For Personal Use Only

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

For Personal Use Only Corpus evidence for prototype-driven alternations: the case of German weak nouns (Draft of August 20, 2014) Roland Schäfer <[email protected]>, Freie Universität Berlin1 In this paper, I present a large study of so-called “weak” nouns in German based on the 9.1 billion token DECOW2012 web corpus. The weak nouns form a small class of masculine nouns with prototypical semantic and phonotactic features as well as case and number affixes which are unusual within the German system of noun inflection. These weak nouns have a certain tendency to be assimilated to the dominant “strong” inflectional pattern. I quantify the strength of this tendency and model it as an effect of the presence or absence of the prototypical features in the nouns as well as paradig- matic (case) features and token frequency. While staying neutral with respect to spe- cific theories, this analysis provides overall evidence for usage-based theories, and it shows that even low-frequency alternations which are typical of non-standard lan- guage can be examined in corpus studies, provided that very large (and therefore nec- essarily web-derived) corpora are available. Keywords prototypeDRAFT theory, alternations, German, noun inflection, web corpora for personal use only 1 I would like to thank the following people (in alphabetical order) for comments, discussion, and sug- gestions: Götz Keydana, Ulrike Sayatz, Christian Zimmer. I also thank the participants of the work- shop “Usage‐ Based Approaches to Morphology” at the Annual Conference of the German Linguistic Society (DGfS) 2013. All remaining errors and inadequacies are mine, and I intend to keep them. 1 1 Overview On July 23, 2012, the German online newspaper Spiegel Online published an article about a controversial statement made by Philipp Rösler, the former leader of the Ger- man liberal democratic party. In that article, there is a comment from a fellow party member quoted in a headline as (1a), but repeated in the text body as (1b).2 (1) a. Auf welchem Planeten lebt er? on which planet lives he On which planet does he live? b. Auf welchem Planet lebt er? On which planet lives he In (1a), the “weak” masculine noun Planet (‘planet’) takes the inflectional marker -en in the dative, but it does not in (1b). The form in (1b) represents a non-standard alterna- tion, because dative singular forms without a suffix are characteristic of the dominant “strong” masculine inflection pattern. While it is impossible to find out which variant was originally uttered in this case, many native speakers would agree that dropping the -en in this case might be stylistically dispreferred in written standard German, but that it is not unusual in colloquialDRAFT spoken German, and that it is far from full ungrammatical- ity. Examples of an accusative and afor genitive personal of a weak noun use (Mensch only, ‘man, human’) 2 http://www.spiegel.de/politik/deutschland/philipp-roesler-empoert-fdp-freunde-mit-griechenland- aeusserung-a-845980.html 2 which inflect according to the strong pattern (accusative Mensch instead of Mensch-en and genitive Mensch-es instead of Mensch-en) can be found in (2) and (3).3,4 (2) Gibt es einen Mensch, der stetig wächst? gives it a man who constantly grows Is there any man who grows constantly? (3) Das Leben eines Mensches wird zu politischen Zwecken the life of.a man becomes for political reasons aufs Spiel gesetzt. on.the game put The live of a man is put at risk for political reasons. The weak nouns form a small class of just over 450 masculine nouns (compared to thousands of strong masculine nouns), and they have remarkable prototypical semantic and phonotactic properties (Köpcke [1995]), for example human denotation or non-final accent (cf. Section 2). In addition, their case and number forms are quite remarkable within the German declension system in that they use -en as a non-nominative-singular marker (Thieroff [2003]). All forms except for the nominative singular take the suffix -en (see Section 2). Not surprisingly, given the low type frequency of the weak nouns and their non-canonicalDRAFT inflection, some of them have been fully assimilated to the strong inflectional pattern (Wurzel for[1985], personal Joeres [1996]). Additionally,use only nouns which are still predominantly weak have alternative strong forms as exemplified in (1) – (3). No extensive corpus study which shows whether the presence or the absence of the pro- totypical semantic and phonotactic features of weak nouns influences the alternation 3 http://www.flegel-g.de/wachstum-wachstum.html 4 http://www.mumia.de/doc/aktuell/991201ai00.html 3 strength has been presented. By “alternation strength”, I mean the probability that a weak noun occurs in a strong form. The lack of corpus evidence was—in my view— mostly due to the lack of adequate corpora, and the present paper aims to remedy