Basic Japanese Cheat Sheet

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Basic Japanese Cheat Sheet Basic Japanese Formal Verbs Informal Verbs Verb Usages WA - Topic Marker Written with hiragana “ha”, but pronounced “wa” ・Marks a limited amount or extent of something. (”not more than”) だけ ・Used with できる (and other potential-form verbs) to mean “as ~ as possible” Present/Future Tense + ます Present/Future Tense + ば + なければ は ・Marks the topic of a sentence. (non-grammatical relationship) DAKE Past Tense + ました Past Tense If... + と If not... + ないと ・Contrasts one option or thing against others. (A vs. B, C, D) いちじかん だけ まって くだ さ い 。 できる だけ はやく かいて くだ さ い 。 かける だけ かいて くだ さ い 。 Progressive Tense + います Progressive Tense + いる + ら + なかったら ・Used when elaborating on things already known to the listener. Please wait for just one hour. Please write it as quickly as you can. Please write as much as you can. DOUBLE MEANING Negative Present/Future + ません Negative Present/Future + ない + ないと いけない くる ま は わたし が うんてん しました。 たなかさん は おかね を もっています。 Must/have to... Marks a firm extent/limit (usually one which presents a problem for the speaker). Negative Past Tense + ませんでした Negative Past Tense + なかった + なければ ならない As for the car, I was the one who drove it. Mr. Tanaka has money. (he is rich) ・ しか ・Emphasizes the negation of anything beyond the extent/limit. Command Form + くだ さ い Command Form + から SHIKA (U-Verbs) GA - Subject Marker Therefore, しか is always is used with a negative verb form. + なさい する → しろ After... + ら せんえん しか もっていません。 しゃしん で しか みた こと が ない。 あなた に しか できない。 Usually considered rude! (RU-Verbs) + くる こい ろ → + あとで が ・Marks the subject of a verb. (grammatical relationship) I haven’t but one thousand yen. I’ve never seen it except in pictures. Only you can do it. Negative Command + ないでください Negative Command + ないで Before... + まえに ・Distinguishes one option or thing from others. (A vs. ???) + な Because... + から/ので ・Used when adding new things/information to a conversation. ・Marks a degree or extent of something. (not small) Potential Form (U-Verbs) + ます Potential Form (U-Verbs) + る In order to... + ために ・Used when describing specific things with adjectives. ほど ・Marks an approximate number or length of time. = can/ able to (RU-Verbs) (RU-Verbs) Marks the greater element in a negative comparison. (”not as ~ as”) + られます + られる While... (at the same time) + ながら ・Marks objects of potential-form verbs. (can/able to) HODO ・ する→ できます くる → こられます する→ できる くる → これる Even if/when... + も ・(Used as an conjunction [between phrases]) : “..., but...” or “even though ..., ...” そのくるま は このくるま ほど はやくないです。 はら が いたく なった ほど わらいました。 (permissible) That car is not as fast as this car. I laughed so hard that my stomach became painful. Want to... + DOUBLE MEANING Want to... + May... たいです たい + も + いい にほんご が すこし はなせます。 たなかさん が おかね を もっています。 Don’t want to... + たくないです Don’t want to... + たくない Try to... to attempt & check the result + みる (I) can speak Japanese a little. Mr. Tanaka has the money. (with him now) Marks the lesser element in a positive comparison. (”more than”) Let’s... + ましょう Let’s... to make an effort + とする ・ WA vs. GA (Detailed Article) - http://nihonshock.com/waga より “From ~” (when closing a private letter) ・ Shall we.../Shall I ~ for you? + ましょうか ・ Shall we.../Shall I ~ for you? + か Must not... + はいけない YORI ・ Passive Form (U-Verbs) + Passive Form (U-Verbs) + To do in advance... + れます れる おく O - Object Marker Written with hiragana “wo”, but the “w” sound is very faint. その くるま は この くるま より はやい です。 = to be ~ed (RU-Verbs) + られます (RU-Verbs) + られる To do too much... + すぎる That car is faster than this car. する→ されます くる → こられます する→ される くる → こられる To do... and so on. + り + する ・Marks the object of a verb. を ・Marks a possibility or suggestion. Causative Form (U-Verbs) + せます Causative Form (U-Verbs) + せる To (irreversibly) do... + しまう ・With verbs of motion, shows where the motion occurs. ・Turns interrogative words into any~ words. = to allow/force (RU-Verbs) (RU-Verbs) Combination Verb でも someone to ~ + させます + させる + VERB 2 くつ を かいました。 そら を とぶ じぶん の こと を はなす。 DEMO ・(Used as an conjunction [before a phrase]) : “even so/but/however, ...” する→ させます くる → こさせます する→ させる くる → こさせる Chaining Phrases + PHRASE 2 (I) bought shoes. To fly (in) the sky To talk about (things of ) one’s self. おちゃ でも のみましょうか。 いつでも でんわ して くだ さ い 。 Shall we drink some tea or something? Please call me anytime. Conjugating Verbs Hiragana NI - The “Aiming” Particle Marks an approximate or negotiable degree, amount, or length of time. U-VERB RU-VERB する くる U-VERB TE/TA ENDINGS あ か が さ ざ た だ な は ば ぱ ま や ら わ ・Marks an indirect object (the person/thing an action is done for/directed toward) ・ Always use these endings. Always に (speaker feels that the stated number/extent is a fair and/or reasonable estimate) A KA GA SA ZA TA DA NA HA BA PA MA YA RA WA ・Marks a destination, direction or specific place for an action.(to/at) くらい はな. さ たべ. X し こ Final Kana of Verb KURAI Alternate form: ぐらい ・Marks a specific point in time (ie. Monday / 3pm / 1972 / July) ・ はな. し たべ. X し き う, つ, る → って った い き ぎ し じ ち ぢ に ひ び ぴ み り かんじ を じゅっかい ぐらい かく と わすれない よ。 はな. す たべ. る する くる I KI GI SHI JI CHI JI NI HI BI PI MI RI ・Marks a period of time that something repeats in. (ie. twice a week) If you write a kanji about 10 times, you won’t forget it. ぶ, む, ぬ → んで んだ はな. せ たべ. れ すれ くれ ・Marks direct objects for: なる (to become), あう (to meet), のる (to ride) く → いて いた う く ぐ す ず つ づ ぬ ふ ぶ ぷ む ゆ る ん はな. そ う たべ. よ う しよう こよう U KU GU SU ZU TSU ZU NU FU BU PU MU YU RU N かのじょ に ほん を あげた。 どようび に こうえん に いきましょう。いちにち に コーヒー を にはい のむ。 ・Means “only”, or “to the exclusion of other things” ぐ いで いだ (I) gave her a book. Let’s go to the park on Saturday. (I) drink 2 cups of coffee a day. ばかり Shift final sound たべ. て して きて → ・Informally, can be pronounced ばっかり / ばっか / ばっかし for emphasis. according to え け げ せ ぜ て で ね へ べ ぺ め れ BAKARI Hiragana chart. たべ. た した きた す → して した E KE GE SE ZE TE DE NE HE BE PE ME RE TO - The “Glue” Particle When quoting, と can become って (informal) あの きょうしつ は おんなのこ ばかり います。 かれ の いうこと は うそ ばっかり だ。 いく (to go) in TE/TA-form becomes いって/いった That classroom has nothing but girls in it. Everything he says is a lie. EXCEPTIONS → お こ ご そ ぞ と ど の ほ ぼ ぽ も よ ろ を ・Marks a person that an action is done with. The ending for verbs ending in is: O KO GO SO ZO TO DO NO HO BO PO MO YO RO WO と → う わ ・Links nouns to create a complete “and” group. ・Marks a starting place or time. ともだち から ききました。 いる (to need), はいる (to enter), しる (to know), きる (to cut) ・Marks quoted speech or phrasal objects: おもう (to think), かく (to write) I heard about it from a friend. U-VERBS Katakana から ・Marks a source of information. (THAT LOOK LIKE RU-VERBS) ・Shows a natural/predictable “if-then” relationship between clauses. さいふ を わすれた から はらえない。 