Memory Dysfunction in Neurological Practice Andrew E Budson, Bruce H Price

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Memory Dysfunction in Neurological Practice Andrew E Budson, Bruce H Price Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 5 February 2007 42 Practical Neurology HOW TO UNDERSTAND IT Pract Neurol 2007; 7: 42–47 Memory dysfunction in neurological practice Andrew E Budson, Bruce H Price A E Budson Geriatric Research Educational Clinical Center, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA; Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston University, Boston, MA; Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA; Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA B H Price Department of Neurology, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Correspondence to: Dr A E Budson Building 62, Room B30, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, 200 Springs Road, Bedford, MA 01730, USA; [email protected] Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 5 February 2007 Budson, Price 43 omplaints of impaired memory are Episodic memory is the explicit and declarative among the most common symptoms reported to neurologists. Moreover, memory system that we all use to recall our C impairment of memory is one of the personal experience most disabling aspects of many neurological disorders, including neurodegenerative dis- eases, strokes, tumours, head trauma, collection of mental abilities that use hypoxia, cardiac surgery, malnutrition, atten- different systems within the brain. A memory tion deficit disorder, depression, anxiety, system is a way that the brain processes medication adverse effects, and just normal information in order to make it available for aging. This memory loss often impairs the use at a later time. Some systems are patient’s daily activities, profoundly affecting associated with conscious awareness (explicit) not just them but also their families. and can be consciously recalled (declarative), Memory research that began with neuro- whereas others are typically unconscious psychological studies of patients with focal (non-declarative) and are instead expressed brain lesions now includes new methods such by a change in behaviour (implicit). as positron emission tomography, functional Here we will describe the four systems that MRI, and event-related potentials which have are of clinical relevance: episodic memory, all provided a more refined and improved semantic memory, procedural memory, and classification system. Instead of conceptualis- working memory (table 1). Additional details ing memory as ‘‘short-term’’ versus ‘‘long- may be found in Budson and Price1 and the term’’, we now think of memory as a other references listed below.2–6 TABLE 1 Selected memory systems Length of Main anatomical Memory System Examples Awareness storage structures Episodic memory Remembering a short story, Explicit Minutes to Medial temporal lobes, what you had for dinner years anterior thalamic nucleus, last night, and what you mamillary body, fornix, did on your last birthday prefrontal cortex Semantic memory Knowing who was the first Declarative Minutes to Inferior lateral temporal President of the US, the Explicit years lobes colour of a lion, and how a fork and comb are different Procedural memory Driving a standard Declarative Minutes to Basal ganglia, cerebellum, transmission car (explicit), Implicit years supplementary motor area and learning the sequence of numbers on a touch-tone phone without trying (implicit) Working memory Phonological: keeping a phone Non-declarative Seconds to Phonological: prefrontal number ‘‘in your head’’ before Explicit minutes; cortex, Broca’s area, dialing information Wernike’s area Spatial: mentally following a Declarative actively Spatial: prefrontal cortex, route, or rotating an object in rehearsed or visual association areas your mind manipulated www.practical-neurology.com Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 5 February 2007 44 Practical Neurology visual scenes. Distortions of memories, and false memories, may occur due to frontal lobe dysfunction, such as when information Prefrontal Thalamocingulate Cingulate gyrus becomes associated with the wrong context, cortex tract or specific details are incorrect. One way to think about how the frontal and medial temporal lobes work together is by Anterior thalamic an oversimplified but useful analogy: nucleus Fornix N the frontal lobes are the ‘‘file clerks’’ of Thalamus the episodic memory system Mammillothalamic tract N the medial temporal lobes are the ‘‘recent Mammillary body memory file cabinet’’ N other cortical regions are the ‘‘remote memory file cabinet’’. Parahippocampal Amygdala gyrus So if the frontal lobes are impaired, it is Hippocampal difficult, though not impossible, to get formation information in and out of storage. And the information may be distorted due to ‘‘impro- per filing’’ leading to inaccurate context or Figure 1 EPISODIC MEMORY sequence. If the medial temporal lobes are Episodic memory. The medial temporal Episodic memory is the explicit and declara- damaged, it will be difficult or impossible for lobes, including the hippocampus and