Inflection Generation for Spanish Verbs Using Supervised Learning

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Inflection Generation for Spanish Verbs Using Supervised Learning Inflection Generation for Spanish Verbs using Supervised Learning Cristina Barros Dimitra Gkatzia Elena Lloret Department of Software School of Computing Department of Software and Computing Systems Edinburgh Napier University and Computing Systems University of Alicante Edinburgh, EH10 5DT, UK University of Alicante Apdo. de Correos 99 [email protected] Apdo. de Correos 99 E-03080, Alicante, Spain E-03080, Alicante, Spain [email protected] [email protected] Abstract verbs used for denoting modality - statement of facts, desires, commands, etc.). To create lexicons We present a novel supervised approach for all the verb inflections and moods would be to inflection generation for verbs in Span- a very time-consuming and costly task, so in this ish. Our system takes as input the verb’s context the use of machine learning approaches lemma form and the desired features such can benefit the inflection of unseen verb forms. as person, number, tense, and is able to Based on this, the research challenge we tackle predict the appropriate grammatical conju- is defined as follows: given a Spanish verb in its gation. Even though our approach learns base form (i.e., its lemma), we want to automati- from fewer examples comparing to pre- cally generate all the inflections for that verb. This vious work, it is able to deal with all is very useful for tasks involving natural language the Spanish moods (indicative, subjunc- generation (e.g., text generation, machine transla- tive and imperative) in contrast to previous tion), since the generated texts would sound more work which only focuses on indicative and natural and grammatically correct. subjunctive moods. We show that in an Our contributions to the field are as follows: we intrinsic evaluation, our system achieves present a novel and efficient method for tackling 99% accuracy, outperforming (although the challenge of inflection generation for Spanish not significantly) two competitive state-of- verbs using an ensemble of algorithms; we pro- art systems. The successful results ob- vide a high-quality dataset which includes inflec- tained clearly indicate that our approach tion rules of Spanish verbs for all the grammatical could be integrated into wider approaches moods (i.e. indicative, subjunctive and imperative, related to text generation in Spanish. being this last one do not tackled by the current approaches); our models are trained with fewer re- 1 Introduction sources than the state-of-art methods; and finally, Existing Natural Language Generation (NLG) ap- our method outperforms the state-of-the-art meth- proaches are usually applied to non morphological ods achieving a 2% higher accuracy. rich languages, such as English, where the mor- The rest of the paper is shaped as follows: In phological inflection of the word during the gener- the next section (Section2) we refer to the related ation process can be addressed using simple hand- work on inflection generation. In Section3, we written rules or existing libraries such as Sim- describe the overall methodology and the dataset pleNLG (Gatt and Reiter, 2009). In contrast, when used to train our model. In Section4, we present it comes to morphological rich languages, such as a comparison to the state-of-art inflection genera- Spanish, the use of rules can lead to incorrect in- tion approaches and in Section5, we discuss the flection of a word, thus generating ungrammatical results. Finally, in Section6, directions for future or meaningless texts. Our ultimate goal is to im- work are discussed. plement a morphological inflection approach for Spanish sentences within an NLG system based on 2 Related Work the use of lexicons. However, lexicons lack some verbs’ information, specifically, regarding gram- Morphological inflection has been addressed from matical moods (i.e., the grammatical features of different perspectives within the area of Compu- 136 Proceedings of the First Workshop on Subword and Character Level Models in NLP, pages 136–141, Copenhagen, Denmark, September 7, 2017. c 2017 Association for Computational Linguistics. tational Linguistics, commonly for morphological 3 Methodology rich languages, such as German, Spanish, Finnish In order to perform the inflection task, we first or Arabic, as well as less morphological rich lan- created a dataset to be used for training machine guages such as English. learning algorithms to inflect verbs in Spanish. As Previous work has used supervised or semi- part of this submission we will make our dataset supervised learning (Durrett and DeNero, 2013; freely available1. Then, we trained a model ca- Ahlberg et al., 2014; Nicolai et al., 2015; Faruqui pable of predicting the appropriate inflection of a et al., 2016) to learn from large datasets of mor- verb automatically, given a verb base form. Next, phological rules on word forms in order to ap- each of the stages of the proposed approach are ply them to inflect the