonstrate multiple

availability in the varying habitats occupied [7,12,22]. spectral sensitivity classes in rays. Recordings in inverte-

More recent behavioural observations, however, demon- brates are mostly limited to early work, confirming mono

strate that in one species of triggerfish, Rhinecanthus or dichromacy in crabs and other crustaceans (for review

aculeatus, double cone members are certainly used sepa- [5]). More recently they have been used to determine the



rately in the construction of a trichromatic colour sense sensitivities of stomatopod photoreceptors [30,31,32 ],



with the other single cone present [24 ]. It seems likely, confirming the general pattern of results from MSP

therefore, that double cone function is variable, possibly [18,31] but also identifying an extensive population of

Figure 3

Pervagor spilosoma Chromis verater 1.0 1.0

0.8 0.8

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4

0.2 0.2

0.0 0.0 300 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700

Forcipiger flavissimus Chromis hanui 1.0 1.0

0.8 0.8 Spectral Sensitivity

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4

0.2 0.2

0.0 0.0 300 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700

Wavelength (nm)

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

The spectral sensitivities of four species of reef fish from the same photic microhabitat. Normalized sensitivities include filtering by ocular media, in

non-UV sensitive species often close to 400 nm. Note the diversity of peak positions, spectral sensitivity number and mix of single and double



cones. Rods, black, single cones, red and double cones, blue curves.Adapted from [4 ,22].

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 www.sciencedirect.com

Colour vision in marine organisms Marshall, Carleton and Cronin 89

UV-sensitive photoreceptors (Figure 1 [30]). An interest- indicate a different approach to processing of their twelve

ing recent study of the ocular media filtering mechanisms channel input. Our current hypothesis is that the relative

stomatopods use to construct their UV sensitivities spectral excitations of these twelve bins are read off as a

revealed a rare instance of an animal multiplying spectral serial linear excitation pattern. Such an unconventional

channels using variable cut-off filtering and a single visual way to conduct colour vision requires specific subretinal

pigment [33]. wiring, currently under investigation. Comfortingly it

does explain away the worry of dodecachromatic colour



Ultimately it is the behaviour of the animal relative to vision [50 ].

colour that allows us to state what type of colour vision it

has. Reaction to colour varies from simple phototaxis,

Too many opsins?

moving to or away from coloured areas as shown in beach

It is increasingly common to estimate spectral sensitivi-

amphipods [34], through wavelength-specific behaviours,

ties through examining opsin expression. Many insects

to complex colour association and categorisation tasks

are molecularly trichromatic, with a UV, blue and green

(see [35,36] for useful summaries). Behavioural evidence

(or LWS for Long Wave Sensitive) classes of opsins. Of

in elasmobranch rays is consistent with the earlier electro-

 marine invertebrates, so far only stomatopods and possi-

physiology [28,29 ], identifying likely trichromacy in the

bly fiddler crabs reveal more than two spectral classes

giant shovel-nosed ray Glaucostegus typus [37]. Aside from 

 [13,47,51 ]. Early in vertebrate evolution, at least as early as

the triggerfish studies already mentioned [24 ], the reef

lampreys, five classes of opsin arose [20,52]. These are the

damselfish, Pomacentrus ambionensis, is known to use

RH1 class found in rods and four cone opsin classes:

colour choice in operant conditioning and may extend

SWS1 — very short (often UV) sensitive, SWS2 — short

this to more complex tasks in the UV including individual

wavelength (often blue) sensitive, RH2 — a medium

facial recognition [38,39]. As indicated both by opsin

wavelength, green-sensitive rod-like opsin, and LWS —

types [40] and ocular media transmission [41], damselfish 

long wavelength (up to red) sensitive class [2 ].

and other small fish on the reef [42] could use UV as a

private short-distance communication channel, as larger

It is surprising how often gene duplications, deletions and

predatory fish lack UV sensitivity [41]. Colour-vision

substitutions multiply and change opsins. For example,

models and field observation reveal that ‘cleaner blue’

among the visually unexciting ostracods, Oakley and

is used as a colour code by several cleaner fish and

colleagues identify eight opsin types [53] while in sto-

crustaceans [43]. In addition, such modelling shows that

matopods, Porter and colleagues find over thirty opsins in

fish with UV sensitivity are at a great advantage, not just 

some species [51 ]. In fish, it is not uncommon to find

with UV spectra or UV silhouetted prey becoming more

seven to ten or more opsins expressed at a functional level

visible [39,41] but due to boosted colour discrimination 

[2 ,54]. Serial duplications, deletions, gene conversions

across colour space [16].