parahippocampus, form the core of the tive memory system that we all use to recall recent information to be retained. Older episodic memory system. Other brain our personal experience, framed in our own information that has been ‘‘consolidated’’ regions are also necessary for episodic context, such as remembering a short story or over months to years is thought to be stored memory to function correctly. (From Budson AE, Price BH. Memory what we had for dinner last night. Episodic in other cortical regions, and will therefore be dysfunction. N Engl J Med memory loss follows a predictable pattern available even when the medial temporal 2005;352:692–9. Copyright known as Ribot’s law: recently acquired lobes are damaged. Massachusetts Medical Society. All memories are more vulnerable to loss than Alzheimer’s disease is the most common rights reserved.) longer established ones. This memory system disorder that disrupts episodic memory. Recent depends on the medial temporal lobes research suggests that these patients suffer (including the hippocampus, entorhinal and from memory distortions and false memories in perirhinal cortex). Other critical structures addition to their failure to remember informa- (some of which are associated with a circuit tion. Other common disorders that disrupt described by Papez in 1937) include the basal episodic memory are listed in table 2. forebrain with the medial septum and the If a patient presents with an inability to diagonal band of Broca’s area, the retro- remember recent information and experi- splenial cortex, the mammillothalamic tract, ences, a disorder of episodic memory should and the anterior nucleus of the thalamus. A be suspected. The evaluation should include a lesion in any one of these structures may detailed history, with emphasis on the time cause the impairment that is characteristic of course of the memory disorder. For example: dysfunction of the episodic memory system N degenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s (fig 1). start insidiously and progressively worsen The frontal lobes are also important for N vascular dementia and other multifocal certain aspects of episodic memory, including disorders such as multiple sclerosis tend the registration, acquisition, or encoding to start suddenly and progress in a (learning) of information, retrieval of infor- stepwise manner mation without contextual and other cues, N staticdisorderssuchasstrokesand recalling the source of information, and traumatic brain injuries are typically assessing the temporal sequence and recency maximal at onset, improve, and are then of events. The left medial temporal and stable frontal lobes are most involved when learning N transient disorders of episodic memory words, whereas the right medial temporal and include those attributable to a concussion, frontal lobes are most involved when learning a seizure, and transient global amnesia. Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 5 February 2007 Budson, Price 45 Interviewing a family member or other informant is usually necessary because the TABLE 2 Four memory systems and the common clinical disorders patient will invariably not remember impor- which disrupt them tant aspects of their history. Any history of other cognitive deficits should also be elicited, Episodic Semantic Procedural Working such as attention, language, and visuospatial Disease memory memory memory memory skills. Medical and neurological examination Alzheimer’s disease +++ ++ – ++ should be performed to look for signs of Frontotemporal dementia ++ ++ – +++ neurodegenerative disorders, focal neurologi- Semantic dementia + +++ ?– cal injury, and systemic illness. Lewy body dementia ++ ?? ++ There are many cognitive tests that can be Stroke and vascular +++ ++ used to assess a patient’s episodic memory dementia Parkinson’s disease + + +++ ++ function. But the relationships between a Huntington’s disease + + +++ +++ patient’s cognitive functions, test perfor- Progressive supranuclear + + ++ +++ mance, and their ‘‘real life’’ activities are palsy often complex. Test performance is rarely Korsakoff syndrome +++ –– ¡ pathognomonic of a specific disorder. And no Multiple sclerosis + ¡ ? ++ single test is capable of evaluating all aspects Transient global amnesia +++ ¡ –– of memory. Factors confounding test inter- Hypoxic–ischaemic injury ++ –– ¡ pretation include aphasia, dyslexia, low intel- Head trauma ++¡ ++ ¡¡¡ ¡ lectual function, alterations in mood and Tumours ¡ ¡ motivation, confusion or delirium, psychosis, Depression + ++ Anxiety + –– ¡ and medication adverse effects. Cognitive
Recommended publications
  • Psychogenic and Organic Amnesia. a Multidimensional Assessment of Clinical, Neuroradiological, Neuropsychological and Psychopathological Features
