14 Patrick Suppes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

14 Patrick Suppes 14 Patrick Suppes Lucie Stern Professor of Philosophy, Emeritus Stanford University, USA 1. Why were you initially drawn to computational or in­ formational issues? I begin with some history. I am not sure just when I first thought about the use of computers for computation, but I do remember the late 1950s when Dick Atkinson and I were writing a book on applications of learning models and stimulus-response reinforce­ ment concepts to simple game situations. The book is full of ex­ tended computations supported by use of the I.B.M. 650 Com­ puter at Stanford, which was one of the very earliest made avail­ able to academic communities to have the possibility of going be­ yond the old days of computing with hand calculators. We say this in the preface of Suppes and Atkinson (1960): "Evan Linick has been indispensable in programming many problems for the I.B.M. 650 Computer at Stanford; we are also indebted to Richard Hill of the Western Data Processing Center for his cooperation." This brief acknowledgement is all we had to say about what was really a first, big-time, move into computing from my own standpoint. At that time, the only task was facilitating the many statistical estimates of parameters and the like for the models introduced in the book. It is important to realize how fundamental the change was in feasible applications of statistics with the introduction of digital computers in the 1950s. Even very simple formulations of linear-programming or linear-regression problems, that were prac­ tically unsolvable, could now be used to address all kinds of inter­ esting questions in theories of behavior and social interaction. I was pleased to be on the initial wave of this range of applications (for those reading this that do not know much about my back­ ground, I have spent as much of my:time as a scientist as I have as a philosopher.) 144 14. Patrick Suppes The next step was a significant one, and in fact an important predecessor in a long career of being concerned with the use of computers for instruction. The shift from data analysis to in­ struction is conceptually a big one: this direction involved a much deeper commitment to thinking about computation in many dif­ ferent ways. It began with something rather simple: the deliv­ ery of elementary exercises in arithmetic, mainly in the form of drill and practice, initially to students in elementary schools close to Stanford. In fact, the very first experiments began early in the 1960s when John McCarthy and I, with grants we had avail­ able, shared the expense of purchasing one of the early PDP-1's from Digital Equipment Corporation. To give a flavor of early work on computers, where we were concerned with the real-time performance for instructional purposes, I quote from an early book I co-authored on these matters (Suppes, Jerman, and Brian, 1968) recounting experiences in 1965-1966. In fact, the book's title is Computer-Assisted Instruction: Stanford's 1965-66 Arithmetic Program. Here is a quotation from Chapter 9 on the nature of the time-sharing system and CAl programming: This chapter briefly describes the operating system and programming language used to implement the drill­ and-practice system. The complete process of coding, inputting, and debugging is described in connection with several examples. This description is of the op­ erating system in effect during 1966-67, which is an improvement on that running during 1965-66. For a detailed description of the general time-sharing system in effect during 1965-66, see McCarthy et al (1967). The hardware used is a highly modified PDP-1 made by Digital Equipment Corporation. The PDP-1 is a small to medium-sized binary computer with 32,000 words of 5-microsecond core memory. The system op­ erates in a general time-sharing mode. The heart of the time-sharing system is a 131,000 word­ swapping drum. Each user program is normally read into memory in turn and allowed to run for a short period of time. Then the program is re-written on the drum and the next program is brought in. This process of interchanging two user programs from core memory to drum is known as program swapping. The process takes 32 milliseconds on the PDP-l. The new program 14. Patrick Suppes 145 swapped in is commonly allowed to run for 64 millisec­ onds if it does not create some situation causing it to be terminated early. Normal early terminations will consist of things such as waiting for input or output to be completed. For example, if a program tries to read a keyboard but there has been no input on the key­ board, the program will be dismissed until the input becomes available. A program so terminated will not be swapped into memory until the cause for its termi­ nation is removed. When input which was unavailable becomes available, the program is marked active and is put back in line to be run when its turn comes. To introduce the system response cycle, we take as an example a program which reads input (such as check­ ing for a correct single character answer to a question), and then responds with some output to the teletype. The period from the time the key is struck until the printer responds with the next message from the pro­ gram is known as the system response time. This time should be fast enough to give a human user the impres­ sion that there was little or no system response time. This system response time can be compared with re­ sponse times in devices such as automatic self-service elevators and telephones (for example, dial or busy tones). A system