Model Systems in Behavioral Ecology: Integrating Conceptual, Theoretical, and Empirical Approaches

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Model Systems in Behavioral Ecology: Integrating Conceptual, Theoretical, and Empirical Approaches Model Systems in Behavioral Ecology: Integrating Conceptual, Theoretical, and Empirical Approaches. Edited by Lee Alan Dugatkin Model Systems in Behavioral Ecology: Integrating Conceptual, Theoretical, and Empirical Approaches by Lee Alan Dugatkin Review by: Reviewed by Lawrence M Dill The Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol. 77, No. 3 (September 2002), pp. 361-362 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/345260 . Accessed: 10/12/2012 15:15 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Quarterly Review of Biology. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded by the authorized user from 192.168.72.225 on Mon, 10 Dec 2012 15:15:17 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Volume 77, No. 3 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY September 2002 REVIEWS AND BRIEF NOTICES History, Philosophy & Ethics .....................307 Plant Sciences ......................................328 General Biology ....................................312 Animal Sciences ....................................331 For Tyros & Laics ..................................314 Aquatic Sciences ...................................346 Paleontology ........................................315 Environmental Sciences ..........................347 Molecular Biology .................................317 Neural Sciences ....................................355 Cellular Biology ....................................317 Behavior .............................................360 Genetics & Evolution ..............................318 Human Biology & Health ........................362 Reproduction & Development ..................326 Biomedical Sciences ...............................364 Microbiology ........................................327 HISTORY, PHILOSOPHY & ETHICS occurs before selection on the rest of the pheno- type is well laid out. Forsdyke concludes that, if his THE ORIGIN OF SPECIES Revisited: A Victorian interpretation is correct, Romanes and Bateson Who Anticipated Modern Developments in were light years ahead of their contemporaries. Darwin’s Theory. This assumes that we can equate the modern-day By Donald R Forsdyke. Montreal and Ithaca (New knowledge that aspects of species-specific base %Gםcomposition in viral DNA (in particular, C ם York): McGill-Queen’s University Press. $49.95. xi 275 p; ill.; index. ISBN: 0-7735-2259-X. 2001. differences) indicate some physiological basis for From the dust jacket, I expected this to be an his- selection and, specifically, for speciation of organ- torical treatment into antecedents to Darwin’s the- isms other than viruses, which is a real stretch. ory on the origin of species. It is part history—“[i]n Romanes and Bateson were seeking a physiologi- places I am drawn to ask questions of a historical cal mechanism. Whether we have found that in .G% differences in viruses remains to be seenםnature” (p 3)—but the history takes the form of C looking for hints in the 19th century for recent Other aspects of the phenotype of the gene (pat- physiological and molecular approaches to the ori- terns of methylation, imprinting) are not consid- gin of species. Forsdyke believes that he has found ered; whether any aspects of the heritable pheno- such hints in the works of Romanes, Gulick, and type of the gene relate to speciation (initiation or Bateson, the latter being well known for his redis- adaptation) remains to be proven. What Forsdyke covery of Mendel’s paper. Romanes was a staunch does is remind us that individuals other than Dar- supporter of Darwin. Gulick (for most of his life a win were seeking alternate mechanisms of speci- missionary) is known to some for his discovery of ation. speciation in isolated, but contiguous populations Brian K Hall, Biology, Dalhousie University, Hali- of Hawaiian snails. Less well known is that Gulick fax, Nova Scotia, Canada anticipated “organic selection” (the Baldwin effect) by 11 years. Forsdyke admits that his approach is unabashed Whig history, which histo- Dear Mr Darwin: Letters on the Evolution of rians are not supposed to do. But Forsdyke is not Life and Human Nature. an historian, he is a biochemist and molecular biol- By Gabriel Dover. Berkeley (California): University of .p; ill.; index 268 ם ogist. California Press. $27.50. xvi As his mechanism of speciation, Darwin opted ISBN: 0-520-22790-5. [First published in Great for natural selection over the production of vari- Britain in 1999 by Weidenfeld & Nicolson.] 