<<

Journal of Studies in Learning and Teaching English Volume.Journal 6, Issue. of Studies 2, Ser. in 12, Learning (2017), 27-53 and Teaching English Volume. 6, Issue. 2, Ser. 12, (2017), 27-53 Unaccusative and Unergative Acquisition: Effects of Implicit, Explicit, and Mixed Oral UnaccusativeCorrective and Unergative Feedback Verb Acquisition: Effects of Implicit, Explicit, and Mixed Oral CorrectiveSeyedeh Maryam Feedback Meraji Ph.D Candidate DepartmentSeyedeh Maryamof Foreign Languages Meraji Shiraz Branch,Ph.D Islamic Candidate Azad University DepartmentShiraz, of Foreign Iran Languages ShirazEmail: Branch, mary [email protected] Azad University Shiraz, Iran Firooz Sadighi Email: mary [email protected]∗ Professor Department of Foreign Languages Firooz Sadighi∗ Shiraz Branch,Professor Islamic Azad University DepartmentShiraz, of Foreign Iran Languages ShirazEmail: Branch, [email protected] Islamic Azad University Shiraz, Iran Abstract. PersianEmail: L2 [email protected] learners of English over-passivize the unac- cusatives. Therefore, it is necessary that teachers identify erroneous over-passivization of unaccusatives and provide the necessary explana- Persian L2 learners of English over-passivize the unac- Abstract.tion of the differences between unaccusatives and unergatives. Issues cusatives. Therefore, it is necessary that teachers identify erroneous such as type of instructions provided by teachers in the Second Lan- over-passivization of unaccusatives and provide the necessary explana- guage Acquisition (SLA) classrooms still suffers from unanswered ques- tion of the differences between unaccusatives and unergatives. Issues tions. This study focused on the acquisition of the differences between such as type of instructions provided by teachers in the Second Lan- unaccusatives and unergativesby intermediate and advanced learners guage Acquisition (SLA) classrooms still suffers from unanswered ques- before and after receiving oral Corrective Feedback (CF). 106 students tions. This study focused on the acquisition of the differences between who scored 1 standard deviation below and above the mean score of unaccusatives and unergativesby intermediate and advanced learners the quick Oxford placement test (SD= 6.11, mean= 36.94), and also before and after receiving oral Corrective Feedback (CF). 106 students passed an elimination test examining their knowledge of passive forma- who scored 1 standard deviation below and above the mean score of tion rules, were selected as the participants. They were divided into 4 the quick Oxford placement test (SD= 6.11, mean= 36.94), and also groups (3 experimental and 1 control) according to the type of oral CF passed an elimination test examining their knowledge of passive forma- they were supposed to receive and the university courses they had to tion rules, were selected as the participants. They were divided into 4 enroll in. A Written Elicited Production Task (WEPT) and a Grammat- groups (3 experimental and 1 control) according to the type of oral CF icality Judgment Task (GJT) were administered as the pre-tests. Each they were supposed to receive and the university courses they had to experimental group received one type of oral CF as the treatment (ex- enroll in. A Written Elicited Production Task (WEPT) and a Grammat- plicit, implicit, or mixed) and the control group received no CF. Then icality Judgment Task (GJT) were administered as the pre-tests. Each Received:experimental February group 2017; received Accepted: one April type 2017 of oral CF as the treatment (ex- ∗Correspondingplicit, implicit, author or mixed) and the control group received no CF. Then

Received: February 2017; Accepted: April27 2017 ∗Corresponding author

27 28 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

the WEPT and GJT were administered as the post-tests. The results revealed that while students had the ability to judge the grammaticality of the sentences with unergatives before receiving oral CF, they did not have such ability for sentences with unaccusatives, and it was only after receiving the oral CF when they acquired the required knowledge.

Keywords: Unaccusative , unergative verbs, oral corrective feed- back, L2 acquisition

1. Introduction

Unaccusative and unergative verbs Perlmutter (1979) proposed Unaccusative hypothesis as a syntactic hy- pothesis that divides the intransitive verbs into two classes: unergatives and unaccusatives. According to this hypothesis, while the unaccusative verbs are verbs whose subjects have no responsibility for the action of the verb and the surface subject is not the real subject, unergative verbs are verbs with real subjects responsible for the action of the verb.

Logical problem of unaccusative and unergative verbs acquisi- tion As Sorace (1993) claimed, syntactic distinction between unaccusatives and unergatives varies in different languages. For example, Sorace ex- plained that in Italian, unaccusative verbs select the auxiliary “be” while unergative verbs select the auxiliary “have”. Another example was given by KarimiDoostan (1997), elaborating that Persian lets its native speakers alternate between causatives and unergatives by replacing the “do/KARDAN” with “be or become/ SHODAN”. Therefore, Persian speakers can use many simple or compound transitive verbs intransi- tively in their L1. In English, however, this alternation depends mainly on the transitivity or intransitivity of the verb, and when L2 learn- ers tend to resort to the verb ”SHODAN” construction (NP-be-V-en) in English, it will result in over-passivization of unaccusatives (Abbasi Bagherianpoor, 2010). As Abbasi Bagherianpoor (2010), Rezai and Ariamanesh (2012), and Abbasi and Mirjani Arjenan (2014) suggested, Persian L2 learners of English over-passivize the unaccusatives. Therefore, it is necessary that teachers identify such erroneous over-passivization of unaccusatives and Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 29 provide the learners with the necessary explanation of the differences between unaccusatives and unergatives.

Corrective feedback Errors are an inevitable part of language learning; learners of either the first or the second language may commit errors in their language pro- duction. George (1972) perceived children’s errors as transitional forms. He referred to native speakers’ errors as slip of the tongue and consid- ered second language learners’ errors as the unwanted forms. According to the existing literature, we can find different approaches toward errors in a second language learning context (James, 2013; Littlewood, 1984; Maicusi, Maicusi, & Lopez, 2000). In the behaviorist approach, an error is considered as a negative element that should be avoided; it is a sign, showing teaching is not effective, and therefore teachers should systematically prevent error oc- currence by intensive drilling, over-repetition, and over-teaching. An- other approach, with Chomsky’s (1965) ideas as its base, explains that errors lead to learners’ progress. In this mentalist approach, learners gain the necessary knowledge of the language by a trial and error pro- cess rather than over-learning. While the behaviorist approach regards errors as negative, the mentalist approach considers it as positive. In a cognitive approach, errors are viewed as reflections of social norms as well as learners’ cognitive processes; therefore, they are perceived as an outcome of the social-cognitive interaction. Following these different approaches, different researchers perceive and define errors from different perspectives. For example, Hawkins (1987) elaborated on the concept of error from an intrinsically ratio- nal point of view. He demonstrated that errors are not fixed and they change according to the index that they refer to. Furthermore, he ex- plained that errors can be identified through interaction. While Richards, Platt, and Platt (1992) considered errors of SLA as a linguistic misuse of an item which resulted from an incomplete learning, Selinker (1972) used the term interlanguage to refer to this misuse. In his view, interlan- guage was a unique language system created and used by learners while learning a second or foreign language. 30 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

