<<

LE JOURNAL CANADIEN DES SCIENCES NEUROLOGIQUES

Functional Implications of Changes in the Senescent Brain: A Review

CHARLES H.M.BECK

SUMMARY: The morphological, chem­ and altered affect. Cholinergic hippo- change, a reduction in ambition and ical, and physiological changes in I he brain campal and neocortical systems were activity, an increased difficulty in accompanying are reviewed. The chemically and physiologically abnormal comprehension and an increase in time deterioration of the striatal and hypo­ in the aged. The implications for slowed and effort necessary for performance thalamic dopaminergic systems were im­ cognitive processing and persistance of the of familiar duties. Presenile plicated in the onset of age related trace are presented. is viewed as an acceleration of this Parkinsonian-like slowing of performance process of mental deterioration (Kolb, 1968). RESUME: L'auteur passe en revue les emotions modifiee. Les systemes choliner- The first section of this paper changements morphologiques, physiolo- giques de I'hippocampe et du neocortex describes the physically visable, i.e., manifestent aussi chez les vieillards des giques, el chimiques qui accompagnent la morphological changes, that can be vieiles.se. La deterioration des systemes anormalites physiologiques et chimiques. seen in . Although the dopaminergiques stries et hypothala- L'auteur discute les implications de ces miques est associee chez les vieillards aux changements pour la domaine des pro­ discussion will be restricted to the phenomenes de ralentissement de per­ cessus cognitifs el pour la persistence des literature on normally aging indi­ formance du type Parkinsonien. et aux traces de memoire. viduals and people with presenile and senile dementia, it is noteworthy that a higher than normal incidence of the Dementia refers to a progressive same physical signs have been loss of the mental faculties. The most observed in Down's syndrome common cause of a progressive retardates (Ohara, 1972) and Parkin­ impairment of the mental faculties in son patients (Hirano, 1970). Down's the older adult population is the syndrome retardates develop the degenerative brain disease known as symptoms by age 35 years. In addition senile dementia. If the disease begins to reviewing the physical signs of before the age of 65 years it is referred dementia, the first section of the paper to as presenile dementia or Alzheimer's will discuss the functional implications disease. Presenile and senile dementia of these signs. are diseases of nervous tissue as The second portion of the paper opposed to atherosclerotic dementia describes the chemical and physio­ which is a degenerative disease of the logical correlates of the aging process circulatory system. Together these two and outlines their functional implica­ categories account for the majority of tions. In the third and final section all cases of degenerative brain disease there is a detailed analysis of the in the aged. The incidence of senile behavioral scientist's perspective of dementia is twice as high in women as aging. in men. The proportions are reversed for atherosclerotic dementia (Roth, reported by Kent, 1977). MORPHOLOGICAL SIGNS OF AGING The mental symptoms of senile IN THE BRAIN dementia, from the clinician's Physical evidence for a diagnosis of viewpoint, include flattening of affect, presenile dementia includes the Reprint requests to: Charles Beck. Department of Psychology. Biological Sciences Building. University confusion in spatial and temporal presence in the brain of (I) senile of Alberta, l-dmonton. Alberta. T6G 2E9, Canada. orientation, impairment of memory plaques containing amyloid, an

Supported by a grant from the Medical Research especially for recent events, and a immunoglobulin byproduct; (2) Council. general slowing of movements and tangles of neurofibrils, normally

Krom the Neuropsychology Unit. Department of though processes. The onset is longitudinally arranged in the nerve Psychology. University of Alberta. Edmonton. Alberta. gradual, beginning with a dislike for fiber and thought to participate in the

Vol. 5 No. 4 NOVEMBER 1978-417 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGICAL SCIENCES

transport of vital substances along been related to memory impairments Samorajski, 1975). This results in a ; (3) areas of degeneration, in humans (Milner, 1970; Victor et al, reduction in oxygen turnover (Chen et consisting of silver staining granules 1971). The memory deficits of the al., 1972). One would predict from this surrounded by spaces or vacuoles; (4) demented and aged have been a reduced cerebral blood flow with age collections of lipofuscin or age attributed to hippocampal damage and with increasing dementia and this pigment inside neurons, representing (Ball, 1977; Malamud, 1972). Other is indeed the case (Lassen et al., I960; an accumulation of oxidized nerve authors (Brizzee et al., 1974; Scheibel Ingvar and Lassen, 1970; Simard, cell membranes; and (5) loss of nerve et al., 1975, 1976) prefer to emphasize 1971). Slowing of cerebral blood flow cells. the importance of the diffuseness of occurs over the temporal region in the It is widely held that the pervasive­ the signs of physical damage brains of