response time longer than 1-2 sec­ onds will create impatience and disinterest, and will be very annoying to users. The 131D data channel is connected to an IBM 1301 model II random-access disk file. The total capacity of this file is on the order of 336 million bits (56 million 6-bit characters of 1S-1/2 million IS-bit words). Two independent-access arms are provided. Maximum seek (arm movement) time is 180 millisec­ onds. When the appropriate arm is positioned there is a rotational delay of 0-34 milliseconds before the transfer may begin. The average delay is 17 millisec­ onds. The transfer rate is one IS-bit word about every 35 microseconds or every seventh memory cycle. The rate is comparable with that of high-speed magnetic tape drive~. However, the relatively fast random ac­ cess has a very distinct advantage over (serial-access) magnetic tape units in a time-sharing environment, 146 14. Patrick Suppes because of the rapid switching between user programs and hence between users' files (Suppes, Jerman, and Brian, 1968, pp. 291-92). These paragraphs seem really quaint forty years later. Just about every computing device, including current cell phones, has more computer power than we had available then. There is more progress between the PDP-1 and current computer facilities widely available than there is between the earlier introduction of the Model-T Ford in the 1920s and current automobiles, a much more radical conceptual evolution over a much shorter period. Notice however, that I am still concerned here with using com­ puters to deliver a product that is different, of course, than it could ever be delivered by other means, but still delivered as a rather standard product. To emphasize how differences are possi­ ble, I stress that the important psychological measure of response latency (i.e. the time it takes for a student to make each response), can be measured and studied carefully, something that is not pos­ sible in the classroom or even, really, by an individual tutor with no computer facilities. I can elaborate on this point, but will not here. In any case, practical involvement with computers had its theo­ retical consequences, however, and during the 1960s my thinking about many conceptual questions relevant to computing changed forever. One of the things I got involved in was the controversy between behaviorists and cognitivists, or linguists such as Noam Chomsky, on the nature of the mind, about which more in answer to the next question. 2.What examples from your work (or the work of others) best illustrate the fruitful use of computational and/or in­ formational approach for foundational researches and/or applications? The appearance of Chomsky's famous review (1959) of Skinner's Verbal Behavior (1957), marked a change that proved permanent in the acceptance of behaviorism, in particular the learning theory, of the stimulus-response variety that was dominant in American psychology from the 1920s to the latter part of the 1950s. Skin­ ner's Verbal Behavior, with its non-mathematical formulation, but with rather strong claims made in much too qualitative and vague a way, was an easy target for Chomsky's detailed critique. One of 14. Patrick Suppes 147 the primary effects was to disturb, again in a way that was a per­ manent change, the theoretical complacency of many behavioral psychologists. Since I followed this development rather closely, because of my attempts with Bill Estes and others to give a sharp mathematical formulation of stimulus-response theory, I was sometimes called "the white knight of behaviorism". Very early in the 1960s, I re­ member a famous public debate I had on these questions with Chomsky at Stanford. The aisles were crowded, there was no place to sit down, the students were enthralled, not because of me, but because of Chomsky.
Recommended publications
  • * Supplied by EMS Are the Best That Can Be Made from the Original
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 115 128 FL 007 330 AUTHOR Thogmartin, Clyde TITLE A Bibliography of Empirical Investigations of Certain Aspects of Foreign Language Teaching and Learning, 1925-1975. PUB DATE 75 NOTE 67p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.76 HC-$3.32 Plus Postage DESCRIPTORS Academic Achievement; *Bibliographies; Bilingual Education; Bilingualism; Educational Equipment; *Educational Research; Error Patterns; Fles; Individual Differences; Instructional Materials; Instructional Media; Interference (Language Learning); *Language Instruction; Language Proficiency; *Language Research; Language Skills; Language Tests; Psycholinguistics; *Second Language Learning; Student Attitudes; Teaching Methods; Transfer of Training; Vocabulary Development ABSTRACT This 647-item bibliography is limited to reports on empirical research and discussions of research reports on various aspects of foreign language learning, mostly drawn from American and British journals and Ph.D. dissertations since 1925 and from ERIC documents. Principal topics include content of teaching materials, cognitive effects of bilingualism, measurement of bilingual proficiency, psychological studies of bilinguals, relationships between various language skills, the psycholinguistics of second language learning, transfer and interference, error analysis, vocabulary learning, bilingual education, foreign languages in the elementary schools, individual differences in language learning success, prognosis, teaching methods, equipment, media, testing, language difficulty, and effects of language learning
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology of School Learning: Views of the Learner