The way a teacher recognizes the causes and origins of errors commit- ted by second/foreign language learners determines the attitude he/she may choose to deal with those errors. As Han (2002) and Lightbown and Spada (1999) described, behaviorists and audio-lingual proponents focused on the relationship between the negative transfer of the mother tongue experiences to the second language learning process. They be- lieved that the causes of errors can be found in the failed habit forma- tions that should be prevented; otherwise, errors may end with negative fossilization. On the other hand, Chomsky’s (1965) universal grammar theory ex- plained that human beings are equipped with a biologically language learning ability which brings them the creativity in language learn- ing. This creativity can be explained by the systematic productions of learners which are perceived as a sign of progress in learning. So, errors made by language learners should not be prevented because they are indeed signs of learning, not symptoms of failure. With errors being an inevitable part of learning a second language, teachers are driven into a pedagogical context in which providing learn- ers with feedback is really essential. The first definition of feedback in language learning defined it as any type of information provided for the learner after receiving the learner’s response to a question or to a task (Wager & Wager, 1985). This definition was too broad and included all post-response information given to the learner, therefore, researchers tried to find a more relevant definition which was more limited. Asa result, it was defined as multiple strategies which do not immediately give the correct answers to students, and instead, provide elaborated responses that help learners complete the task successfully (Annett, 1969; Bilodeau, 1969; Narciss, 2008). Another was proposed by Lee- man (2007),he argued that feedback can be defined as a reaction to the learner’s utterance, but evidence can be given to the learner at any stage of his/her production. Researchers also found out that corrective feedback strategies can be provided for the learner in implicit or explicit ways. In an explicit corrective feedback, a formal explanation is provided after the error com- mitted, and implicit corrective feedback involves some ways that indicate Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 31 the incorrectness of the learner’s utterance and ask for a reformulation (Campillo, 2003).

Oral corrective feedback Lyster and Ranta (1997) drew on strategies from previous studies and added new strategies based on their analysis of teacher-student interac- tions and identified explicit correction, elicitation, clarification requests, recasts, metalinguistic feedback, and repetition of error corrective feed- back. While the explicit correction is considered an explicit corrective feedback strategy, the latter strategies provide the corrective feedback implicitly. Since these feedback strategies proved to be useful in teacher- student interactions, the following strategies were selected to be used as oral corrective feedback strategies of the study: recasts (which involve the teachers’ reformulation of all or part of the students’ utterance minus the error part); explicit feedback (which refers to the explicit provision of the correct form); clarification request ( which shows the student that either the teacher has misunderstood the utterance or the utterance is ill-formed and a reformulation is needed); metalinguistic feedback (in- volves comments or questions regarding the well-formedness of the stu- dents’ utterance without explicitly mentioning the correct form); elici- tation (which consists of three techniques: a. teacher utters a sentence then pauses and lets the student fill the required answer; b. teacher uses questions to elicit the correct forms; c. teacher occasionally asks students to reformulate their sentences.); repetition (is the teacher’s repetition of the students incorrect utterance) suggested by Lyster and Ranta (1997); and prompt or negotiation of form (in which the teacher withholds the reformulation and the student is encouraged to correct the error himself/herself) proposed by Lyster and Mori (2006). Unergative and unaccusative verbs appear in the learning materials at different levels of learning, and acquiring them is necessary. Since corrective feedback strategies proved to be effective on syntactic acqui- sition of L2 learners (Kawaguchi & Ma, 2012), this study focused on the effects of explicit and implicit oral corrective feedback strategies, on the acquisition of unaccusative and unergative verbs in an Iranian EFL context. 32 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

Literature Review

Due to importance of corrective feedback in different language learning theories and the effects of feedback strategies on L2 grammar acquisition, many studies have been conducted on the issue (e.g., Bowles & Montrul, 2009; Kawaguchi and Ma, 2012; Zohrabi and Ehsani, 2014; Tamayo Maggi and Quishpe,2017). Bowles and Montrul (2009) investigated the possible role of explicit teaching and negative evidence on a-personal (a grammatical feature of Spanish) learning. They explained that form-focused instruction either in the form of grammatical explanation or in the form of corrective feedback, during or even before exposure to L2, could provide students with explicit information. The researchers conducted their study by 72 participants who were divided into two groups: one group consisted of 12 native speakers of Spanish who lived in different countries at the time of the study and another group consisted of 60 low-intermediate language learners whose L1 was English. The participants of the second group were divided into two groups, one received instructions on the targeted structure and served as an experimental group while the other did not receive any instruction and just took the pre-and the post-tests. The results of the obtained data revealed that the researchers’ instruction had the desired effect, and after providing the learners with instruction and feedback, they were able to accept the grammatical forms more easily than the ungrammatical ones. In another study, Kawaguchi and Ma (2012) investigated the role of oral corrective feedback and negotiation of meaning on L2 learners’ grammar development. They followed an Interactionist Approach intro- duced by Long (1996). The participants of their study were 6 adult in- formants: two of them were English native speakers, two were non-native speakers with high proficiency level of English and the other two were non-native speakers with low proficiency level of English. The researchers used board game to elicit past tense marking in the sentences, interac- tion conversation to obtain all types of grammatical items and picture describing and drawing to elicit subject-verb agreement, plural forms and question formation. They observed that the provided lin- Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 33 guistic environment of non-native speakers and non-native speakers was comparable with that of native speakers and non-native speakers. The researchers concluded that the non-native-non-native combination either with high proficient learners or the low proficient ones provided a bet- ter outcome of corrective feedback compared with the non-native-native combinations. Kawaguchi and Ma also suggested that the low proficient level learners received the best type of corrective feedback along with the best type of negotiation of meanings from the high proficient level learners. Their results revealed that since all learners had fewer errors in their post-tests compared with their pre-test, the provided corrective feedback was considered to be positively effective. Focusing on the effects of different types of corrective feedbacks on grammar accuracy and learners’ awareness, Zohrabi and Ehsani (2014) asked 60 pre-intermediate EFL students to translate simple present and simple past tense sentences from Persian to English. They taught present and the past tense before the administration of the translation test. After correcting the tests, they provided the explicit feedback group with explicit written feedback for their errors, and the implicit feedback group just had their errors specified and no corrective feedback was pro- vided. Based on the obtained results, Zohrabi and Ehsani concluded that the Iranian EFL learners’ improved after receiving the treatment. Considering acquisition of unaccusative and unergative verbs, Ab- basi and MirjaniArjenan(2014) used 38 subjects with different educa- tional backgrounds to investigate the translation challenges that the translators faced while translating English unaccusative verbs into Per- sian. The researchers argued that among the main problems that the translators usually faced was the translation of the and the unaccusative verbs. They used a corpus, and three available Persian translations of it, and also a questionnaire to obtain readers’ responses to different kinds of translation. The results of their study revealed that passives and unaccusatives were treated differently by translators. Ab- basi and MirjaniArjenan concluded that readers preferred active trans- lation of passive forms to the passive one, and unaccusative translation of the unaccusatives to other translated forms. 34 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