and other transformations allow great plasticity, espe-

cially when coupled to subsequent differential gene

Marine invertebrates with behavioural evidence for col- 

expression and modification [55 ,56]. For example,

our vision include the fiddler crabs [44] and blue crab [45].

in a recent survey of percomorph fish species, a new

Crab colour sense is most likely based both on two- 

class of SWS2 gene was described [57 ]. Other recent

channel UV/violet — green retinal sensitivities and on

studies find RH2 gene loss in pufferfish [58] and loss of

achromatic cues [46]. Molecular evidence for more than 

SWS1 and LWS in marine mammals [15 ]. Having this

two spectral sensitivities does exist in fiddler crabs [47];

variation available may contribute to rapid speciation in

however, it is not clear to what extent this reflects 

fish [2 ].

seasonal or other environmental tuning as opposed to

an extension of dichromacy.

Hofmann et al. showed that in damselfish, varying light

Stomatopods have been known for some time to possess environment has shaped opsin diversity, particularly in

true colour vision in colour versus grey-card tests [48], the SWS and LWS classes [40]. Important to note is that

hardly a surprise given their diverse spectral sensitivities not all opsins are necessarily functional at any one time,

and colourful lifestyle [49]. Early speculation pointed and how and when each subclass is used throughout life is

towards a series of dichromatic mechanisms, based on poorly understood. A number of factors including the

anatomy and subretinal neural connections. However following require careful consideration.

more recent results indicate a far more interesting system



of processing. Thoen et al. [32 ] demonstrate that sto- (a) Co-expression of different visual pigments in the

matopods discriminate wavelengths up to ten times less same photoreceptor is now recognised in both

 

well than other animals, such as humans, honey bees and invertebrates [51 ] and vertebrates [59 ]. In some

goldfish. Theoretically, if their colour sense were based examples this widens the overall spectral response of

on photoreceptor comparisons, mantis shrimp hue dis- the cell; however, it may sometimes reflect a

crimination should be extremely fine, so these results photoreceptor transitory phase.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94

90 Molecular biology of sensation

(b) Spectral sensitivities may change during the life of an species as their retinas often mirror the vertical

animal on a developmental, seasonal, sexual, or other change in photic environment underwater. Higher

basis (see e below). This may be achieved through sensitivity double cones are dense in the dorsal retina,

opsin expression differences and/or a change in which looks into dim deeper water, while more single

chromophore from A1 to A2, a transformation that cones for colour specific tasks are present in the

extends sensitivities to longer wavelengths [7]. ventral and medial retina [62]. In this way, retinal

Almost all marine fish, including tuna [56] or bream sensitivities vary across the retina to match the

[60] go through a larval stage, and where examined spatially varying light environment. This above any

these often have a different opsin quota to the adult. other reason may explain the supernumerary spectral

Functionally this may reflect a change in foraging sensitivity diversity seen in fish.

mode and/or light environment. (d) Short-term spectral sensitivity plasticity is known in

(c) Retinal functional subdivision is common in inverte- both fish and invertebrates, a surprise given our

brates with compound eyes but is also found in fish, assumption that an animal’s colour sense was fixed for

with different areas expressing different opsins life or at least long-term life periods. Diurnal opsin

 

[59 ,61 ]. Although examples of this are currently expression changes have been recorded in freshwater

limited to freshwater, it is also likely in marine cichlids and killifish [63,64]. Sensitivities also change

Figure 4

(a) (b)

(c) (d)

1.0 1.0

0.8 0.8

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4 Sensitivity 0.2 0.2

0.0 Normalised Reflectance / 0.0

Normalised Reflectance

300 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700

(e) (f) 1.0 1.0

0.8 0.8

0.6 0.6

0.4 0.4 Radiance 0.2 0.2

0.0 0.0

Normalised Reflectance

Normalised Reflectance /

300 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700

Wavelength (nm)