2000. ants—most evolutionary biologists would not In this imaginary correspondence with Charles define variation as “like does not produce like,” or Darwin the author asserts that many of the ideas inheritance as “like produces like,” as Forsdyke of William Hamilton, George Williams, Robert does (p 16). Bateson wanted to include reproduc- Trivers, and John Maynard Smith are based upon tive criteria in his definition of a species. Romanes’s the “illusion” that genes are the ultimate units of thesis that reproductive or physiological selection selection (p 70), that evolutionary factors addi- 307 This content downloaded by the authorized user from 192.168.72.225 on Mon, 10 Dec 2012 15:15:17 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 308 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW OF BIOLOGY Volume 77 tional to natural selection have been overlooked into her account, Ceccarelli adds an important (such as neutral drift, “exaptation,” “adoptation,” contribution to our understanding of how science and “molecular drive”), and that DNA, RNA, and develops. proteins have cooperated and coevolved since the The book is separated into four sections. The origin of life. The important writings of Richard first three are case studies of scientists attempting Dawkins on these subjects are repeatedly carica- to inspire interdisciplinarity through inspirational tured and scornfully dismissed, and Edward O Wil- monographs; the fourth section then offers con- son is called “the father of evolutionary psychol- clusions concerning the genre, including how it ogy” (p 233). Also, the author finds evidence from contributes to the rhetoric of science, the history his teary experiences in watching football (soccer) of science, and interdisciplinary studies. Each case matches (p 186) and in listening to music (p 242) study is further separated into two chapters. The that human behavior is not genetically determined first chapter places the volume within its historical and therefore that we have free will. How, he asks, context, outlining the social, conceptual, and theo- could the behavior of team athletes and their sup- retical debates on the eve of the book’s publication. porting fans be under the control of selfish genes The second chapter then offers a close reading of if they are unrelated and from such diverse back- the text itself combined with an examination of its grounds? reception within the scientific community. Through The author does not provide the background this admixture of the rhetoric of science and the information needed by his intended audience history of science, textual analysis, and reader (general readers) to evaluate this truly astonishing response, Ceccarelli is able to explain how the suc- mishmash of confused and ignorant assertions. cessful inspirational monograph was rhetorically Anyone wishing to gain a clear understanding of designed to unite the social and conceptual divi- the topics discussed in this book should go to one sions among disciplines to form new interdisciplin- of the recently published introductory textbooks ary fields of research. on sociobiology or evolutionary psychology, or to The three volumes Ceccarelli examines are The- the original writings of the authors mentioned odosius Dobzhansky’s Genetics and the Origin of Spe- above. The same course of action is recommended cies (1937. New York: Columbia University Press), for the author. Erwin Schro¨dinger’s What Is Life?: The Physical William F Zimmerman, Biology, Amherst College, Aspect of the Living Cell (1944. Cambridge: Cam- Amherst, Massachusetts bridge University Press), and Edward O Wilson’s Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge (1998. New York: Knopf). Although the first two books helped usher Shaping Science with Rhetoric: The Cases of in the modern synthesis and molecular biology, Dobzhansky, Schro¨dinger, and Wilson. respectively, the third has arguably ended in fail- By Leah Ceccarelli. Chicago (Illinois): University of Chi- ure. The most exciting and useful conclusion cago Press. $55.00 (hardcover); $20.00 (paper). xi comes from a comparison of why, rhetorically, p; ill.; index. ISBN: 0-226-09906-7 (hc); 0- these volumes succeeded or failed. Ceccarelli 204 ם 226-09907-5 (pb). 2001. defines two specific rhetorical strategies used to Despite the book’s title, this is no disciplinary appeal to two different disciplines. The first, con- defense of how rhetoric is responsible for the crea- ceptual chiasmus, is a rhetorical strategy that tion of scientific knowledge. Rather, the author has encourages each discipline to view the
Recommended publications
  • EXTRA-PAIR MATING and EFFECTIVE POPULATION SIZE in the SONG SPARROW (Melospiza Melodia)
    EXTRA-PAIR MATING AND EFFECTIVE POPULATION SIZE IN THE SONG SPARROW (Melospiza melodia) By Kathleen D. O'Connor B.A., Skidmore College, 2000 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FUFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES THE FACULTY OF FORESTRY Department of Forest Sciences Centre for Applied Conservation Research We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA April 2003 © Kathleen D. O'Connor, 2003 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Department of Fpce_t Sciences The University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada Date 2HApc. 20O5 DE-6 (2/88) Abstract Effective population size is used widely in conservation research and management as an indicator of the genetic state of populations. However, estimates of effective population size for socially monogamous species can vary with the frequency of matings outside of the social pair. I investigated the effect of cryptic extra-pair fertilization on effective population size estimates using four years of demographic and genetic data from a resident population of song sparrows (Melospiza melodia Oberholser 1899) on Mandarte Island, British Columbia, Canada.