    Behavioural Neurology 18 (2007) 53–64 53 IOS Press Psychogenic and organic amnesia. A multidimensional assessment of clinical, neuroradiological, neuropsychological and psychopathological features Laura Serraa,∗, Lucia Faddaa,b, Ivana Buccionea, Carlo Caltagironea,b and Giovanni A. Carlesimoa,b aFondazione IRCCS Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy bClinica Neurologica, Universita` Tor Vergata, Roma, Italy Abstract. Psychogenic amnesia is a complex disorder characterised by a wide variety of symptoms. Consequently, in a number of cases it is difficult distinguish it from organic memory impairment. The present study reports a new case of global psychogenic amnesia compared with two patients with amnesia underlain by organic brain damage. Our aim was to identify features useful for distinguishing between psychogenic and organic forms of memory impairment. The findings show the usefulness of a multidimensional evaluation of clinical, neuroradiological, neuropsychological and psychopathological aspects, to provide convergent findings useful for differentiating the two forms of memory disorder. Keywords: Amnesia, psychogenic origin, organic origin 1. Introduction ness of the self – and a period of wandering. According to Kopelman [33], there are three main predisposing Psychogenic or dissociative amnesia (DSM-IV- factors for global psychogenic amnesia: i) a history of TR) [1] is a clinical syndrome characterised by a mem- transient, organic amnesia due to epilepsy [52], head ory disorder of nonorganic origin. Following Kopel- injury [4] or alcoholic blackouts [20]; ii) a history of man [31,33], psychogenic amnesia can either be sit- psychiatric disorders such as depressed mood, and iii) uation specific or global. Situation specific amnesia a severe precipitating stress, such as marital or emo- refers to memory loss for a particular incident or part tional discord [23], bereavement [49], financial prob- of an incident and can arise in a variety of circum- lems [23] or war [21,48].
    [Show full text]
  • What Is It Like to Be Confabulating?
    What is it like to be Confabulating? Sahba Besharati, Aikaterini Fotopoulou and Michael D. Kopelman Kings College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London UK Different kinds of confabulations may arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders. This chapter first offers conceptual distinctions between spontaneous and momentary (“provoked”) confabulations, as well as between these types of confabulation and other kinds of false memories. The chapter then reviews current explanatory theories, emphasizing that both neurocognitive and motivational factors account for the content of confabulations. We place particular emphasis on a general model of confabulation that considers cognitive dysfunctions in memory and executive functioning in parallel with social and emotional factors. It is argued that all these dimensions need to be taken into account for a phenomenologically rich description of confabulation. The role of the motivated content of confabulation and the subjective experience of the patient are particularly relevant in effective management and rehabilitation strategies. Finally, we discuss a case example in order to illustrate how seemingly meaningless false memories are actually meaningful if placed in the context of the patient’s own perspective and autobiographical memory. Key words: Confabulation; False memory; Motivation; Self; Rehabilitation. 1 Memory is often subject to errors of omission and commission such that recollection includes instances of forgetting, or distorting past experience. The study of pathological forms of exaggerated memory distortion has provided useful insights into the mechanisms of normal reconstructive remembering (Johnson, 1991; Kopelman, 1999; Schacter, Norman & Kotstall, 1998). An extreme form of pathological memory distortion is confabulation. Different variants of confabulation are found to arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • How the Brain and Memory Works 10
    Caring For A Loved One With Dementia 10 How the Brain and Memory Works Introduction The way our brain stores memories is a complex process across many areas of the brain. Luckily, memories are not all stored in one place. They are spread out across different brain regions, or lobes, and allow us to keep and recall memories even if one area of the brain is damaged. Although the brain’s process for storing memories is sometimes compared to a filing cabinet, the processes are extremely complex and still not fully understood. 2 Creating memories 3. Store information The human brain is made up of neurons. Neurons are nerve cells that talk to each other through a synapse- a connection This is the process of retaining the information in short term, or between cells that sends information. Neurons receive and more permanently in long term memory. An area of the brain carry information to the parts of the brain to process or store called the Hippocampus plays an important role in storing long information. The brain has approximately 100 billion nerve term memories. cells, give or take 15 billion. 4. Recall To create memories, the brain must accomplish the following processes: Memories that are frequently recalled become stronger than those accessed less frequently. The neurons linked to this 1. Encode information information create a neural pathway- a road to that memory. Think of it as walking along a path. The more frequently you This process allows something of interest to be stored in the walk on the same path, the more defined the trail becomes.