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Fine colour pattern combinations through resolution breakdown in Thalassoma lunare (moon wrasse, a, c, e) and Pygoplites diacanthus (angelfish,

b, d, f). Fish colours (a,b) measured using reflectance spectrophotometry (c,d) where curves are colour-coded to fish colours in a and b, combine

over a few metres underwater, especially to the relatively poor-resolution eyes of fish. Combined curves plotted in red in e and f. The complex

colours of wrasse and parrotfish, combine to give a good match to background blue spacelight, at least for the spectrum up to 600 nm, which

encompasses most of the blue-shifted sensitivities known in reef fish. Yellow and blue, frequent in reef fish colouration for a variety of reasons

combine to a uniform grey spectral reflection, good camouflage in several contexts and in marked contrast to the highly conspicuous colour

combination close up.Adapted from [67].

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 www.sciencedirect.com

Colour vision in marine organisms Marshall, Carleton and Cronin 91

with the spectral content of light available, correlating

Fluorescence of stomatopods and fish (among other ani-

to different settlement depths as larvae take up their

mals including jumping spiders and birds) has been

adult habitat [65].

implicated as a means of boosting colour communication

(e) Unconventional colour vision, such as that seen in

[68,69]. The fluorescent emission of stomatopods



stomatopods (or on land in butterflies [50 ]) may

increases the underlying colour signal by 15%, enough

result in a large number of opsin types, aimed not at

to be seen; however, behavioural evidence of the signifi-

allowing finer colour chromatic evaluation, but

cance of the fluorescent component is lacking. Triple-

either to deliver to stomatopods the required

fins, wrasse, and now more than 180 other species of

spectral coverage that their spectral-pattern recog-

marine fish [70] are known to fluoresce and may do so at

nition system requires or to add sensitivities for

specific depths where the longer wavelengths (yellow-

specific colour tasks. This latter wavelength-

red) of these blue-water-excited emissions are prominent.

specific colour vision, with or without accompa-

Despite several attempts [71], behavioural evidence for

nying retinal subdivision, is worth more attention in

fluorescent function in fish remains unconvincing, as it

the ocean.

fails to differentiate other colour pattern elements. Many

pigments in nature fluoresce, and it remains possible that

fluorescence in fish serves no visual function.

Colours, colour vision and spectral sensitivity

spacing

Conclusions and future perspectives

The colours and colour vision systems around coral reefs

There is great diversity in opsin complements amongst

have received recent attention [16,21,22,43,66,67].

marine animals suggesting excellent colour vision capa-

These studies re-confirm many of the original visual

bilities. However, before assuming animals with multiple

ecological observations of Lythgoe and colleagues, such

opsins are attempting higher levels of spectral discrimi-

as the use of yellow and blue in marine waters for

nation, two other explanations must be considered. First-

efficient signal transmission [7]. The relatively poor

ly, large opsin numbers are clearly a dynamic way to tune

spatial acuity of fish vision is also important in colour

colour vision to the needs of animal on a variety of

communication, as many of the fine colour patterns of 

evolutionary or within-lifetime timescales [57 ]. Addi-

reef fish, for example, while conspicuous close up, add

tionally, multiple spectral sensitivities may also reflect

together through resolution breakdown to give grey or

unconventional processing and/or a colour sense that is

match to blue background space-light at a distance [67]

functionally subdivided across the retina.

(Figure 4). The blue-shifted spectral sensitivities of

reef fish also deliver another camouflage surprise, as

At present, there is a great need for behavioural observa-

yellow (a common colour on the reef) is in fact well

tion to back up hypotheses and ideas gleaned from more

matched to an average coral colour up to around 600 nm

immediate methods, and this necessarily includes in situ

[67]. Yellow fish seem conspicuous to us, as our primate

observation of how colour is used. The ocean is not an

colour vision is particularly concerned with ripening

easy place to work, but there is clearly a great diversity of

fruit and leaves in this spectral region. As with colour

colour vision systems within it that make it worth the

mixing, yellow may be both conspicuous and camou-

effort, and our greater knowledge of freshwater systems

flaged as required on the reef, as it contrasts well to a

provides tantalising glimpses of discoveries-in-waiting.

blue water background but matches the yellow/green

We also suggest that the top-down approach of phyloge-

corals [16,67].

ny, evolution and molecular genetics needs a more effec-

tive dialogue with the functional, anatomical or

Characterisation of reef fish colour vision has revealed a

neurobiological side of colour vision as identified in the

greater diversity than might be expected (Figure 3) 

 recent review of Kemp et al. [72 ].

