**********************S**************************** VOLIAM Twgxty
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
The Position of Enggano Within Austronesian
7KH3RVLWLRQRI(QJJDQRZLWKLQ$XVWURQHVLDQ 2ZHQ(GZDUGV Oceanic Linguistics, Volume 54, Number 1, June 2015, pp. 54-109 (Article) 3XEOLVKHGE\8QLYHUVLW\RI+DZDL L3UHVV For additional information about this article http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/ol/summary/v054/54.1.edwards.html Access provided by Australian National University (24 Jul 2015 10:27 GMT) The Position of Enggano within Austronesian Owen Edwards AUSTRALIAN NATIONAL UNIVERSITY Questions have been raised about the precise genetic affiliation of the Enggano language of the Barrier Islands, Sumatra. Such questions have been largely based on Enggano’s lexicon, which shows little trace of an Austronesian heritage. In this paper, I examine a wider range of evidence and show that Enggano is clearly an Austronesian language of the Malayo-Polynesian (MP) subgroup. This is achieved through the establishment of regular sound correspondences between Enggano and Proto‒Malayo-Polynesian reconstructions in both the bound morphology and lexicon. I conclude by examining the possible relations of Enggano within MP and show that there is no good evidence of innovations shared between Enggano and any other MP language or subgroup. In the absence of such shared innovations, Enggano should be considered one of several primary branches of MP. 1. INTRODUCTION.1 Enggano is an Austronesian language spoken on the southernmost of the Barrier Islands off the west coast of the island of Sumatra in Indo- nesia; its location is marked by an arrow on map 1. The genetic position of Enggano has remained controversial and unresolved to this day. Two proposals regarding the genetic classification of Enggano have been made: 1. -
Workpapers in Indonesian Languages and Cultures
( J WORKPAPERS IN INDONESIAN LANGUAGES AND CULTURES VOLUME 6 - MALUKU ,. PATTIMURA UNIVERSITY and THE SUMMER INSTITUTE OP LINGUISTICS in cooperation with THE DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION AND CULTURE WORKPAPERS IN INDONESIAN LANGUAGES AND CULTURES VOLUME 6 - MALUKU Nyn D. Laidig, Edi tor PAT'I'IMORA tJlflVERSITY and THE SUMMER IRSTlTUTK OP LIRGOISTICS in cooperation with 'l'BB DBPAR".l'MElI'1' 01' BDUCATIOII ARD CULTURE Workpapers in Indonesian Languages and cultures Volume 6 Maluku Wyn D. Laidig, Editor Printed 1989 Ambon, Maluku, Indonesia Copies of this publication may be obtained from Summer Institute of Linguistics Kotak Pos 51 Ambon, Maluku 97001 Indonesia Microfiche copies of this and other publications of the Summer Institute of Linguistics may be obtained from Academic Book Center Summer Institute of Linguistics 7500 West Camp Wisdom Road l Dallas, TX 75236 U.S.A. ii PRAKATA Dengan mengucap syukur kepada Tuhan yang Masa Esa, kami menyambut dengan gembira penerbitan buku Workpapers in Indonesian Languages , and Cultures. Penerbitan ini menunjukkan adanya suatu kerjasama yang baik antara Universitas Pattimura deng~n Summer Institute of Linguistics; Maluku . Buku ini merupakan wujud nyata peran serta para anggota SIL dalam membantu masyarakat umumnya dan masyarakat pedesaan khususnya Diharapkan dengan terbitnya buku ini akan dapat membantu masyarakat khususnya di pedesaan, dalam meningkatkan pengetahuan dan prestasi mereka sesuai dengan bidang mereka masing-masing. Dengan adanya penerbitan ini, kiranya dapat merangsang munculnya penulis-penulis yang lain yang dapat menyumbangkan pengetahuannya yang berguna bagi kita dan generasi-generasi yang akan datang. Kami ucapkan ' terima kasih kepada para anggota SIL yang telah berupaya sehingga bisa diterbitkannya buku ini Akhir kat a kami ucapkan selamat membaca kepada masyarakat yang mau memiliki buku ini. -
Materials for a Rejang-Indonesian-English Dictionary