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 104 859 SP 009 097 AUTHOR Bart, William M., Ed.; Wong, Martin R., Ed. TITLE Psychology of School Learning: Views of the Learner. Volume I: Environmentalism. INSTITUTION MSS Information Ccrp., New York, N.Y. PUB DATE 74 'NOTE 249p. AVAILABLE FROM MSS Information 'orporation, 655 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10021 ($6.25 paper, $12.00 cloth, 20 percent discount on orders of $200.00 or more) EDRS PRICE MF-$0.76 HC-$12.05 PLUS POSTAGE DESCRIPTORS Behavioral Objectives; Computer Assisted Instruction; Educational Philosophy; *Educational Psychology; *Environmental Influences; Higher Education; *Learning; *Learning Specialists; Reinforcement; Teacher Education ABSTRACT This document is the first of three volumes presenting essays from three schools of thought regarding learning. Volume one consists of readings from psychologists, philosophers, and learning theorists concerning the view that the learner isa product primarily of environmental factors. The list of essays includes the following:(a) "Ideas and Their Origin," (b) "The Free and Happy Student," (c) "The Technology of Teaching," (d) "Treatment of Nonreading in a Culturally Deprived Juvenile Delinquent: An Application of Reinforcement Principles," (e) "Production and Elimination of Disruptive Classroom Behavior by Systematically Varying Teacher's Behavior," (f) "Learning Theory Approaches to Classroom Management: Rationale and Intervention Techniques," (g) "A Token Reinforcement Program in a Public School: A Replication and Systematic Analysis," (h) "Educational
    [Show full text]
  • Commemorative Meeting for Alfred Tarski Stanford University-November 7, 1983
    Patrick Suppes received his Ph.D. in Philosophy in 1950 at Columbia Univer­ sity, where he worked with Ernest Nagel. He has been at Stanford since 1950 and is now Professor ofPhilosophy and Statistics. Suppes' main research inter­ ests include the philosophy of science, theory of measurement, decision theory and probability, and computer-assisted education. He is a member of the Na­ tional Academy of Sciences. Jon Barwise received his Ph.D. in Mathematics in 1967 at Stanford University, where he .worked with Solomon Feferman. After teaching at U.C.L.A., Yale, and Wisconsin, he returned to Stanford as Professor of Philosophy in 1983. Barwise's main research interests include mathematical logic, especially model theory, set theory, and generalized re­ cursion theory; and applications of logic to the semantics of natural language. At Stanford, Barwise has been Director of the Center for Study of Language and Information, and is currently chairman of the Program in Symbolic Sys­ tems. Solomon Feferman received his Ph.D. in Mathematics in 1957 at the University of California, Berkeley, where he studied with Alfred Tarski. He has been at Stanford since 1956, where he is Professor of Mathematics and Phi­ losophy. His research interests are in mathematical logic and the foundations of mathematics, especially proof theory and constructive and semiconstructive mathematics, as well as in the history of modern logic. Feferman is Editor-in­ Chief of the Collected Works of Kurt Godel; he is currently Chairman of the Department of Mathematics. Commemorative Meeting for Alfred Tarski Stanford University-November 7, 1983 PATRICK SUPPES, JON BARWISE, AND SOLOMON FEFERMAN, SPEAKERS INTRODUCTION The meeting was held in memory of Professor Alfred Tarski, who died at the age of 82 on October 28, 1983.
    [Show full text]
  • Calculus for a New Century: a Pump, Not a Filter
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 300 252 SE 050 088 AUTHOR Steen, Lynn Arthur, Ed. TITLE Calculus for a New Century: A Pump, Not a Filter. Papers Presented at a Colloquium _(Washington, D.C., October 28-29, 1987). MAA Notes Number 8. INSTITUTION Mathematical Association of America, Washington, D.C. REPORT NO ISBN-0-88385-058-3 PUB DATE 88 NOTE 267p. AVAILABLE FROMMathematical Association of America, 1529 18th Street, NW, Washington, DC 20007 ($12.50). PUB TYPE Viewpoints (120) -- Collected Works - Conference Proceedings (021) EDRS PRICE MF01 Plus Postage. PC Not Available from EDRS. DESCRIPTORS *Calculus; Change Strategies; College Curriculum; *College Mathematics; Curriculum Development; *Educational Change; Educational Trends; Higher Education; Mathematics Curriculum; *Mathematics Instruction; Mathematics Tests ABSTRACT This document, intended as a resource for calculus reform, contains 75 separate contributions, comprising a very diverse set of opinions about the shap, of calculus for a new century. The authors agree on the forces that are reshapinc calculus, but disagree on how to respond to these forces. They agree that the current course is not satisfactory, yet disagree about new content emphases. They agree that the neglect of teaching must be repaired, but do not agree on the most promising avenues for improvement. The document contains: (1) a record of presentations prepared fcr a colloquium; (2) a collage of reactions to the colloquium by a variety of individuals representing diverse calculus constituencies; (3) summaries of 16 discussion groups that elaborate on particular themes of importance to reform efforts; (4) a series of background papers providing context for the calculus colloquium; (5) a selection of final examinations from Calculus I, II, and III from universities, colleges, and two-year colleges around the country; (6) a collection of reprints of documents related to calculus; and (7) a list of colloquium participants.