desired words. Other described in more detail. approaches have relied on linguistic informa- tion, such as morphemes and phonology (Cot- 3.1 Dataset Creation terell et al., 2016); morphosyntactic disambigua- For the purposes of this research, we created a par- tion rules (Suarez´ et al., 2005); and, graphical allel dataset of Spanish base forms and their cor- models (Dreyer and Eisner, 2009). responding inflected form. The Spanish verbs can Recently, the morphological inflection has been be divided into regular and irregular verbs, where also addressed at SIGMORPHON 2016 Shared all the regular verbs share the same inflection pat- Task (Cotterell et al., 2016) where, given a lemma terns whereas, the irregular ones do not and can with its part-of-speech, a target inflected form had completely vary from one verb to another, as it is to be generated (task 1). This task was addressed shown in Figure1. through several approaches, including align and transduce (Alegria and Etxeberria, 2016; Nico- lai et al., 2016; Liu and Mao, 2016); recurrent neural networks (Kann and Schutze¨ , 2016; Aha- roni et al., 2016; Ostling¨ , 2016); and, linguistic- inspired heuristics approaches (Taji et al., 2016; Sorokin, 2016). Overall, recurrent neural net- Figure 1: Differences between regular and irregu- works approaches performed better, being (Kann lar verbs in Spanish, for the first singular person of and Schutze¨ , 2016) the best performing system in the present tense and in the subjunctive mood. the shared task, obtaining around 98%. Therefore, we constructed a dataset, contain- Furthermore, the work described here differs ing the necessary examples of inflection for all from existing statistical surface realisation meth- the tenses in the Spanish language, by consulting ods which use phrase-based learning (e.g., (Kon- the Real Academia Espanola˜ 2 and the Enciclope- stas and Lapata, 2012)) since they do not usu- dia Libre Universal en Espanol˜ 3. We further con- ally include morphological inflection. In this re- sidered that a verb can be divided in three parts: spect, our work is more similar to (Dusekˇ and (1) ending, (2) ending stem, and (3) penSyl. An Jurcˇ´ıcekˇ , 2013), where the inflected word forms example is shown in Figure2. This information are learnt through multi-class logistic regression will be later used as features within the dataset. by predicting edit scripts. The aforementioned In Spanish, the verbs can be classified depending data-driven methods achieve high accuracy in pre- on their ending, specifically, the verbs ended by “- dicting the appropriate inflection by learning from ar”, “-er” and “-ir” belong to the first, second an huge datasets. For example, Durret and DeNero third conjugation, respectively. Moreover, for the (2013) use 11400 amount of data (i.e. the total feature penSyl, the previous syllable of the end- number of instances or rules used to predict the ing, formed by the whole syllable, or its dominant inflections of a verb). In contrast, we use almost vowel is extracted. Finally, the ending stem is the half to train our system (4556 instances), and we closest consonant to the ending. achieve comparable or better results for Spanish. Finally, the work presented here relies on ensem- 1Our dataset for the Spanish verbs inflection is available bles of classifiers which has been proved success- here: https://github.com/cbarrosua/infDataset 2http://www.rae.es/diccionario-panhispanico-de- ful for content selection in data-to-text systems dudas/apendices/modelos-de-conjugacion-verbal (Gkatzia et al., 2014). 3http://enciclopedia.us.es/index.php/Categor´ıa:Verbos 137 tures for the inflection. In this manner, the base form was divided into syllables, taking the penul- timate one to obtain the penSyl feature. Since all verbs in Spanish always end with “-ar”, “-er” and “-ir”, as described in the previous section, we split the last syllable into the ending and ending stem features. Then, for each model we predicted its potential inflection using these extracted features Figure 2: Division of the Spanish verb to begin combined with the ones related to the verb tense, and its inflection for the first singular person of the i.e., the number, person, etc. Finally, the predicted present tense and in the subjunctive mood. inflections were employed to replace the features previously identified in the base form, leading to the reconstruction of the base form into the desired Besides the previous features obtained from the inflection, as it can be seen in Figure3. verb, other features, such as suff1, suff2 or stemC1 were included because in Spanish some verbs have several variations of an inflection for the same tense, person and number. Therefore, our dataset is finally composed of the following features: (1) ending, (2) ending stem, (3) penSyl, (4) person, (5) number, (6) tense, (7) mood, (8) suff1, (9) suff2, (10) stemC1, (11) stemC2, (12) stemC3. In partic- ular, suff1 and suff2 are the inflection predicted for the suffix of the verb form; and stemC1, stemC2 and stemC3, refer to the inflection predicted for the penSyl of the verb form.