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging : an Ecology Model of Consumer Behaviour?
    This is a repository copy of Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour?. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/122432/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Wells, VK orcid.org/0000-0003-1253-7297 (2012) Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour? Marketing Theory. pp. 117-136. ISSN 1741-301X https://doi.org/10.1177/1470593112441562 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Foraging: an ecology model of consumer behavior? Victoria.K.Wells* Durham Business School, UK First Submission June 2010 Revision One December 2010 Revision Two August 2011 Accepted for Publication: September 2011 * Address for correspondence: Dr Victoria Wells (née James), Durham Business School, Durham University, Mill Hill Lane, Durham, DH1 3LB & Queen’s Campus, University Boulevard, Thornaby, Stockton-on-Tees, TS17 6BH, Telephone: +44 (0)191 334 0472, E-mail: [email protected] I would like to thank Tony Ellson for his guidance and Gordon Foxall for his helpful comments during the development and writing of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM
    Mimicry - Ecology - Oxford Bibliographies 12/13/12 7:29 PM Mimicry David W. Kikuchi, David W. Pfennig Introduction Among nature’s most exquisite adaptations are examples in which natural selection has favored a species (the mimic) to resemble a second, often unrelated species (the model) because it confuses a third species (the receiver). For example, the individual members of a nontoxic species that happen to resemble a toxic species may dupe any predators by behaving as if they are also dangerous and should therefore be avoided. In this way, adaptive resemblances can evolve via natural selection. When this phenomenon—dubbed “mimicry”—was first outlined by Henry Walter Bates in the middle of the 19th century, its intuitive appeal was so great that Charles Darwin immediately seized upon it as one of the finest examples of evolution by means of natural selection. Even today, mimicry is often used as a prime example in textbooks and in the popular press as a superlative example of natural selection’s efficacy. Moreover, mimicry remains an active area of research, and studies of mimicry have helped illuminate such diverse topics as how novel, complex traits arise; how new species form; and how animals make complex decisions. General Overviews Since Henry Walter Bates first published his theories of mimicry in 1862 (see Bates 1862, cited under Historical Background), there have been periodic reviews of our knowledge in the subject area. Cott 1940 was mainly concerned with animal coloration. Subsequent reviews, such as Edmunds 1974 and Ruxton, et al. 2004, have focused on types of mimicry associated with defense from predators.