かえる (to go home), すべる (to slide), ける (to kick) KARA ・Marks a reason when used after . ア カ ガ サ ザ タ ダ ナ ハ バ パ マ ヤ ラ ワ ・Marks onomatopoeic words. (words that mimic sounds) I can’t pay because I forgot my wallet. A KA GA SA ZA TA DA NA HA BA PA MA YA RA WA “And” group Phrasal object Forms of である (to be) Caution! かいぎ は いちじ から さんじ まで です。 ボス と じょうし と けんか しました。 いす が こわれた と おもいます。 こそこそ と はなす ・Marks an ending place or time. イ キ ギ シ ジ チ ヂ ニ ヒ ビ ピ ミ リ the meeting is from 1 o’clock until 3 o’clock. FORMAL INFORMAL SO N (I) had an argument with (my) boss and manager. (I) think the chair broke. to speak in whispers まで ・When followed by に, I KI GI SHI JI CHI JI NI HI BI PI MI RI means “by/before” あした までに レポート を かいて くだ さ い 。 Present Tense MADE Please write the report by tomorrow. です だ ウ ク グ ス ズ ツ ヅ ヌ フ ブ プ ム ユ ル ン DE - The “Enabling” Particle Past Tense でした だった ソ ン U KU GU SU ZU TSU ZU NU FU BU PU MU YU RU N Marks a place, area, or length of time inside which something occurs. ほうが Comparison Give/Receive Probable でしょう だろう で ・ TSU SHI Superior/Outsider Negative Present Tense ではありません じゃな い エ ヶ ゲ セ ゼ テ デ ネ ヘ ベ ペ メ レ ・Marks a method, tool or circumstance under which something happens. ・ Marks the greater element in a positive comparison E KE GE SE ZE TE DE NE HE BE PE ME RE Negative Past Tense ではありませんでした じゃな かった ・Marks a material or substance; composition.
Recommended publications
  • The Japanese Writing Systems, Script Reforms and the Eradication of the Kanji Writing System: Native Speakers’ Views Lovisa Österman
    The Japanese writing systems, script reforms and the eradication of the Kanji writing system: native speakers’ views Lovisa Österman Lund University, Centre for Languages and Literature Bachelor’s Thesis Japanese B.A. Course (JAPK11 Spring term 2018) Supervisor: Shinichiro Ishihara Abstract This study aims to deduce what Japanese native speakers think of the Japanese writing systems, and in particular what native speakers’ opinions are concerning Kanji, the logographic writing system which consists of Chinese characters. The Japanese written language has something that most languages do not; namely a total of ​ ​ three writing systems. First, there is the Kana writing system, which consists of the two syllabaries: Hiragana and Katakana. The two syllabaries essentially figure the same way, but are used for different purposes. Secondly, there is the Rōmaji writing system, which is Japanese written using latin letters. And finally, there is the Kanji writing system. Learning this is often at first an exhausting task, because not only must one learn the two phonematic writing systems (Hiragana and Katakana), but to be able to properly read and write in Japanese, one should also learn how to read and write a great amount of logographic signs; namely the Kanji. For example, to be able to read and understand books or newspaper without using any aiding tools such as dictionaries, one would need to have learned the 2136 Jōyō Kanji (regular-use Chinese characters). With the twentieth century’s progress in technology, comparing with twenty years ago, in this day and age one could probably theoretically get by alright without knowing how to write Kanji by hand, seeing as we are writing less and less by hand and more by technological devices.
