Cognitive Ethology: a New Approach for Studying Human Cognition

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cognitive Ethology: a New Approach for Studying Human Cognition Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 317 The British Psychological British Journal of Psychology (2008), 99, 317–340 Society q 2008 The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Cognitive Ethology: A new approach for studying human cognition Alan Kingstone1*, Daniel Smilek2 and John D. Eastwood3 1Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, British Columbia, Canada 2Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada 3Department of Psychology, York University, Ontario, Canada We all share a desire to understand and predict human cognition and behaviour as it occurs within complex real-world situations. This target article seeks to open a dialogue with our colleagues regarding this common goal. We begin by identifying the principles of most lab-based investigations and conclude that adhering to them will fail to generate valid theories of human cognition and behaviour in natural settings. We then present an alternative set of principles within a novel research framework called ‘Cognitive Ethology’. We discuss how Cognitive Ethology can complement lab-based investigations, and we show how its levels of description and explanation are distinct from what is typically employed in lab-based research. The study of human cognition has been punctuated by three historical stages of advance (Van Kleeck & Kosslyn, 1991). The first stage, beginning in the late 1950s to early 1960s was marked by a rapid progression propelled by the methods of traditional psychophysics and experimental psychology. The second stage, beginning by the mid-1970s, was fuelled by computational analysis that signalled the arrival of cognitive science. The third phase, which began in the mid-1980s, incorporated evidence from neuropsychology and animal neurophysiology, and most recently an ever increasing array of techniques for scanning the brain of alert participants. In the present article, we take as our starting-point a critical problem that continues to bedevil the study of human cognition that arose precisely from the original and remarkably successful methods of experimental psychology. Those methods, which involved simplifying the issue of investigation by making the experimental context both impoverished and controlled, sought to discover causal relationships between one factor and another. The intention was that by minimizing the complexity of the environment and maximizing the experimental control, investigators could create theories that would be universally valid. However, by the mid-1970s it had become very * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Alan Kingstone, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada (e-mail: [email protected]). DOI:10.1348/000712607X251243 Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 318 Alan Kingstone et al. clear that most statements were true if, and only if, particular laboratory conditions were met. In other words, the relationship between factor A and factor B was predictable if, and only if, specific conditions were established within the lab; the relationship between factors became unpredictable when these laboratory situations were not met. Thus, for example, memory experiments found that what people remembered depended on factors such as (a) what processing they performed on the stimulus materials; (b) what stimulus materials they expected to receive; (c) what materials were actually presented; (d) what people were doing before their memory was measured; (e) how their memory was measured, and so on and so forth. The take home message was that cognitive processes vary and are affected by what is happening elsewhere within the cognitive system, and therefore cognitive processes depend critically on the specific situational context in which a subject is embedded. The field’s response to the above fact has generally taken one of the two forms. One reaction is to deny that there is a problem. This ‘response’ enables one to maintain the initial assumption that cognitive processes are invariant and unaffected by what is happening elsewhere, and thus allows one to continue to create and study laboratory- specific phenomenon like ‘nonword repetition memory’ or ‘inhibition of return’. The other reaction is to acknowledge that there is a problem, but then continue to conduct research predicated on the assumption that cognitive processes are invariant. Both responses are what Broadbent (1991) has called ‘pathological’. Occasionally, investigators like Donald Broadbent and Ulric Neisser have tried a third response. They acknowledged that cognitive processes change with situational changes and worked hard to bring the implications of this fact to the awareness of others. Perhaps their only mistake was to trust that the next generation of researchers would take their words to heart and try to find a solution to the issue. In hindsight, this faith has proven to be grossly misplaced, as the next generation of researchers have adopted one of the pathological responses of the past and grounded their neuroimaging investigations on the false assumption that cognitive processes are invariant across situations. It is precisely this false assumption that allows researchers to make the remarkable claim that the cognitive processes that they engage and measure in a simple, artificial brain neuroimaging situation captures the same fundamental cognitive processes and associated neural systems that are engaged in a complex natural situation. The aim of the present paper is modest but against this historical backdrop, we believe it is vital. We aim to initiate a dialogue among researchers regarding the fact that cognitive processes vary substantially with changes in context. We also hope to stimulate researchers to find a response to this issue that is not ‘pathological’. By putting forward a possible solution of our own, a novel