    [Show full text]
  • Shell Resource Partitioning As a Mechanism of Coexistence in Two Co‑Occurring Terrestrial Hermit Crab Species Sebastian Steibl and Christian Laforsch*
    Steibl and Laforsch BMC Ecol (2020) 20:1 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12898-019-0268-2 BMC Ecology RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Shell resource partitioning as a mechanism of coexistence in two co-occurring terrestrial hermit crab species Sebastian Steibl and Christian Laforsch* Abstract Background: Coexistence is enabled by ecological diferentiation of the co-occurring species. One possible mecha- nism thereby is resource partitioning, where each species utilizes a distinct subset of the most limited resource. This resource partitioning is difcult to investigate using empirical research in nature, as only few species are primarily limited by solely one resource, rather than a combination of multiple factors. One exception are the shell-dwelling hermit crabs, which are known to be limited under natural conditions and in suitable habitats primarily by the avail- ability of gastropod shells. In the present study, we used two co-occurring terrestrial hermit crab species, Coenobita rugosus and C. perlatus, to investigate how resource partitioning is realized in nature and whether it could be a driver of coexistence. Results: Field sampling of eleven separated hermit crab populations showed that the two co-occurring hermit crab species inhabit the same beach habitat but utilize a distinct subset of the shell resource. Preference experiments and principal component analysis of the shell morphometric data thereby revealed that the observed utilization patterns arise out of diferent intrinsic preferences towards two distinct shell shapes. While C. rugosus displayed a preference towards a short and globose shell morphology, C. perlatus showed preferences towards an elongated shell morphol- ogy with narrow aperture. Conclusion: The two terrestrial hermit crab species occur in the same habitat but have evolved diferent preferences towards distinct subsets of the limiting shell resource.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Relations Chapter 8 Behavioral Ecology
    Social Relations Chapter 8 Behavioral Ecology: Study of social relations. Studies interactions between organisms and the environment mediated by behavior 1 Copyright © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Some differences between males and females…besides the obvious!!! • Females produce larger, more energetically costly gametes. • Males produce smaller, less energetically costly gametes. Female reproduction thought to be limited by resource access. Male reproduction limited by mate access. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v =NjnFBGW3Fmg&feature=related2 Hermaphrodites • Hermaphrodites Exhibit both male and female function. Most familiar example is plants. So, how do organisms choose their mate??? 3 Mate Choice • Sexual Selection Differences in reproductive rates among individuals as a result of differences in mating success. Intrasexual Selection: Individuals of one sex compete among themselves for mates. Intersexual Selection: Individuals of one sex consistently choose mates among members of opposite sex based on a particular trait. http://www.youtube.co m/watch? v=eYU4v_ZTRII 4 How much is too much??? • Darwin proposed that sexual selection would continue until balanced by other sources of natural selection • Given a choice, female guppies will mate with brightly colored males. However, brightly colored males attract predators. It’s a trade-off! 5 Class Activity!!! • In groups, using guys in a bar looking for dates example, come up with a scenario of sexual selection either going too far or succeeding!!! Be creative!!! 6 Sexual Selection in Plants??? Nonrandom Mating Found Among Wild Radish • Wild radish flowers have both male (stamens) and female (pistils) parts, but cannot self-pollinate. • Marshall found non-random mating in wild radish populations.
    [Show full text]
  • Where Is Behavioural Ecology Going?
    Opinion TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.21 No.7 July 2006 Where is behavioural ecology going? Ian P.F. Owens Division of Biology and NERC Centre for Population Biology, Imperial College London, Silwood Park, Ascot, Berkshire, UK, SL5 7PY Since the 1990s, behavioural ecologists have largely wove together the theories developed over the preceding abandoned some traditional areas of interest, such as decade and championed a new empirical approach to optimal foraging, but many long-standing challenges investigating behaviour. The key element of this approach remain. Moreover, the core strengths of behavioural was the use of adaptation as the central conceptual ecology, including the use of simple adaptive models to framework, which gave behavioural ecologists a precise investigate complex biological phenomena, have now a priori expectation: behaviours should evolve to maxi- been applied to new puzzles outside behaviour. But this mise the fitness of the individuals showing strategy comes at a cost. Replication across studies is those behaviours. rare and there have been few tests of the underlying Krebs and Davies also stressed the importance of two genetic assumptions of adaptive models. Here, I attempt other principles [27]. The first was the need to quantify to identify the key outstanding questions in behavioural variation in behaviour accurately. Drawing attention to ecology and suggest that researchers must make the new quantitative work that was being performed in greater use of model organisms and evolutionary some areas of ethology [28,29], they showed how this genetics in order to make substantial progress on approach could be applied to a variety of behaviours.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Foraging and the Behavioral Ecology of Intragroup Resource Transfers
    UC Davis UC Davis Previously Published Works Title Social foraging and the behavioral ecology of intragroup resource transfers Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5565r5zn Journal Evolutionary Anthropology, 5(2) ISSN 1060-1538 Author Winterhalder, B Publication Date 1996 DOI 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1996)5:2<46::AID-EVAN4>3.0.CO;2-U Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California 46 Evolutionary Anthropology Social Foraging and the Behavioral Ecology of Intragrou p Resource Transfers BRUCE WINTERHALDER Two chimpanzees stalk, isolate, and kill a red colobus monkey. An attendant question: is sharing as practiced primatologist notes that parts of the prey are relinquished selectively to onlooking among modem foragers an early or scroungers (Fig. 1). A human forager returns to camp mid-afternoonwith a freshly late development in our prehistory? killed, medium-sized ungulate. Later in the day, an ethnographer observes that The models of behavioral ecology shared portions of the animal have found their way into the cooking pots of most or (boldface indicates a term defined in all of those in the small band. Examining a prehistoric scatter of food residues, an the Glossary) described here empha- ethnoarcheologist wonders when early hominids began to scrounge or share food, size the importance of intragroup re- and with what consequences for our evolution. All of these settings represent one source transfer among social problem: the analysis of intragroup resource transfers among social foragers. New foragers, but give them different and studies in the behavioral ecology of transfers show them to be more commonplace more diverse explanations than those in nature, more complicatedand variable, and more subject to comparativeanalysis just cited.