Tamayo Maggi and Quishpe (2017) investigated the effects of corrective feedback on grammatical accuracy of students’ oral interactions. Their 18-week study involved twenty-eight participants in two classes. While class 1 received metalinguistic corrective feedback, class 2 received re- formulation. The results revealed that the provision of the two types of corrective feedback led to a significant correction of statements produced by students in specific linguistic structures. The researchers claimed that the performance of the group of students who received the metalinguistic corrective feedback was better than the group of students who received reformulation. The findings of this study suggested that language teach- ers should use more metalinguistic feedback for the treatment of EFL students’ errors when interacting orally. Despite the studies conducted on the effectiveness of corrective feed- back (e.g. Bitchener, Young, & Cameron, 2005; Russell & Spada, 2006; Bowles & Montrul, 2009; Sauro, 2009; Tamayo Maggi and Quishpe, 2017), and the studies focused on acquisition of unaccusative and unerga- tive verbs (e.g. Oshita, 1997; Hirakawa, 2000; Park and Lakshmanan, 2007; Mortazavi, 2012; Abbasi Bagherianpoor, 2010; Rezai & Aria- manesh, 2012; Abbasi & MirjaniArjenan, 2014), the existing literature lacks thorough research on the effects of oral corrective feedback on the acquisition of unaccusative/unergative verbs and the aim of the present study is to fill this gap in the literature.

Objectives and Research Questions of the Study

Syntax acquisition plays an important role in second/foreign language acquisition. Although the literature on the use of corrective feedback strategies is extensive, it suffers from neglecting the role of feedback strategies on some specific parts of syntax like unergatives and unac- cusatives in EFL contexts with a non-Roman language as the L1. Hence, one of the objectives of this study is to provide the L2 ped- agogical context with information about the effectiveness of oral cor- rective feedback. Furthermore, the study aims at informing L2 teachers of the most appropriate feedback strategies in teaching unergative and unaccusative verbs. It also tries to provide language instructors with Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 35 information on the possible differences between L2 learners with differ- ent levels of proficiency (intermediate and advanced) and the type of corrective feedback which is more effective for learners of each profi- ciency level. Based on the above objectives and to fill the present gap of the applied linguistics literature, the present study seeks to answer the following research questions: Q1. Does providing oral corrective feedback affect the Iranian EFL learners’ acquisition of unaccusatives and unergatives? Q2. Which type of corrective feedback (implicit, explicit or mixed) is more effective in the acquisition of unaccusatives and unergatives? Q3. At which level of language learning (intermediate or advanced) can learners best take advantage of the corrective feedback while learning unergatives and unaccusatives?

Method

Participants Participants of this study were 137 Iranian EFL learners chosen based on a combination of random and nonprobability sampling procedures. The participants were studying either teaching English as a foreign language or English translation at the Islamic Azad University and Payam-E-Noor University of Shiraz, and their age ranged from 19 to 40 years. The re- searchers selected their classes randomly from among the courses that they were teaching at the time of the study. From among the 137 par- ticipants, those who gained %66.66 of the Elimination Test score and did not participate in any other English classes outside the university were chosen for the study (106 participants). The selected participants were then randomly divided into 4 different groups according to different classes they had to enroll in and according to the type of oral correc- tive feedback they were supposed to receive, based on a nonprobability convenience sampling. Instruments Elimination Test (ET). The ET was taken from Mortazavi’s (2012) 36 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi study and evaluated the participants’ knowledge of passive construction in English. The test consisted of 9 fill-in-the-blank items and participants with 3 or more errors out of 9 were excluded from the study as their judgments were likely to be due to the lack of necessary knowledge of the passive forms. The reliability of the test was calculated at the pilot study stage of the study and the coefficient of equivalence was .94 which showed a high index of reliability. Oxford Placement Test (OPT). OPT is a standard language profi- ciency placement test, consisting of two different parts (a grammar test and a listening test), each with 50 multiple choice items which evaluate learners’ correct use of English. Since the aim of the present study was to investigate the syntactic ability of the participants and not their lis- tening ability, the grammar test of OPT was used and the listening test was eliminated. Reliability of the test which was calculated at the pilot study stage of the study revealed a coefficient of equivalence of .89, and a reliability index of .94 which showed a high index of reliability. Written Elicited Production Task (WEPT). The WEPT was taken from Hirakawa’s (2000) study and was designed to elicit information about EFL learners’ ability to distinguish passive structures from unac- cusative verbs in English. It targeted incorrect use of passive morphology to unaccusative verbs and consisted of 24 items. The test reliability was calculated and it was .81, which was acceptable. Grammaticality Judgment Task (GJT). The GJT was adopted from Hirakawa’s (2000) study and aimed at evaluating participants’ abil- ity of distinguishing between unaccusatives and unergatives. Pseudopas- sive and constructions were the structures which were used in the GJT in order to determine the participants’ ability to distinguish un- accusatives from unergatives and passives. The Cronbach’s Alpha index of reliability for the GJT was reported .75.

Procedures

Data collection. At the beginning of the study, the researchers ex- plained the voluntary nature of the study, the anonymity of participants’ Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 37 responses and also the purpose of the study to the participants. Then, the participants completed the OPT and the ET. The results of ET were used to eliminate the participants who did not know the passive rules. The mean score and the standard deviation of all participants’ OPT scores were calculated (mean=36.94, SD= 6.11) and those partic- ipants who scored one standard deviation above and below the mean (i.e., participants with scores ranging from 43.05 to 30.83) were selected to complete the WEPT and the GJT. The participants of the study (N=106) were randomly divided into 4 different groups according to different classes they had to enroll in and according to the type ofcor- rective feedback they were supposed to receive. While 3 groups were the experimental groups of the study and received implicit oral CF, explicit oral CF, and implicit and explicit oral CF, one group was the control group of the study and received no CF. The participants of all 4 groups followed a similar procedure for data collection of the study. At the beginning they were all asked to complete the WEPT and the GJT in regular class hours. They had 40 minutes to answer the WEPT and 25 minutes for the OPT. The necessary in- struction for the tasks was provided by the researchers and in cases of clarification requests, some explanation was provided in the participants’ L1. For both tasks an additional 5 to 10 minutes were allowed in case the participants needed extra time to complete the task. The first ad- ministrations of the WEPT and the GJT were used as the pretests of the study. In the following sessions, the nature and concept of unergative and unaccusative verbs were taught to all groups, The WEPT and GJT were corrected and returned to the participants. Participants of the groups chosen for the oral CF received their CF during a short one by one interview with the researcher. Each interview lasted 2-4 minutes and all interviews were conducted after the official class hours. Participants of the explicit oral CF received feedback in the form of comments regarding the well-formedness of the responses or the explicit provision of the correct response (Example 1).

Example 1: Researcher: ... we don’t use the passive structure here. As you remem- 38 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi ber, “increase” is an , so the answer is “increased”. Participant: Oh. I remember that. – For this item, explicit corrective feedback was provided (i.e. the re- searcher provides the explicit provision of the correct form). The implicit oral CF group received their CF in the form of recast, clarification request, repetition, elicitation, and metalinguistic feedback showing that a reformulation is needed (Example 2 and Example 3).

Example 2: Researcher: I can’t understand. Why did you use “was died”? Participant: Because she was dead [Pause and stop answering]. Researcher: Excuse me? Can you explain it more? Participant: Ehh[pause and think], wait, oh I think the answer is just “died”. Is it right? – For this item, the corrective feedback was provided in the form of clar- ification request (i.e. the researcher shows the student that she hasmis- understood, or the utterance is ill-formed and reformulation is needed).