memory impaired senile ness of these signs is positively throughout the brain, in accounting patients, and in the parieto-occipito- correlated with the degree of dementia for general slowing of information temporal region in patients with both for patients stricken before age 65 processing accompanying aging. aphasia symptoms (Hagberg and and for those afflicted later in life Prior to the disintegration of the Ingvar, 1976; Obristetal., 1970). Since (Blessed et al., 1968). However, it is nerve cell body there is an age related old people have a greater arterio­ not generally recognized that virtually deterioration of axonal (output) and venous 02 content difference, all people by 80 years of age present dendritic (input) terminal processes indicating the brain is extracting more similar, though less invidious, ex­ (Bondareff and Geinisman, 1976; 02 per unit of blood (Dastur et al., amples of the same physical pathology Machado-Salas et al., 1977; Scheibel 1963), and since the cerebral capillary (Dayan, 1970; Gellerstedt, 1931; et al., 1975, 1976). network of the senescent brain is more Matsuyama et al., 1966). There is a In summary, morphological changes extensive than that of the younger positive correlation between age in the brain with age provide a picture person (Huziker et al., 1978), the related cell loss, severity of tangle of gradual degeneration and cell loss, diminished cerebral blood flow is formation, and degree of granulo- especially in structures involved in more likely a product of metabolic vacuolar degeneration in both memory. The normal aging process is than morphological variables. normals and dements. However, the accelerated in Alzheimer's disease and In presenile and senile dementia, but yearly rate of cell loss is five times as Down's syndrome. Theories of aging not in atherosclerotic dementia, the great in the Alzheimer patients as in emphasize either the relation between occurrence of neurofibrillary tangles is normal persons (Ball, 1977; Ball and hippocampal degeneration and mem­ proportional to the accumulation of Lo, 1977). ory deficits or that between diffuse aluminum in the brain cells (Crapper degeneration and slowed processing. Brody (1955) hypothesized that the et al., 1976). The concentration of The functional implications of neuro- aluminum in Alzheimer patients is degree of dementia is directly related filbrillary tangles and cell membrane to the incidence of cell loss. Meier- four times that in normal patients instability will be detailed in the next (Crapper, 1974; Crapper et al., 1973). Ruge (1975) suggested that this section of the paper. hypothesis is untenable because Implantation of aluminum into the subsequent studies of senescent rodent brains of animals results in neuro­ brains have revealed less than 1% cell CHEMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL fibrillary tangles indentical to those in losses (Brizzee et al., 1968; Klein and SIGNS OF AGING IN THE BRAIN senile humans' brains (Terry and Michel, 1977). However, these studies The most prominent theory of aging Pena, 1965). Aluminum disrupts examined only the rodent neocortex. is the error catastrophe theory of Orgel protein synthesis in nerve cells (Miller Studies of the old rodent's medial (1963; Comfort, 1974). In all tissue, the and Levine, 1974). The origin of the dorsal nucleus of the thalamus (Stein RNA transcription of the genetic code metabolic error causing the aluminum and Firl, 1976) and for protein has an increased error rate accumulation is unknown. (Vijayan, 1977) showed serious with age, resulting in the age related Another anabolic process which degeneration. In humans the physical decline in levels of critical enzymes. deteriorates with aging is that of the signs enumerated above appear in Thus there is an age associated decline synthesis, release, and uptake of many parts of the brain including the in uptake, turnover, and utilization of neurotransmitters. Old brains ex­ neocortex. However, the pathology amino acids and a decline in RNA bio­ perience 25% slower rate and 50% has a predilection for the hippo­ synthesis of enzymes (Tonna and reduced volume of axonal transport of campus and in Alzheimer's disease for Singh, 1976). In general, biochemical substances essential for transmitter the posterior hippocampus in par­ anabolic functions deteriorate production (Geinisman et al., 1977a, ticular (Ball, 1977; Brizee et al., 1974; through the loss of enzymes control­ 1977b), a shortage of the enzymes Gellerstedt, 1931). The magnitude of ling carbohydrate, intermediate necessary for transmitter synthesis the hippocampal cell loss by 70 years protein, and lipid metabolism (McGreer et al., 1977; Spillane et al., of age is fairly substantial. There is (Roberts et al., 1976). With age, a 1977; Vijayan, 1977), an impairment approximately 12% cell loss in decrease occurs in glucose oxidation of waste reducing uptake of unused normals and 60% in presenile dements (Patch, 1977) and in formation and neurotransmitter (Jonec and Finch, (Ball, 1977). Damage to the medial utilization of ATP, an energy source 1975; Sun, 1976), fewer transmitter dorsal thalamus and hippocampus has (Ermini et al., 1971; Sun and receptor binding sites (White et al.,