    [Show full text]
  • Rodent Foraging Is Affected by Indirect, but Not by Direct, Cues of Predation Risk
    Behavioral Ecology Vol. 15 No. 3: 433–437 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arh031 Rodent foraging is affected by indirect, but not by direct, cues of predation risk John L. Orrock,a Brent J. Danielson,a and R. Jory Brinkerhoffb aEcology and Evolutionary Biology Interdepartmental Graduate Program, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011, USA, and bDepartment of Zoology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695, USA We used foraging trays to determine whether oldfield mice, Peromyscus polionotus, altered foraging in response to direct cues of predation risk (urine of native and nonnative predators) and indirect cues of predation risk (foraging microhabitat, precipitation, and moon illumination). The proportion of seeds remaining in each tray (a measure of the giving-up density [GUD]) was used to measure risk perceived by mice. Mice did not alter their GUD when presented with cues of native predators (bobcats, Lynx rufus, and red foxes, Vulpes vulpes), recently introduced predators (coyotes, Canis latrans), nonnative predators (ocelots, Leopardus pardalis), a native herbivore (white-tailed deer, Odocoileus virginianus), or a water control. Rather, GUD was related to microhabitat: rodents removed more seeds from foraging trays sheltered beneath vegetative cover compared with exposed trays outside of cover. Rodents also removed more seeds during nights with precipitation and when moon illumination was low. Our results suggest that P. polionotus used indirect cues rather than direct cues to assess risk of vertebrate predation. Indirect cues may be more reliable than are direct scent cues for estimating risk from multiple vertebrate predators that present the most risk in open environments. Key words: foraging, giving-up densities, Peromyscus polionotus; predator recognition; prey behavior; risk assessment; Savannah River Site.
    [Show full text]
  • Rana Clamitans and R. Catesbeiana) Susan Z
    University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn Doctoral Dissertations University of Connecticut Graduate School 8-23-2013 Ecological and Behavioral Interactions Between Two Closely Related North American Frogs (Rana clamitans and R. catesbeiana) Susan Z. Herrick Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations Recommended Citation Herrick, Susan Z., "Ecological and Behavioral Interactions Between Two Closely Related North American Frogs (Rana clamitans and R. catesbeiana)" (2013). Doctoral Dissertations. 214. https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations/214 Ecological and Behavioral Interactions Between Two Closely Related North American frogs ( Rana clamitans and R. catesbeiana ) Susan Zator Herrick, PhD University of Connecticut, 2013 Abstract Resource partitioning within the ecological niche space in which there is a high level of overlap between species can alleviate the tendency toward competitive exclusion. When competitive ability is asymmetrical due to predation or other ecological factors, it may be more effective for the less competitive species to lessen direct competition by contracting their use of local resources. Species occurring in mixed assemblages may come into direct contact with each other throughout their respective breeding seasons. Where competition for breeding habitat and acoustic space exists, the level of interference is expected to vary widely, depending upon the ecological and breeding similarities between the species involved and the relative importance of the resource. In this study, I investigated the breeding season interspecific interactions of two species of ranid frog in eastern North America, the American bullfrog ( Rana catesbeiana ) and the green frog ( R. clamitans ). The ecological and behavioral similarities between these species combined with phylogenetic relatedness and comparable natural distributions make them an ideal system for studying interspecific dynamics related to their breeding ecology.