    [Show full text]
  • William Kaye Estes Was Spread Upon the Permanent Records of the Faculty
    At a meeting of the FACULTY OF ARTS AND SCIENCES on November 6, 2012, the following tribute to the life and service of the late William Kaye Estes was spread upon the permanent records of the Faculty. WILLIAM KAYE ESTES BORN: June 17, 1919 DIED: August 17, 2011 William Kaye Estes, Daniel and Amy Starch Professor of Psychology, Emeritus, earned recognition early on as a member of a rare category containing those who exhibited both sterling personal leadership and outstanding intellectual contributions. His creative scholarship centered on learning theory, to which Harvard University’s own B. F. Skinner contributed in a major way. Many psychologists believed that the hard facts of learning theory needed a formal structure that could give rise to precise predictions. Bill Estes filled this need in 1950 in a classic paper, “Towards a Statistical Theory of Learning,” which described how organisms sampled the elements of a conditioning stimulus over successive trials so that, in time, the stimulus reliably evoked a conditioned response. Fred Skinner was not pleased that one of his best graduate students had abandoned the strict orthodoxy of behaviorism and theorized about mental events. Bill’s argument was so elegant it became one of the seedbeds for what evolved into the sub-discipline of mathematical psychology with a journal and a society founded in 1964 and 1977 respectively. Bill, along with Kenneth Arrow, Patrick Suppes, Richard Atkinson, R. R. Bush, and Duncan Luce, made pioneering contributions to many cognitive domains over a period spanning more than a half century. After spending his undergraduate and graduate years at the University of Minnesota and obtaining a Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Curricular Implications of Mathematical Concepts of the Preschool Child
    1 This dissertation has been microfilmed exactly as received 70-6763 DICKERSCHEID, Harold, 1927- CURRICULAR IMPLICATIONS OF MATHEMATICAL CONCEPTS OF THE PRESCHOOL CHILD. The Ohio State University, Ph.D., 1969 Education, theory and practice University Microfilms, Inc,, Ann Arbor, Michigan Copyright by Harold Dickerscheid 1970 CURRICULAR implications o f mathematical CONCEPTS OF THE PRESCHOOL CHILD DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Harold Dickerscheid, B.S., M.Ed. The Ohio State University 1969 Approved by Adviser College of Education ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer gratefully acknowledges his adviser, Dr, Paul R, Klohr, Curriculum and Foundations Faculty, for his insightful suggestions and critical analyses throughout the planning and writing of this study. Special acknowledgment is made to Dr, Nathan Lazar who gave impetus and encouragement to the study. Appreciation is extended to Dr, James K, Duncan for his time in reading and evaluating the manuscript. Gratitude is given to the staff and student teachers of the Campbell Hall Child Development Laboratory and to the children who participated in this study. Special thanks is given to Mrs, Donna Deichert, head teacher, and Mrs, Gretchen Batra, graduate assistant, for their cooperation with testing and observations. Thanks are also due to Mrs. Arlene Flocken and the children in her Head Start classes at Windsor School for their contribution. To my wife, Jean, and children
    [Show full text]
  • Mathematical Sciences Employment Register
    Program for the Annual Meeting in Cincinnati begins on page 20 Notices of the American Mathematical Society January 1982, Issue 215 Volume 29, Number 1, Pages 1-128 Providence, Rhode Island USA ISSN 0002-9920 CALENDAR OF AMS MEETINGS THIS CALENDAR lists all meetings which have been approved by the Council prior to the date this issue of the Notices was sent to press. The summer and annual meetings are joint meetings of the Mathematical Association of America and the Ameri· can Mathematical Society. The meeting dates which fall rather far in the future are subject to change; this is particularly true of meetings to which no numbers have yet been assigned. Programs of the meetings will appear in the issues indicated below. First and second announcements of the meetings will have appeared in earlier issues. ABSTRACTS OF PAPERS presented at a meeting of the Society are published in the journal Abstracts of papers presented to the American Mathematical Society in the issue corresponding to that of the Notices which contains the program of the meet· ing. Abstracts should be submitted on special forms which are available in many departments of mathematics and from the office of the Society in Providence. Abstracts of papers to be presented at the meeting must be received at the headquarters of the Society in Providence, Rhode Island, on or before the deadline given below for the meeting. Note that the deadline for ab· stracts submitted for consideration for presentation at special sessions is usually three weeks earlier than that specified below. For additional information consult the meeting announcement and the list of organizers of special sessions.