The Senescent Brain Downloaded418-NOVEMBE from https://www.cambridge.org/coreR 1978 . IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 LE JOURNAL CANADIEN DES SCIENCES NEUROLOGIQUES

1977), reduced number of neurotrans­ 1978; Gygax et al., 1978) and alleviate parietal, temporal, and occipital mitter vesicles (Sun, 1976), the complaints of the aged (Bazo, cortices of Alzheimer patients (Perry and lowered sensitivity of the chemical 1973; Hoffbrand et al., 1976). No one et al., 1977). Normal old people do not response of the receiving to has described the effects of the ergot experience significant declines in delivered neurotransmitter (Govoni et alkaloids on the physical signs of acetylcholine enzyme levels outside al., 1977, Spano et al., 1975). cerebral senescence. the hippocampus (Perry et al., 1977). Conversely, products which de­ Dopamine is released by certain The dysfunction in Alzheimer patients grade or catabolize neurotransmitters nerve terminals in the retina in re­ is presynaptic, i.e., in acetylcholine increase with aging. Acid phos­ sponse to light (Kramer, 1971). In aged synthesis, rather than an impairment phatases, perioxidases, monamine mammals the cells receiving the in postsynaptic receptor site sensitivity oxidase, and catechol-o-methyl trans­ dopamine become hypersensitive to (Perry etal., 1977; Spillaneetal., 1977; ferase all increase in the old brain released dopamine (Spano et al., 1975) White et al., 1977). Such abnormality (Broch, 1973; Brunk and Ericcson, in an attempt to compensate for is consistent with electron microscopic 1972; Robinson et al., 1977). The latter functional dopaminergic dennerva- evidence of gross degenerative changes two molecules degrade the cate­ tion (Govoni et al., 1977). It is possible in presynaptic structures and the cholamine transmitters, dopamine that such paradoxical hypersensitivity normal appearance of receptor mem­ and norepinephrine. may contribute to the longer dark branes (Wisniewski and Terry, 1976). adaptation time (Reading, 1968) and The greater the depletion of the choli­ If the neurotransmitter changes nergic enzyme, the greater the degree referred to above affected systems the increased aggravation from light of neurofibrillary tangles evident controlling behaviors which deteri­ glare (Wolf, 1960) experienced by old (White et al., 1977). Geinisman et al. orated with age, then it is apparent people. Usually the effects are (1977a, 1977b) suspect that the tangles that the behavioral deficits of the aged attributed to increased opacity of the may be partly responsible for the re­ would be ascribable to metabolic lens and cornea. dysfunction rather than frank cell loss duced availability of the enzyme at Calcium ion concentrations in­ presynaptic membranes. as the primal causal agent. The crease in muscle (Gutmann and evidence for this view is growing. The Hanzlikova, 1972) and nervous tissue In response to , the medulla of aging brain undergoes a significant with aging (Sun and Seaman, 1977). the adrenal gland liberates a number decline in the functional effectiveness This may be related to abnormally low of neurotransmitter substances into of the dopamine system in the striatum levels of parathyroid hormone in the the blood stream. The urinary and (Finch, 1973; Govoni et al., 1977; senescent (Fujita et al., 1976). Calcium blood plasma levels of epinephrine, McGeer et al., 1977; Robinson et al., plays an important role in neurotrans­ norephinephrine, and dopamine beta- 1977). There is also a decrease in the mitter release. The redressing of the hydroxylase increase with age number of cells in the substantia nigra Ca^ imbalance has been suggested as (Freedman et al., 1972; Giorginoet al., with age (McGeer et al., 1977). the mechanism underlying the efficacy 1969; Kvetnansky et al., 1978) as does Behaviorally, this would mean that the of chronic chlorpromazine therapy in the size of adrenal medulla (Kvetnan­ normal aging process would move reducing neuronal lipofuscin accumu­ sky et al., 1978; Sotgiu et al., 1960). people toward the Parkinsonian ratio lation (Samorajski and Rolsten, 1976). Hyperfunction of the adrenal medulla of excess acetycholine to dopamine in Chlorpromazine has multiple effects in the aged coupled with their the critical system linking the sub­ on release and uptake of dopamine exaggerated sensitivity to its hor­ stantia nigra and the striatum and norepinephrine. mones (Frolkis et al., 1972) may be (McGreer et al., 1977). Thus one related to increased incidence of would expect slowness of movement, Chemical blocking of pathways hypertension, atherosclerosis, and rapid fatiguing, tardy movement requiring the neurotransmitter acetyl­ cardiac infarction in old people initiation, loss of associated move­ choline impairs performance on (Kvetnansky et al., 1978). ments, and diminished kinetic grace, memory tests. The deficits were similar characteristic of Parkinson patients, to those observed in normal old The hippocampus is capable of in­ to become more obvious with persons (Drachman and Leavitt, hibiting the secretion of adreno- advancing age. Evidence that the same 1974). Many neurons terminating in corticotrophic hormone from the dopaminergic deficit may be responsi­ the hippocampus release acetyl­ anterior pituitary. This hormone ble for a flattening of affect and a loss choline. There is a significant age controls secretions of other hormones, of incentive motivation is available related decline in the hippocampal e.g., corticosterioids, from the cortex (Phillips and Fibiger, 1973). concentration of the enzyme necessary of the adrenal gland. Neurons of the for acetylcholine synthesis (Perry et hippocampus have receptor sites for The ergot alkaloids, e.g., dihydro- al., 1977; Vijayan, 1977). This decline one of the corticosteriods, namely ergotoxine, which stabilize the is even more marked in Alzheimer corticosterone. Corticosterone modu­ terminal membranes of dopaminergic patients, but not in chronic schizo­ lates the genetic processes governing neurons (Goldstein et al., 1978; Spano phrenics, chronic unipolar depres- anzyme synthesis in the hippocampus and Trabucchi, 1978) protect the sives, or multi-infarct dements. It is (Lee et al., 1977). These enzymes are organism against the metabolic effects significant in degree not only in the necessary for neurotransmitter pro­ of reduced blood flow (Boismare et al.. hippocampus but also in the frontal, duction and, as noted above, are