[4 ,19,22]. Fish from very similar light micro-habitats

possess widely differing spectral sensitivity placement,

and while it is easy to state that this must somehow be Conflicts of interest

related to their species-specific colour task needs, these Nothing declared.

are not obvious. Modelling of a number of colour tasks

with different spectral sensitivity combinations suggests Acknowledgements

that the exact placement of the colour channels may vary The opinion expressed here are very clearly a distillation of many years



work, not just from ourselves, but from many exceptional and enlightened

without greatly affecting performance [4 ,16]. This is

colleagues. These include John Lythgoe, Horace Barlow, Bill McFarland,

also seen in the variability of spectral sensitivities in

Ellis Loew, Craig Hawryshyn, Tom Kocher, Roy Caldwell, George Losey,



40 species of flower-visiting hymenopterans [4 ]. The Misha Vorobyev and Daniel Osorio. We apologise for necessarily citing

secondary references that also summarise their seminal work.

idea of neutral drift in the evolution of spectral sensitivity

tuning may be uncomfortable but is worth considering

until well defined behaviour to colour task connections

JM is supported by funding from the Australian Research Council, the Asian

are found. Office of Aerospace Research and Development and the Airforce Office of

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94

92 Molecular biology of sensation

Scientific Research (AFOSR). TC through the National Science Pinnipeds also are thought to have lost blue cones. Colour vision there-

Foundation (NSF) and AFOSR. KC through NSF. fore seems unlikely or at least rudimentary in marine mammals, although

there remains behavioural evidence suggesting that some dolphins and

References and recommended reading

some pinnipeds do make colour choices. This may be the result of rod-

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,

cone dichromatic-like subretinal processes.

have been highlighted as:

16. Marshall NJ, Vorobyev M: The design of color signals and color

 of special interest vision in fishes. In Sensory Processing in Aquatic Environments.

 of outstanding interest Edited by Collin SP, Marshall NJ. Springer; 2003:194-222.

17. Hart NS, Lisney TJ, Marshall NJ, Collin SP: Multiple cone visual

1. Wald G: The molecular basis of visual excitation. Nature 1968,

pigments and the potential for trichromatic colour vision in

219:800-807.

two species of elasmobranch. J Exp Biol 2004, 207:4587-4594.

2. Hunt DM, Hankins MW, Collin SP, Marshall NJ (Eds): Evolution of

18. Cronin TW, Marshall NJ: A retina with at least ten spectral

 Visual and Non-visual Pigments. Springer; 2014.

types of photoreceptors in a mantis shrimp. Nature 1989,

This recent review captures the latest research on visual pigments, within 339:137-140.

photoreceptors and elsewhere, across both invertebrates and verte-

brates. Data from several streams including electrophysiological, beha- 19. Losey GS, McFarland WN, Loew ER, Zamzow JP, Nelson PA,

vioural and molecular biology are included with highlights being the Marshall NJ: Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. I.

recent advances in opsin expression and visual pigment function. Ocular transmission and visual pigments. Copeia 2003,

3:433-454.

3. Barlow HB: What causes trichromacy? A theoretical analysis

using comb-filtered spectra. Vis Res 1982, 22:635-643. 20. Yokoyama S: Evolution of dim-light and color vision pigments.

Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet 2008, 9:259-282.

4. Cronin TW, Johnsen S, Marshall NT, Warrant EJ: Visual Ecology.

 Princeton University Press; 2014. 21. Marshall NJ, Jennings K, McFarland WN, Loew ER, Losey GS:

This is a much needed update of the 1979 volume, The Ecology of Vision, Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. II. Colors of

by John Lythgoe, and reviews and expands this area of visual neu- Hawaiian coral reef fish. Copeia 2003, 3:455-466.

roscience. The subdiscipline of visual ecology recognises the link

22. Marshall NJ, Jennings K, McFarland WN, Loew ER, Losey GS:

between the design of visual systems and the environment, as well as

Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. III. Environmental

examining behavioural and evolutionary aspects. Visual Ecology contains

light and an integrated approach to the ecology of reef fish

many chapters relevant to marine colour vision, including examinations of

vision. Copeia 2003, 3:467-480.

photic habitat and colour vision.