    [Show full text]
  • Primary and Secondary Prevention Interventions for Cognitive Decline
    2016 Primary and secondary prevention interventions for cognitive decline and dementia Overview of reviews Published by The Norwegian Institute of Public Health Section for evidence summaries in the Knowledge Centre Title Primary and secondary prevention interventions for cognitive decline and dementia Norwegian title Primær‐ og sekundærforebyggende tiltak for kognitiv svikt og demens Responsible Camilla Stoltenberg, direktør Authors Gerd M Flodgren, project leader, researcher, the Knowledge Centre Rigmor C Berg, Head of Unit, for Social Welfare Research at the Knowledge Centre ISBN 978‐82‐8082‐745‐6 Projectnumber 798 Type of publication Overview of reviews No of pages 69 (110 inklusiv vedlegg) Client Nasjonalforeningen for folkehelsen MeSH terms Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, cognition, cognitive impairment, cognitive disorders, memory complaints, primary prevention, secondary prevention Citation Flodgren GM, Berg RC. Primary and secondary prevention interventions for cognitive decline and dementia. [Primær‐ og sekundærforebyggende tiltak for kognitiv svikt og demens] Rapport −2016. Oslo: Folkehelseinstituttet, 2016. 2 Table of contents Table of contents TABLE OF CONTENTS 3 KEY MESSAGES 5 EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 6 Background 6 Objectives 6 Methods 6 Results 6 Discussion 8 Conclusions 8 HOVEDFUNN (NORSK) 9 SAMMENDRAG (NORSK) 10 Bakgrunn 10 Problemstillinger 10 Metoder 10 Resultat 10 Diskusjon 12 Konklusjon 12 PREFACE 13 OBJECTIVES 15 BACKGROUND 16 Description of the condition 16 How the interventions may work 18 Why is it important to do this
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of Phosphatidylserine Administration on Symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children S
    AGRO SET_OTT_06.qxp 27-10-2006 10:14 Pagina 16 Effect of phosphatidylserine administration on symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children S. HIRAYAMA1*,Y. MASUDA2,R. RABELER3 *Corresponding author 1. Department of Early Childhood Education and Care, Kurashiki City College, 160 Hieda, Kurashikishi, Okayama, Japan 2. Kojima first High School, Okayama, Japan 3. Cargill Food Ingredients GmbH, Freising, Germany PURPOSE the emotional response in the frontal lobe, due to a problem of disinhibition (1). Disinhibition consists of disinhibition of To clarify whether the administration of phosphatidylserine attention (inattention) and that of behaviour (hyperactivity ("PS") can improve the attention-deficit ("AD") and and impulsiveness). hyperactivity disorder ("HD") symptoms in children. with AD/HD patients are classified into inattention-predominant Infant nutrition AD/HD. type, hyperactivity and impulsiveness -dominant type and mixed type. Each symptom causes problems in learning and relation between family members. Though the cause of STUDY DESIGN AND SUBJECTS disorders has yet to be identified (2), central stimulants (a type of psycho stimulant) are used in the treatment. These A pilot study in 15 AD/HD children 6 to 12 years old (including drugs can alleviate the AD/HD symptoms to some extent (3, 6 suspected to have AD/HD) who had rarely received 4). However, there is no consensus on the long term use of medication before. These 15 children took 200 mg/day of PS these drugs and adverse events (adverse reactions) may in a capsule every day for 2 months. The following items were occur during or years after the treatment (5). Accordingly, investigated at the start of study ("pre-study") and upon supplementary and substitute medication is frequently completion of study ("post-study): 1) AD/HD symptoms advised.