PACIFIC LING U1STICS Series D - No. 58 MATERIALS FOR A REJANG - INDONESIAN - ENGLISH DICTIONARY collected by M.A. Jaspan With a fragmentary sketch of the . Rejang language by W. Aichele, and a preface and additional annotations by P. Voorhoeve (MATERIALS IN LANGUAGES OF INDONESIA, No. 27) W.A.L. Stokhof, Series Editor Department of Linguistics Research School of Pacific Studies THE AUSTRALIAN NATIONAL UNIVERSITY Jaspan, M.A. editor. Materials for a Rejang-Indonesian-English dictionary. D-58, x + 172 pages. Pacific Linguistics, The Australian National University, 1984. DOI:10.15144/PL-D58.cover ©1984 Pacific Linguistics and/or the author(s). Online edition licensed 2015 CC BY-SA 4.0, with permission of PL. A sealang.net/CRCL initiative. PACIFIC LINGUISTICS is issued through the Linguistic Circle of Canberra and consists of four series: SERIES A - Occasional Papers SERIES B - Monographs SERIES C - Books SERIES D - Special Publications EDITOR: S.A. Wurm ASSOCIATE EDITORS: D.C. Laycock, C.L. Voorhoeve, D.T. Tryon, T.E. Dutton EDITORIAL ADVISERS: B.W. Bender K.A. McElhanon University of Hawaii University of Texas David Bradley H.P. McKaughan La Trobe University University of Hawaii A. Capell P. MUhlhiiusler University of Sydney Linacre College, Oxford Michael G. Clyne G.N. O'Grady Monash University University of Victoria, B.C. S.H. Elbert A.K. Pawley University of Hawaii University of Auckland K.J. Franklin K.L. Pike University of Michigan; Summer Institute of Linguistics Summer Institute of Linguistics W.W. Glover E.C. Polome Summer Institute of Linguistics University of Texas G.W. Grace Malcolm Ross University of Hawaii University of Papua New Guinea M.A.K. -
A Tutorial on Lexical Classes
A tutorial on lexical classes Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero University of Manchester INTRODUCTION Goals of the tutorial §1 Our brief: L to address the key issues arising from exponence phenomena in which the lexicon is divided into arbitrary subsets, notably (a) inflectional classes and (b) cophonologies. §2 Our focus: L to explore the advantages and drawbacks of theories that model such phenomena by means of diacritic features. Overview §3 The discussion will touch upon the following questions: • Architectural issues How must the grammar be organized in order to guarantee the syntactic inertness of inflectional class features? Can syntactic features like gender be sensitive to inflectional class membership, and can inflectional class membership be determined by phonological properties? • Perspectives on inflectional class features (1): denial To what extent can inflectional class features be eliminated by (a) storing complex items in the lexicon and (b) invoking replacive mechanisms of exponence (‘overwriting’)? • Perspectives on inflectional class features (2): strengthening Instead of eliminating inflectional class features, do we rather need a more elaborate theory (e.g. one based on feature decomposition) capable of accounting for a wider range of phenomena (e.g. cross-class syncretism)? • Class features in phonology: cophonologies Can patterns of overlap between cophonologies be captured through the decomposition of cophonology diacritics, just like syncretisms between inflectional classes? Can derived environment effects in phonology be analysed in terms of the percolation of cophonology diacritics within morphological structures? http://www.bermudez-otero.com/WOTM4.pdf Workshop on Theoretical Morphology 4, Großbothen, 20 June 2008 2 ARCHITECTURAL ISSUES Basic properties of inflectional classes §4 Inflectional classes are traditionally defined by two properties: • arbitrariness and • syntactic inertness. -
CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION This Chapter Presents Background of The
CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION This chapter presents background of the research, statement of problem, research objective, research significance, and definition of key terms. 1.1 Background Morphology is a branch of linguistics which deals with the study of a word and its internal structure which affects the change of meaning. Moreover, Geert (2005: 7) states that morphology term refers to a study of the structure of the word, systematic word, the formation of word that has a relationship with its meaning. Morphology also covers how users of a particular language understand complex words, and find lexical items. Thus, Morphological studies in this research are used to analyze the word and how the word is formed that the word has a form and a meaning. Morphological process is the formation of a word in the form and meaning according to need in a single act of substitutions. Moreover, morphological process is a process of word formation by connecting one morpheme to other morphemes. The definition of morphological process according to Chaer (2012: 25) is the formation of a word from a base form of words through affixation and reduplication process. The formation of words will undergo another one of two processes, inflection and derivation. Derivation in particular is a process of forming a word that produces new lexeme. According to Ba’dulu and Herman (2006:12) 1 2 morphological derivation takes one word and turns it into another word. In other words, derivation is creating a new lexical word or creating a word from another syntactic category. Derivation is uses lexical meaning to make (or derive) new words and to change a word from grammatical class to another. -