Example 3: Teacher: “Clothes were dried in the sun”? “were dried”? Participant: Should I use present tense, or the sentence is not passive? Teacher: Do you remember the unaccusatives? Participant: Eeee, no, not exactly. Teacher: [The teacher briefly explains the characteristics of unaccusatives], now can you answer the question? Participant: Oooh, now I can remember! it should be “dried” – For this item, the corrective feedback was provided in the form of rep- etition (i.e. the teacher repeats the incorrect part) followed by an ex- planation to encourage the student to correct the answer (i.e. prompt). Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 39

The mixed oral CF group received a combination of both explicit and implicit CFs. Participants of the group with no CF received their papers and did not receive any type of feedback at all. After providing the experimental groups with the feedbacks, and the control group with no feedback, the researcher asked all participants to return their the WEPT and the GJT papers (the same session they received the paper and the feedback). Eight weeks after the first admin- istration of the WEPT and the GJT, the tasks were administered again as the post-tests. Collecting the necessary data, the researcher checked and listed all the results for data analysis.

Data analysis. Items of the WEPT and the GJT had four types of verbs (transitive, unergative, alternating unaccusative and non-alternating un- accusative). The SPSS Version 16 was used to compare the participants’ performance in each type of verb in both pre- and post-tests by the t-test and ANOVA analysis.

Results

Results of WEPT The WEPT was used to evaluate the participants’ knowledge of un- accusatives and unergatives at the beginning and at the end of the study. To provide answer to the first research question of the study, Paired samples t-test was run. The results are shown in Table 1. As the results in Table 1 show, the reported mean differences of all groups were negative, which indicates the score in the post-test was higher. The significant level of the no feedback group (the control group) was larger than .05, for the three verb types, indicating that the difference observed in no feedback group was not statistically significant. The significance level (p < .05) of all experimental groups indicated that the difference was statistically significant.

WEPT was used to evaluate participants’ knowledge of unaccusatives and unergatives, at the beginning and at the end of the study. To provide answer to the first research questionof the 40 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi study, Paired samples t-test was run. The results are shown in Table 1.

TableTable 1 1: Paired-samples t-tests on alternating unaccusative verbs of Paired-samples t-tests on alternating unaccusativeWEPT verbs of WEPT Paired Differences 95% Confidence Std. Interval of the Sig. Error Difference (2- Effect Verb Group Test Mean SD Mean Lower Upper t df tailed) size alternating EO CF pre - post -1.214 .787 .149 -1.519 -.909 -8.167 27 .000 .711 unaccusative IO CF pre - post -1.229 .808 .136 -1.506 -.951 -9.001 34 .000 .704 MO pre - post -1.300 .675 .213 -1.738 -.817 -6.091 9 .000 .804 CF No CF pre - post -.091 .579 .101 -.296 .114 -.902 32 .374 - non- EO CF pre - post -1.107 .629 .119 -1.351 -.863 -9.316 27 .000 .771 alternating IO CF pre - post - -1.457 .611 .103 -1.667 -1.247 34 .000 .854 unaccusative 14.113 MO pre - post -1.600 .516 .163 -1.969 -1.231 -9.798 9 .000 .914 CF No CF pre - post -.152 .508 .088 -.331 .028 -1.715 32 .096 - unergative EO CF pre - post -.929 1.152 .218 -1.375 -.482 -4.264 27 .000 .402 IO CF pre - post -.771 1.003 .169 -1.116 -.427 -4.552 34 .000 .378 MO pre - post -.900 .738 .233 -1.428 -.372 -3.857 9 .004 .623 CF No CF pre - post -.121 .696 .121 -.368 .126 -1.000 32 .325 - EO=Explicit Oral, IO=Implicit Oral, MO=Mixed Oral, CF =Corrective Feedback

The effectAs the results size in Tablewas calculated1 show, the reported for each mean differences experimental of all groups group were negative,to determine thewhich relative indicates magnitudethe score in the of post the-test difference was higher. The between significant thelevelscores of the no offeedback the pre- andgroup post-tests (the control of group) each was group. larger Thethan .05, effect for the sizes three verb of all types experimental, indicating that thegroups were considered as large effect sizes.

After comparing the pre-and post-test18 scores of each group and cal- culating the effect sizes, a One-way ANOVA with different types of cor- rective feedback received by the experimental groups as between-group factor( i.e. implicit, explicit or mixed), examined the difference in stu- dents’ gain sores from one group to others to answer the second research question of the study (Table 2). The summarized results of One-way ANOVA, as presented in Table 2, in- dicated that no statistically significant difference was foundp ( = .754 > .05). The results suggested that providing explicit/implicit/mixed feed- back has no significant effect on the participants’ learning of unac- cusatives and unergatives.

difference observed in no feedback group was not statistically significant. The significance level

(p < .05) of all experimental groups indicated that the difference was statistically significant. The effect size was calculated for each experimental group to determine the relative magnitude of the difference between the scores of the pre- and post-test of each group. The effect sizes of all experimental groups were considered as large effect sizes.

After comparing the pre- and post-test scores of each group and calculating the effect sizes, a One-way ANOVA with different types of corrective feedback received by the experimental groups as between-group factor( i.e. implicit, explicit or mixed), examined the difference in students’ gain sores from one group to others to answer the second research question of the study (Table 2).

Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 41 Table 2 Table 2: One-way ANOVA analysis for mean of gain scores of all One-way ANOVA analysis for mean of gain scores of all experimental experimental groups groups Alternating unaccusatives Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Groups .051 2 .026 .296 .754 Within Groups .520 6 .087 Total .572 8

TheResults summarized of GJT results of One-way ANOVA, as presented in Table 2, indicated that no The GJT was used to determine intermediate/advanced students ability statisticallyto significant differentiate difference unergative was found verbs (p= from . 754 unaccusative > .05). The verbs. results While suggested a scale that of (+2) represented the maximum score accepting a sentence, a scale of (- providing 2)explicit/implicit/mixed represents the maximum feedback score has rejectingno significant the sentence. effect on Therefore,the participants’ pos- learning itive mean scores on the acceptable sentences and negative mean scores of unaccusatives and unergatives. on the unacceptable sentences showed students’ ability to judge gram- maticality of the sentences correctly. Pseudopassive and resultative con- structions were the structures used19 in the GJT in order to determine in- termediate/advanced students ability to distinguish unaccusatives from unergatives. Resultative structure. Resultatives aimed at examining students’ deep knowledge of unaccusative and unergative verbs. While resultatives are compatible with transitive objects and alternating unaccusative sub- jects, they are not compatible with unergative subjects. Therefore, a mean score of (-2) for unergative verbs and (+2) for unaccusative verbs showed the students’ maximum acquisition of unergative and un- accusative verbs, respectively. In Figure 1, the bar-graphs of the pre-test mean scores of the interme- diate and advanced level students of each group are illustrated. 42 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