Beck NOVEMBER 1978-419 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGICAL SCIENCES

depleted in old age (Perry et al., 1977). suggests that the speeded and memory hypofunction of the dopaminergic There is a positive correlation between categories of subtests are measuring system discussed above. McGeer's the degree of hippocampal neuron de­ the same thing. For example, memory (1977) hypothesis was that the generation in the individual and the tests would produce "speed" deficits if dopaminergic bias in the aged is resting levels of corticosterone in the the pace of stimulus presentation and toward that of the Parkinsonian blood plasma (Landfield and Lynch, response production is too rapid for neuropathology. Parkinson patients 1977). Resting plasma corticosterone the old person. When taken to exhibit not only slowed movement level decreases in old age. Continued identical original criteria, times but also slowed pre-electromyo- degeneration of the hippocampus with young and old are no different on graphic reaction times (Brumlick and senescence may result from increased subsequent (Hulicka, 1965). Boshes, 1966). Parkinson patients sensitivity to the circulating hormones When allowed to pace themselves, the persist in an error response for a longer (Frolkis et al., 1972). It would be elderly took longer to learn the task duration on tracking tasks than do interesting to measure corticosterone than did young persons but did just as normal individuals (Angel et al., 1970; levels and sensitivity in Alzheimer well as the young on recall tests Bowen et al., 1972). The degree to patients. (Adamowicz, 1976). Total learning which simple stimuli persist in In summary, the senescent brain time available rather than rate of memory, as measured by the critical experiences dysfunctional biases presentation is the critical variable flicker fusion test, increases with toward the Parkinsonian state of (Winn and Elias, 1977). increasing severity of Parkinson striatal dopamine depletion and If the slowed processing were a movement dysfunction (Riklan et al., consequent slowing of movement as general characteristic, one would 1970). Personality inflexibility as well as toward impairment expect an increased deficit with the measured by the F-Scale increases in precipitated by hypofunction of the amount of information processed on the same manner (Ploski et al., 1966). hippocampal and cortical cholingergic verbal tasks (Anders and Fozard, Comparable examples of stimulus systems. A common age related 1973; Klauser and Klein, 1978) and on trace persistance and response mode deterioration of the genetically nonverbal tasks (Benton, 1977; persistance for the aged will be controlled transcription of protein Fozard et al., 1976); slowed retrieval described below. may underly both trends. search of both primary and secondary Another mechanism that may memory (Anders and Fozard, 1973), contribute to slowed processing is and slowed rotation and comparison RELATED BEHAVIORAL SIGNS dysfunction of the hippocampal of spatial depth figures (Gaylord and OF AGING cholinergic system. Damage to the Marsh, 1975). The studies cited have Whether intelligence as defined by hippocampus causes the organism to demonstrated these effects with the fail to adjust to changing reinforce­ the traditional intelligence test does normal aged. decline with age (Birren and Morrison, ment contingencies and to fail to 1961; Williams, 1970) or does not Partitioning of the total response habituate to novel stimuli (Kimble, (Barton et al., 1975; Ford and Roth, time into reaction time and movement 1968). The organism tends to persist in 1977; Schaie et al., 1973) appears to time revealed that both were slow in performing current or habitual modes depend on methodological issues the aged and that the reaction time was of responding. It is possible that the which are not central to theme of this responsible for the greatest part of the slowness of the aged is partly the result review. Two main categories of deficit (Spirduso, 1975). Retarded of a tendency to prolong the response subtests in these and more recent movement time was attributable to to internally or externally generated batteries (Branconnier and Cole, 1978; slower peripheral nerve conduction stimuli (Axelrod, 1963; Botwinick, Shader et al., 1974) appear to be age velocity (Retzlaff and Fontaine, 1965), 1973). If the subsidence of the neural sensitive. These are speeded subtests loss of muscle fiber mass (Gutmann response to the initial stimulus is slow, and the memory subtests. and Hanzlikova, 1976), decreased the perception of the second stimulus The memory deficit of the aged is frequency of miniature end-plate would be retarded and slowed not specific for any particular potentials and slowed contraction processing of information would mnemonic subfunction such as coupling time (Gutmann and Hanzli­ result. If this were the case one would memory storage capacity (Eber, 1976; kova, 1972). look for evidence in the aged of the cf. Salthouse, 1978); level of memory Several mechanisms may contribute persistance of brief images at the processing (Eysenck, 1974; Zelinski et to the slowed reaction time. Raised registration level and for the per­ al., 1978; cf. Mergler et al., 1977); sensory thresholds (Clark and Mehl, severation of sets at the cognitive level. ignoring irrelevant stimuli (Rabbitt, 1971; Dyck et al., 1972, 1974; Harkins Exaggerated persistance of the 1965; cf. Klauser and Kleim, 1978); and Chapman, 1976; Kokmen et al., iconic trace has recently been registration as opposed to retrieval 1978; Whanger and Wang, 1974) and demonstrated to result in superior (Adamowicz, 1976), or on long versus slowed conduction time of brain performance in the recognition of short delays of recall (Schneider et al., neurons would at best contribute 10% fragmented words in the aged 1975). Rather, the deficit seems to be a to the total retardation of the reaction compared to the young (Kline and general slowing of information time (Retzlaff and Fontaine, 1965). A Orme-Rogers, 1978). Complementary processing (Adamowicz, 1976). This more significant factor would be the halves of a single word were flashed on