Recommended publications
  • Tagalog Pala: an Unsurprising Case of Mirativity
    Tagalog pala: an unsurprising case of mirativity Scott AnderBois Brown University Similar to many descriptions of miratives cross-linguistically, Schachter & Otanes(1972)’s clas- sic descriptive grammar of Tagalog describes the second position particle pala as “expressing mild surprise at new information, or an unexpected event or situation.” Drawing on recent work on mi- rativity in other languages, however, we show that this characterization needs to be refined in two ways. First, we show that while pala can be used in cases of surprise, pala itself merely encodes the speaker’s sudden revelation with the counterexpectational nature of surprise arising pragmatically or from other aspects of the sentence such as other particles and focus. Second, we present data from imperatives and interrogatives, arguing that this revelation need not concern ‘information’ per se, but rather the illocutionay update the sentence encodes. Finally, we explore the interactions between pala and other elements which express mirativity in some way and/or interact with the mirativity pala expresses. 1. Introduction Like many languages of the Philippines, Tagalog has a prominent set of discourse particles which express a variety of different evidential, attitudinal, illocutionary, and discourse-related meanings. Morphosyntactically, these particles have long been known to be second-position clitics, with a number of authors having explored fine-grained details of their distribution, rela- tive order, and the interaction of this with different types of sentences (e.g. Schachter & Otanes (1972), Billings & Konopasky(2003) Anderson(2005), Billings(2005) Kaufman(2010)). With a few recent exceptions, however, comparatively little has been said about the semantics/prag- matics of these different elements beyond Schachter & Otanes(1972)’s pioneering work (which is quite detailed given their broad scope of their work).
    [Show full text]
  • The Moods of Verbs Reteaching
    Name Date Lesson 5 The Moods of Verbs Reteaching The mood of a verb indicates the status of the action or condition it describes. Some actions and conditions are factual, while others exist only as possibilities or ideas. Indicative mood is used to make statements and ask questions about factual actions and conditions. STATEMENT Variety shows were popular in the early days of television. QUESTION Were ventriloquists more popular than acrobats? Imperative mood is used to give a command or make a request. The understood subject in a command is you. COMMAND Pick a flower from this bunch. Subjunctive mood is used primarily to express a wish or to refer to actions or conditions that are contrary to fact. The subjunctive form of a verb matches the past form of that verb. The subjunctive form of be is were. If a variety show were popular now, I might appear as a juggler. Another subjunctive form is used in formal writing to refer to a request or command. The top-rated singer insisted that we give him a better dressing room. A. Identifying the Mood of a Verb Indicate the mood of each underlined verb by labeling it with IND for indicative, IMP for imperative, or SUBJ for subjunctive. 1.Call your parents when you will be late. ____________ 2. Harry Truman became president after the death of Franklin Delano Roosevelt. ____________ 3. Take a moment to look over your term paper before you pass it in. ____________ CHAPTER 4 4. The Faculty Advisor urges that the meetings be over by 3:30.
    [Show full text]
  • Lesson-10 in Sanskrit, Verbs Are Associated with Ten Different
    -------------- Lesson-10 General introduction to the tenses. In Sanskrit, verbs are associated with ten different forms of usage. Of these six relate to the tenses and four relate to moods. We shall examine the usages now. Six tenses are identified as follows. The tenses directly relate to the time associated with the activity specified in the verb, i.e., whether the activity referred to in the verb is taking place now or has it happened already or if it will happen or going to happen etc. Present tense: vtIman kal: There is only one form for the present tense. Past tense: B¥t kal: Past tense has three forms associated with it. 1. Expressing something that had happened sometime in the recent past, typically last few days. 2. Expressing something that might have just happened, typically in the earlier part of the day. 3. Expressing something that had happened in the distant past about which we may not have much or any knowledge. Future tense: B¢vÝyt- kal: Future tense has two forms associated with it. 1. Expressing something that is certainly going to happen. 2. Expressing something that is likely to happen. ------Verb forms not associated with time. There are four forms of the verb which do not relate to any time. These forms are called "moods" in the English language. English grammar specifies three moods which are, Indicative mood, Imperative mood and the Subjunctive mood. In Sanskrit primers one sees a reference to four moods with a slightly different nomenclature. These are, Imperative mood, potential mood, conditional mood and benedictive mood.