-

Figure 1. Pre-test mean scores of verbs with resultative structure

As it is illustrated in Figure 1, the mean scores of the pre-test items focusing on unergative verbs indicate that all groups had the ability to determine the ill-formedness of the sentences with unergative verbs with the resultative structure. On the other hand, regarding the group performance on unaccusative verbs with resultative structure in the pre- test, the bar-graphs indicate that except the advanced level students of the mixed oral groups, all other groups failed to identify the well- formedness of the sentences. As the focus of the study was on the effects of corrective feedback, mean of the post-test scores of students was also calculated for each group. Figure 2 illustrates the performance of each group after receiving the corrective feedback. Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 43

-

Figure 2. Post-test mean scores of verbs with resultative structure structure

As it is illustrated in Figure 2, the mean scores of students’ judgments on the well-formedness of sentences with unergative verbs are higher compared to mean scores of the students’ judgments on the pre-test. Re- garding the unaccusative verbs, Figure 2 illustrates that while students’ judgments on pre-test indicated no or little ability of identifying the well-fromedness of sentences, the bars of the post-test are in the posi- tive side which showed students ability to differentiate the correct forms from the incorrect ones. As the comparison of the bars direction and their magnitude from the pre-test to post-test indicate an improvement in all experimental groups, the mean differences of the pre- and post-tests of the different levels of the experimental groups were calculated (Table 3). As it is shown in Table 3, the intermediate-level students, who re- ceived a combination of implicit and explicit oral corrective feedback, had the highest improvement in judging the grammaticality of sentences with unaccusative (1.02) and unergative verbs (1.07). 44 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

Table 3: Mean differences of pre- and post-test scores of verbs with 44 S.resultative M. Meraji structure and F. Sadighi Table 3 TableMean 3: differencesMean differences of pre- and of postpre--test and scores post-test of verbs scores with of verbs with resultative structure resultative structure Group 44 LevelS. M. MerajiExplicit and F. Sadighi Implicit Mixed oral oral oral TableUnaccusative 3: Mean differencess Intermediate of pre-0.70 and post-test0.85 scores1.02 of verbs with Advancedresultative structure0.82 1.01 0.60 Unergatives Intermediate 0.63 0.61 1.07 Advanced 0.71 0.55 0.10

As it Pseudopassiveis shown in Table Structure. 3, the intermediatePseudopassive-level students, structures, who like received resultative a combination of structures, aimed at examining the students’ deep knowledge of unac- implicit and cusative explicit and oral unergative corrective verbs. feedback, Unlike had resultatives, the highest the improvement pseudopassive in judging the structure is allowed with transitive and unergative verbs’ subjects, but Pseudopassive Structure. Pseudopassive structures, like resultative not allowed with unaccusative verbs’ subjects. Therefore, a mean score grammaticalitystructures, of sentences aimed with at unaccusative examining the (1.02) students’ and unergative deep knowledge verbs (1.07). of unac- of (+2) for unergative verbs and (-2) for unaccusative verbs showed the cusative and unergative verbs. Unlike resultatives, the pseudopassive students’ maximum acquisition of unergative and unaccusative verbs, structure is allowed with transitive and unergative verbs’ subjects, but Pseudopassiverespectively.Structure In Figure.Pseudopassive 3, the pre-tests structure, mean scores like of resultative the intermediate structure, aimed at not allowed with unaccusative verbs’ subjects. Therefore, a mean score and advanced level students of each group are illustrated. of (+2) for unergative verbs and (-2) for unaccusative verbs showed the examining students’Pseudopassive deep knowledge Structure. of unaccusativePseudopassive and structures, unergative like verbs. resultative Unlike resultatives, structures,students’ maximum aimed at acquisition examining the of unergative students’ deep and unaccusative knowledge of verbs, unac- pseudopassivecusativerespectively. structure and is In unergativeallowed Figure with 3, verbs. the transitive pre-tests Unlike and mean resultatives, unergative scores of theverbs’ the pseudopassive intermediate subjects, but not allowed structureand advanced is allowed level students with transitive of each and group unergative are illustrated. verbs’ subjects, but with unaccusativenot allowed verbs’ withsubjects.Therefore, unaccusative verbs’ a mean subjects. score Therefore,of (+2) for a unergative mean score verbs and (-2) of (+2) for unergative verbs and (-2) for unaccusative verbs showed the for unaccusativestudents’ verbs maximum showed students’ acquisition maximum of unergative acquisition and unaccusative of unergative verbs, and unaccusative respectively.Figure 3. Pre-test In Figure mean 3, the scores pre-tests of verbs mean with scores Pseudopassivestructure of the intermediate verbs, respectively.and advanced level students of each group are illustrated.

In FigureFigure 3, the 3. prePre-test-tests mean scores ofof verbsthe intermediate with Pseudopassivestructure and advanced level students of each group are illustrated.

Figure 3. Pre-test mean scores of verbs with Pseudopassivestructure

23

Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 45

As it is shown in Figure 3, both intermediate and advanced level stu- dents had the ability to judge the grammaticality of the sentences with unergative verbs and pseudopassive structure. The bar-graphs of stu- dents’ judgments on sentences with unaccusative verbs illustrates that all groups but the advanced-level students, who were supposed to re- ceive a combination of implicit and explicit corrective feedback, failed to judge the grammaticality of sentences with unaccusative verbs and pseudopassive structure. As the focus of the study was to compare the students’ judgments before and after receiving corrective feedback, Figure 4 illustrates the performance of each group after receiving the corrective feedback. The bar-graphs of Figure 4 indicate a change in students’ grammat- icality judgments for both unergative and unaccusative verbs. However, the students’ grammaticality judgment of unergative verbs in the pre- tests revealed that the judgments’ scores were on the positive side of the axis. The post-test results indicated that the students’ ability to judge the grammaticality of sentences with unergative verbs had improved.

Figure 4. Post-test mean scores of verbs with Pseudopassivestructure

As the comparison of the bars’ direction and their magnitude from pre- test to post-test indicates an improvement in all groups, the mean differ- 46 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

ences of the pre-and post-tests of the different levels of all groups were

calculated (Table 4). Table 4 Table 4: Mean differences of pre-and post-test scores of verbs with Mean differences of pre- and post-test scores of verbs with pseudopassive structure pseudopassive structure Group Level Explicit Implicit Mixed oral oral oral Unaccusatives Intermediate 0.71 0.79 0.96 Advanced 0.82 0.96 0.95 Unergatives Intermediate 0.59 0.58 1.04 Advanced 0.69 0.51 0.05

As it is shownAs it is in shown Table in 4 Table, the intermediate 4, the intermediate-level-level students, students, who who received received a combination of a combination of implicit and explicit oral corrective feedback, had the implicit and explicithighest oral improvement corrective in judging feedback, the grammaticality had the highest of the improvement sentences with in judging the unaccusative (0.96) and unergative verbs (1.04). grammaticality of theTo sentences provide answer with for unaccusative the third research (0.96) question and unergative of the study, verbs and (1.04). to find out at which level of language learning (intermediate or advanced) can learners best take advantage of the corrective feedback, an inde- To providependent answer samples for the t-tests third was research run for question intermediate-and of the study, advanced-level and to find out at which learners’ performance on resultative and pseudopassive structures in all level of languageexperimental learning (intermediate groups (Table 5). or advanced) can learners best take advantage of the As it is shown in Table 5, none of the structures (i.e., resultative corrective feedback,& pseudopassive) an independent revealed samples no significant t-tests was difference run for intermediate between the- and perfor- advanced-level mance of advanced-and intermediate-level students. Therefore, despite learners’ performanceof the different on resultative mean andscores pseudopassive obtained by structuresdifferent groups, in all experimentalno significant groups difference was found. (Table 5).