Chapter One Introduction
CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION 0 Chapter One Introduction 1.0 Background :- In spite of the fact that English morphology studies have been incepted lately in linguistics; these studies have acquired an overwhelming importance in the field of English language learning and acquisition. According to Leiber (2009) ‘morphology is the study of word formation, including the ways new words are coined in the languages of the world and the way forms of words are varied depending on how they are used in sentence’. Thus morphology studies the formation of words in English language and how this formation is largely dependent on sentence structure in which the word is deployed. Forming words in English language takes two main ways; deriving new lexemes from other words by adding a prefix or a suffix, e.g. “happy”, “unhappy” and “happiness”. This process happens to be called derivational morphology which is concerned with creating a new word though a related one. The other way is forming words using inflectional processes, which result in a modified version of a word (Delahunty & Garvey: 76). These processes are often applied to words in sentences in order to meet the grammatical needs of the sentences such as tense, gender or number. For instance the variant forms of the verb ‘to walk’ ‘walk’ and ‘walks’ are used for the present simple whereas “walked” is used for the past simple. 1 As stated above, words may be constructed of more than one morpheme, a morpheme is the smallest form (i.e, spoken or written units) that has meaning or grammatical function (Delahunty & Garvey:2010 ; p 76). -
Between Inflection and Derivation Paradigmatic Lexical Functions in Morphological Databases
Between Inflection and Derivation Paradigmatic Lexical Functions in Morphological Databases Maarten Janssen ILTEC, Lisboa, Portugal [email protected] Abstract This article shows the use of lexical functions in the MorDebe lexical database system. Lexical functions are used in MorDebe to bridge the gap between derivation and inflection – to model relations between word-forms that are derivational from one perspective but inflectional from another. However, lexical functions in MorDebe range over lemmas rather than word-meanings and are hence significantly different from the normal lexical function. Абст‡кт В этой ст‡тье обсужд‡етсfl использние лексических функций ‚ лексической б‡зе д‡нных MorDebe. Лексические функции используютсfl ‚ MorDebe длfl того, чтобы сок‡тить ‡зы‚ между сло‚ооб‡знием и сло‚оизменением – длfl моделиниfl отношений между сло‚офом‡ми, котоые можно ‡ссм‡т肇ть с одной точки зениfl к‡к де肇ционные, ‡ с дугой - к‡к сло‚оизменительные. Одн‡ко, лексические функции ‚ MorDebe опеделflютсfl скоее длfl лексем, т.е. слных ‚ходо‚, ‡ не длfl смысло‚ых единиц и, следтельно, отлич‡ютсfl от лексических функций ‚ обычном поним‡нии. 1. Introduction Despite its central role in linguistics, the distinction between inflection and derivation is far from clear-cut. Modern day morphological theories often posit in-between cases - behaving partly like inflections, but partly like derivations. However, the introduction of marginal cases is problematic from the perspective of morphological database: an actual database which lists inflections and/or derivations, either has to treat them on a par, or model any individual form either as derivational or inflectional. This article will show how this problem manifests itself in the MorDebe morphological database, and how lexical functions are used to (partially) overcome the problem. -
1 on Agent Nominalizations and Why They Are Not Like Event