    [Show full text]
  • The Importance of Sleep in Fear Conditioning and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
    Biological Psychiatry: Commentary CNNI The Importance of Sleep in Fear Conditioning and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Robert Stickgold and Dara S. Manoach Abnormal sleep is a prominent feature of Axis I neuropsychia- fear and distress are extinguished. Based on a compelling tric disorders and is often included in their DSM-5 diagnostic body of work from human and rodent studies, fear extinction criteria. While often viewed as secondary, because these reflects not the erasure of the fear memory but the develop- disorders may themselves diminish sleep quality, there is ment of a new safety or “extinction memory” that inhibits the growing evidence that sleep disorders can aggravate, trigger, fear memory and its associated emotional response. and even cause a range of neuropsychiatric conditions. In this issue, Straus et al. (3 ) report that total sleep Moreover, as has been shown in major depression and deprivation can impair the retention of such extinction mem- attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, treating sleep can ories. In their study, healthy human participants in three improve symptoms, suggesting that disrupted sleep contri- groups successfully learned to associate a blue circle (condi- butes to the clinical syndrome and is an appropriate target for tioned stimulus) with the occurrence of an electric shock treatment. In addition to its effects on symptoms, sleep (unconditioned stimulus) during a fear acquisition session. disturbance, which is known to impair emotional regulation The following day, during extinction learning, the blue circle and cognition in otherwise healthy individuals, may contribute was repeatedly presented without the shock. The day after to or cause disabling cognitive deficits. For sleep to be a target that, extinction recall was tested by again repeatedly present- for treatment of symptoms and cognitive deficits in neurop- ing the blue circle without the shock.
    [Show full text]
  • The Three Amnesias
    The Three Amnesias Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute University of Florida PO Box 100165 HSC Gainesville, FL 32610-0165 USA Bauer, R.M. (in press). The Three Amnesias. In J. Morgan and J.E. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press. The Three Amnesias - 2 During the past five decades, our understanding of memory and its disorders has increased dramatically. In 1950, very little was known about the localization of brain lesions causing amnesia. Despite a few clues in earlier literature, it came as a complete surprise in the early 1950’s that bilateral medial temporal resection caused amnesia. The importance of the thalamus in memory was hardly suspected until the 1970’s and the basal forebrain was an area virtually unknown to clinicians before the 1980’s. An animal model of the amnesic syndrome was not developed until the 1970’s. The famous case of Henry M. (H.M.), published by Scoville and Milner (1957), marked the beginning of what has been called the “golden age of memory”. Since that time, experimental analyses of amnesic patients, coupled with meticulous clinical description, pathological analysis, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging, has led to a clearer understanding of the nature and characteristics of the human amnesic syndrome. The amnesic syndrome does not affect all kinds of memory, and, conversely, memory disordered patients without full-blown amnesia (e.g., patients with frontal lesions) may have impairment in those cognitive processes that normally support remembering.
