<<

Education and debate

Statistics Notes Blinding in clinical trials and other studies Simon J Day, Douglas G Altman

Leo Human behaviour is influenced by what we know or the treatment received. Such blind assessment of Pharmaceuticals, Princes Risborough, believe. In there is a particular risk of expecta- outcome can often also be achieved in trials which are Buckinghamshire tion influencing findings, most obviously when there is open (non-blinded). For example, lesions can be HP27 9RR some subjectivity in assessment, leading to biased photographed before and after treatment and assessed Simon J Day results. Blinding (sometimes called masking) is used to manager, clinical by someone not involved in running the trial. Indeed, biometrics try to eliminate such bias. blind assessment of outcome may be more important ICRF Medical It is a tenet of randomised controlled trials that the than blinding the administration of the treatment, Statistics Group, treatment allocation for each patient is not revealed especially when the outcome measure involves subjec- Institute of Health until the patient has irrevocably been entered into the Sciences, Oxford tivity. Despite the best intentions, some treatments have OX3 7LF trial, to avoid . This sort of blinding, better unintended effects that are so specific that their occur- Douglas G Altman referred to as allocation concealment, will be discussed rence will inevitably identify the treatment received to professor of statistics in a future statistics note. In controlled trials the term in medicine both the patient and the medical staff. Blind blinding, and in particular “double blind,” usually assessment of outcome is especially useful when this is Correspondence to: refers to keeping study participants, those involved SJDay a risk. with their management, and those collecting and ana- In epidemiological studies it is preferable that the lysing clinical data unaware of the assigned treatment, BMJ 2000;321:504 identification of “cases” as opposed to “controls” be so that they should not be influenced by that kept secret while researchers are determining each knowledge. subject’s exposure to potential risk factors. In many The relevance of blinding will vary according to such studies blinding is impossible because exposure circumstances. Blinding patients to the treatment they can be discovered only by interviewing the study have received in a controlled trial is particularly impor- tant when the response criteria are subjective, such as participants, who obviously know whether or not they alleviation of pain, but less important for objective cri- are a case. The risk of differential recall of important teria, such as death. Similarly, medical staff caring for related events between cases and controls must 2 patients in a randomised trial should be blinded to then be recognised and if possible investigated. As a treatment allocation to minimise possible bias in minimum the sensitivity of the results to differential patient management and in assessing disease status. recall should be considered. Blinded assessment of For example, the decision to withdraw a patient from a patient outcome may also be valuable in other study or to adjust the dose of medication could easily epidemiological studies, such as cohort studies. be influenced by knowledge of which treatment group Blinding is important in other types of research the patient has been assigned to. too. For example, in studies to evaluate the perform- In a double blind trial neither the patient nor the ance of a diagnostic test those performing the test must caregivers are aware of the treatment assignment. be unaware of the true diagnosis. In studies to evaluate Blinding means more than just keeping the name of the of a measurement technique the the treatment hidden. Patients may well see the observers must be unaware of their previous measure- treatment being given to patients in the other ment(s) on the same individual. treatment group(s), and the appearance of the drug We have emphasised the risks of bias if adequate used in the study could give a clue to its identity. Differ- blinding is not used. This may seem to be challenging ences in taste, smell, or mode of delivery may also the integrity of researchers and patients, but bias asso- influence efficacy, so these aspects should be identical ciated with knowing the treatment is often subcon- for each treatment group. Even colour of medication scious. On average, randomised trials that have not 1 has been shown to influence efficacy. used appropriate levels of blinding show larger In studies comparing two active compounds, blind- treatment effects than blinded studies.3 Similarly, diag- ing is possible using the “double dummy” method. For nostic test performance is overestimated when the ref- example, if we want to compare two medicines, one erence test is interpreted with knowledge of the test presented as green tablets and one as pink capsules, we result.4 Blinding makes it difficult to bias results could also supply green placebo tablets and pink intentionally or unintentionally and so helps ensure placebo capsules so that both groups of patients would the credibility of study conclusions. take one green tablet and one pink capsule. Blinding is certainly not always easy or possible. Single blind trials (where either only the investigator or 1 De Craen AJM, Roos PJ, de Vries AL, Kleijnen J. Effect of colour of drugs: of perceived effect of drugs and their effectiveness. BMJ only the patient is blind to the allocation) are 1996;313:1624-6. sometimes unavoidable, as are open (non-blind) trials. 2 Barry D. Differential recall bias and spurious associations in case/control In trials of different styles of patient management, studies. Stat Med 1996;15:2603-16. 3 Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes R, Altman DG. Empirical evidence of bias: surgical procedures, or alternative therapies, full blind- dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treat- ing is often impossible. ment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 1995;273:408-12. 4 Lijmer JG, Mol BW, Heisterkamp S, Bonsel GJ, Prins MH, van der In a double blind trial it is implicit that the Meulen JH, et al. Empirical evidence of design-related bias in studies of assessment of patient outcome is done in ignorance of diagnostic tests. JAMA 1999;282:1061-6.

504 BMJ VOLUME 321 19-26 AUGUST 2000 bmj.com