On agent nominalizations and why they are not like event nominalizations1 Mark C. Baker and Nadya Vinokurova Rutgers University and Research Institute of Humanities -Yakutsk Abstract: This paper focuses on agent-denoting nominalizations in various languages (e.g. the finder of the wallet), contrasting them with the much better studied action/event- denoting nominalizations. In particular, we show that in Sakha, Mapudungun, and English, agent-denoting nominalizations have none of the verbal features that event- denoting nominalizations sometimes have: they cannot contain adverbs, voice markers, expressions of aspect or mood, or verbal negation. An apparent exception to this generalization is that Sakha allows accusative-case marked objects in agentive nominalizations. We show that in fact the structure of agentive nominalizations in Sakha is as purely nominal as in other languages, and the difference is attributable to the rule of accusative case assignment. We explain these restrictions by arguing that agentive nominalizers have a semantics very much like the one proposed by Kratzer (1996) for Voice heads. Given this, the natural order of semantic composition implies that agentive nominalizers must combine directly with VP, just as Voice heads must. As a preliminary to testing this idea typologically, we show how a true agentive nominalization can be distinguished from a headless subject relative clause, illustrating with data from Mapudungun. We then present the results of a 34-language survey, showing that indeed none of these languages allow clause-like syntax inside a true agentive nominalization. We conclude that a generative-style investigation into the details of particular languages can be a productive source of things to look for in typological surveys. -
Obtaining a Better Understanding of Distributional Models of German Derivational Morphology
Obtaining a Better Understanding of Distributional Models of German Derivational Morphology Max Kisselew∗ Sebastian Pado´∗ Alexis Palmer∗ Jan Snajderˇ † ∗Institut fur¨ maschinelle Sprachverarbeitung, Stuttgart University, Germany †Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, University of Zagreb, Croatia kisselmx,pado,palmeras @ims.uni-stuttgart.de [email protected] { } Abstract Predicting the (distributional) meaning of derivationally related words (read / read+er) from one another has recently been recognized as an instance of distributional compositional meaning construc- tion. However, the properties of this task are not yet well understood. In this paper, we present an analysis of two such composition models on a set of German derivation patterns (e.g., -in, durch-). We begin by introducing a rank-based evaluation metric, which reveals the task to be challenging due to specific properties of German (compounding, capitalization). We also find that performance varies greatly between patterns and even among base-derived term pairs of the same pattern. A regression analysis shows that semantic coherence of the base and derived terms within a pattern, as well as coherence of the semantic shifts from base to derived terms, all significantly impact prediction quality. 1 Introduction Derivation is a major morphological process of word formation (e.g., read read+er), which is typically → associated with a fairly specific semantic shift (+er: agentivization). It may therefore be surprising that the semantics of derivation is a relatively understudied phenomenon in distributional semantics. Recently, Lazaridou et al. (2013) proposed to consider the semantics of a derived term like read+er as the result of a compositional process that combines the meanings of the base term read and the affix +er. -
Morphological Representation and Semantic Interpretation
Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 11, 1983, 01-18 doi: 10.5774/11-0-104 MORPHOLOGICAL REPRESENTATION AND SEMANTIC INTERPRETATION Rudolf P. Botha Introduction TI1is paper deals with the tenability of the following general linguistic condition on morphological representation: (1) TIle Compositionality Condition The morphological representation assigned to a complex word must provide the formal structure required by an optimal specification of the semantic interpretation of the word. This condition entails, on the one hand, that if the semantic interpreta tion (or meaning) of a complex word can be specified as a simple function of the meanings of its constituents, these constituents must be bracketed and labelled in such a way as to make such a specification possible. On the other hand, the condition disallows a bracketing and/or labelling which, for this specification, requires objectionable devices. A device is objectionable if it (a) has unacceptable empirical consequences either in the grammar or in the general linguistic theory, (b) is ad hoc in a specific sense, (c) represents or creates a conceptual redundancy within the