    [Show full text]
  • The Functions and Evolution of Topic and Focus Markers
    The Functions and Evolution of Topic and Focus Markers by Paula Kadose Radetzky B.A. (Columbia University) 1991 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 1996 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Richard A. Rhodes, Co-chair Professor Eve E. Sweetser, Co-chair Professor H. Mack Horton Spring 2002 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Abstract The Functions and Evolution of Topic and Focus Markers by Paula Kadose Radetzky Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Professors Richard A. Rhodes and Eve E. Sweetser, Co-chairs This dissertation examines the notions of topic and focus from both synchronic and diachronic points of view. Previous works have almost exclusively treated these concepts synchronically, and the historical studies which do exist have not successfully traced and motivated the individual stages of development. The sections on topic first propose and give cross-linguistic evidence for the following path of grammaticalization: locative/ contrastive topic marker > marker > marker This overview is followed by two text-based studies, one of the Japanese topic marker wa and the other of the Greek particle de. Because of their long written traditions, these two languages allow us to contextually view and motivate the intermediate stages of grammaticalization. The last part of the dissertation is a discussion of focus. It begins by developing a synchronic theory involving different levels of highlighting, and then it presents case studies of data primarily from Japanese and Korean, examining in detail the mechanisms by which demonstratives and copulas become focus markers in these languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Montague Grammatical Analysis of Japanese Case Particles
    Montague Grammatical Analysis of Japanese Case Particles Mitsuhiro Okada and Kazue Watanabe Keio University Sophia University 1 Introduction The Montague Grammar for English and other Indo-European languages in the literature is based on the subject-predicate relation, which has been the tradition since Aristotle in the Western language analysis. In this sense, for example, an object and a transitive verb are part of a verb phrase, that is, of a predicate, and the types of a transitive verb and of an object are assigned after the type of predicate (verb phrase) is fixed. When we try to construct categorial grammar and denotational semantics following this tradition, we meet the following deficiencies; • The type structure for transitive verbs and objects, hence their denota- tions, becomes very complicated. For example, the type for transitive verbs is; ((e t) —+ t) (e -4 t) • In order to get an appropriate type for a sentence within the ordinary categorial grammar, we first concatenate a transitive verb with an object and then a subject with the result of the first concatenation. Hence, the resulting grammar becomes very much word-order sensitive. It is easily seen that this kind of operation is not suitable for languages which have word-order flexibility like Japanese, where the following sentences are equivalent in meaning "I ate a cake."; (1) a. Watashi ga cake wo tabeta. I NOM cake ACC ate b. Cake wo watashi ga tabeta. cake ACC I NOM ate We employ the following notational convention in this paper. To each ex- ample sentence of Japanese, we append a gloss which shows the corresponding English expression to each Japanese word and an English translation.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Japanese Computational Linguistics Francis Bond and Timothy Baldwin
    1 Introduction to Japanese Computational Linguistics Francis Bond and Timothy Baldwin The purpose of this chapter is to provide a brief introduction to the Japanese language, and natural language processing (NLP) research on Japanese. For a more complete but accessible description of the Japanese language, we refer the reader to Shibatani (1990), Backhouse (1993), Tsujimura (2006), Yamaguchi (2007), and Iwasaki (2013). 1 A Basic Introduction to the Japanese Language Japanese is the official language of Japan, and belongs to the Japanese language family (Gordon, Jr., 2005).1 The first-language speaker pop- ulation of Japanese is around 120 million, based almost exclusively in Japan. The official version of Japanese, e.g. used in official settings andby the media, is called hyōjuNgo “standard language”, but Japanese also has a large number of distinctive regional dialects. Other than lexical distinctions, common features distinguishing Japanese dialects are case markers, discourse connectives and verb endings (Kokuritsu Kokugo Kenkyujyo, 1989–2006). 1There are a number of other languages in the Japanese language family of Ryukyuan type, spoken in the islands of Okinawa. Other languages native to Japan are Ainu (an isolated language spoken in northern Japan, and now almost extinct: Shibatani (1990)) and Japanese Sign Language. Readings in Japanese Natural Language Processing. Francis Bond, Timothy Baldwin, Kentaro Inui, Shun Ishizaki, Hiroshi Nakagawa and Akira Shimazu (eds.). Copyright © 2016, CSLI Publications. 1 Preview 2 / Francis Bond and Timothy Baldwin 2 The Sound System Japanese has a relatively simple sound system, made up of 5 vowel phonemes (/a/,2 /i/, /u/, /e/ and /o/), 9 unvoiced consonant phonemes (/k/, /s/,3 /t/,4 /n/, /h/,5 /m/, /j/, /ó/ and /w/), 4 voiced conso- nants (/g/, /z/,6 /d/ 7 and /b/), and one semi-voiced consonant (/p/).
