<<

MENTAL RETARDATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES RESEARCH REVIEWS 7: 200–210 (2001)

INFECTIOUS AND IMMUNE FACTORS IN THE OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS:EPIDEMIOLOGY,HYPOTHESES, AND ANIMAL MODELS

Mady Hornig and W. Ian Lipkin* Emerging Laboratory, Gillespie Neuroscience Research Facility, University of California, Irvine, California

Both genetic and environmental factors contribute to the pathogen- Perinatal central nervous system (CNS) [Chess, esis of a wide variety of neurodevelopmental disorders, including , 1971; Gillberg and Gillberg, 1983; Hoon and Reiss, 1992; Barak mental retardation, and . Some heritable disorders approach 100% penetrance; nonetheless, even in these disorders, subtle aspects of et al., 1999] and disturbed neuroimmune networks [Warren et clinical expression may be influenced by the environment. In other al., 1986; 1987; 1990; 1991; 1994; 1995; Singh et al., 1991; disorders with genetic influences, exogenous factors, and the timepoint(s) 1993; 1997a,b; 1998; Singh, 1996; 1997; Burger and Warren, during nervous system development at which they are introduced, modu- 1998; Warren, 1998] have been proposed as factors in the late expression of disease. Elucidation of the mechanisms guiding this intri- cate interplay between host response genes, environmental agents, and the pathogenesis of disorders. An immune or in- neurodevelopmental context within which these interactions occur, is nec- fectious basis for autism is supported by epidemiologic studies essary to understand the continuum of clinical outcomes. This chapter will suggesting an increased rate of autism in particular geographic review the evidence that infectious and immune factors may contribute to regions [Gillberg et al., 1991; Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; De- the pathogenesis of neurodevelopmental disorders, describe an animal model of neurodevelopmental disorders based upon viral infection, identify partment of Developmental Services, 1999; Gillberg and Wing, processes by which neural circuitry may be compromised, and outline areas 1999], season-of-birth effects [Bartlik, 1981; Kostantareas et al., for future research. © 2001 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 1986; Burd, 1988; Tanoue et al., 1988; Atlas, 1989; Gillberg, MRDD Research Reviews 2001;7:200–210. 1990; Bolton et al., 1992; Mouridsen et al., 1994; Barak et al., 1995; 1999; Ticher et al., 1996; Torrey et al., 1997; Stevens et Key Words: neurodevelopmental disorders; autism; ; immune; Borna al., 2000], and linkage to viral and/or immune factors [Chess, disease virus; animal models 1971; Warren et al., 1986; 1987; 1990; 1991; 1994; 1995; Singh et al., 1991; 1993; 1997a,b; 1998; Singh, 1996; 1997; Burger and Warren, 1998; Warren, 1998; Stevens et al., 2000]. Heri- table predisposing factors may include immunologically relevant o established animal model for autism exists that ade- influences such as linkages to major histocompatibility complex quately addresses autism’s: 1) likely perinatal origins (MHC) genes [Warren et al., 1992; 1996a,b; Warren and Singh, N[Gillberg and Gillberg, 1983; Hoon and Reiss, 1992; 1996; Warren, 1998]; increased frequency of the null allele of Bauman and Kemper, 1994]; 2) linkage to viral and/or immune the complement component 4b locus, located in the MHC factors [Chess, 1971; Warren et al., 1986; Singh et al., 1991; complex [Warren et al., 1991]; and increased frequency of a 1993; 1997a,b; Singh, 1996; 1997; Warren, 1998]; 3) association family history of autoimmune disorders [Comi et al., 1999]. with hippocampal, amygdalar, and cerebellar dysfunction [Bau- Animal models and the , then, suggest man and Kemper, 1994]; 4) connection with dopamine and that genetic and environmental components work in concert to serotonin disturbances [Cook, 1990; Anderson, 1994; Cook and cause disease. Frustration with the ability to move beyond Leventhal, 1996; Cook et al., 1997]; and 5) wide spectrum of simple allelic associations to genetic causation has led to models neurobehavioral derangements (motor, postural, and sensory based on interactions of multiple genes, and reintroduction of deficits; hypotonia; stereotypies; poor eye contact; mental retar- the environment into the nature-nurture equation. Although dation; islands of normal to supernormal functioning) superim- controversial, reports that the rate of autism spectrum disorders posed on impairments in social interaction, communication, and behavior [Wing, 1997]. Additionally, although perinatal expo- sure to infectious agents and toxins is linked to the pathogenesis Grant sponsor: National Institutes of Health; Grant numbers: NS29425, MH57467, HD37546, MH01608; Grant sponsor: the MIND Institute of UC Davis; Grant of neuropsychiatric disorders, the mechanisms by which envi- sponsor: the CAN Foundation; Grant sponsor: the Association. ronmental triggers interact with developing immune and neural *Correspondence to: W. Ian Lipkin or Mady Hornig, Emerging Diseases Laboratory, 3101 GNRF, University of California, Irvine, CA, 92697-4292. elements to create neurodevelopmental disturbances are poorly E-mail: [email protected], [email protected] understood.