total grammar or general linguistic theory, or (d) is insuf ficiently constrained in regard to descriptive power. The use of devices with one or more of these properties makes a specification of the semantic interpretation of a word non-optimal. And morphological representations necessitating the use of such devices are suspect. Condi tion (1)' clearly attaches more value to morphological representations which maximize com positionality in the specification of the meaning of complex words, hence the name "Compositionality Condition". Of course, the condition cannot be enforced in the case of complex words which have idiosyncratic elements of meaning that cannot be predicted on the basis of the meanings of their . -
East Nusantara: Typological and Areal Analyses Pacific Linguistics 618
East Nusantara: typological and areal analyses Pacific Linguistics 618 Pacific Linguistics is a publisher specialising in grammars and linguistic descriptions, dictionaries and other materials on languages of the Pacific, Taiwan, the Philippines, Indonesia, East Timor, southeast and south Asia, and Australia. Pacific Linguistics, established in 1963 through an initial grant from the Hunter Douglas Fund, is associated with the Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies at The Australian National University. The authors and editors of Pacific Linguistics publications are drawn from a wide range of institutions around the world. Publications are refereed by scholars with relevant expertise, who are usually not members of the editorial board. FOUNDING EDITOR: Stephen A. Wurm EDITORIAL BOARD: John Bowden and I Wayan Arka (Managing Editors), Mark Donohue, Nicholas Evans, David Nash, Andrew Pawley, Malcolm Ross, Paul Sidwell, Jane Simpson, and Darrell Tryon EDITORIAL ADVISORY BOARD: Karen Adams, Arizona State University Marian Klamer, Universiteit Leiden Alexander Adelaar, University of Melbourne Harold Koch, The Australian National Peter Austin, School of Oriental and African University Studies Frantisek Lichtenberk, University of Byron Bender, University of Hawai‘i Auckland Walter Bisang, Johannes Gutenberg- John Lynch, University of the South Pacific Universität Mainz Patrick McConvell, Australian Institute of Robert Blust, University of Hawai‘i Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander David Bradley, La Trobe University Studies Lyle Campbell, University of Utah William McGregor, Aarhus Universitet James Collins, Universiti Kebangsaan Ulrike Mosel, Christian-Albrechts- Malaysia Universität zu Kiel Bernard Comrie, Max Planck Institute for Claire Moyse-Faurie, Centre National de la Evolutionary Anthropology Recherche Scientifique Soenjono Dardjowidjojo, Universitas Atma Bernd Nothofer, Johann Wolfgang Goethe- Jaya Universität Frankfurt am Main Matthew Dryer, State University of New York Bambang Kaswanti Purwo, Universitas Atma at Buffalo Jaya Jerold A. -
Linguistics 1A Morphology 3 Compounding and Derivation
Linguistics 1A Morphology 3 Compounding and derivation In the previous lecture we saw that it is possible to combine two free morphemes to make one complex word, a compound. In principle, the two morphemes can be of any lexical category. Indeed, many different combinations of lexical categories can be found in English compounds: (1) a. N + N apron string, kitchen towel, bathroom, police car b. A + N high school, smallpox, dimwit, greenbelt c. P + N overdose, underdog, afterthought, uptown d. V + N swearword, rattlesnake, whetstone, scrubwoman (2) a. N + A headstrong, honey-sweet, skin-deep, nationwide b. A + A red-hot, worldy-wise, widespread c. P + A overabundant, underripe, underprivileged d. V + A --- (3) a. N + V colour-code, breast-feed, chain-smoke, pan-fry b. A + V sharpshoot, dry-clean, whitewash c. P + V outperform, underfeed, overreact d. V + V stirfry Some of the combinations are more productive than others, meaning that it is easier to coin new compounds of that type. The N+N type, for example, is completely productive: any combination of two nouns is a possible compound of English. At the other extreme, V+A combinations do not seem to occur at all. This is an arbitrary gap in the English lexicon: a look at other languages quickly reveals that there is nothing intrinsically wrong with a V+A compound, as the Dutch examples in (4) show. (4) fonkel V-nieuw A druip V-nat A shine new drip wet ‘brand new’ ‘soaking wet’ The resulting meaning of the combination of two morphemes is largely unpredictable.