this situation by using a very large web corpus of German for a data-driven study. I pursue two major goals in this paper: a theoretical and a methodological one. On the theoretical side, I consider this study to contribute evidence for the adequacy of usage- based models of inflectional morphology, without making strong claims in favor of a specific version of, for example, Construction Morphology such as advocated in Booij (2010), who is not very much concerned with inflection, anyway. A prototype or schema approach as proposed by Köpcke (1988,1995,1998) suggests itself, not only because it was applied to German nominal inflection within those publications. The advantage of a schema approach is that it specifically allows for central and non-central membership. Lexical items less strongly associated with a certain schema can be assumed to disasso- ciate from that schema and associate with another one more readily than strongly asso- ciated items. In the case at hand, I suggest that such a theory predicts that the alternation strength of highly prototypical weak nouns should be lower than that of less prototypi- cal weak nouns. My corpus study not only confirms my hypothesis but also quantifies the strength of the influenceDRAFT of the different semantic, phonotactic, and morphological properties. It is a difficult question whetherfor personaland how such corpus use data only can be interpreted in “cognitive” terms. I carefully subscribe to a view of corpus linguistics as “psy- chologically informed (cognitively-inspired) usage-based linguistics” (Gries [2010:334]). In this vein, the inspiration for the present study comes from the cognitive schema-based theories by Köpcke. Also, the study contributes—by producing results 4 from corpus data which are highly compatible to those theories—evidence to support the assumption that reflexes of cognitive representations can indeed be measured in cor- pus data. As for the second (methodological) goal, I demonstrate that data-driven corpus studies of rare event alternations—even if they predominantly occur in unedited colloquial writ- ten language—are possible, if corpora of the appropriate size and nature are available. I suggest web corpora in the region of 10 billion tokens as the appropriate source of data. I consider this methodological goal to be equally important as the theoretical one. I now briefly describe the system of noun inflection in German as well as the position of the weak nouns within that system in Section 2. Section 3 presents the corpus study including the statistical results. Section 4 summarizes the findings. 2 German weak nouns 2.1 German noun inflection In this section, I briefly describe noun inflection in current German, mostly for readers who are unfamiliar with German. I follow the overviews in Augst (1979), Eisenberg (2000, 2013), and Schäfer (2014, submitted). German has a three-wayDRAFT gender distinction (neuter, masculine, feminine), with four cases (nominative, accusative, dative,for genitive) personal and a two-way use number only distinction (sin- gular, plural).5 Leaving only the weak nouns aside (cf. Section 2.2), case inflection in nouns reduces to two simple rules: The dative plural takes -(e)n (after the plural marker) 5 Many syncretisms can be observed in the case system, but if we take the whole range of case-marked parts-of-speech (nouns, adjectives, pronouns, and determiners), non-ambiguous NP forms can be found for each of the four cases. 5 whenever it is phonotactically possible, and the genitive singular of masculine and neuter nouns always takes -(e)s.6 Furthermore, grammatical gender and inflection are tightly coupled in as much as a noun’s gender largely predetermines its plural inflection. Leaving aside the weak nouns and some other small classes once again, the general masculine and neuter (strong) plu- ral marker is -e (with or without stem umlaut) as in Stuhl/Stühl-e (‘chair/s’) and Gurt/Gurt-e (‘belt/s’). The general feminine plural marker is -en as in Nadel/Nadel-n (‘needle/s’). Sometimes, the prototypical masculine and neuter plural suffixes occur with feminine nouns and vice versa. Examples are masculine Staat-en (‘states’) or femi- nine Wänd-e (‘wall/s’). The class of weak nouns also takes the atypical masculine plural marker -en. I now turn to the additional peculiarities of weak nouns in Section 2.2. 2.2 Weak nouns In addition to taking the rare masculine plural -en, weak nouns deviate from the other- wise strict pattern of case marking in that they take the suffix -en in all singular forms except for the nominative. In other words, -en becomes a non-nominative-singular marker instead of a simple plural marker. In this section, I briefly summarize the rele- vant previous work onDRAFT weak nouns, especially regarding their special status within the German declension system. for personal use only 6 In inflectional affixes, orthographic <e> always corresponds phonologically to schwa.