research approach that we call ‘Cognitive Ethology’, our intention is to encourage other researchers to develop and advance their own positive responses. While what follows for the remainder of this paper focuses primarily on instances of cognition as it pertains to the investigation of human attention, we think that the issues we raise here can be readily extended to other research domains of human cognition. Laboratory research Laboratory research in the field of human cognition is founded on the critical assumption that human cognition is subserved by processes that are invariant and regular across situations. This invariance assumption enables one to conduct a study in Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Cognitive Ethology 319 the laboratory and then to propose that the process being measured is expressed in everyday life. Importantly, there is a second assumption that falls out of the first. Given that processes are assumed to be invariant across situations, it follows that one can reduce situational variability without compromising the nature of the process one is measuring. Indeed, a basic objective of the experimental environment in the laboratory is to gain as much control over a situation as is possible so that any change can be attributed to the variable that is being manipulated. Together, these assumptions provide a powerful one–two punch. The assumption of process stability enables the scientist to be concerned with real-life situations without ever having to leave the laboratory. In addition, the assumption of control drives the scientist increasingly away from complex real-life situations to paradigms that are simple, contrived, and artificial. These assumptions are not, however, without their risks. For instance, the assumption of invariance eliminates any need or even obligation for the scientist to confirm that the process being manipulated and measured in the laboratory actually expresses itself in the real world. Investigators do, of course, through the process of replication, check that their lab-based effects are regular within the laboratory environment. Unfortunately, a result that is invariant within the strict confines of the laboratory does not mean that it is reproducible outside the lab. Indeed, even a cursory examination of the literature reveals that there are many instances where even the most minor change within a laboratory situation will compromise the replicability of an effect (e.g. Atchley & Kramer, 2001; Berry & Klein, 1993; Bindemann, Burton, & Langton, 2008; Soto-Faraco, Morein-Zamir, & Kingstone, 2005; Wolfe & Pokorny, 1990). In addition, as any researcher knows all too well, failed replications that are published represent just the smallest tip of a very large iceberg of failed replications that are obtained in the laboratory and never published. Upon closer consideration, there is a good reason why lab-based effects should be so remarkably fragile. After all there is a large, well established, and growing body of literature indicating that process stability is tied intimately to the situation used to create it, with participants’ strategies and associated brain configurations changing from one situation to the next (see for instance Duncan & Owen, 2000 for a review). Neisser (1976) referred to these dynamic configurations as ‘schemata’, Monsell (1996) has spoken
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Psychology
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY PSYCH 126 Acknowledgements College of the Canyons would like to extend appreciation to the following people and organizations for allowing this textbook to be created: California Community Colleges Chancellor’s Office Chancellor Diane Van Hook Santa Clarita Community College District College of the Canyons Distance Learning Office In providing content for this textbook, the following professionals were invaluable: Mehgan Andrade, who was the major contributor and compiler of this work and Neil Walker, without whose help the book could not have been completed. Special Thank You to Trudi Radtke for editing, formatting, readability, and aesthetics. The contents of this textbook were developed under the Title V grant from the Department of Education (Award #P031S140092). However, those contents do not necessarily represent the policy of the Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. Unless otherwise noted, the content in this textbook is licensed under CC BY 4.0 Table of Contents Psychology .................................................................................................................................................... 1 126 ................................................................................................................................................................ 1 Chapter 1 - History of Cognitive Psychology ............................................................................................. 7 Definition of Cognitive Psychology
    [Show full text]
  • Tribute to Glyn W. Humphreys, 1954-2016
    cortex 107 (2018) 1e3 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Special issue: Editorial Tribute to Glyn W. Humphreys, 1954e2016 Martin Edwards a, Monika Harvey b and Julie Snowden c,d,* a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite Catholique de Louvain, Belgium b School of Psychology, University of Glasgow, UK c Cerebral Function Unit, Neuroscience Centre, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, UK d Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, UK article info (obituary Cortex 2016; 75: A1eA2) is a profound loss to cognitive neuroscience. Article history: It is fitting that a Special Issue dedicated to Glyn'smemoryshould Received 3 July 2018 be published in Cortex, whose stated remit is “the study of cognition Accepted 4 July 2018 and the relationship between the nervous system and mental pro- Published online 26 July 2018 cesses”. The description encapsulates Glyn's work. Glyn started his career at the University of Bristol in the 1970s. His PhD involved letter and word perceptual recognition, using state-of-the-art technology of the time, combined with clever priming experimental design (Humphreys, 1978). His scientific aptitude and creativity were early indicators of what was to come. In 1979, Glyn moved to Birbeck College, where he met his wife and scientific partner, Jane Riddoch. Together, they formed a team, located at the University of Bir- mingham in 1989 and at the University of Oxford in 2011, that changed the face of cognitive neuropsychology. Their meticulous studies of patients with brain disorders are a model par excellence of how neuropsychological investigation can inform understanding of brain function.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology and Life