Tlie Senescent Brain Downloaded420-NOVEMBE from https://www.cambridge.org/coreR 1978 . IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 LE JOURNAL CANAD1EN DES SCIENCES NEUROLOG1QUES

a screen one after the other. If the may modulate cognitive deficits in the cessing, but that slowing may occur as persistance of the trace of the initial elderly (Birkhill and Schaie, 1975; a result of other causes, for example word half in the mind of the viewer was Hoyer et al., 1973; Plemons et al., excessive caution (Birkhill and Schaie, superimposed on the flash of the 1978). Physical seems 1975). second half of the word, recognition of definitely to influence physiological the word would result. Old people performance (Retzlaff and Fontaine, REFERENCES were especially superior for the longer 1965; de Vries, 1971) and reaction time ADAMOW1CZ, J. K. (1976). Visual short-term intervals between word fragments. In (Spirduso, 1975; Spirduso and memory and aging. Journal of Gerontology addition, old persons show enhanced Clifford, 1978). The last study is 31. 39-46. persistance of complementary after­ especially provocative since exercising ANDERS. T. A. and FOZARD. J. I.. (1973). images (Kline and Nestor, 1977), and, men in their sixties performed with Effects of age upon retrieval from primary as one might expect, poor temporal reaction times comparable to men in and secondary memory. Developmental resolution of shocks to the skin their twenties. One would like to know Psychology 9. 411-415. ANGEL. R. A.. ALSTEN, VV. and HIGGINS, (Axelrod et al., 1968), slowed what would be the iconic trace per­ J. R. (1970). Control of movement in Park­ temporal processing of auditory sistance times and retrieval search inson's disease. Brain 93. 1-15. stimuli (Corso, 1975), lowered visual times of those active oldsters. AXELROD, S. (1963). Cognitive tasks in several critical flicker fusion thresholds modalities. In Processes of Aging, edited by (Misiak, 1951; McFarland et al., In conclusion, evidence has been R. H. Williams. C. Tibbitts and W. Donahue, 1958), lowered auditory click fusion presented for an age dependent Atherton Press, New York, Vol. 1, pp. 132- thresholds (Weiss, 1959), protracted slowing of information processing. 145. susceptibility to backward masking Such an impairment may relate to AXELROD, S., THOMPSON. L. W. and persistance of sensory traces and COHEN, L. D. (1968). Effects of senescence (Kline and Szafran, 1975), and longer on the temporal resolution of somesthetic lasting visual evoked responses to perseveration of cognitive sets. stimuli presented to one hand or both. photic stimulation (Mundy-Castle, Persistance would be an advantage Journal of Gerontology 2. 191-195. 1953). It is easy to see how such only in tasks in which retention of a BALL, M. J. (1977). Neuronal loss, neurofibril­ persistance, if also applicable to items single item or mode of response was lary tangles and granulovascular degeneration in the hippocampus with aging and dementia. retrieved from primary of secondary required. Serial search or serial learning tasks would produce deficits A quantitative study. Acta Neuropathologica memory, would slow down retrieval 37. 11-118. search time (Anders and Fozard, in performance, unless there was adequate time for shifts between items. BALL. M. J. and LO. P. (1977). Granulovacuo- 1973). lar degeneration in the aging brain and in Evidence has been presented that dementia. Journal of Neuropathology and When one speaks of secondary and dysfunction in dopaminergic and Experimental Neurology. 36, 474-487. permanent memory one usually cholinergic brain systems may underly BARTON. E. M.. PLEMONS, J. K.. WILLIS. speaks of perseveration of sets rather these persistance effects. Avenues for S. L. and BALTES. P. B. (1975). Recent findings on adult and gerontological than persistance of traces. Old people future research suggest themselves. intelligence: changing a stereotype of perseverate in viewing ambiguous Persistance measures have not been decline. American Behavioral Scientist 19, figures in only one mode (Botwinick et applied to the other types of human 224-236. al., 1959), persist in viewing the Necker abiotrophies (Bowen et al., 1976) or BAZO, A. G. (1973). An ergot aklaloid prepar­ cube in only one view (Heath and their animal models (Crapper, 1974; ation (hydergine) versus papaverine in Orbach, 1963), and fail to reverse the Ungerstedt, 1971). One disturbing treating common complaints of the aged: a double blind study. Journal of American figure ground relationship in order to possibility is that schizophrenia which Geriatric Society 21. 63-71. read ambiguous words (Kline et al., supposedly has a dopaminergic bias BENTON, A. L. (1977). Interactive effects of 1977). Tasks requiring a shifting of opposite in direction to that of age and brain disease on reaction time. set are expecially Parkinsonism is also said to be Archives of Neurology 34. 369-370. taxing for the aged (Botwinick et al., characterized by slow information BIRKHILL. W. R. and SCHAIE. K.W.( 1975). 1957, 1958, 1959; Goodrick, 1972; processing (Davidson and Neale, The effect of differential reinforcement of Heglin, 1956). Old people have 1974; Saccuzzo et al., 1974; Yates, cautiousness in intellectual performance among the elderly. Journal of Gerotology anecdotally described the persistance 1966; Yates and Korboot, 1970) and 30. 578-583. with which unwanted tunes and trace persistance (Gruzelier et al., B1RREN.J. E.and MORRISON. D. K. (1961). thoughts run through their minds. 1972). It would be helpful to know Analysis of the WAIS subtests in relation to What can be done to postpone the whether the persistence measures in age and education. Journal of Gerontology development of senility? As previously the aged are changed by exercise or 16. 363-369. noted the ergot alkaloids and drug therapy. The hypothesis that BLESSED. G.. TOMLINSON. B. E. and persistance and perseveration beget ROTH. M. (1968). The association between phenothiazines may offer some hope. quantitative measures and senile changes in At the present time the largest cognitive slowing needs to be the cerebral gray matter of elderly subjects. contribution to longevity variance in examined. Are the three tightly British Journal of Psychiatry 114. 797-802. the healthy rat is the factor controlled correlated or can they be dissociated? BOISMARE. F.. LE PONCIN. M. and by diet (Ooka et al., 1978; Ross and Possibly it will be found that LEFRANCOIS. .1. (1978). Biochemical and behavioral effects of hypox hypoxia in rats: Lustbader, 1976; Segall et al., 1978). persistance and perseveration always result in slowing of information pro­ Study of the protection afforded by ergot There is some evidence that training alkaloids. Gerontology 24, Suppl. I, 6-13.