Recommended publications
  • Tip #4 INFINITIVES 不定詞
    Tip #4 INFINITIVES 不定詞 Basic Guidelines Infinitive means “without limit”, and is one of a group of three special word forms called “verbals”: Verbal Type Functions Forms 1) infinitive (不定詞) (weak) noun to + verb (to) verb to find help (to) do 2) gerund (動名詞) (strong) noun verb-ing finding 3) participle (分詞) adjective verb-ing verb-ed having verb-ed finding found having found acting acted having acted To conquer unknown areas of science is Taro’s desire. “to conquer” = noun(名詞) and is used as the subject of the sentence The desire to conquer unknown areas of science is Taro’s. “to conquer” = adjective(形容詞) and describes what kind of desire Taro desires to conquer unknown areas of science. “to conquer” = a noun phrase that actually functions to describe “desire” Writers too often misuse the infinitive because they were mistaught, or because the concepts were never learned properly. When thinking of the word “infinitive”, associate the word “infinity” “without limitations”. Infinitive use more commonly communicates uncertainty and doubt—though more psychologically—and is often used in situations in which logic and future possibilities are stressed. The gerund(V-ing 動名詞)—likely mistaught as well—stresses the concept of certainty much more the infinitive, and often refers to things done or finished in the past; thus, things that are known or certain. In short many writers overuse the infinitive in today’s technical, scientific, and even business writing; and use the gerund too little. For numerous reasons dating back to the 1980s, the gerund has become more and more frequently used for stressing facts or factual-like information.
    [Show full text]
  • Old English: 450 - 1150 18 August 2013
    Chapter 4 Old English: 450 - 1150 18 August 2013 As discussed in Chapter 1, the English language had its start around 449, when Germanic tribes came to England and settled there. Initially, the native Celtic inhabitants and newcomers presumably lived side-by-side and the Germanic speakers adopted some linguistic features from the original inhabitants. During this period, there is Latin influence as well, mainly through missionaries from Rome and Ireland. The existing evidence about the nature of Old English comes from a collection of texts from a variety of regions: some are preserved on stone and wood monuments, others in manuscript form. The current chapter focusses on the characteristics of Old English. In section 1, we examine some of the written sources in Old English, look at some special spelling symbols, and try to read the runic alphabet that was sometimes used. In section 2, we consider (and listen to) the sounds of Old English. In sections 3, 4, and 5, we discuss some Old English grammar. Its most salient feature is the system of endings on nouns and verbs, i.e. its synthetic nature. Old English vocabulary is very interesting and creative, as section 6 shows. Dialects are discussed briefly in section 7 and the chapter will conclude with several well-known Old English texts to be read and analyzed. 1 Sources and spelling We can learn a great deal about Old English culture by reading Old English recipes, charms, riddles, descriptions of saints’ lives, and epics such as Beowulf. Most remaining texts in Old English are religious, legal, medical, or literary in nature.
    [Show full text]
  • Systematic Homonymy and the Structure of Morphological Categories: Some Lessons from Paradigm Geometry
    Jason Johnston (1997) Systematic Homonymy and the Structure of Morphological Categories: Some Lessons from Paradigm Geometry. PhD thesis, University of Sydney Note to the PDF Edition This PDF version of my dissertation is as close as I have been able to make it to the copies deposited with the Faculty of Arts and the Department of Linguistics at the University of Sydney. However this version is not in any sense an image capture of those copies, which remain the only ‘official’ ones. The differences between this document and the printed copies are very minor and revolve entirely around the use of Greek and IPA fonts. Essentially, one instance of the Greek font has been rewritten in transliteration (the quotation on page 41), and the change to an IPA font lacking a subscripted dot has entailed some modification of words containing that diacritic. Either these have been rewritten without diacritics (the words ‘Panini’ and ‘Astadhyayi’ throughout), or proper IPA retroflex glyphs have been substituted where appropriate (in all other cases). Additionally, the substitute IPA font has slightly different metrics from the one used originally, which has caused some enlargement of tables containing IPA characters or (more commonly) diacritics, and therefore occasionally some repagination. In all cases this is compensated for within a few pages at most. There are other small differences in the appearance of some tables, diagrams, and accented characters. This document contains a ‘live’ table of contents (‘bookmarks’ in Adobe Acrobat terminology). In addition, endnote references and bibliographic citations, though not specifically so marked, are hyperlinked to their corresponding text or entry, as the case may be.