    [Show full text]
  • Processing and Acquisition of Scrambled Sentences by Learners of Japanese As a Second Language
    Processing and Acquisition of Scrambled Sentences by Learners of Japanese as a Second Language Item Type text; Electronic Dissertation Authors Shigenaga, Yasumasa Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 23/09/2021 15:41:09 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/344218 PROCESSING AND ACQUISITION OF SCRAMBLED SENTENCES BY LEARNERS OF JAPANESE AS A SECOND LANGUAGE by Yasumasa Shigenaga __________________________ Copyright © Yasumasa Shigenaga 2014 A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the GRADUATE INTERDISCIPLINARY DOCTORAL PROGRAM IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND TEACHING In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY In the Graduate College THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA 2014 2 THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA GRADUATE COLLEGE As members of the Dissertation Committee, we certify that we have read the dissertation prepared by Yasumasa Shigenaga, titled Processing and Acquisition of Scrambled Sentences by Learners of Japanese as a Second Language and recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy. _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 08/28/2014 Thomas G. Bever _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 08/28/2014 Kenneth Forster _______________________________________________________________________ Date: 08/28/2014 Janet Nicol Final approval and acceptance of this dissertation is contingent upon the candidate’s submission of the final copies of the dissertation to the Graduate College. I hereby certify that I have read this dissertation prepared under my direction and recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Santa Barbara Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UC Santa Barbara UC Santa Barbara Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Washi, Momo: Nontraditional Pronoun Usage by a Kansai Japanese Vlogger Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4qc633pf Author Tsai, Karen Publication Date 2018 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Santa Barbara Washi, Momo: Nontraditional Pronoun Usage by a Kansai Japanese Vlogger A Thesis submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Master of Arts in Linguistics by Karen Tsai Committee in charge: Professor Matthew Gordon, Chair Professor Mary Bucholtz Professor Argyro Katsika December 2018 The thesis of Karen Tsai is approved. Mary Bucholtz Argyro Katsika Matthew Gordon, Committee Chair December 2018 Washi, Momo: Nontraditional Pronoun Usage by a Kansai Japanese Vlogger Copyright © 2018 by Karen Tsai iii ABSTRACT Washi, Momo: Nontraditional Pronoun Usage by a Kansai Japanese Vlogger by Karen Tsai Previous literature on Japanese first-person singular (1SG) pronouns deals primarily with encoding of social information such as the speaker’s gender, age, and politeness or formality in Standard Japanese speakers. However, this study investigates the nontradi- tional 1SG pronoun usage of Momona, a 21-year-old Kansai Japanese YouTube vlogger. This study also expands the research on variation of 1SG pronoun usage in conversational speech and casual registers, which are underrepresented in the literature. I argue that Momona’s usage of washi, wai, and washa convey her quirky personality and her identity as a young woman from the Kansai region of Japan, and that her use of atashi, watashi, and watakushi challenge traditional interpretations of Standard Japanese pronoun us- age.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring Learners' Investment and Meaning-Making
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations Dissertations and Theses July 2019 Language Learners as Agentive Meaning-Makers: Exploring Learners' Investment and Meaning-Making Shinji Kawamitsu Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2 Part of the Bilingual, Multilingual, and Multicultural Education Commons, Curriculum and Instruction Commons, and the Language and Literacy Education Commons Recommended Citation Kawamitsu, Shinji, "Language Learners as Agentive Meaning-Makers: Exploring Learners' Investment and Meaning-Making" (2019). Doctoral Dissertations. 1647. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2/1647 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations and Theses at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LANGUAGE LEARNERS AS AGENTIVE MEANING-MAKERS: EXPLORING LEARNERS’ INVESTMENT AND MEANING-MAKING A Dissertation Presented by SHINJI KAWAMITSU Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY May 2019 College of Education Language, Literacy and Culture © Copyright by Shinji Kawamitsu 2019 All Rights Reserved LANGUAGE LEARNERS AS AGENTIVE MEANING-MAKERS: EXPLORING LEARNERS’ INVESTMENT AND MEANING-MAKING A Dissertation Presented by SHINJI KAWAMITSU Approved as to style and content by: Theresa Austin, Chair Margaret Gebhard, Member Yuki Yoshimura, Member ____________________________________ Jennifer Randall Associate Dean of Academic Affairs College of Education DEDICATION To my parents who gave me access to so many things ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would first like to thank my students who participated in my study.