    免费考研网www.freekaoyan.com Instructor’s Manual for Gerrig and Zimbardo Psychology and Life Sixteenth Edition prepared by John N. Boyd Allyn and Bacon Boston London Toronto Sydney Tokyo Singapore 免费考研网www.freekaoyan.com 免费考研网www.freekaoyan.com Copyright © 2002 by Allyn & Bacon A Pearson Education Company 75 Arlington Street Boston, Massachusetts 02116 Internet: www.ablongman.com All rights reserved. The contents, or parts thereof, may be reproduced for use with Psychology and Life, Sixteenth Edition, by Richard Gerrig and Philip Zimbardo, provided such reproductions bear copyright notice, but may not be reproduced in any form for any other purpose without written permission from the copyright owner. ISBN 0-205-34454-2 Printed in the United States of America 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 05 04 03 02 免费考研网www.freekaoyan.com 免费考研网www.freekaoyan.com PSYCHOLOGY AND LIFE, 16th Edition Instructor’s Manual Please note that the transcription to PDF can result in unintended reformatting. Page numbers in the table of contents may not correspond to the book's interior. The publisher regrets any inconvenience caused by this error.) TABLE OF CONTENTS PREFACE Open-Book Testing i Teaching the Introductory Course ii Why Read This Instructor’s Manual? xxv CHAPTERS Chapter 1: The Science of Psychology in Your Life 1 Chapter 2: Research Methods in Psychology 19 Chapter 3: The Biological Bases of Behavior 33 Chapter 4: Sensation 51 Chapter 5: Perception 71 Chapter 6: Mind, Consciousness, and Alternate States 85 Chapter 7: Learning and Behavior Analysis 105 Chapter 8:
    [Show full text]
  • Broadbent's Filter Theory Cherry: the Cocktail Party Problem
    194 Part II ● Cognitive psychology KEY STUDY EVALUATION — Cherry Cherry: The cocktail party problem The research by Colin Cherry is a very good example of how a psycholo- gist, noticing a real-life situation, is able to devise a hypothesis and carry Cherry (1953) found that we use physical differences between out research in order to explain a phenomenon, in this case the “cocktail the various auditory messages to select the one of interest. party” effect. Cherry tested his ideas in a laboratory using a shadowing These physical differences include differences in the sex of the technique and found that participants were really only able to give infor- speaker, in voice intensity, and in the location of the speaker. mation about the physical qualities of the non-attended message When Cherry presented two messages in the same voice to (whether the message was read by a male or a female, or if a tone was both ears at once (thereby removing these physical differences), used instead of speech). Cherry’s research could be criticised for having moved the real-life phenomenon into an artificial laboratory setting. the participants found it very hard to separate out the two However, this work opened avenues for other researchers, beginning with messages purely on the basis of meaning. Broadbent, to elaborate theories about focused auditory attention. Cherry (1953) also carried out studies using a shadowing task, in which one auditory message had to be shadowed (repeated back out aloud) while a second auditory message was presented to the other ear. Very little information seemed to be obtained from the second or non-attended message.
    [Show full text]
  • Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention 1 Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention
    Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention 1 Applied History of Psychology/History of Research on Attention There has been a large increase in research activity in the area of attention since the 1950s. This research has focused not only on attention, but also how attention is related to memory and executive functioning. Human learning and behaviour are dependent on our ability to pay attention to our environment, retain and retrieve information, and use cognitive strategies. An understanding of the development of attention is also critical when we consider that deficits in attention often lead to difficulties in school and in the work force. Thus, attention is an important topic in the study of psychology, specifically in the areas of development (see Part II of this book), learning (Part III), and psychological disorders (see the section on ADHD in Part IV). There is no doubt that an understanding of attention and related concepts is critical to our understanding of human cognition and learning. Introduction to The History of Research on Attention The study of attention is a major part of contemporary cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Attention plays a critical role in essentially all aspects of perception, cognition, and action, influencing the choices we make. The study of attention has been of interest to the field of psychology since its earliest days. However, many ideas about attention can be traced to philosophers in the 18th and 19th centuries, preceding the foundation of the field of psychology. The topic of attention was originally discussed by philosophers. Among the issues considered were the role of attention on conscious awareness and thought, and whether attention was directed voluntarily or involuntarily toward objects or events.