    [Show full text]
  • Prototype-Driven Alternations: the Case of German Weak Nouns to Appear in Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory
    Prototype-driven alternations: the case of German weak nouns To appear in Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory Roland Schäfer Abstract: Over the past years, multifactorial corpus-based explorations of alternations in grammar have become an accepted major tool in cognitively oriented corpus lin- guistics. For example, prototype theory as a theory of similarity-based and inherently probabilistic linguistic categorization has received support from studies showing that alternating constructions and items often occur with probabilities influenced by proto- typical formal, semantic or contextual factors. In this paper, I analyze a low-frequency alternation effect in German noun inflection in terms of prototype theory, based on strong hypotheses from the existing literature that I integrate into an established theo- retical framework of usage-based probabilistic morphology, which allows us to ac- count for similarity effects even in seemingly regular areas of the grammar. Specifical- ly, the so-called weak masculine nouns in German, which follow an unusual pattern of case marking and often have characteristic lexical properties, sporadically occur in forms of the dominant strong masculine nouns. Using data from the nine-billion-token DECOW12A web corpus of contemporary German, I demonstrate that the probability of the alternation is influenced by the presence or absence of semantic, phonotactic, and paradigmatic features. Token frequency is also shown to have an effect on the al- ternation, in line with common assumptions about the relation between frequency and entrenchment. I use a version of prototype theory with weighted features and polycen- tric categories, but I also discuss the question of whether such corpus data can be taken as strong evidence for or against specific models of cognitive representation (proto- types vs.
    [Show full text]
  • The Finnish Noun Phrase
    Università Ca’ Foscari di Venezia Facoltà di Lingue e Letterature Straniere Corso di Laurea Specialistica in Scienze del Linguaggio The Finnish Noun Phrase Relatore: Prof.ssa Giuliana Giusti Correlatore: Prof. Guglielmo Cinque Laureanda: Lena Dal Pozzo Matricola: 803546 ANNO ACCADEMICO: 2006/2007 A mia madre Table of contents Acknowledgements ………………………………………………………….…….…… III Abstract ………………………………………………………………………………........ V Abbreviations ……………………………………………………………………………VII 1. Word order in Finnish …………………………………………………………………1 1.1 The order of constituents in the clause …………………………………………...2 1.2 Word order and interpretation .......……………………………………………… 8 1.3 The order of constituents in the Nominal Expression ………………………… 11 1.3.1. Determiners and Possessors …………………………………………………12 1.3.2. Adjectives and other modifiers …………………………………………..… 17 1.3.2.1 Adjectival hierarchy…………………………………………………………23 1.3.2.2 Predicative structures and complements …………………………………26 1.3.3 Relative clauses …………………………………………………………….... 28 1.4 Conclusions ............……………………………………………………………. 30 2. Thematic relations in nominal expressions ……………………………………….. 32 2.1 Observations on Argument Structure ………………………………….……. 32 2.1.1 Result and Event nouns…………………………………………………… 36 2.2 Transitive nouns ………………………………………………………………... 38 2.2.1 Compound nouns ……………….……………………………………... 40 2.2.2 Intransitive nouns derived from transitive verbs …………………… 41 2.3 Passive nouns …………………………………………………………………… 42 2.4 Psychological predicates ……………………………………………………….. 46 2.4.1 Psych verbs ……………………………………………………………….
    [Show full text]
  • German Grammar Articles Table
    German Grammar Articles Table Is Tedd fornicate when Franky swivelling railingly? Douglis plucks his prefigurations promised saucily, but powerless Elwood never faxes so pronto. Traumatic and unassisted See hydrogenized his gatecrasher retrogrades jewel cross-legged. And the gender is it is important to get if available in german grammar table for each noun is spoken among the german article, we have to tell you to In German there's a lawn more to determining which article to crank than just. German Grammar Indefinite articles Vistawide. And crash the nouns with both definite article in the key below as first. German Grammar Songs Accusative and Dative Prepositions. Experienced German teachers prepared easy articles and simple conversations in German for. German grammar Nouns Verbs Articles Adjectives Pronouns Adverbial phrases Conjugation Sentence structure Declension Modal particles v t e German articles are used similarly to the English articles a crawl the film they are declined. Why by a fresh male a fit female exit a window neutral in German Though these might. Because as these might be already discovered German grammar is. General sites Helpful articles about teaching grammar Animated German Grammar Tutorials. The German definite article d- with growing its forms is getting essential tool. A new wrath of making chart German is easy. German Nominative and Accusative cases audio. How To our Understand The Frustrating Adjective Italki. Contracted Preposition-Determiner Forms in German Tesis. German language Kumarika. German Definite Articles Der Die Das Everything may Need. To give table above certain adjectival pronouns also talking like the background article der. You dig insert a 'k-' in front seven any voyage of 'ein-' in the constant of the against to.