    [Show full text]
  • A Case Study in Language Change
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Honors Theses Lee Honors College 4-17-2013 Glottopoeia: A Case Study in Language Change Ian Hollenbaugh Western Michigan University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/honors_theses Part of the Other English Language and Literature Commons Recommended Citation Hollenbaugh, Ian, "Glottopoeia: A Case Study in Language Change" (2013). Honors Theses. 2243. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/honors_theses/2243 This Honors Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Lee Honors College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. An Elementary Ghau Aethauic Grammar By Ian Hollenbaugh 1 i. Foreword This is an essential grammar for any serious student of Ghau Aethau. Mr. Hollenbaugh has done an excellent job in cataloguing and explaining the many grammatical features of one of the most complex language systems ever spoken. Now published for the first time with an introduction by my former colleague and premier Ghau Aethauic scholar, Philip Logos, who has worked closely with young Hollenbaugh as both mentor and editor, this is sure to be the definitive grammar for students and teachers alike in the field of New Classics for many years to come. John Townsend, Ph.D Professor Emeritus University of Nunavut 2 ii. Author’s Preface This grammar, though as yet incomplete, serves as my confession to what J.R.R. Tolkien once called “a secret vice.” History has proven Professor Tolkien right in thinking that this is not a bizarre or freak occurrence, undergone by only the very whimsical, but rather a common “hobby,” one which many partake in, and have partaken in since at least the time of Hildegard of Bingen in the twelfth century C.E.
    [Show full text]
  • Torres Bustamante Dissertation
    © 2013 Teresa Torres Bustamante ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ON THE SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS OF MIRATIVITY: EVIDENCE FROM SPANISH AND ALBANIAN By TERESA TORRES BUSTAMANTE A Dissertation submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Program in Linguistics written under the direction of Dr. Mark Baker and approved by New Brunswick, New Jersey October, 2013 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION ! On the Syntax and Semantics of Mirativity: Evidence from Spanish and Albanian By TERESA TORRES BUSTAMANTE Dissertation Director: Mark Baker In this dissertation, I examine mirative constructions in Spanish and Albanian, in which past tense morphology is used to convey speaker's surprise and does not seem to contribute its usual temporal meaning to the asserted proposition. I put forward an analysis that makes the following claims. First, mirative sentences are assertions that include a modal component. This modal component brings up the speaker's beliefs in a way that entails the opposite of what the assertion expresses. Thus, a clash is generated between the speaker's beliefs and the assertion, and this triggers a sense of surprise. Second, the past tense morphology is analyzed as being a real past ! ""! tense, following recent proposals for counterfactual conditionals. In the case of miratives, the past tense keeps its normal semantics, but is interpreted in the CP domain as the time argument of the modal base, rather than in TP. The beliefs that are contrasted with the assertion are therefore past beliefs up to the discovery time (which usually coincides with the speech time), in which the actual state of affairs is encountered by the speaker.
    [Show full text]
  • 2018. the Oxford Handbook of Modality and Mood. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 688 Pp Cameron Morin
    Review of Nuyts, Jan & Johan van der Auwera (eds.) 2018. The Oxford Handbook of Modality and Mood. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 688 pp Cameron Morin To cite this version: Cameron Morin. Review of Nuyts, Jan & Johan van der Auwera (eds.) 2018. The Oxford Handbook of Modality and Mood. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 688 pp. 2019. hal-03120215 HAL Id: hal-03120215 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03120215 Submitted on 8 Feb 2021 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Morin, Cameron. 2019. Review of Nuyts, Jan & Johan van der Auwera (eds.) 2018. The Oxford Handbook of Modality and Mood. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 688 pp. Linguist List 30: 4831. SUMMARY “The Oxford Handbook of Modality and Mood”, edited by Jan Nuyts and Johan van der Auwera (2018), provides a state of the art overview of the linguistic domains of modality and mood across various research perspectives, including morphosyntax, semantics, pragmatics, typology, language variation and change, theoretical frameworks, and more. It is composed of 23 chapters by leading researchers in the field. The book aims to offer a structured and neutral approach to modality and mood out of a vast expanse of literature, which has sometimes displayed wide conceptual and terminological divergence.