© 2001 Wiley-Liss, Inc. may be increasing [Gillberg et al., 1991; tion. Alternatively, tissue damage and or a virally-triggered autoimmune dia- Department of Developmental Services, disease may be the indirect result of a thesis. Only limited conclusions may be 1999; Gillberg and Wing, 1999], and that host immune response to microbial gene drawn from these studies, as they are this increase is restricted to certain geo- products present in neural cells. Immune based on small, incompletely character- graphic regions [Baron-Cohen et al., responses to microbial agents can also ized, heterogeneous populations differing 1999; Department of Developmental lead to breakdown of tolerance to host in etiology and course, and often diag- Services, 1999; Gillberg and Wing, antigens and result in tissue damage nosed through superficial screening using 1999], provide further support for envi- [Zhao et al., 1998]. The agent responsi- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) ronmental factors in the pathogenesis of ble for induction of autoimmunity need criteria alone, without use of standard- these disorders. not be present in CNS at the time of ized diagnostic instruments such as the These factors may include perinatal clinical presentation. Furthermore, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised central nervous system (CNS) infection original infection may have been periph- (ADI-R) or Autism Diagnostic Observa- [Desmond et al., 1967; Chess, 1971; eral, as is the case in Sydenham’s chorea, tion Schedule (ADOS). Occasionally, Blattner, 1974; Peterson and Torrey, or as is proposed for and obsessions similar immune disturbances are noted in 1976; Chess, 1977; DeLong et al., 1981; following streptococcal infection. Yet first degree relatives or neurologic or Schwab, 1982; Gillberg and Gillberg, another mechanism for brain disease is healthy controls; however, in most stud- 1983; Markowitz, 1983; Ivarsson et al., persistent noncytopathic viral infection. ies, comparison groups are not screened 1990; Mason-Brothers et al., 1990; Ritvo Such can profoundly impact for presence of autism spectrum disorder et al., 1990; Gillberg and Coleman, 1992; neurotransmitter function or brain devel- symptoms or disorder. Controls for such Hoon and Reiss 1992; Tepper et al., opment, yet remain cryptic unless spe- factors are time of blood collection, se- 1998; Barak et al., 1999] and toxins cific reagents are used for detecting viral rum/plasma vs. intracellular sources of [Anonymous, 1999; Rice and Barone, gene products [Lipkin et al., 1988a,b; cytokines, medications, age, IQ, psycho- 2000; Weiss and Landrigan, 2000]. Oldstone 1989a,b,c]. Diverse neuropsy- social factors, immunization and infec- chiatric outcomes may even result from tious disease history, or family history of EVIDENCE TO SUPPORT A infection with a single agent, depending immune-based disorders. Findings of im- ROLE FOR VIRAL INFECTION on: 1) timing of infection relative to the mune disturbances in peripheral blood IN THE PATHOGENESIS OF status of neural or immune systems (in specimens have generally not been as- NEURODEVELOPMENTAL utero, juvenile, adult); 2) genetic context sessed or corroborated in cerebrospinal DISORDERS (e.g., MHC antigens determining im- fluid (CSF) samples and immune-medi- The implication of a role for pre- mune response and subsequent neuropa- ated mechanisms have not been defini- natal and/or postnatal viral infections in thology); and/or 3) other environmental tively supported by or brain im- the etiology of autism stems from retro- factors (infectious agents, toxins, psycho- aging studies. Nonetheless, although the spective epidemiologic studies of in utero social stress). degree of convergence of epidemiologic, or perinatal viral exposures, of seasonal Despite a finding of season-of- genetic, immunologic, and biochemical and geographic factors, and, more indi- birth effects in several studies [Bartlik, findings across or within subsets of chil- rectly, from studies of immune aberra- 1981; Burd, 1988; Gillberg, 1990; Barak