    [Show full text]
  • The Old Saxon Language Grammar, Epic Narrative, Linguistic Interference
    Irmengard.Rauch The Old Saxon Language Grammar, Epic Narrative, Linguistic Interference PETER LANG New York • San Francisco • Bern • Baltimore Frankfurt am Main • Berlin • Wien • Paris Contents Plate I Ms. C Fit 1: lines 1-18a xvii Plate II Ms. M Fit 2-3: lines 117-179a x vi i i -x ix Plate III Ms. S Fit 7: lines 558b-582a; Fit 8: lines 675-683a; lines 692-698 xx-xxi Plate IV Versus de Poeta & interprete huius codicis xxii Plate V Map of Old Saxon Speech Area x x i i i Introduction xxv Symbols and Abbreviations xli Chapter One Reading an Old Saxon Text; Early Cognitive Decisions; The Verb in the Lexicon; The Strong Verb Dictionary Finder Chart 1. The Old Saxon Text: Narrative Discourse 1 2. The OS Sentence, Grammatical Constituents, Lexicon 2 3. The Verb in the Lexicon 3 3.1 The Fundamental Identifying Form (FIF) of the Verb 3 3.2 Strong, Weak, Modal Auxiliary, Anomalous Verb Types 4 VIII 3.3 Strong Verb ABLAUT 5 3.3.1 The Strong Verb Dictionary Finder Chart 6 3.3.2 Variation in the Seven Strong Verb Sets 12 Chapter Two After the First Search; Diachronic Synchrony and Linguistic Explanation; Linear Syntax: Independent Sentence; Pragmatic Strategies; Nonlinear Micro-syntax: Morphology; Inflection of Verb Present Tense 4. Linguistic Generalization in Diachronic Synchrony 19 5. Completing the Search 23 6. Linear Syntax: The Independent Sentence 24 6.1 The Independent Declarative Sentence: (X)VbSO 24 6.2 The Unmarked Interrogative and the Unmarked Imperative Sentence: (X)VbSO 25 6.3 The Marked Independent Sentence: (X)SVbO and (X)SOVb 26 6.4 Textual and Pragmatic Strategies in Linear Syntax 27 7.
    [Show full text]
  • Minimum of English Grammar
    Parameters Morphological inflection carries with it a portmanteau of parameters, each individually shaping the very defining aspects of language typology. For instance, English is roughly considered a ‘weak inflectional language’ given its quite sparse inflectional paradigms (such as verb conjugation, agreement, etc.) Tracing languages throughout history, it is also worth noting that morphological systems tend to become less complex over time. For instance, if one were to exam (mother) Latin, tracing its morphological system over time leading to the Latin-based (daughter) Romance language split (Italian, French, Spanish, etc), one would find that the more recent romance languages involve much less complex morphological systems. The same could be said about Sanskrit (vs. Indian languages), etc. It is fair to say that, at as general rule, languages become less complex in their morphological system as they become more stable (say, in their syntax). Let’s consider two of the more basic morphological parameters below: The Bare Stem and Pro- drop parameters. Bare Stem Parameter. The Bare Stem Parameter is one such parameter that shows up cross-linguistically. It has to do with whether or not a verb stem (in its bare form) can be uttered. For example, English allows bare verb stems to be productive in the language. For instance, bare stems may be used both in finite conjugations (e.g., I/you/we/you/they speak-Ø) (where speak is the bare verb)—with the exception of third person singular/present tense (she speak-s) where a morphological affix{s}is required—as well as in an infinitival capacity (e.g., John can speak-Ø French).