    [Show full text]
  • Donald E. Broadbent (1926-1993)
    Donald E. Broadbent (1926-1993) Donald Broadbent's great contribution to psychology was ceded by a selection device and a temporary storage buffer. his elaboration of the insight that human behavior can be This was a radical departure from the S-R approach of understood in terms of information flowing through the behaviorism; among other things the information-process­ organism. His most important book, Perception and Com­ ing approach embodied the idea that behavior was influ­ munication (1958), was the first systematic treatment of enced as much by the possible stimuli that might have been the human organism as an information-processing system, present as by the actual stimuli presented to the senses. and in it he proposed a structure of cognition that was The publication of Perception and Communication was specific enough to inspire a program of experimental re­ clearly one major foundation stone of the cognitive revolu­ search the influence of which may still be felt. tion. There were other important influences too, but Born on May 6, 1926, in Birmingham, England, Broadbent's book differed in that it laid out a testable model Donald grew up in Wales and attended Winchester Col­ and explicitly encouraged experimental challenges to as­ lege, an English public school. He entered the Royal Air pects of the model. The challenges were taken up, of course, Force in 1944 and, as his interests were in the natural and it is to Broadbent's great credit that when Gray and sciences, elected to take a short course in engineering at Wedderburn showed in 1960 that attention switched from Pembroke College, University of Cambridge.
    [Show full text]
  • Problem Solving Skill Pdf
    Problem solving skill pdf Continue Definition of quality glossary: Troubleshooting is the act of defining a problem. Determining the cause of the problem; Identify, prioritize, and select alternatives to your solution. Implement the solution. Troubleshooting Process Troubleshooting Resource Troubleshooting Chart Troubleshooting Process esplanaq should guide employees and develop troubleshooting techniques to effectively manage and execute a successful organization. You can follow the basic four-step troubleshooting process and methodology described below to find the right solution for your problem. Step Characteristics 1. Cause Information status of disagreements By identifying specific issues related to each faction, determine which process is wrong so that you do not try to resolve the problem without data 2. Alternative solutions create alternative solutions that initially involve all relevant individuals involved in generating alternatives and seek alternatives that can solve brainstorming problems with the idea of others specifying alternatives that match the goals of the organization 3. Evaluate alternative assessment alternatives compared to target standards to explicitly evaluate the results and possible results of alternative assessment evaluation alternatives to the established goals. Implement a pilot test of alternative collection feedback selected from all affected parties seeking acceptance or agreement from all affected parties to implement follow-up measures for the solution plan and to evaluate and monitor long-term results on the basis of final solution 1. Define the problem to diagnose the situation so that the problem occurs as well as the symptoms. Useful troubleshooting techniques include a diagram of the causes and effects of using flowcharts to identify the expected steps in the process and to define and analyze the root cause.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Felt Thought: Neuroscience, Modernism and the Intelligence of Poetry Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4948m8v9 Author Langione, Matthew Paul Publication Date 2016 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Felt Thought: Neuroscience, Modernism and the Intelligence of Poetry by Matthew Paul Langione A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in English and the Designated Emphases in Critical Theory and in Science and Technology Studies in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Charles Altieri, Chair Professor Mitchell Breitwieser Professor Dorothy J. Hale Professor David A. Hollinger Professor John R. Searle Spring 2016 To Aynslie, M & D, Barry O’C, Jaime and the brothers G ~i~ TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction…………………………………………………………………………………..1 Poetic Intelligence in the Age of Intelligent Machines Chapter I…………………………………………………………………………………….55 A Geometry of One’s Own Chapter II……………………………………………………………………………………97 Probability, Pragmatism and the Problem of Personality Chapter III…………………………………………………………………………………136 The Science of Sensibility Chapter IV…………………………………………………………………………………186 Prufrock and the Poetics of Observation Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………………242 Intelligent Examples Epilogue……………………………………………………………………………………248 Minding the Gap: The Value of Neuroscience to Literary Criticism Bibliography……………………………………………………………………………....289 ~ii~ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A dissertation is a life’s work even if it springs into being in the white heat of a few months’ labor. My first debts, therefore, are to my parents, Paul and Joanne Langione, who put me in a position to dream of such a thing, to James Kloppenberg who gave me faith that I could do it, and to Barry O’Connell who never allowed me to forget how much it mattered if I did.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Psychology