    [Show full text]
  • Disordered Recognition Memory: Recollective Confabulation
    cortex xxx (2013) 1e12 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Special issue: Research report Disordered recognition memory: Recollective confabulation Chris J.A. Moulin* Laboratoire d’Etude de l’Apprentissage et du De´veloppement, CNRS UMR 5022, Universite´ de Bourgogne, Dijon, France article info abstract Article history: Recollective confabulation (RC) is encountered as a conviction that a present moment is a Received 31 January 2012 repetition of one experienced previously, combined with the retrieval of confabulated Reviewed 12 April 2012 specifics to support that assertion. It is often described as persistent de´ja` vu by family Revised 24 September 2012 members and caregivers. On formal testing, patients with RC tend to produce a very high Accepted 24 January 2013 level of false positive errors. In this paper, a new case series of 11 people with dementia or Published online xxx mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and with de´ja` vu-like experiences is presented. In two experiments the nature of the recognition memory deficit is explored. The results from Keywords: these two experiments suggest e contrary to our hypothesis in earlier published case re- Dementia ports e that recollection mechanisms are relatively spared in this group, and that patients De´ja` vu experience familiarity for non-presented items. The RC patients tended to be overconfident Reduplicative paramnesia in their assessment of recognition memory, and produce inaccurate assessments of their Familiarity performance. These findings are discussed with reference to delusions more generally, and Metacognition point to a combined memory and metacognitive deficit, possibly arising from damage to temporal and right frontal regions.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Loss What Can You Do About
    Joyce Cheng RN Community Health Nurse November 16, 2016 Dementia- an umbrella term used to describe a set of symptoms, including impairment in memory, reasoning, judgment, language and other thinking skills Normal age-related memory loss doesn’t prevent you from living a full and productive life. These changes in memory are generally manageable and do not disrupt your ability to work, live independently or maintain a social life Alzheimer’s disease Vascular dementia (multi-infarct dementia) Frontotemporal dementia Lewy body dementia • Alzheimer's is the most common form of dementia, accounts for 60 to 80 percent of dementia cases. • Alzheimer's is not a normal part of aging, although the greatest known risk factor is increasing age, and the majority of people with Alzheimer's are 65 and older • Alzheimer's worsens over time. Alzheimer's is the sixth leading cause of death in the United States. • Alzheimer's has no current cure, but treatments for symptoms are available and research continues. Asking the same questions repeatedly Forgetting common words when speaking Mixing words up Taking longer to complete familiar tasks Misplacing items in inappropriate places Getting lost while walking or driving around a familiar neighborhood Undergoing sudden changes in mood or behavior for no apparent reason Becoming less able to follow directions Vascular cognitive impairment- Sleep deficiency- Medications Nutritional Deficiency Stress, Anxiety, and Depression Caused by reduced blood flow to the brain or blockage. Reduced blood flow lead to depriving of oxygen and essential nutrients. Hypertension High Cholesterol Stroke- forgetfulness may be an early warning sign of stroke Sleep Apnea- wake up with a headache, daytime fatigue, snoring.
    [Show full text]
  • Nutrition and Brain Aging: Role of Fatty Acids with an Epidemiological Perspective
    THESIS AWARD Nutrition and brain aging: role of fatty acids with an epidemiological perspective Cecilia SAMIERI Abstract: In the absence of identified etiologic treatment for dementia, the potential Pascale BARBERGER-GATEAU preventive role of nutrition may offer an interesting perspective. The objective of the thesis of C. Samieri was to study the association between nutrition and brain aging in Inserm, U897, 1,796 subjects, aged 65 y or older, from the Bordeaux sample of the Three-City study, Equipe Epidemiologie de la nutrition with a particular emphasis on fatty acids. Considering the multidimensional nature of et des comportements alimentaires, nutritional data, several complementary strategies were used. At the global diet level, Universite Bordeaux Segalen, dietary patterns actually observed in the population were identified by exploratory Case 11, methods. Older