    [Show full text]
  • Group Selection As Behavioral Adaptation to Systematic Risk
    Group Selection as Behavioral Adaptation to Systematic Risk Ruixun Zhang1, Thomas J. Brennan2, Andrew W. Lo3,4* 1 MIT Department of Mathematics, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America, 2 Northwestern University School of Law, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America, 3 MIT Sloan School of Management, CSAIL, and EECS, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America, 4 AlphaSimplex Group, LLC, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America Abstract Despite many compelling applications in economics, sociobiology, and evolutionary psychology, group selection is still one of the most hotly contested ideas in evolutionary biology. Here we propose a simple evolutionary model of behavior and show that what appears to be group selection may, in fact, simply be the consequence of natural selection occurring in stochastic environments with reproductive risks that are correlated across individuals. Those individuals with highly correlated risks will appear to form ‘‘groups’’, even if their actions are, in fact, totally autonomous, mindless, and, prior to selection, uniformly randomly distributed in the population. This framework implies that a separate theory of group selection is not strictly necessary to explain observed phenomena such as altruism and cooperation. At the same time, it shows that the notion of group selection does captures a unique aspect of evolution—selection with correlated reproductive risk–that may be sufficiently widespread to warrant a separate term for the phenomenon. Citation: Zhang R, Brennan TJ, Lo AW (2014) Group Selection as Behavioral Adaptation to Systematic Risk. PLoS ONE 9(10): e110848. doi:10.1371/journal.pone. 0110848 Editor: Claude Wicker-Thomas, CNRS, France Received July 15, 2014; Accepted September 25, 2014; Published October 29, 2014 Copyright: ß 2014 Zhang et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Predation Risk on Group Size, Vigilance, and Foraging Behavior In
    Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published April 6, 2014 Behavioral The official journal of the ISBE Ecology International Society for Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2014), 00(00), 1–12. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru050 Original Article Effects of predation risk on group size, vigilance, and foraging behavior in an African Downloaded from ungulate community a a b Scott Creel, Paul Schuette, and David Christianson http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ aDepartment of Ecology, Montana State University, 310 Lewis Hall, Bozeman, MT 59717, USA and bSchool of Natural Resources and the Environment, University of Arizona, 325 Biological Sciences East, Tucson, AZ 85745, USA Received 12 November 2013; revised 25 February 2014; accepted 2 March 2014. Predators alter prey dynamics by direct killing and through the costs of antipredator responses or risk effects. Antipredator behavior includes proactive responses to long-term variation in risk (e.g., grouping patterns) and reactive responses to short-term variation in at Libraries-Montana State University, Bozeman on April 14, 2014 risk (e.g., intense vigilance). In a 3-year field study, we measured variation in antipredator responses and the foraging costs of these responses for 5 ungulates (zebra, wildebeest, Grant’s gazelle, impala, and giraffe) that comprised more than 90% of the prey community available to the 2 locally dominant predators, lions and spotted hyenas. Using a model-selection approach, we examined how vigilance and group size responded to attributes of the predator, prey, and environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Factors Affecting the Evolution and Behavioral Ecology of the Modern Bears Author(S): Ian Stirling and Andrew E
    Factors Affecting the Evolution and Behavioral Ecology of the Modern Bears Author(s): Ian Stirling and Andrew E. Derocher Source: Bears: Their Biology and Management, Vol. 8, A Selection of Papers from the Eighth International Conference on Bear Research and Management, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada, February 1989 (1990), pp. 189-204 Published by: International Association of Bear Research and Management Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3872919 Accessed: 03/01/2009 19:54 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=iba. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
    [Show full text]