    [Show full text]
  • Sanskrit Sahakarin
    Vyakr[m! Salutation to you O Goddess Sarasvati, who is giver of boons, and who has a beautiful form! I begin my studies. Let there be success for me always. s<Sk&t shkairn! Page 1 Vyakr[m! Prepared by Dr. Kumud Singhal [email protected] 408-934-9747 Version #2 Version #1 available at http://www.arshavidyacenter.org/sanskrit_sahakarin.pdf Presented by Arsha Vidya Center Many thanks to our Sanskrit Teacher Çré Vijay Kapoor s<Sk&t shkairn! Page 2 Vyakr[m! Table of Contents 1. Introduction ......................................................................................................................................... 5 2. The Sanskrit Alphabet ........................................................................................................................ 6 3. Sandhi Rules ........................................................................................................................................ 7 3.1 Vowel Sandhi .................................................................................................................................... 7 3.2 Visarga Sandhi .................................................................................................................................. 8 3.3 Consonant Sandhi ........................................................................................................................... 10 4. Nouns .................................................................................................................................................. 13 4.1 Usage & Examples
    [Show full text]
  • Unaccusativity and the Syntax of Imperatives in East Circassian Ksenia Ershova*
    2017. Proc Ling Soc Amer 2, 36:1-14. https://doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v2i0.4091. Unaccusativity and the syntax of imperatives in East Circassian Ksenia Ershova* Abstract. This paper presents novel evidence for the syntactic distinction between unergative and unaccusative verbs in East Circassian (or Kabardian). The evidence concerns a particular strategy of forming imperatives – simultaneous causativization and reflexivization – which is only applicable to unaccusative predicates. I argue that this type of imperative involves the promotion of the internal argument to the a higher position through the use of the causative morpheme which has been grammaticalized to mark imperative mood. The observed patterns suggest that imperative mood, while generally associated with the CP-layer, must be sensitive to the structure of vP. Keywords. unaccusativity; imperative mood; causative; East Circassian; Kabardian; Northwest Caucasian 1. Introduction. This paper addresses the syntactic distinction between unergative and unac- cusative verbs in East Circassian, a language that has not been previously observed to draw such a divide. The evidence for unaccusativity comes from an unlikely source – the morphology of imperative mood. In particular, unaccusative verbs may form a special type of imperative that is unavailable for unergative verbs. This imperative form involves the transitivization of the verb via a synthetic causative morpheme and reflexivization of the causee. An example of this imperative form can be seen in (1)1 – I will refer to this form as the reflexive causative strategy of imperative formation throughout this paper. (1) z-o-m@-Ke-gw@bzˆ REFL.ABS-2SG.ERG-NEG-CAUS-be.angry ‘Don’t be angry (lit.
    [Show full text]
  • An Imperative?
    A Critical Study of Imperatives by Andrea Suzanne Leszek Submitted to the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Linguistics at the MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY February, 1995 © Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1995. All Rights Reserved. Author Linguistics and Philosophy January 20, 1995 Certified by ................................... .......................................... David Pesetsky Professor Linguistics and Philosophy Accepted by ...... ,..,..... ............................................ Wayne O'Neil Chairman, Departmental Committee Linguistics and Philosophy -'HCHIVLS -MRCi 1995 MAR 1. 1995 A Critical Study of Imperatives by Andkea Suzanne Leszek Submitted to the Department of Linguistics and Philosophy on January 20, 1995, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Linguistics Abstract The central concern of this thesis is the syntactic nature of imperatives. It explores themes which are common to imperatives cross-linguistically. This study is largely based on the work of Laka (1990) and Harris (1994). It is argued that the syntactic properties of imperatives result from an imperative feature, [IMP], which is a feature of an abstract syntactic category known as 1, which heads a £ Phrase. There is some parametric variation across languages as to how the syntax of imperatives interacts with Tense and negation. This study also adopts a mechanism from Harris (1994) by which the Morphological Structure Component allows imperatives to be realized in S- Structure. Chapter One states that there are three types of languages with respect to imperatives. It provides an example of each type of language with data from that language. Chapter Two contains general observations about imperatives cross-linguistically.