26

Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 47

Table 5: Independent Samples t-test for intermediate- and Table 5 advanced-level learners Independent Samples t-test for intermediate- and advanced-level learners Levene's Test for Equality t-test for Equality of Means of Variances 95% Std. Sig. Mean Confidence Error F Sig. t df (2- Differenc Interval of the Differen tailed) e Difference ce Lower Upper Equal variances .306 .592 1.213 10 .253 .18167 .14976 -.15201 .51535

assumed e Equal variances 1.213 8.548 .258 .18167 .14976 -.15986 .52319

Resultativ not assumed Equal variances 1.314 .278 .715 10 .491 .11500 .16085 -.24340 .47340 assumed

ssive Equal variances .715 7.785 .496 .11500 .16085 -.25772 .48772

Pseudopa not assumed

As it is shown in Table 5, none of the structures (i.e., resultative&pseudopassive) Discussion and Conclusion revealed any significant difference between the performance of advanced- and intermediate-level

Consideringstudents. Therefore, the despite findings of the from different the mean scoresWEPT, obtained the scores by different of groups, the post-tests no of all three groups who received oral corrective feedback were signifi- cantlysignificant different difference was from found. the scores of the pre-tests. Regarding the unergative verbs, the results revealed that the post-tests performance of all groups indicated an improvementDiscussion and the and effect conclusion sizes of all three groups who received oral corrective feedback, were considered as large. The results Considering the findings from WEPT, the scores of the post-tests of all three groups which also revealed that the effect size of the group who received a combina- tionreceived of oral implicit corrective and feedback explicit were oral significantly corrective different feedback from the was scores larger of the than pre-tests. the other two groups who received either explicit or implicit oral corrective feedback.Regarding the Althoughunergative verbs, the the groups results revealed who receivedthat the post oral-tests performance corrective of feedback all groups had different effect sizes, the results of the One-way ANOVA revealed indicated an improvement and the effect sizes of all three groups which received oral corrective no significant difference between the explicit, implicit and mixed oral strategies.feedback, were considered as large. The results also revealed that the effect size of the group which received a combination of implicit and explicit oral corrective feedback was larger than the other two groups which received either explicit or implicit oral corrective feedback. Although

27

48 S. M. Meraji and F. Sadighi

A comparison of the results of the present study with those of Kawa- guchi and Ma (2012) showed that in both studies oral corrective feed- back was found to be effective for L2 learners’ grammar development. Kawaguchi and Ma evaluated the participants’ performance on noun plural forms, verb past tense, subject-verb agreement, and question for- mation. They stated that oral corrective feedback and negotiation of meaning resulted in better performance by the participants. The result of the present study also revealed the same findings for the effective- ness of oral corrective feedback for the acquisition of unergative and unaccusative verbs. The results of the present study were also in-line with the results of the studies conducted by Bowles and Montrul (2009) in that both stud- ies revealed that a combination of explicit and implicit oral corrective feedback would result in the acquisition of grammar. Findings of Russell and Spada (2006) were also compatible with the findings of the present study in that both studies found that oral corrective feedback would help L2 learners of English acquire the grammar. Although the results of the present study showed agreement with the studies which found the positive effects of corrective feedback, the present study provided the literature with information on the fact that the three types of oral corrective feedback can equally result in the ac- quisition of unergative and unaccusative verbs. The results of the GJT revealed no significant difference between the performances of advanced- and intermediate-level students. Therefore, despite of the different mean scores obtained by the different groups, no significant difference was found. Regarding the unaccusative verbs, the results of the pre-test revealed that all of the students had no or little ability of judging the grammaticality of sentences with unac- cusative verbs. A comparison of these results with those of unergative verbs showed that sentences with unaccusative verbs (either with resul- tative or pseudopassive structure) were problematic for our students. Al- though students had no or little ability of judging the grammaticality of sentences with unaccusative verbs at the beginning of the study, af- ter receiving instruction along with corrective feedback, their results improved. The results of all groups’ post-tests indicated an increase of Unaccusative and Unergative Verb Acquisition: ... 49 ability to judge the grammaticality of sentences with unaccusative verbs. The results of this study provide further support for the results of the previous studies such as the one done by Rezai and Ariamanesh (2012), as both studies indicated that Iranian EFL learners had difficulty with judging the grammaticality of sentences with unaccusative verbs before receiving instructions. Since Rezai and Ariamanesh did not provide their participants with instructions or corrective feedback, comparison of post- tests results is not applicable. Results of Abbasi Bagherianpoor’s (2010) study supported the find- ings of the present study, indicating that learners had difficulty in struc- tures with unaccusative verbs and although learners were screened into intermediate and advanced levels, their performance was not signifi- cantly different. Compared with AbbasiBagherianpoor’s findings, the present study revealed that students of intermediate and advanced-levels had difficulty with unaccusative verbs. However, what makes the re- sults of the present study different from the previous ones is that inthe present study the students’ performance is also evaluated after receiving the corrective feedback and explicit instruction. According to the results of the post-test, although both levels’ ability to judge grammaticality of sentences increased, advanced-level learners had slightly outperformed intermediate-level learners for items with unaccusative verbs; however, this difference was not considered significant. Since the body of research points to universal acquisition challenges for unaccusatives (Zobl, 1989; Oshita, 1997; Hirakawa, 2000; Abbasi- Bagherianpoor, 2010; & Mortazavi, 2012), these findings provide a fur- ther support for the afore-mentioned challenges and also suggest solution (i.e., explicit instruction & oral corrective feedback) to overcome and re- duce the effects of those challenges on L2 acquisition. Although the results did not indicate any difference between im- plicit, explicit, mixed oral corrective feedback strategies, a comparison of different groups revealed that a combination of implicit and explicit strategies resulted in the highest mean differences from the pre-to the post-test. So, it can be argued that using a combination of explicit and implicit corrective feedback strategies can help the students understand unaccusative and unergative rules better. It can also be concluded that 5050 S.S. M. M. Meraji Meraji and and F. F. Sadighi Sadighi sincesince there there is isnot not any any significant significant difference difference between between the the different different correc- correc- tivetive feedback feedback strategies, strategies, it itis isteachers’ teachers’ decision decision to to determine determine which which type type oror types types of of corrective corrective feedback feedback can can help help the the students. students.