    [Show full text]
  • Models in Science
    Models in Science Reference: Frigg, Roman, Hartmann, Stephan "Models in Science", The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2006 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.). Online at http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/models-science/ Models are of central importance in many scientific contexts. The centrality of models such as the billiard ball model of a gas, the Bohr model of the atom, the MIT bag model of the nucleon, the Gaussian-chain model of a polymer, the Lorenz model of the atmosphere, the Lotka-Volterra model of predator-prey interaction, the double helix model of DNA, agent-based and evolutionary models in the social sciences, or general equilibrium models of markets in their respective domains are cases in point. Scientists spend a great deal of time building, testing, comparing and revising models, and much journal space is dedicated to introducing, applying and interpreting these valuable tools. In short, models are one of the principle instruments of modern science. Philosophers are acknowledging the importance of models with increasing attention and are probing the assorted roles that models play in scientific practice. The result has been an incredible proliferation of model-types in the philosophical literature. Probing models, phenomenological models, computational models, developmental models, explanatory models, impoverished models, testing models, idealized models, theoretical models, scale models, heuristic models, caricature models, didactic models, fantasy models, toy models, imaginary models, mathematical models, substitute models, iconic models, formal models, analogue models and instrumental models are but some of the notions that are used to categorize models. While at first glance this abundance is overwhelming, it can quickly be brought under control by recognizing that these notions pertain to different problems that arise in connection with models.
    [Show full text]
  • Scientific Progress on the Semantic View
    Scientific Progress On The Semantic View An Account of Scientific Progress as Objective Logical and Empirical Strength Increments Anders Solli Sal Master Thesis IFIKK, Faculty of Humanities University of Oslo Autumn 2013 1 Theories are our own inventions, our own ideas; they are not forced upon us, but are our self-made instruments of thought: this has been clearly seen by the idealist. But some of these theories of ours can clash with reality; and when they do, we know that there is a reality; that there is something to remind us of the fact that our ideas may be mistaken. And this is why the realist is right. - Karl Popper 2 Abstract The aim of this master thesis is to make a convincing argument that scientific progress can be spoken of in objective terms. In order to make this argument I will propose a philosophical theory of scientific progress. Two concepts will be constructed with this aim in mind, both which are types of strength measures on scientific theories. The first concept, that of logical strength, pertains to the way a theory may exclude, or permit less, model classes compared to another theory. The second concept, that of empirical strength, pertains to an objective measure of the informational content of data models, defined in terms of Kolmogorov complexity. This latter idea stems from communication and computational theory. Scientific progress is then defined as the interaction, or the stepwise increases, of these two strength measures. Central for the conception of a scientific theory is the philosophical framework known as The Semantic View of Scientific Theories.