subjects with a ‘‘healthy’’ pattern, who consumed more than 3.5 weekly 146 rue Leo-Saignat, servings of fish in men and more than 6 daily servings of fruits and vegetables in women, F-33076 Bordeaux cedex, showed a better cognitive and psychological health. Adherence to the Mediterranean France diet, measured according to a score-based confirmatory method, was associated with <[email protected]. slower global cognitive decline after 5 y of follow-up. At the nutrient biomarker level, fr> higher plasma eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), a long-chain omega-3 fatty acid, was associated with a decreased dementia risk, and the omega-6-to-omega-3 fatty acids ratio to an increased risk, particularly in depressed subjects. EPA was also related to slower working memory decline in depressed subjects or in carriers of the e4 allele of the ApoE gene.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Dysfunction
    The new england journal of medicine review article current concepts Memory Dysfunction Andrew E. Budson, M.D., and Bruce H. Price, M.D. From the Geriatric Research Education emory function is vulnerable to a variety of pathologic Clinical Center, Edith Nourse Rogers Me- processes including neurodegenerative diseases, strokes, tumors, head trau- morial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, Mass., m ma, hypoxia, cardiac surgery, malnutrition, attention-deficit disorder, de- the Department of Neurology, Boston Uni- 1,2 versity, Boston, and the Department of pression, anxiety, the side effects of medication, and normal aging. As such, memory Neurology, Division of Cognitive and Be- impairment is commonly seen by physicians in multiple disciplines including neurol- havioral Neurology, Brigham and Wom- en’s Hospital, Boston (A.E.B.); and Har- ogy, psychiatry, medicine, and surgery. Memory loss is often the most disabling feature vard Medical School, Boston, and McLean of many disorders, impairing the normal daily activities of the patients and profoundly Hospital, Belmont, Mass. (B.H.P.). Address affecting their families. reprint requests to Dr. Budson at GRECC, Bldg. 62, Rm. B30, Edith Nourse Rogers Some perceptions about memory, such as the concepts of “short-term” and “long- Memorial Veterans Hospital, 200 Springs term,” have given way to a more refined understanding and improved classification Rd., Bedford, MA 01730, or at abudson@ systems. These changes result from neuropsychological studies of patients with focal partners.org. brain lesions, neuroanatomical studies in humans and animals, experiments in animals, N Engl J Med 2005;352:692-9. positron-emission tomography, functional magnetic resonance imaging, and event- Copyright © 2005 Massachusetts Medical Society.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Sleep Disruption a Cause Or Consequence of Alzheimer's
    International Journal of Molecular Sciences Review Is Sleep Disruption a Cause or Consequence of Alzheimer’s Disease? Reviewing Its Possible Role as a Biomarker Maria-Angeles Lloret 1, Ana Cervera-Ferri 2,* , Mariana Nepomuceno 3, Paloma Monllor 3 , Daniel Esteve 3 and Ana Lloret 3 1 Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, University Clinic Hospital of Valencia, Avda. Blasco Ibanez, 17, 46010 Valencia, Spain; [email protected] 2 Department of Human Anatomy and Embryology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Valencia, Avda. Blasco Ibanez, 15, 46010 Valencia, Spain 3 Department of Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Valencia, Health Research Institute INCLIVA, Avda. Blasco Ibanez, 15, 46010 Valencia, Spain; [email protected] (M.N.); [email protected] (P.M.); [email protected] (D.E.); [email protected] (A.L.) * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +34-9639-835-07 Received: 27 December 2019; Accepted: 8 February 2020; Published: 10 February 2020 Abstract: In recent years, the idea that sleep is critical for cognitive processing has gained strength. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia worldwide and presents a high prevalence of sleep disturbances. However, it is difficult to establish causal relations, since a vicious circle emerges between different aspects of the disease. Nowadays, we know that sleep is crucial to consolidate memory and to remove the excess of beta-amyloid and hyperphosphorilated tau accumulated in AD patients’ brains. In this review, we discuss how sleep disturbances often precede in years some pathological traits, as well as cognitive decline, in AD. We describe the relevance of sleep to memory consolidation, focusing on changes in sleep patterns in AD in contrast to normal aging.
    [Show full text]