LimitationsLimitations of of the the Study Study and and Suggestions Suggestions for for Further Further ResearchResearch

TheThe present present study, study, which which targeted targeted Iranian Iranian EFL EFL majors, majors, has has some some lim- lim- itationsitations in in relation relation to to the the participants participants and and the the instruments. instruments. Since Since the the participantsparticipants of of the the present present study study were were chosen chosen based based on on a nonprobability a nonprobability samplingsampling and and from from among among an an accessible accessible population, population, they they were were Persian Persian speakers.speakers. As As Abbasi Abbasi Bagherianpoor Bagherianpoor (2010) (2010) and and Hirakawa Hirakawa (2000) (2000) claimed claimed thatthat unaccusative/unergative unaccusative/unergative acquisition acquisition in in L2 L2 English English follows follows from from L1 L1 universals,universals, it itis isrecommended recommended that that future future research research replicate replicate the the present present studystudy using using participants participants with with different different L1 L1 backgrounds. backgrounds. Another Another limi- limi- tationtation of of the the present present study study was was unavailability unavailability of of computer-assisted computer-assisted cor- cor- rectiverective feedback. feedback. Since Since technology technology plays plays an an important important role role in in education, education, thethe incorporation incorporation of of computer-assisted computer-assisted feedback feedback into into classes classes may may provide provide thethe learners learners and and teachers teachers with with its its advantages/disadvantages. advantages/disadvantages. Therefore, Therefore, therethere is is a aneed need for for studies studies which which focus focus on on the the incorporation incorporation of of such such programsprograms into into EFL EFL classes. classes.

ReferencesReferences

[1][1] Abbasi, Abbasi, A. A. & & MirjaniArjenan, MirjaniArjenan, F. F. (2014). (2014). Translation Translation of of English English passive passive andand unaccusative unaccusative verbs verbs into into Farsi: Farsi: A A comparative comparative study study of of three three trans- trans- lationslations of of ’Animal ’Animal Farm’by Farm’by three three Iranian Iranian translators. translators.Procedia-SocialProcedia-Social and and BehavioralBehavioral Sciences, Sciences, 98, 98,9-15.9-15.

[2][2] Abbasi Abbasi Bagherianpoor, Bagherianpoor, A. A. (2010). (2010).TheThe acquisition acquisition of ofEnglish English unaccusative unaccusative verbsverbs by by Iranian Iranian EFL EFL majors. majors.UnpublishedUnpublished Ph. Ph. D D dissertation, dissertation, Shiraz Shiraz University,University, Iran. Iran.

[3][3] Annett, Annett, J. J. (1969). (1969).FeedbackFeedback and and human human behaviour. behaviour.Harmondsworth,Harmondsworth, UK: UK: Penguin.Penguin. UnaccusativeUnaccusative and and Unergative Unergative Verb Verb Acquisition: Acquisition: ...... 5151

[4][4] Bilodeau, Bilodeau, E. E. A. A. (1969). (1969). Supplementary Supplementary feedback feedback and and instructions. instructions. In In E. E. A.A. Bilodeau Bilodeau (Ed.), (Ed.),PrinciplesPrinciples of skill of skill acquisition acquisition (pp. (pp. 235-253). 235-253).NewNew York: York: AcademicAcademic Press. Press.

[5][5] Bitchener, Bitchener, J., J., Young, Young, S., S., & Cameron,& Cameron, D. D. (2005). (2005). The The effect effect of ofdifferent different typestypes of correctiveof corrective feedback feedback on on ESL ESL student student writing. writing.JournalJournal of ofSecond Second LanguageLanguage Writing, Writing, 14(3), 14(3), 191-205. 191-205.

[6][6] Bowles, Bowles, M. M. & Montrul, & Montrul, S. (2009).S. (2009). Back Back to tobasics: basics: Incomplete Incomplete knowledge knowledge of Differentialof Differential Object Marking Marking in inSpanish Spanish heritage heritage speakers. speakers.Bilingualism:Bilingualism: LanguageLanguage and and Cognition, Cognition, 12(03), 12(03), 363-383. 363-383.

[7][7] Campillo, Campillo, P. P.S. S.(2003). (2003). An An analysis analysis of implicitof implicit and and explicit explicit feedback feedback on on grammaticalgrammatical accuracy. accuracy.Miscelanea:Miscelanea: A JournalA Journal of ofEnglish English and and American American Studies,Studies, (27) (27), 209-228., 209-228.

[8][8] Chomsky, Chomsky, N. N. (1965). (1965).AspectsAspects of theoryof theory of syntax.of syntax.Cambridge,Cambridge, Mass: Mass: MIT MIT Press.Press.

[9][9] George, George, H. H.V. V.(1972). (1972).CommonCommon errors errors in languagein language learning. learning.Rowley,Rowley, Mass: Mass: NewburyNewbury House. House.

[10][10] Han, Han, Z. Z.(2002). (2002). A study A study of the of the impact impact of recasts of recasts on on tense tense consistency consistency in in L2L2 output. output.TesolTesol Quarterly, Quarterly, 36(4), 36(4), 543-572. 543-572.

[11][11] Hawkins, Hawkins, E. E.(1987). (1987).ModernModern languages languages in thein the curriculum. curriculum.SecondSecond edition, edition, Cambridge:Cambridge: CUP. CUP.

[12][12] Hirakawa, Hirakawa, M. M. (2000). (2000).UnaccusativityUnaccusativity in in second second language language Japanese Japanese and and English.English.(Unpublished(Unpublished doctoral doctoral dissertation). dissertation). McGill McGill University, University, Canada. Canada.

[13][13] James, James, C. C. (2013). (2013).ErrorsErrors in inlanguage language learning learning and and use: use: Exploring Exploring error error analysis.analysis.London:London: Routledge. Routledge.

[14][14] KarimiDoostan, KarimiDoostan, Gh. Gh. (1997). (1997).LightLight verb verb construction construction in Persian.in Persian.(Unpub-(Unpub- lishedlished doctoral doctoral dissertation). dissertation). Essex Essex University, University, England. England.

[15][15] Kawaguchi, Kawaguchi, S. S. & & Ma, Ma, Y. Y. (2012). (2012). Corrective Corrective feedback, feedback, negotiation negotiation of of meaningmeaning and and grammar grammar development: development: Learner-learner Learner-learner and and learner-native learner-native speakerspeaker interaction interaction in inESL. ESL.OpenOpen Journal Journal of ofModern Modern Linguistics, Linguistics, 2(02), 2(02), 57.57.

[16][16] Leeman, Leeman, J. (2007).J. (2007). Feedback Feedback in inL2 L2 learning: learning: Responding Responding to to errors errors dur- dur- inging practice. practice.PracticePractice in in a second a second language: language: Perspectives Perspectives from from Applied Applied LinguisticsLinguistics and and Cognitive Cognitive Psychology, Psychology,111-137.111-137. 5252 S.S. M. M. Meraji Meraji and and F. F. Sadighi Sadighi

[17][17] Lightbown, Lightbown, P. P.M. M. & Spada,& Spada, N. N. (1999). (1999). Instruction, Instruction, first first language language influence, influence, andand developmental developmental readiness readiness in insecond second language language acquisition. acquisition.TheThe Modern Modern LanguageLanguage Journal, Journal, 83(1), 83(1), 1-22. 1-22.

[18][18] Littlewood, Littlewood, W. W. (1984). (1984).ForeignForeign and and second second language language learning: learning: Language Language acquisitionacquisition research research and and its its implications implications for for the the classroom. classroom.Cambridge:Cambridge: CambridgeCambridge University University Press. Press.