    Cognitive PSYCHoLogY 00_Eysenck Groome_Prelims.indd 1 19-Mar-15 1:44:31 PM Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies Series Editors: S. Alexander Haslam, Alan M. Slater and Joanne R. Smith School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter, EX4 4QG sychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies is a new series of texts aimed at stu- P dents and general readers who are interested in understanding issues raised by key studies in psychology. Volumes centre on 12–15 studies, with each chapter providing a detailed account of a particular classic study and its empirical and theoretical impact. Chapters also discuss the important ways in which thinking and research has advanced in the years since the study was conducted. Chapters are written by researchers at the cutting edge of these developments and, as a result, these texts serve as an excellent resource for instructors and students look- ing to explore different perspectives on core material that defines the field of psychology as we know it today. Also available: Social Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies Joanne R. Smith and S. Alexander Haslam Developmental Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies Alan M. Slater and Paul C. Quinn 00_Eysenck Groome_Prelims.indd 2 17-Mar-15 12:36:02 PM 00_Eysenck Groome_Prelims.indd 3 19-Mar-15 1:44:31 PM SAGE Publications Ltd Editorial arrangement and Chapters 1 and 9 Michael W. Eysenck 1 Oliver’s Yard and David Groome 2015 55 City Road Chapter 2 Michael W. Eysenck 2015 London EC1Y 1SP Chapter 3 Vicki Bruce and Yoav Tadmor 2015 Chapter 4 George Mather 2015 SAGE Publications Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Attention (Idea)
    Attention (idea) http://everything2.com/user/fuzzy+and+blue/writeups/Attention?displayty... Attention (idea) (idea) by fuzzy and blue I like it! Fri May 30 2003 at 20:38:58 2 C!s 11 ¶, 70 s, 1377 w, 9611 c Node Your Homework presents: a paper I wrote with a classmate for a cognitive psychology class, posted here in slightly modified form, with my co-author's permission. Theories of Attention 9 October 1998 (updated 30 May 2003) by fuzzy and blue and an anonymous classmate at Harvey Mudd College Attention has been of interest to psychologists ever since William James first wrote about it in his Principles of Psychology in 1890. However, attention is a complicated phenomenon, one that is difficult to define and discuss, much less observe. Because early 20th century psychology was dominated by behaviorist theory, which emphasized observable facts, it took more than 60 years for the first modern theory of attention to develop. E. C. Cherry reawakened interest in theories of attention when he published the results of his investigation of what he called "the cocktail party problem" in 1953 (Hampson & Morris 112). Subsequent theories of attention have since expanded and refined Cherry's ideas to considerably higher levels of sophistication, although the problem of attention is far from a closed one. In this paper, we will be examining the evolution of theories of attention, starting with the results of Cherry's "dichotic listening" experiments and continuing on to modern cognitive theories of divided attention and attentional resources. Cherry's 1953 article "Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears" presented the results of several experiments using the "dichotic presentation" technique, in which subjects listening to two different messages at once (one in each ear, through headphones) were able to "shadow" one message, repeating it as they listened, but retain little to no information about the non-attended message (Komatsu 34).
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Working memory Citation for published version: Logie, R 2015, Working memory: Beyond Baddeley and Hitch. in MW Eysenck & D Groome (eds), Cognitive Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies. SAGE, London, pp. 86-104. Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Cognitive Psychology General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 01. Oct. 2021 1 Working Memory: Beyond Baddeley and Hitch Final submitted copy - There may be minor changes in the final publication. Chapter published in: Eysenck, M.W. & Groome, D. (eds.). Cognitive Psychology: Revisiting the Classic Studies, pp 86-104. London: Sage. 2015 Commentary on Cognitive Classic Paper: Baddeley, A.D. and Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working Memory. In G. Bower (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. VIII, 47-90, New York: Academic Press. Robert H Logie University of Edinburgh, UK Background Crucial for every day functioning is the human ability to retain information on a temporary basis and to keep track of what we are doing moment to moment, allowing completion of a current task or to function in a novel environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Future Skills in a Digital World Skills of the Intelligence Age Contents
    Future Skills in a Digital World Skills of the Intelligence Age Contents 1 Critical thinking 1 1.1 History ................................................. 1 1.2 Etymology ............................................... 1 1.3 Definitions ............................................... 1 1.4 Logic and rationality .......................................... 2 1.4.1 Inductive versus deductive thinking .............................. 2 1.4.2 Critical thinking and rationality ................................ 2 1.5 Functions ............................................... 2 1.6 Procedure ............................................... 2 1.7 Habits or traits of mind ........................................ 3 1.8 Research ................................................ 3 1.9 Education ............................................... 3 1.9.1 Efficacy ............................................ 4 1.10 Importance in academia ........................................ 4 1.11 See also ................................................ 5 1.12 References ............................................... 5 1.13 Further reading ............................................ 6 1.14 External links ............................................. 7 2 Intellectual honesty 8 2.1 See also ................................................ 8 2.2 References ............................................... 8 2.3 Further reading ............................................ 8 3 Collaboration 9 3.1 Classical examples of collaboration .................................. 9 3.1.1 Trade
    [Show full text]