5. Marshall NJ, Kent J, Cronin TW: Visual adaptations in 23. Walls GL: The Vertebrate Eye and its Adaptive Radiation. The

crustaceans: spectral sensitivity in diverse habitats. In Cranbrook Press; 1942.

Adaptive Mechanisms in the Ecology of Vision. Edited by Archer

24. Pignatelli V, Champ C, Marshall J, Vorobyev M: Double cones are

SN, Djamgoz MBA, Loew ER, Partridge JC, Vallerga S. Springer;

1999:285-327.  used for colour discrimination in the reef fish, Rhinecanthus

aculeatus. Biol Lett 2010, 6:537-539.

6. Marshall NJ, Messenger JB: Colour-blind camouflage. Nature

25. Kelber A, Roth LSV: Nocturnal colour vision — not as rare as we

1996, 382:408-409.

might think. J Exp Biol 2006, 209:781-788.

7. Lythgoe J: The Ecology of Vision. Clarendon Press; 1979.

26. Kamermans M, Kraaij DA, Spekreijse H: The cone/horizontal cell

8. Horva´ th G (Ed): Polarized Light and Polarization Vision in Animal network: a possible site for color constancy. Vis Neurosci 1998,

Sciences. Springer; 2014. 15:787-797.

A twenty five chapter book that comprehensively examines recent

27. Barry KL, Hawryshyn CW: Spectral sensitivity of the Hawaiian

advances in polarization vision. This includes the recent notion, advanced

saddle wrasse, Thalassoma duperrey, and implications for

by authors of this article, that the information content in polarisation

visually mediated behaviour on coral reefs. Environ Biol Fishes

signals from animals makes this modality equivalent to colour vision. To

1999, 56:429-442.

some extent, this explains why many marine animals, notably the inver-

tebrates, seem to have adopted polarization over colour in the marine 28. Gruber SH: Duplex vision in the elasmobranchs; histological,

environment. electrophysiological and psychophysical evidence. In Vision in

Fishes: New Approaches in Research. Edited by Ali MA. Plenum

9. Marshall NJ, Cronin TW: Polarisation vision. Curr Biol 2011,

Press; 1975:525-540.

21:R101-R105.

29. Bedore C, Loew E, Frank T, Hueter R, McComb DM, Kajiura S: A

10. Widder EA: Bioluminescence in the Ocean: origins of

 physiological analysis of color vision in batoid

biological, chemical, and ecological diversity. Science 2010,

elasmobranchs. J Comp Physiol A 2013, 199:1129-1141.

328:704-708.

Physiological approaches were used to examine colour and specifically

UV vision in two species of sting ray. ERG revealed multiple sensitivity

11. Munz FW, McFarland WN: The significance of spectral position

peaks, including in the UV although no UV cones were identified. MSP

in the rhodopsins of tropical marine fishes. Vis Res 1973,

13:1829-1874. showed two or three cone visual types, consolidating the previous work of

Hart and colleagues [17] and the behavioural evidence of Van-Eyk

12. Lythgoe J, Muntz W, Partridge JC, Shand J, Williams DM: The et al. [37] showing likely trichromatic colour vision in shovel nosed rays.

ecology of the visual pigments of snappers (Lutjanidae) on the

30. Marshall NJ, Oberwinkler J: The colourful world of the mantis

Great Barrier Reef. J Comp Physiol A 1994, 174:461-467.

shrimp. Nature 1999, 401:873-874.

13. Briscoe AD, Chittka L: The evolution of colour vision in insects.

31. Marshall J, Cronin TW, Kleinlogel S: Stomatopod eye structure

Annu Rev Entomol 2001, 46:471-510.

and function: a review. Arthropod Struct Dev 2007, 36:420-448.

14. Maximov V: Environmental factors which may have led to the

32. Thoen HH, How MJ, Chiou T-H, Marshall NJ: A different form of

appearance of colour vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 2000,

355:1239-1242.  color vision in mantis shrimp. Science 2014, 343:411-413.