    [Show full text]
  • DAKNYI and ANYI the Constructions to Be Described in This Chapter
    CHAPTER SIXTEEN DAKNYI AND ANYI Th e constructions to be described in this chapter are all based on some form of the lexical verbs dakpe ‘to say’, or ale ‘to do’. Th ese two verbs occur in various infl ections and varying degrees of grammaticalisation in a wide variety of constructions, with a dramatic functional prominence in Tshangla discourse. Th e constructions built on each verb will be described in turn.1 In the fi nal section evidence will be given to show that these con- structions are grammaticalised derivatives of a non-fi nal concatenating construction. 16.1 Daknyi 16.1.1 Lexical verb Th e fi rst verb to be described is dakpe ‘to say’. Dakpe shows the behavior common to any lexical verb, participating without restriction in any of the tense, aspect and mirativity infl ections.2 Examples (1), (2), (3) and (4), show the present imperfective mirative and non-mirative, and past perfec- tive mirative and non-mirative infl ections respectively. Example (5) shows dakpe in the imperative mood. (1) Nyi tam thur zhu-wa giwala, cangapa-gi. Pon, dak-la dang . PRT story one tell-NOM COP joker-AGT king say-COP PRT ‘So he told a story, the joker. “King, he says . .” ’ (2) Bumthang nang-ka la, rokte Khengpa dak-ca me. Bumthang in-LOC PRT 3p Khengpa say-COP PRT ‘In Bumthang, they say (i.e. call them) Khengpa.’ (3) Ata brang-ka di-nyi, ji-gi nan yen ma-bi-la elder.brother place-LOC go-NF 1s-AGT 2s teach NEG-give-PRT dak-pa-la.
    [Show full text]
  • Negation in Syntax--On the Nature of Functional Categories and Projections
    NEGATION IN SYNTAX: ON THE NATURE OF FUNCTIONAL CATEGORIES AND PROJECTIONS HIREN ITZIAR LAKA HUGARZA B.A. University of the Basque Country ( 1985) H.A. University of the Basque Country ( 1986) SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS & PHILOSOPHY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREHENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF f HILOSOPHY IN LINGUISTICS at the HASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY August, 1990 c Hiren Itziar Laka Mugarza The author hereby grants to H.I.T. permission to reproduce and to distribute copies of this thesis document in whole or in part Signature of Author /hr- bkJz artment of ~ingui'6tics& Philosophy Cestif ied by r-- Keneth Hale Thesis Supervi~or \ n PA A Wayne 0 '.Neil O'A~~*~H~~#~ZTCY~,OF TE Departanen .el Committee AUG 3 1IS90 The central concern of this work is the syntactic nature of negation in Universal Grammar, and its relation to other functional elements in the Syntax. The study argues that negation is not a syntactic category on its own; rather, it is one of the values of a more abstract syntactic category, named X, which includes other sentence operators, such as affirmation and emphasis (Chapter 2). It is also argued that the syntactic feature [negation] surfaces in other syntactic categories besides X. In particular, the existence of [Nl (negative) Complementizers is defended; this acounts for a range of phenomena in various lanquages: across-the-clause licensing of Negative Polarity Items in English, the ditribution of the -nik complementizer in Basque, and the nature of Dubitative Subjunctive in Romance (Chapter 3). Chapter 1 argues for the existence of a universal requirement that inflectional heads such as negation (Z) must be c-commanded by the syntactic head Tense at S- structure.
    [Show full text]
  • Illocutionary Revelations: Yucatec Maya Bakáan and the Typology Of
    Illocutionary revelations: Yucatec Maya bakaan´ and the typology of miratives* Scott AnderBois Brown University Abstract Miratives have often been thought of as expressing predications which can be schematized as ‘p is Y for the speaker at the time of the utterance’, where Y is some a member of the set surprising, new information, a sudden revelation, . While { } much of the prior literature has discussed the value of Y, this discussion has typically been taken to be primarily a matter of analysis or its conceptual underpinnings rather than an empirical one. In this paper, I examine a new mirative in detail, Yucatec Maya (YM) bakaan´ , using context-relative felicity judgments to argue that bakaan´ conven- tionally encodes sudden revelation rather than these other notions. While I hold that bakaan´ encodes revelation, I argue that this revelation is not in fact about proposi- tional content per se, but rather is about the appropriateness/utility of the illocutionary update the speaker performs. A sudden revelation that a proposition is true is one such revelation, but other kinds are more clearly illocutionary in nature. Evidence for this position comes not only from bakaan´ in declarative sentences, but also its use in im- peratives and interrogatives. I argue that the range of uses that the use of bakaan´ as an illocutionary modifier across sentence types sheds light on the kinds of updates they encode, and in particular supports a theory in which declarative updates are more complex than corresponding imperative and interrogative ones. 1. Introduction Since DeLancey (1997) first brought the term to popular use, the nature of mirativity, its grammatical encoding, and very existence have been much debated.
    [Show full text]