    [Show full text]
  • The APA Guide to Graduate Programs in Philosophy
    2013 The APA GUide To GraduatE ProGrAms in PhilosoPhy University of Delaware Newark, DE 19716 www.apaonline.org All data collected by the American Philosophical Association. The data in this publication have been provided by the departments and programs listed; in the cases where no response was received to repeated requests for information, the most recent data provided, if any, can be found in the previous edition. The APA cannot guarantee the accuracy of this report; because all information has been self- reported, there may be errors or inconsistencies in these data. Readers should therefore examine the available data closely to ensure that any comparisons are made in a fair and reasonable manner. Errors or inconsistencies should be brought to the attention of the American Philosophical Association by email to [email protected]. For clarification, correction, updating, or supplementation of the information provided, contact the department(s) in question. © 2013, 2014 The American Philosophical Association This publication of the American Philosophical Association may not be reproduced for sale or future distribution without the express written consent of the executive director. Inquiries should be directed as follows: The American Philosophical Association University of Delaware, 31 Amstel Avenue, Newark, DE 19716 Phone (302) 831-1112 • Fax (302) 831-8690 [email protected] Last updated March 14, 2014 About the Grad Guide The Guide to Graduate Programs in Philosophy, originally published biennially until the early 2000s, was relaunched in 2012 as an annual online resource. The guide compiles data on both doctoral and master’s degree programs in philosophy at institutions throughout the US and Canada, offering prospective students, job candidates, and other members of the profession a rich resource on post- graduate education and employment in philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Invitational Conference on Testing Problems (New York, October 28, 1967)
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 080 541 TM 003 046 TITLE Invitational Conference on Testing Problems (New York, October 28, 1967).. INSTITUTION Educational Testing Service, Princeton, N.J.. PUB DATE 28 Oct 67 NOTE 139p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$6.58 DESCRIPTORS Computer Assisted Instruction; *Conference Reports; *Curriculum Development; Individualized Curriculum; *Instructional Innovation; Intelligence Tests; *Measurement; Professional Education; Program Evaluation; Public Policy; Simulation; Statistical Analysis; *Testing Problems; Test Interpretation ABSTRACT The 1967 Invitational Conference on Testing 'obiems dealt with various aspects of change in education,Papers pr-5ented in Session I, Evaluation and Research in Curriculum Development, were:(1) "Adapting the Elementary School Curriculum to Individual Performance" by Robert Glaser, and (2)"An Evaluation Model for Professional Education--Medical Education" by Christine H..McGuire. Papers given in Session II, New Approaches to Instruction, were: (1) "Computer-Based Instruction in Initial Reading" by Richard C. Atkinson, and (2) "Academic Games and Learning" by James S..Coleman.. The luncheon address was "Testing and Public Policy" by William Gorhan..Papers preSented at Session III, Measurement Systems, were: (1) "Sample-free Test Calibration and Person Measurement" by Benjamin D. Wright, (2) "Reformation through Measurement in Secondary Education" by Paul R. Lohnes, and (3) "Surveys Undertaken by the Scottish Council for Research in Education" by David A..Walker. (KM) FILMED FROM BEST AVAILABLE COPY U S OEPARTMENT OF HEALTH EOUCATION & WELFARE 1.---1 NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF EDUCATION FH.../ THIS 00CuMENT, HAS BEEN REPRO DucED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM 1.n- THE PERSON OR OPGAN,ZATIoN ORIGIN ATING IT POINTS OF VIER' OR OPINIONS (=) STATED 00 NOT NECESSARILY REPRE SENT OFFICIAL NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF OD EDUCATION POSITION OR POLICY C) 12' C:1 C 31, I Mir Ital .n t.1-...Le Oil_ -1-e.
    [Show full text]
  • Memorial Resolution for Prof
    Memorial Resolution for Prof. Emeritus H. Pierre Noyes Professor Emeritus H. Pierre Noyes, a founding member of the SLAC Theoretical Physics Group, died on September 30, 2016, in Stanford, California, at age 93. H. (Henry) Pierre Noyes was born on December 10, 1923, in Paris, France, to William Albert Noyes and Katharine Howarth Macy, daughter of Jesse Macy. His father was a Professor of Chemistry at the University of Illinois. Pierre graduated Harvard University in 1943 with a degree in physics. While he was at Harvard, his roommate was Thomas Kuhn. Pierre was a reader of an early draft of Kuhn's now famous book \The Structure of Scientific Revolutions". After Harvard, Pierre served two years in the US Navy as Aviation Electronics Technician's Mate, where he worked on radar technology and developed a new design for naval radar antennae. After his military service, Pierre began his postgraduate work culminating in a Ph.D. in 1950 at the University of California, Berkeley, in theoretical physics in 1950. His doctoral advisor was Geoffrey Chew (whose doctoral advisor had been Enrico Fermi). In 1951, Pierre began a term as an Assistant Professor at the University of Rochester, spending the first year as Fulbright scholar in Birmingham, England. During this period Pierre also worked on the classified Project Matterhorn, a magnetic fusion reactor at Princeton University. In 1955, Pierre joined what was to become the Lawrence Livermore National Laboratory, where he worked under directors Edward Teller and Ernest Lawrence on computations related to the theory of nuclear fusion and nuclear weaponry. From 1956 to 1962, he served at Lawrence Livermore National Laboratory as the group leader of the General Research Group.
    [Show full text]