Using both intracellular electrophysiology and behaviour, the colour

15. Meredith RW, Gatesy J, Emerling CA, York VM, Springer MS: Rod vision system of the stomatopod Haptosquilla trispinosa is described.

 monochromacy and the coevolution of cetacean retinal Wavelength discrimination experiments are used to show that, in some

opsins. PLoS Genet 2013, 9:e1003432. ways unexpectedly, stomatopods possess discrimination levels around

Shows that cetaceans have dispensed with all cone pigments and thus ten times worse than those of other animals similarly tested, including

maintain all-rod retinas, other marine mammals express green opsins but humans, butterflies, bees and goldfish. The hypothesis suggested to

no cetacean is known to express a blue-sensitive cone pigment. The explain this relies on a completely new form of colour coding in which the

narrow spectral deep-water foraging habitats of many marine mammals spectrum is coded as a serially related colour pattern, rather than colours

and lack of colour in mate choice may have driven this reduction. being judged from analogue comparison of two (or more) receptor output.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 www.sciencedirect.com

Colour vision in marine organisms Marshall, Carleton and Cronin 93

 

The need to cover the spectrum with this new system therefore explains the increasingly being recognised in vertebrates also, including fish [59 ,61 ].

need for full spectral coverage and twelve spectral sensitivities. The neural Stomatopod colour vision is defined as particularly unconventional with

connection requirements underlying this are currently under investigation. colour coding via spectral pattern recognition over the entire spectrum

rather than spectral channel comparisons as found in all other colour

33. Bok MJ, Porter ML, Place AR, Cronin TW: Biological sunscreens 

vision types [32 ].

tune polychromatic ultraviolet vision in mantis shrimp. Curr

Biol 2014, 24:1636-1642. 51. Porter ML, Speiser DI, Zaharoff AK, Caldwell RL, Cronin TW,

This examination of the UV sensitivities in stomatopods revealed several  Oakley TH: The evolution of complexity in the visual systems of

new features to this unusual visual system. They include: the use of stomatopods: insights from transcriptomics. Integr Comp Biol

mycosporine amino acids (MAAs) within the optical elements of the eye 2013, 53:39-49.

(crystalline cones) as variable UV filters to tune spectral sensitivity; The research described here uses transcriptomics to examine the diver-

selective filtering of visual pigment using short-pass filtering (long-pass sity of opsins in stomatopod crustaceans and attempts to place this in an

is more common); multiplication of UV sensitivities using two visual evolutionary context. Some species express up to 33 opsins while the

pigments and several differential UV filters. This is unusual in the animal retina has a maximum of 16 photoreceptor types at the functional level.

kingdom with animals generally providing a new visual pigment for every

52. Collin SC, Davies W, Hart NS, Hunt D: The evolution of early

new added spectral sensitivity.

vertebrate photoreceptors. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 2009,

34. Ugolini A, Vignali B, Castellini C, Lindstrom M: Zonal orientation 364:2925-2940.

and spectral filtering in Talitrus saltator (Amphipoda,

53. Oakley TH, Huber D: Differential expression of duplicated opsin

Talitridae). J Mar Biol Assoc UK 1996, 76:377-389.

genes in two eyetypes of ostracod crustaceans. J Mol Evol

35. Kelber A, Vorobyev M, Osorio D: Animal colour vision — 2004, 59:239-249.

behavioural tests and physiological concepts. Biol Rev 2003,

78:81-118. 54. Carleton KL, Spady T, Streelman JT, Kidd M, McFarland W,

Loew E: Visual sensitivities tuned by heterochronic shifts in

36. Kelber A, Osorio D: From spectral information to animal colour opsin gene expression. BMC Biol 2008, 6:22.

vision: experiments and concepts. Proc Biol Sci 2010,

277:1617-1625. 55. Hofmann C, Carleton KL: Gene duplication and differential gene

 expression play an important role in the diversification of

37. Van-Eyk SM, Siebeck UE, Champ CM, Marshall NJ, Hart NS: visual pigments in fish. Integr Comp Biol 2009, 49:630-643.

Behavioural evidence for colour vision in an elasmobranch. J

56. Nakamura Y, Mori K, Saitoh K, Oshima K, Mekuchi M, Sugaya T,

Exp Biol 2011, 214:4186-4192.