Beck NOVEMBER 1978-421 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGICAL SCIENCES

BONDAREFF, W. and GEINISMAN, Y. CLARK, W. C. and MEHL, L. (1971). Thermal EYSENCK, M. W. (1974). Age differences in (1976). Loss of synapses in the pain: A sensory decision theory analysis of incidental learning. Developmental Psychol­ of the senescent rat. American Journal of the effect of age and sex on d1, various ogy 10, 936-941. Anatomy 145, 129-136. response criteria and 50% pain threshold. FINCH, C. E. (1973). Catecholamine metabolism BOTWIN1CK, J. (1973). Aging and Behavior, Journal of Abnormal Psychology 78, 202- in the brains of aging male mice. Brain Springer. New York, p. 132. 212. research 52, 261-276. BOTW1N1CK.J..BR1NLEYJ. F.andBlRREN. COMFORT, A. (1974). The position of aging FORD, J. M. and ROTH, W. T. (1977). Do J. E. (1975). Set in relation toage. Journal of studies. Mechanisms of Aging & Develop­ cognitive abilities decline with age? Gerontology 12. 300-305. ment 3, 1-31. Geriatrics 32, 59-62. BOTW1NICK, J., BR1NLEY, J. F. and CORSO, J. F. (1975). Sensory processes in man FOZARD, J. L., THOMAS, J. C, JR. and ROBBIN, J. S. (1958). Task alternation time during motility and senescence. In WAUGH, N. C. (1976). Effects of age and in relation to problem difficulty and age. Neurobiology of Aging, edited by J. M. frequency of stimulus repetitions on two- Journal of Gerontology 13, 414-417. Ordyand K. R. Brizzee, Plenum, New York, choice reaction time. Journal of Gerontology pp. 119-143. 31, 556-563. BOTWIN1CK, J., BRINLEY, J. F. and ROBBIN, J. S. (1959). Modulation of speed CRAPPER, D. R. (1974). Dementia: Recent FREEDM AN, L. S., OH UCHI, T„ GOLDSTEIN, of response with age. Journal of Genetic observations in Alzheimer's disease and M., AXELROD, F„ FISH, I. and DANCIS, Psychology 95, 137-144. experimental aluminum encephalopathy. In J. (1972(. Changes in human serum Frontiers in Neurology and Neuroscience dopamine-/3-hydroxylase activity with age. BOTWINICK, J, ROBBIN. J. S. and BRINLEY, Nature 236, 310-311. J. F. (1959). Reorganization of perceptions Research, edited by P. Seeman and G. M. FROLKIS, W., BEZRUKOV, V. V. and with age. Journal of Gerontology 14, 85-88. Brown, Toronto, University of Toronto Press, pp. 97-111. S1NITSKY, V. N. (1972). Sensitivity of BOWEN, D. M.. SMITH, C. B., WHITE, P. central nervous structures to humoral and DAVISON, A. N. (1976). Neuro- CRAPPER, D. R.. KRISHNAN, S. S. and factors in aging. Experimental Gerontology transmitter-related enzymes and indices of DALTON, A. J. (1973). Brain aluminum 7, 185-194. distribution in Alzheimer's disease and hypoxia in senile dementia and other FUJITA, T., OHATA, M., OTA, K., TSUDA, experimental neurofibrillary degeneration. abiotrophies. Brain 99, 459-496. T., UEZI, A., OKANO, K. and YOSH1- Science 180, 511-513. BOWEN. F. P., HOEHN, M. M. and YAHR, KAWA, M. (1976). Aging and parathyroid M. D. (1972). Cerebral dominance in CRAPPER, D. R., KRISHNAN, S. S. and hormone secretion. Journal of Gerontology relation to tracking and tapping performance QUITTKAT, S. (1976). Aluminum, neuro­ 31, 523-526. in patients with parkinsonism. Neurology fibrillary degeneration and Alzheimer's GAYLORD, S. A. and MARSH, G. R. (1975). 22, 32-39. disease. Brain 99. 67-80. Age differences in the speed of a spatial BRANCONNIER, R. J. and COLE. J. O. DASTUR, D. K., LANE, M. H., HANSEN, cognitive process. Journal of Gerontology (1978). The impairment index as a symptom- D. B.. KETY, S. S., BUTLER, R. N., 30, 674-678. independent parameter of drug efficacy in PERL1N, S. and SOKOLOFF, L. (1963). GEINISMAN, Y., BONDAREFF, W. and geriatic psycho-pharmacology. Journal of Effects of aging on cerebral circulation and TELSER, A. (1977a). Transport of (>H) Gerontology 33. 217-223. metabolism in human aging: A biological frucose labeled glycoproteins in the septo- and behavioral study. Washington, D.C., BRIZZEE, K. R., ORDY. J. M. and KAACK, hippocampal pathway of young adult and Public Health Service Publication, United senescent rats. Brain Research 25, 182-186. B. (1974). Early appearance and regional States Government Printing Office, No. 986. differences in intraneuronal and extraneur- GEINISMAN, Y., BONDAREFF, W. and onal lipofuscin accumulation with age in the DAVIDSON, G. S. and NEALE, J. M. (1974). TELSER, A. (1977b). Diminished axonal brain of a nonhuman primate. Journal of The effects of signal-noise similarity on transport of glycoprotein in the senescent rat Gerontology 29, 366-381. visual information processing in schizo­ brain. Mechanisms of Aging and Develop­ phrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ment 6, 363-378. BRIZZEE, K. R., SHERWOOD, N. and 83, 683-686. T1MIRAS, P. S. (1968). Comparison of cell GELLERSTEDT, E. (1931). Klinisch-generalo- populations at various levels in the cerebral DAYAN, A. D.( 1970). Quantitative histological gischer Nachweis von Erblichkeit bei of young adult and aged Long-Evans rats. studies on the aged brain. II. Senile plaques Pickscher Krankheit. Zeitschrift fur Journal of Gerontology 23, 289-297. and neurofibrillary tangles in senile Neurologische Psychiatrie 136, 464-471. dementia. Acta Neuropathological 16, 95- GIORGINO, R., SCARDAPANE. R., NAR- BROCH, O. J., J R. (1973). The postnatal devel­ 102. DELLI, G. M. and TAFARO, E. (1969). opment of catechol-o-methyl transferase in DRACHMAN, D. A. and LEAV1TT, J. (1974). Adrenal glands and aging: Aspects of the rat brain. Journal of Neurochemistry 20, medullary activity. Folia Endocrinology 22, 847-852. Human memory and the cholinergic system. Archives of Neurology 30, 113-121. 215-224. BRODY, H. (1955). Organization of the cerebral GOLDSTEIN, M., LEW, J. Y.. HATA, F. and cortex. III. A study of aging in the human DYCK, P. J., CURTIS, D. J., BUSHEK, W. L1EBERMAN, A. (1978). Binding inter­ cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative and OFFORD, K. (1974). Description of actions of ergot alkaloids with mono- Neurology 102, 511-556. "Minnesota Thermal Disks" and normal aminergic receptors in the brain. Geron­ values of cutaneous thermal discrimination BRUMLICK. J. and BOSHES, B. (1966). The tology 24. Suppl. I, 76-85. in man. Neurology 24, 325-330. mechanism of bradykinesia in parkinsonism. GOODRICK.C. L. (1972). Learning by mature Neurology 16, 337-344. DYCK, P. J.. SCHULTZ. P. W. and O'BRIEN.. young and aged Wistar albino rats as a P. C. (1972). Quantitation of touch pressure BRUNK, U. and ERICCSON, J. L. E. (1972). function of test complexity. Journal of sensation. Archives of Neurology 26, 465- Electron microscopic studies on rat brain Gerontology 27. 353-357. 473. neurons. Localization of acid phosphatase GOVONI. S.. LODDO. P.. SPANO. R. F. and and mode of formation of lipofuscin bodies. EBER, J. T. (1976). Age differences in learning TRABUCCHI, M. (1977). Dopamine Journal of Ultrastructure Research 38, 1-38. and memory on a digit symbol substitution receptor sensitivity in brain and retina of rats CHEN.J.C. WARSHAW.J. B.andSANADl. task. Experimental Aging Research 2.45-53. during aging. Brain Research 138, 565-570. D. R. (1972). Regulation of mitochondrial ERMINI, M. SZELENYI, J„ MOSER, P. and GRUZEL1ER, J. H., LYKKEN, D. T. and respiration in senescence. Journal of VERZAR, F. (1971). The aging of skeletal VENABLES, P. H. (1972). Schizophrenia Cellular & Comparative Physiology 80, 141- (striated) muscle by changes of recovery and arousal revisited. Archives of General 148. metabolism. Gerontologia 17, 300-311. Psychiatry 26. 427-432.