    [Show full text]
  • Origins of the Verbalizer Affixes in the Japonic Languages
    ORIGINS OF THE VERBALIZER AFFIXES IN THE JAPONIC LANGUAGES Tyler Lau Advisors: Claire Bowern and Stephen R. Anderson May 1st, 2012 ABSTRACT The affix that verbalizes adjectives in the Japonic languages is traditionally viewed as deriving from one of two constructions: *ku a(r)-, an adverbializer + existential verb, or *-sa a(r)-, a nominalizer + existential verb (Martin 1987, Bentley 2001, Chamberlain 1895, etc.). However, Izuyama (1997) argues that this view is taken at face value and ignores phonological correspondences with the southern Japonic languages, notably Yaeyama and Yonaguni. She argues instead that the form originates in the completive forms of three or four reconstructed verbs *s(u), *k(u), *i(r ∼s), all meaning ‘to do’. In my work, I gathered comparative morphological and phonological evidence from wordlists, grammars and my own fieldwork with a speaker of Okinawan, to test these hypotheses. However, my findings also lead me to reject Izuyamas reconstruction of *i(r~s) as a valid reconstruction of ‘to do’ or as relevant to the verbalizer affix. Rather, I establish its origins in a Proto-Ryukyuan verb *er- ‘to get,’ descending from Proto-Japanese *e- that grammaticalized to attach to consonant-stem verbs, to create an inchoative or valency-changed class of vowel-stem verbs. I also tentatively reconstruct the verb ‘to do’ as *as-, a transitive form of the verb *ar- ‘to exist.’ In agreement with Izuyama, however, I found that there is ample evidence supporting the hypothesis that the verbalizer affix originates in the completive and/or past form of verbs meaning ‘to do’ and provide both comparative and theoretical evidence for this claim.
    [Show full text]
  • The Genji Monogatari
    The Genji Monogatari - “A Loose Sequence of Vague Phrases” ? Thomas Evelyn McAuley Ph. D. The School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London ProQuest Number: 10731386 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 10731386 Published by ProQuest LLC(2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 Abstract of Thesis In the thesis I test the hypothesis that Late Old Japanese (LOJ) is not, as has been claimed by a number of scholars, a language that is innately “vague”, but that it is capable of conveying meaning clearly. To prove this I analyse the text of the Genji Monogatari in a number of ways. I study the usage of honorifics in the text and the relationship between honorific usage and court rank. I show that honorific usage very often obviates the need for grammatical subjects and objects, and where honorifics or the context are not sufficient, the author introduces subjects to clarify the meaning of the text. Furthermore, I demonstrate that over brief sections of text, one character might be “tagged” with a particular honorific in order to identify them.