Shigenobu Y, Ojima N, Muta S, Fujiwara A et al.: Evolutionary

38. Siebeck UE, Wallis GM, Litherland L: Colour vision in coral reef changes of multiple visual pigment genes in the complete

fish. J Exp Biol 2008, 211:354-360. genome of Pacific bluefin tuna. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 2013,

110:11061-11066.

39. Siebeck UE, Losey GS, Marshall NJ: UV communication in Fish.

Here the tuna genome has been sequenced to show that opsin genes

In Communication in Fishes, Vol 2. Edited by Laddich F, Collin SP,

duplicate in tandem and also undergo gene conversion.

Moller P, Kapoor BG. Science Publications Inc.; 2006:423-455.

57. Cortesi F, Musilova´ Z, Stieb S, Hart NS, Siebeck UE,

40. Hofmann CM, Marshall NJ, Abdilleh K, Patel Z, Siebeck UE,

 Malmstrøm M, Tørresen OK, Jentoft S, Cheney KL, Marshall NJ,

Carleton KL: Opsin evolution in Damselfish: convergence,

Carleton KL, Salzburger W: Ancestral duplications and highly

reversal, and parallel evolution across tuning sites. J Mol Evol

dynamic opsin gene evolution in percomorph fishes. PNAS

2012, 75:79-91.

2015, 112:1493-1498.

A comprehensive survey of 100 fish species that documents the evolu-

41. Siebeck UE, Marshall NJ: Ocular media transmission of coral

tionary history of the SWS2 gene. A new gene duplication is identified

reef fish — can coral reef fish see ultraviolet light? Vis Res

along with, in some groups, losses and conversions in this violet/blue

2001, 41:133-149.

sensitive opsin class, coinciding with radiation of the highly diverse

percomorphs. One species, , is used to exemplify

42. Losey GS, Cronin TW, Goldsmith T, Hyde D, Marshall NJ,

how gene differences occur within one species ontogenetically.

McFarland WN: The UV visual world of fishes: a review. J Fish

Biol 1999, 54:921-943.

58. Neafsey DE, Hartl DL: Convergent loss of an anciently

duplicated, functionally divergent RH2 opsin gene in the fugu

43. Cheney KL, Grutter AS, Blomberg SP, Marshall NJ: Blue and

and Tetraodon pufferfish lineages. Gene 2005, 350:161-171.

yellow signal cleaning behavior in coral reef fishes. Curr Biol

2009, 19:1283-1287.

59. Dalton BE, Loew ER, Cronin TW, Carleton KL: Spectral tuning by

 opsin coexpression in retinal regions that view different parts

44. Detto T: The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in

of the visual field. Proc Biol Soc 2014, 281:1797.

mate choice. Proc Biol Sci 2007, 274:2785-2790.

60. Shand J, Davies W, Thomas N, Balmer L, Cowing J, Pointer M: The

45. Baldwin J, Johnsen S: The importance of color in mate choice

influence of ontogeny and light environment on the expression

of the blue crab Callinectes sapidus. J Exp Biol 2009,

212:3762-3768. of visual pigment opsins in the retina of the black bream

Acanthopagrus butcheri. J Exp Biol 2008, 211:1495-1503.

46. Baldwin J, Johnsen S: The male blue crab, Callinectes sapidus,

61. Temple SE: Why different regions of the retina have different

uses both chromatic and achromatic cues during mate

 spectral sensitivities: a review of mechanisms and functional

choice. J Exp Biol 2012, 215:1184-1191.

significance of intraretinal variability in spectral sensitivity in

47. Rajkumar P, Rollmann SM, Cook TA, Layne JE: Molecular vertebrates. Vis Neurosci 2011, 28:281-293.

evidence for color discrimination in the Atlantic sand fiddler

62. Fritsches KA, Marshall NJ, Warrant EJ: Retinal specializations in

crab, Uca pugilator. J Exp Biol 2010, 213:4240-4248.

the blue marlin: eyes designed for sensitivity to low light levels.

48. Marshall NJ, Jones J, Cronin TW: Behavioural evidence for Mar Freshwater Res 2003, 54:333-341.

colour vision in stomatopod crustaceans. J Comp Physiol A

63. Halstenberg S, Lindgren K, Samagh S, Nadal-Vicens M, Balt S,

1996, 179:473-481.