    [Show full text]
  • German Grammar in English for International Students
    German Grammar in English for International Students Version 2.6 Prof. Dr. Russell Block FK 13 – General Studies University of Applied Sciences – München Winter Semester 2013 © 2013 Contents: Introduction: .............................................................8 Chapter 1: The Sound of German ............................................9 1 Standard German .....................................................9 2 The standard dialect ...................................................9 3 Overview of the German consonants .....................................9 3.1 Tense vs. lax .................................................11 3.2 The final devoicing rule .........................................11 3.3 Comments on individual consonants . 11 3.3.1 Vogel-V ...............................................11 3.3.2 The origin of <w> .......................................12 3.3.3 The problem of /h/ .......................................12 3.3.4 Ach-Laut – ich-Laut ......................................12 3.3.5 The pronunciation of final <g> . 12 3.3.6 The strange case of /s/ ....................................13 3.3.7 r-peculiarities ...........................................13 3.3.8 Affricates ..............................................13 3.3.9 Foreign sounds ..........................................13 3.3.10 The Glottal Stop ///......................................14 4 Vowels ............................................................14 5 The German vowels ..................................................15 5.1 Vowel length
    [Show full text]
  • Inheritance and Inflectional Morphology: Old High German, Latin, Early New High German, and Koine Greek
    Inheritance and Inflectional Morphology: Old High German, Latin, Early New High German, and Koine Greek By MaryEllen Anne LeBlanc A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in German in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Irmengard Rauch, Chair Professor Thomas Shannon Professor Gary Holland Spring 2014 1 Abstract Inheritance and Inflectional Morphology: Old High German, Latin, Early New High German, and Koine Greek by MaryEllen Anne LeBlanc Doctor of Philosophy in German University of California, Berkeley Professor Irmengard Rauch, Chair The inheritance framework originates in the field of artificial intelligence. It was incorporated first into theories of computational linguistics, and in the last two decades, it has been applied to theoretical linguistics. Inheritance refers to the sharing of properties: when a group of items have a common property, each item is said to inherit this property. The properties may be mapped in tree format with nodes arranged vertically. The most general (i.e. the most widely shared, unmarked) properties are found at the highest nodes, and the most specific (marked) information is found at the lowest nodes. Inheritance is particularly useful when applied to inflectional morphology due to its focus on the generalizations within and across paradigms. As such, it serves as an alternative to traditional paradigms, which may simplify the translation process; and provides a visual representation of the structure of the language's morphology. Such a mapping also enables cross- linguistic morphological comparison. In this dissertation, I apply the inheritance framework to the nominal inflectional morphology of Old High German, Latin, Early New High German, and Koine Greek.
    [Show full text]
  • Notes from Donald Hall's on Writing Well Verbs Verbs Act. Verbs Move. Verbs Do. Verbs Strike, Soothe, Grin, Cry, Exasperate
    Notes From Donald Hall’s On Writing Well Verbs Verbs act. Verbs move. Verbs do. Verbs strike, soothe, grin, cry, exasperate, decline, fly, hurt, and heal. Verbs make writing go, and they matter more to our language than any other part of speech. Verbs give energy, if we use them with energy. If we don’t use them with energy, they sound dull or flat: Verbs are action. Verbs are motion. Verbs are doing. A single verb is stronger than a strung-out verb-adjective or verb-noun combination. I am cognizant of this situation I know it. Look out for the verbs be / is / are combined with nouns and adjectives. He looked outside and became aware of the fact that it was raining. He looked outside and saw that it was raining. He looked outside. It was raining. Look out for the verbs has / have. We had a meeting. We met. The same advice applies to phrases that use verb forms ending in –ing (present participles). He is clearing his throat . He clears his throat . Watch out for false color in verbs. The 200-foot-high derrick was a black, latticed steel phallus raping the hot, virginal blue sky. The train slammed to a stop in the station. Steam vomited from all apertures. Passengers gushed through the barriers and hurled into the night. And fancy verbs. “Depict” is inferior to “paint” or “draw” or “describe” “Emulate” is fancy for “copy,” “ascertain” for “make sure,” and “endeavor” for “try.” Nouns Nouns are the simplest parts of speech, the words least tricky to use.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Structure(S), Form and Interpretation
    FUNCTIONAL STRUCTURE(S), FORM AND INTERPRETATION The issue of how interpretation results from the form and type of syntactic structures present in language is one which is central and hotly debated in both theoretical and descriptive linguistics. This volume brings together a series of eleven new cutting-edge essays by leading experts in East Asian languages which show how the study of formal structures and functional morphemes in Chinese, Japanese and Korean adds much to our general understanding of the close connections between form and interpretation. This specially commissioned collection will be of interest to linguists of all backgrounds working in the general area of syntax and language change, as well as those with a special interest in Chinese, Japanese and Korean. Yen-hui Audrey Li is Professor of Linguistics and East Asian Languages and Cultures at the University of Southern California. Her research focuses on the comparison of grammatical properties in English, Chinese and other East Asian languages. Recent work has considered issues of order and constituency, scope interaction of quantificational expressions, as well as the distribution, structure and interpretation of different types of nominal expressions and relative constructions. Andrew Simpson is a Senior Lecturer at the School of Oriental and African Studies, the University of London. His research interests centre on the comparative syntax of East and Southeast Asian languages, and his work addresses issues relating to processes of language change, DP-structure, the syntax of question formation and the interaction of syntax with phonology. ROUTLEDGECURZON ASIAN LINGUISTICS SERIES Editor-in-Chief: Walter Bisang, Mainz University Associate Editors: R.
    [Show full text]