422-NOVEMBER 1978 The Senescent Brain Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 LE JOURNAL CANADIEN DES SCIENCES NEUROLOGIQUES

GUTMANN, E. and HANZLIKOVA, V. (1972). KLAUSER, D. H. and KLEIM, D. M. (1978). MALAMUD, N. (1972). Neuropathology of Basic mechanisms of aging in the neuro­ Age differences in processing relevant organic bain syndromes associated with muscular system. Mechanisms of Aging and stimuli in multiple-item recognition aging. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging and the Development. I, 327-349. learning. Journal of Gerontology 33, 87-93. Brain. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 63-88. GUTMANN, E. and HANZLIKOVA, V. (1976). KLEIN, A. W. and MICHEL, M. E. (1977). MATSUYAMA. H.. NAM1KI, H. and WA- Fast and slow motor units in . A morphometric study of the neocortex of TANABE. I. (1966). Senile changes in the Gertontology 22, 280-300. young adult and old maze — differentiated brain in the Japanese. Incidence of rats. Mechanisms of Ageing and Develop­ GYGAX, P., WIERNSPERGER, N„ ME1ER- Alzheimer's neurofibrillary change and RUGE, W. and BAUMANN, T. (1978). ment 6, 441-452. senile plaques. In International Congress of Effect of Papaverine and dihydroergotoxine KLINE, D. W., CULLER, M. P. and SUCEC, Neuropathology, 5th, Zurich, edited by F. mesylate on cerebral microflow, EEG and J. (1977). Differences in inconspicuous word Luthy and A. Bischoff. Amsterdam, p02 in oligemic hypotension. Gerontology identification as a function of age and Excerpta Medica Foundation, pp. 979-980. 24 Suppl. I, 14-22. reversible — figure training. Experimental Aging Research 3, 203-213. MCFARLAND, R. A., WARREN, B. and HAGBERG, B. and INGVAR, D. H. (1976). KAVIS, C. (1958). Alterations in critical Cognitive reduction in presenile dementia KLINE, D. W. and NESTOR, S. (1977). The flicker frequency as a function of age and related to regional abnormalities of the persistence of complementary after images light: dark ratio. Journal of Experimental cerebral blood flow. British Journal of as a function of adult age and exposure Psychology 56, 529-538. Psychiatry 128, 209-222. deviation. Experimental Aging Research 3, 191-201. MCGEER, P. L, MCGEER, E. G. and HARK1NS, S. W. and CHAPMAN, R. (1976). SUZUKI, J. S. (1977). Aging and KLINE, D. W. and ORME-ROGERS, C. Detection and decision factors in pain extrapyramidal function. Archives of (1978). Examination of stimulus persistence perception in young and elderly men. Pain 2, Neurology 34, 33-35. 253-264. as the basis for superior visual identification performance among older adults. Journal of MEIER-RUGE, W. (1975). Experimental HEATH, A. and ORBACH, J. (1963). Reversi­ Gerontology 33, 76-81. pathology and pharmacology in brain bility of the necker cube: IV Responses of research and aging. Life Sciences 17, 1627- elderly people. Perceptual and Motor Skills KLINE, D. W. and SZAFRAN, J. (1975). Age 1636. 17, 625-626. differences in backward monoptic visual noise masking. Journal of Gerontology 30, MERGLER.N. L, DUSIK, J. B.and HOYER, HEGLIN, H. J. (1956). Problem solving set in 307-311. different age groups. Journal of Gerontology W. J. (1977). Central/incidental recall and II, 310-317. KOKMEN, E., BOSSEMEYER, R. W., JR. and selective in young and elderly WILLIAMS, W. J. (1978). Quantitative adults. Experimental Aging Research 3, 49- HIRANO, A. (1970). Neurofibrillary changes evaluation of joint motion sensation in an 60. in conditions related to Alzheimer's disease. aging population. Journal of Gerontology MILLER, C. A. and LEVINE. E. M. (1974). In Alzheimer's Disease and Related 33, 62-67. Conditions, edited by G. E. W. Wolsten- Effects of aluminum salts on cultured neuro­ holme and M. O'Connor, London, Chur­ KOLB, L. C. (1968). Noyes' Modern Clinical blastoma cells. Journal of Neurochcmistry chill, pp. 185-200. Psychiatry, Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders, 22, 751-758. pp. 250-258. HOFFBRAND, B. 1„ MACLAY, W. P. and MILNER, B. (1970). Memory and the medial TURNER, P. (1976). Ergot compounds — KRAMER, S. G. (1971). Dopamine: a retinal temporal regions of the brain. In Biology of the changing scence. Post-graduate Medical neurotransmitter. I. Retinal uptake, storage Memory, edited by K. H. Pribram and D. E. Journal 52, Suppl. 1, 1-62. and light-stimulated release of ['H] Broadbent, New York, Academic Press, pp. dopamine in vivo. Investigative Ophthal­ 29-50. HOYER, W. J., LABOUVIE, G. V. and mology 10, 438-445. BALTES, P. B. (1973). Modification of re­ MISIAK, H. (1951). The decrease of critical sponse speed deficits and intellectual KVETNANSKY, R., JAHNOVA, E..TORDA, flicker frequency with age. Science I 13, 551- performance in the elderly. Human T., STRBAK, V., BALAZ, V. and 552. Development 16, 233-242. MACHO, L. (1978). Changes of adrenal MUNDY-CASTLE, A. C. (1953). An analysis catecholamines and their synthesizing en­ of central responses to photic stimulation in HULICKA, 1. M. (1965). Age differences for zymes during ontogenesis and aging in rats. intentional and incidental learningand recall normal adults. Electroencephalography & Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 7, Clinical Neurophysiology 5. 1-22. scores. Journal of the American Geriatrics 209-216. Society 13, 639-684. OBR1ST, W. D..CHIV1AN, E., CRONQVIST, LANDF1ELD, P. W. and LYNCH, G. S. (1977). S. and INGVAR, D. H. (1970). Regional HUZIKER, O., ABDEL'AL, S., FREY, H„ Brain aging and plasma steroid levels: cerebral blood flow in senile and presenile VETEAU, M-J and ME1ER-RUGE, W. quantitative correlations. Neuroscience dementia. Neurology 20, 315-322. (1978). Quantitative studies in the cerebral Abstracts 3, 111. cortex of aging humans. Gerontology 24, OHARA, P. T. (1972). Electron microscropial 27-31. LASSEN, N. A., FE1NBERG, I. and LANE, study of the brain in Down's syndrome. M. H. (I960). Bilateral studies of cerebral Brain 95, 681-684. INGVAR, D. H. and LASSEN, N. A. (1970). oxygen uptake in young and aged normal Cerebral blood flow and cerebral metabo­ subjects and in patients with organic de­ OOKA, H., SEGALL, P. E. and TIM1RAS, lism. Triangle 9, 234-243. mentia. Journal of Clinical Investigation 39, P. S. (1978). Neural and endocrine 491-500. development after chronic tryptophan JONEC, V. and FINCH, C. E. (1975). Aging deficiency in rats. II. Pituitary-thyroid axis. and dopamine uptake by subcellular frac­ LEE, K. S., ETGEN, A. M. and LYNCH, G. S. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 7, tions of the C57I3L/6J male mouse brain. (1977). Corticosterone modification of the 19-24. Brain Research 91, 197-215. synthesis of specific proteins in the central nervous system. Neuroscience Abstracts 3, ORGEL, L. E. (1963). The maintenance of the KENT, S. (1977). Classifying and treating 350. accuracy of protein synthesis and its organic brain syndromes. Geriatrics 32, 87- relevance to aging. Proceedings of the MACHADO-SALAS, J., SCHEIBEL, M. E. 89, 93-96. National Academy Sciences 49, 517-521. and SCHEIBEL, A. G. (1977). Neuronal KIMBLE, D. P. (1968). Hippocampus and changes in the aging mouse; spinal cord and PATCH, M. S. (1977). Age-dependent changes internal inhibition. Psychological Bulletin lower brain stem. Experimental Neurology in the oxidative metabolism in rat brain. 70, 285-295. 54, 504-512. Journal of Gerontology 32, 643-646.

Beck NOVEMBER 1978-423 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203 THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGICAL SCIENCES