    [Show full text]
  • Marking Topic Or Marking Case: a Comparative Investigation of Heritage Japanese and Heritage Korean
    Marking Topic or Marking Case: A Comparative Investigation of Heritage Japanese and Heritage Korean The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Laleko, Oksana and Maria Polinsky. 2013. Marking Topic or Marking Case: A Comparative Investigation of Heritage Japanese and Heritage Korean. Heritage Language Journal 10(2): 40-64. Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:11879946 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Marking topic or marking case: A comparative investigation of Heritage Japanese and Heritage Korean∗ Oksana Laleko (SUNY New Paltz) Maria Polinsky (Harvard) Abstract In this paper, we examine the relationship between grammatical and discourse-related domains of linguistic organization in heritage speakers by comparing their knowledge of categories mediated at different structural levels: grammatical case marking, which is mediated within the structure of the clause, and the marking of information structure, grammatically mediated at the syntax-discourse interface. To this end, we examine the knowledge of case and topic particles in heritage speakers and L2 learners of Japanese and Korean as assessed through a series of rating tasks. We find that heritage speakers in both languages experience different degrees of difficulty with elements that belong to different linguistic modules: phenomena which involve semantic and discourse computation are found to be more difficult than phenomena governed primarily by structural syntactic constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • Borrowing-Overview
    Intercultural Communication Studies XI:4 2002 Hoffer – Language Borrowing-Overview Language Borrowing and Language Diffusion:* an Overview Bates L. Hoffer Trinity University Perhaps the most frequently encountered product of cultural contact is the set of loanwords that follow from intercultural communication. This "Language Borrowing" section of ICS XI-4 begins with an introduction to some of the major contributions to the linguistic study of the field of language borrowing and a general treatment of some of the latest approaches Part I gives an overview of the scholarly approaches to the topic, primarily those since 1950 but including some of the earlier European work. Part II gives a contemporary case study on massive language borrowing and the subsequent diffusion of the loanwords throughout a society's language use. Part III gives some examples from many languages of the world of the most advanced stage of language borrowing and diffusion: dual language neologisms in daily use. PART I History of the Study of Borrowing Introduction Borrowing is the process of importing linguistic items from one linguistic system into another, a process that occurs any time two cultures are in contact over a period of time. Haugen's 1950 article on borrowing marks the beginning of the current interest in the topic. Much of the earlier work had dealt with historical linguistics and much of the following work has dealt with other areas of linguistics. The early study of the process of borrowing and its results emphasized items from the linguistic systems such as vocabulary, phonology, grammar. In the last four decades added emphasis has been given to the study of the borrowing of other elements in communication systems.
    [Show full text]
  • And Topic Markers Indicate Gender and Number by Means of the Pronominal Article They Follow (Ho-Kwe M.TOP, Ko-Kwe F.TOP and Mo-Kwe PL-TOP)
    The elusive topic: Towards a typology of topic markers (with special reference to cumulation with number in Bolinao and gender in Nalca) Bernhard Wälchli (Stockholm University) [email protected] Helsinki; January 22, 2020 1 The elusive topic: Towards a typology of topic markers (with special reference to cumulation with number in Bolinao and gender in Nalca) Bernhard Wälchli (Stockholm University) Abstract At least since the 1970s, topic has been widely recognized to reflect an important category in most different approaches to linguistics. However, researchers have never agreed about what exactly a topic is (researchers disagree, for instance, about whether topics express backgrounding or foregrounding) and to what extent topics are elements of syntax or discourse or both. Topics are notoriously difficult to distinguish from a range of related phenomena. Some definitions of topic are suspiciously similar to definitions of definiteness, subject, noun and contrast, so the question arises as to what extent topic is a phenomenon of its own. However, topics are also internally diverse. There is disagreement, for instance, as to whether contrastive and non-contrastive topics should be subsumed under the same notion. This talk tries to approach the category type topic bottom-up by considering cross-linguistic functional diversity in marked topics, semasiologically defined as instances of topics with explicit segmental topic markers. The first part of the talk considers the question as to whether topic markers can be defined as a gram type with one or several prototypical functions that can be studied on the basis of material from parallel texts and from descriptive sources.
    [Show full text]