[19][19] Long, Long, M. M. H. H. (1996). (1996). The The role role of of the the linguistic linguistic environment environment in in second second lan- lan- guageguage acquisition. acquisition. In In Ritchie, Ritchie, W. W. C., C., & & Bahtia, Bahtia, T. T. K. K. (eds.), (eds.),HandbookHandbook ofof second second language language acquisition acquisition (pp. (pp. 413-68). 413-68).NewNew York: York: Academic Academic Press. Press. ReprintedReprinted in in Ortega, Ortega, L. L. (ed.), (ed.),SecondSecond language language acquisition: acquisition: Critical Critical con- con- ceptscepts in in linguistics. linguistics.London:London: Routledge. Routledge.

[20][20] Lyster, Lyster, R. R. & & Mori, Mori, H. H. (2006). (2006). Interactional Interactional feedback feedback and and instructional instructional counterbalance.counterbalance.StudiesStudies in in Second Second Language Language Acquisition, Acquisition, 28(02), 28(02), 269-300. 269-300.

[21][21] Lyster, Lyster, R. R. & & Ranta, Ranta, L. L. (1997). (1997). Corrective Corrective feedback feedback and and learner learner up- up- take.take.StudiesStudies in in Second Second Language Language Acquisition, Acquisition, 19(01), 19(01), 37-66. 37-66.

[22][22] Maicusi, Maicusi, T., T., Maicusi, Maicusi, P., P., & Lopez,& Lopez, M. M. J. C.J. C. (2000). (2000). The The error error in inthe the second second languagelanguage acquisition. acquisition.Encuentro.Encuentro. Revista Revista de de investigacion investigacion de de innovacion innovacion enen la laclase clase de de idiomas, idiomas, 11, 11,1999-2000.1999-2000.

[23][23] Mortazavi, Mortazavi, M. M. (2012). (2012). Overpassivization Overpassivization of of unaccusatives unaccusatives in in ESL: ESL: Effects Effects fromfrom tense tense and and have. have.EFLEFL Journal, Journal, 3(1).33-45. 3(1).33-45.

[24][24] Narciss, Narciss, S. S. (2008). (2008). Feedback Feedback strategies strategies for for interactive interactive learning learning tasks.tasks.HandbookHandbook of of research research on on educational educational communications communications and and technol- technol- ogy,ogy, 3, 3,125-144.125-144.

[25][25] Oshita, Oshita, H. H. (1997). (1997).TheThe unaccusative unaccusative trap: trap: L2 L2 acquisition acquisition of of English English in- in- transitivetransitive verbs. verbs.UnpublishedUnpublished Ph.D Ph.D dissertation. dissertation. University University of of Southern Southern California,California, Los Los Angeles. Angeles.

[26][26] Park, Park, K. K. S. S. & & Lakshmanan, Lakshmanan, U. U. (2007). (2007). The The unaccusative-unergative unaccusative-unergative dis- dis- tinctiontinction in in resultatives: resultatives: Evidence Evidence from from Korean Korean L2 L2 learners learners of of English. English.InIn ProceedingsProceedings of of the the 2nd 2nd conference conference on on generative generative approaches approaches to to language language acquisitionacquisition North North America America (GALANA) (GALANA) (pp. (pp. 328-338). 328-338).

[27][27] Perlmutter, Perlmutter, M. M. (1979). (1979). Age Age differences differences in adults’in adults’ free free recall, recall, cued cued recall, recall, andand recognition. recognition.JournalJournal of of Gerontology, Gerontology, 34(4), 34(4), 533-539. 533-539. UnaccusativeUnaccusative and and Unergative Unergative Verb Verb Acquisition: Acquisition: ...... 5353

[28][28] Rezai, Rezai, M. M. J. &J. Ariamanesh,& Ariamanesh, A. A. A. A. (2012). (2012). Acquisition Acquisition of Englishof English unerga- unerga- tivetive and and unaccusative unaccusative structures structures by by Persian Persian EFL EFL learners. learners.JournalJournal of of TeachingTeaching Language Language Skills, Skills, 4(2), 4(2), 53-85. 53-85.

[29][29] Richard, Richard, J. C., J. C., Platt, Platt, J., J., & Platt, & Platt, H. H. (1992). (1992).DictionaryDictionary of languageof language teach- teach- inging & applied& applied linguistics. linguistics.Essex:Essex: Longman. Longman.

[30][30] Russell, Russell, J. J.& &Spada, Spada, N. N. (2006). (2006). The The effectiveness effectiveness of ofcorrective corrective feedback feedback forfor the the acquisition acquisition of of L2 L2 grammar. grammar.SynthesizingSynthesizing Research Research on on Language Language LearningLearning and and Teaching, Teaching,133-164.133-164.

[31][31] Sauro, Sauro, S. S.(2009). (2009). Computer-mediated Computer-mediated corrective corrective feedback feedback and and the the devel- devel- opmentopment of L2of L2 grammar. grammar.LanguageLanguage Learning Learning & Technology,& Technology, 13(1), 13(1), 96-120. 96-120.

[32][32] Selinker, Selinker, L. L. (1972). (1972). Interlanguage. Interlanguage.IRAL-InternationalIRAL-International Review Review of ofApplied Applied LinguisticsLinguistics in inLanguage Language Teaching, Teaching, 10(1-4), 10(1-4), 209-232. 209-232.

[33][33] Sorace, Sorace, A. A. (1993). (1993). Incomplete Incomplete vs. vs. divergent divergent representations representations of of unac- unac- cusativitycusativity in innon-native non-native grammars grammars of Italian.of Italian.SecondSecond Language Language Research, Research, 9(1),9(1), 22-47. 22-47.

[34][34] Tamayo Tamayo Maggi, Maggi, M. M. R. R. & Quishpe, & Quishpe, D. D. C. C. C. C. (2017). (2017). EFL EFL teachers’ teachers’ correc- correc- tivetive feedback feedback and and its its effect effect on onlearners’ learners’ error error repair repair in inspeaking. speaking.RevistaRevista CientificaCientifica Axioma, Axioma, 16, 96-104. 16, 96-104.

[35][35] Wager, Wager, W. W. & & Wager, Wager, S. S. (1985). (1985). Presenting Presenting questions, questions, processing processing re- re- sponses,sponses, and and providing providing feedback feedback in inCAI. CAI.JournalJournal of ofInstructional Instructional Devel- Devel- opment,opment, 8(4), 8(4), 2-8. 2-8.

[36][36] Zobl, Zobl, H. H. (1989). (1989). Canonical Canonical typological typological structures structures and and ergativity ergativity in in En- En- glishglish L2 L2 acquisition. acquisition.LinguisticLinguistic Perspectives Perspectives on on Second Second Language Language Acquisi- Acquisi- tion,tion,203-221.203-221.

[37][37] Zohrabi, Zohrabi, K. K. & Ehsani, & Ehsani, F. F. (2014). (2014). The The role role of implicit of implicit & explicit & explicit corrective corrective feedbackfeedback in in Persian-speaking Persian-speaking EFL EFL learners’ learners’ awareness awareness and and accuracy accuracy in in EnglishEnglish grammar. grammar.Procedia-SocialProcedia-Social and and Behavioral Behavioral Sciences, Sciences, 98, 98,2018-2024.2018-2024.