PERRY, E. K„ PERRY, R. H.,GIBSON, P. H., SCHEIBEL, M. E., TOMIYASU, U. and phosphatase histochemistry and electron BLESSED, G. and TOML1NSON, B. E. SCHEIBEL, A. B. (1976). Dendritic changes probe analysis. Journal of Neuropathology (1977). A cholinergic connection between in the aging human rhinencephalon. and Experimental Neurology 24, 200-210. normal aging and senile dementia in the Transactions of the American Neurological TONNA, E. A. and SINGH. I. J. (1976). An human hippocampus. Neuroscience Letters Association 101, 23-26. autoradiographic investigation of -'H- 6, 85-89. SCHNEIDER, N. G., GR1TZ, E. R. and Uridine utilization by aging mouse cartilage PHILLIPS, A. G. and F1BIGER, H. C. (1973). JARVIK, M. E. (1975). Age differences in cells. Experimental Gerontology II, 231- Dopaminergic and noradrenergic substrates learning, immediate and one week delayed 241. of postive reinforcement: Different effects of recall. Gerontologia 21, 10-20. UNGERSTEDT, W. (1971). Mechanism of D- and L-amphetamine. Science 179, 575- SEGALL. P. E., OOK.A, H., ROSE. K. and action of L-DOPA studied in an experimen­ 577. TIMIRAS, P. S. (1978). Neural and tal Parkinson model. In Monoamines, Noyaux Gris Centraux et Syndrome de PLEMONS. J. K., WILLIS,S. Land BALTES, endocrine development after chronic Parkinson, edited by J. de Ajuriaguerra and P. B. (1978). Modifiability of fluid trypotphan deficiency in rats: I. Brain G. Gauthier, Paris, Masson and Georg. pp. intelligence in aging: A short-term monomine and ptituitary responses. 165-170. longtitudinal training approach. Journal of Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 7, Gerontology 33, 224-231. 1-17. VICTOR, M., ADAMS, R. D. and COLLINS, SHADER, R. 1., HARMATZ, J. S. and G. H. (1971). The Wernicke-Korsakoff PLOSKI, H. A., LEVITA, E. and R1KLAN, M. SALTZMAN, C. (1974). A new scale for Syndrome, Philadelphia, R. A. David, pp. (1966). Impairment of voluntary movement clinical assessment in geriatric patients: 71-135. in parkinson's disease in relation to Sandoz clinical assessment in geriatrics VIJ AYAN, V. K. (1977). Cholinergic enzymes in activation level autonomic malfunction and (SCAG). Journal of the American Geriatric the cerebellum and the hippocampus of the personal rigidity. Psychosomatic Medicine Society 22, 107-113. senescent mouse. Experimental Gerontol­ 28, 70-77. SIMARD, D. (1971). Regional blood and its ogy, 12, 7-11. RABB1TT, P. (1965). An age-decrement in the regulation in dementia. Brain 94, 273-288. VR1ES, H. A., DE (1971). Exercise intensity ability to ignore irrelevant information. threshold for improvement of cardiovascular- Journal of Gerontology 20, 233-238. SOTGIU G., CUSSINI, G., CHIER1CI, F. and VANCINI, B. (I960). Senile adrenal respiratory function in older men. Geriatrics READING, V. (1968). Disability glare and age. hyper-medullism. Panminerva Medicine 2, 26, 94-101. Vision Research 8, 207-214. 1-5. WEISS, A. D. (1959). Sensory functions. In Handbook of Aging and the Individual, RETZLAFF, E. and FONTAINE, J. (1965). SPANO, P. F., KUMAKURA, K., TONON, edited by J. E. Birren, Chicago, University of Functional and structural changes in motor G. C, GOVONI, S. and TRABUCCHI, M. Chicago Press, pp. 503-542. neurons with age. In Behavior, Aging and (1975). LSD and dopamine sensitive the Nervous Systems, Springfield, Illinois, adenylate cylase in various rat brain areas. WHANGER, A. D. and WANG, H. S. (1974). C. C. Thomas, pp. 340-352. Brain research 93, 164-167. Clinical correlates of vibratory sense in the elderly psychiatric patients. Journal of RIKLAN, M., LEVITA, E. and M1S1AK, H. SPANO, P. F. and TRABUCCHI. M. (1978). Gerontology 29, 39-45. (1970). Critical flicker frequency and Interaction of ergot alkaloids with integrative functions in parkinsonism. dopaminergic receptors in the rat striatum WHITE, P., GOODHARDT, M. J., KEET, J. P., H1LEY, C. R.. CARRASCO, L. H„ Journal of Psychology 75, 45-51. and nucleus accumbens. Gerontology 24, Suppl. I, 106-114. WILLIAMS, I. E. I. and BOWEN, D. M. ROBERTS, J., GOLDBERG, P. B. and BASKIN, (1977). Neocortical cholinergic neurons in S. I. (1976). Biochemical changes in the SPILLANE. J. A., WHITE P., GOODH ARDT, elderly people. Lancet, 1,668-671. central nervous system with age in the rat. M. J., FLACK, R. H. A., BOWEN, D. M. WILLIAMS, M. (1970). The psychological Experimental Aging Research 2, 61-74. and DAVISON, A. N. (1977). Selective assessment of geriatric patients. In the vulnerability of neurons in organic ROBINSON, D. S., SOURKES, T. L, NIES, Psychological Assessment of Mental and dementia. Nature 266, 558-559. A., HARRIS, L S., SPECTOR, S., Physical Handicaps edited by P. J. Mittler, BARTLETT, D. L. and KAYE, I. S. (1977). SPIRDUSCO, W. W. (1975). Reaction and London, Methuen, pp. 319-339. Monoamine metabolism in human brain. movement time as a function of age and WINN, F. J. and ELIAS, J. W. (1977). Age, Archives of General Psychology 34, 89-92. physical activity level. Journal of Gerontol­ rate and instructional conditions: empirical ogy 30. 435-440. ROSS, M. H. and LUSTBADER, E. (1976). support against the pacing variable. Dietary practices and growth responses as SP1RDUSO, W. W. and CLIFFORD, P. (1978). Experimental Aging Research 3. 305-324. Replication of age physical activity effects predictors of longevity. Nature 262, 548-553. W1SN1EWSKI. H. M. and TERRY, R. D. on reaction and movement time. Journal of (1976). Neuropathology of the aging brain. SACCUZZO, D. P., H1RT, M. and SPENCER, Gerontology 33, 26-30. T. (1974). Backward masking as a measure of In Neurobiology of Aging, edited by R. D. attention in schizophrenics. Journal of STEIN, D. G. and F1RL, A. C. (1976). Brain Terry and S. Gershon, New York, Raven Abnormal Psychology 83, 512-522. damage and reorganization of function in Press, pp. 265-280. old age. Experimental Neurology 52, 157- SALTHOUSE, T. A. (1978). The role of memory WOLF, E. (I960). Glare and age. Archives of 167. Ophthalmology 64, 502-514. in the age decline in digit-symbol SUN, A. Y. (1976). Aging and in vivo norepine- YATES, A. J. (1966). Data processing levels substitution performance. Journal of phrine-uptake in mammalian brain. and thought disorder in schizophrenics. Gerontology 33, 232-238. Experimental Aging Research 2, 207-220. Australian Journal of Psychology 18, 103- SAMORAJSK1, T and ROLSTEN, C. (1976). SUN, A. Y. and SAMORAJSK1, T. (1975). The 107. Chlorpromazine and aging in the brain. effects of age and alcohol on Na-K ATPase Experimental Gerontology II, 141-147. YATES, A. J. (1966). Data processing levels activity of the whole homogenate and and thought disorder in schizophrenics. SCHAIE, K. W., LAVOUV1E, G. V. and synaptosomes prepared from mouse and Australian Journal of Psychology 18, 103- BUECH, B. V. (1973). Generational and human brain. Journal of Neurochemistry 24, 107. 161-164. cohort-specific differences in adult cognitive YATES. A. J. and KORBOOT P. (1970). Speed functioning. Developmental Psychology 9, SUN, A. Y. and SEAMAN, R. N. (1977). The of perceptual functioning in chronic 3 151-166. effect of aging on synaptosomal (Ca *) nonparanoid schizophrenics. Journal of SCHE1BEL. M. E., LINDSAY, R. D., transport in the brain. Experimental Aging Abnormal Psychology 76, 453-461. TOMIYASU, U. and SCHEIBEL, A. B. Research 3, 107-116. ZEL1NSKI, E. M„ WALSH, D. A. and (1975). Progressive dendritic changes in TERRY, R. D. and PENA, C. (1965). Experi­ THOMPSON, L. W. (1978). Orienting task aging human cortex. Experimental Neurol­ mental production of neurofibrillary effects on EDR and in three age ogy 47, 392-403. degeneration. 2. Electron microscopy. groups. Journal of Gerontology 33, 239-245.

424-NOVEMBER 1978 The Senescent Brain Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.33.19, on 28 Sep 2021 at 20:45:14, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0317167100024203