Fernald R: Diurnal rhythm of cone opsin expression in the teleost

49. Caldwell RL, Dingle H: Stomatopods. Sci Am 1976, 234:80-89. fish Haplochromis burtoni. Vis Neurosci 2005, 22:135-141.

50. Marshall NJ, Arikawa K: Unconventional colour vision. Curr Biol 64. Johnson AM, Stanis S, Fuller RC: Diurnal lighting patterns and

 2014, 24:R1150. habitat alter opsin expression and colour preferences in a

Unconventional colour vision is explored and defined. It is suggested that killifish. Proc R Soc Lond B 2013:280.

multiple colour channels, including up to nine in butterflies and 12 in This paper shows opsin expression varying on a diurnal basis, following

stomatopods are not attempts at increasing colour space dimensionality. to some extent lighting levels. While this example is in freshwater fish,

In the butterflies, spectral channels are added to conduct specific colour this is likely to occur in the marine environment also, reflecting changes

tasks or wavelength specific behaviours. Segregation of the retina also in light over the 24 h period. Such changes as with [63] are quantitative

contributes to spectral sensitivity multiplication, with different photore- amplification of expression within one opsin type rather appearance or

ceptor combinations examining different regions of visual space. This is disappearance of different types.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94

94 Molecular biology of sensation

65. Cronin TW, Caldwell RL, Marshall NJ: Sensory adaptation — the potentially new fluorescing compounds that it identifies. It does point

tunable colour vision in a mantis shrimp. Nature 2001, out in passing that the yellow filters found in the ocular media of several

411:547-548. fish, including many wrasse, may help enhance the fluorescent signal.

66. Marshall NJ: The visual ecology of reef fish colours. In Animal 71. Gerlach T, Sprenge D, Michiels NK: Fairy perceive and

Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication. respond to their deep red fluorescent coloration. Proc R Soc

Edited by Espmark Y, Amundsen T, Rosenqvist G. Tapir Press; Lond B 2014, 281:20140787.

2000:83-120. Part of an extended effort by Michiels and colleagues to show that

fluorescence is used in visual behaviour in fish. Mate choice tests

67. Marshall NJ: Conspicuousness and camouflage with the same

demonstrate that fish including the fluorescent component of their col-

‘bright’ colours. Colours for concealment and camouflage in

ours are favoured over those that have it removed by selective lighting.

reef fish. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 2000, 355:1243-1248.

However behavioural arena lighting conditions are not natural and fail to

differentiate the added component of the fluorescence that forms part of

68. Mazel C, Cronin TW, Caldwell RL, Marshall NJ: Fluorescent

an underlying and otherwise obvious colour pattern.

enhancement of signaling in a mantis shrimp. Science 2004,

303:51.

72. Kemp DJ, Herberstein ME, Fleishman LJ, Endler JA, Bennett ATD,

 Dyer AG, Hart NS, Marshall NJ, Whiting MJ: An integrative

69. Michiels N, Anthes N, Hart NS, Herler J, Meixner A,

framework for the appraisal of coloration in nature. Am Nat

Schleifenbaum F, Schulte G, Siebeck UE, Sprenger D,

2015. (in press).

Wucherer M: Red fluorescence in reef fish: a novel signalling

This review or synopsis examines current methods and approaches in

mechanism? BMC Ecol 2008, 8:16.

colour communication and colour vision in animals. It provides guidance

70. Sparks JS, Schelly RC, Smith WL, Davis MP, Tchernov D: The in methods without being prescriptive and identifies two different views;

covert world of fish biofluorescence: a phylogenetically first is the ‘top-down’ evolutionary/behavioural ecology school and sec-

widespread and phenotypically variable phenomenon. PLoS ond the more mechanistic ‘bottom-up’ neurophysiological/colour quan-

ONE 2014, 9:e83259. tification school. Between these sit colour vision models which, as the

This extensive survey of fish fluorescence using high-intensity excitation article explains, are being misused due to a lack of communication

sources notes over 180 species of fish that fluoresce. Whether this between the two schools. Ways to remedy this and form a more unified,

fluorescence is also visually relevant to the fish themselves under ambient and to some extent standardised, approach to understanding aspects of

illumination is not demonstrated and this work may be more interesting for colour vision are suggested.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2015, 34:86–94 www.sciencedirect.com