dren with autism spectrum disorders is as tions in the blood of children with autism et al., 1995; 1999; Ticher et al., 1996; yet insufficient to warrant firm conclu- [Mason-Brothers et al., 1990; Gillberg Stevens et al., 2000]—many of which sions regarding pathogenesis, the data and Coleman, 1992; Delgiudice-Asch suggest a preponderance of March provide intriguing clues about potential and Hollander, 1997; Warren, 1998; and/or August births or a second trimes- mechanisms that may contribute to neu- Trottier et al., 1999]. A number of con- ter pathogen exposure amongst children ral injury. genital microbial exposures have been re- with autism—other studies have not Several reports are consistent with ported in association with autistic fea- confirmed this link [Landau et al., 1999]. diminished Th1 and increased Th2 re- tures, including rubella virus [Desmond Even if a microbial link is confirmed for sponses in autism: 1) mitogen-stimulated et al., 1967; Chess, 1971; Deykin and a subset of children with autism, other T proliferation is decreased in some MacMahon, 1979], cytomegalovirus factors may also vary seasonally, (e.g., [Stubbs and Crawford 1977; Warren et [Blattner, 1974; Stubbs and Crawford, nutritional differences) and influence ex- al., 1986], but not all [Ferrari et al., 1988] 1977; Markowitz, 1983; Stubbs et al., pression of disease [Gillberg and studies; 2) Th1 (IFN-␥ϩ CD4ϩ and 1984; Ivarsson et al., 1990; Ritvo et al., Coleman, 1992]. IL-2ϩ CD4ϩ cells) and TC1 (IFN-␥ ϩ 1990], herpes simplex virus [Peterson and Studies of immunologic function CD8ϩ and IL-2ϩ CD8ϩ cells) T cells are Torrey 1976; DeLong et al., 1981; Ritvo in children with autism reveal a wide reduced, and Th2 (IL-4ϩ CD4ϩ cells) et al., 1990], varicella zoster virus [Stubbs array of abnormalities, including de- and TC2 (IL-4ϩ CD8ϩ cells) T cells are and Crawford 1977; Deykin and Mac- creased cellular immune capacity [War- reportedly increased [Gupta et al., 1998]; Mahon, 1979], enteroviruses [Sells et al., ren et al., 1986; 1990; Wright et al., 3) natural killer cell activity is reduced 1975], human immunodeficiency virus 1990; Yonk et al., 1990; Denney et al., [Warren et al., 1987], despite unchanged [Tepper et al., 1998], and syphilis [Stubbs 1996]; decreased plasma complement numbers of natural killer cells [Warren et and Crawford, 1977; Schwab, 1982]. component C4b [Warren et al., 1994; al., 1990]; and 4) increased levels of se- Establishing a causal relationship 1995]; and increased humoral immune rum IgE [Gupta et al., 1996; Trottier et between infection with a microbial agent and autoantibody responses [Weizman et al., 1999]. Furthermore, as Th2 cells are and a specific brain disease can be com- al., 1982; Singh et al., 1993]. These ab- implicated in systemic (nonorgan spe- plex [Lipkin and Hornig, 1998]. In some normalities provide general support for cific) autoimmune disorders [De Carli et instances, for example, herpes simplex the hypothesis that children with autism al., 1994; Singh et al., 1999], the in- encephalitis, the agent is readily impli- may be predisposed to respond abnor- creased autoantibody production [Todd cated: the virus is present in brain and mally to viral infections either through and Ciaranello, 1985; Todd et al., 1988; destroys infected tissue through replica- the establishment of persistent infections Plioplys et al., 1989a,b; Yuwiler et al.,

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN 201 1992; Singh et al., 1993, 1997a,b; 1998; cules, but negative for IL-2 receptor or NEONATAL BORNAVIRUS Connolly et al., 1999] and family history CD25) in autism [Plioplys et al., 1994], INFECTION OF LEWIS RATS: of autoimmune disorders [Money et al., consistent with the pattern seen in several AN ENVIRONMENTAL MODEL 1971; Raiten and Massaro, 1986; Gill- autoimmune disorders [Burmester et al., OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL berg et al., 1992; Comi et al., 1999] 1984; Hafler et al., 1985; Bergroth et al., DAMAGE reported for children with autism lend 1988], levels of DRϩ IL2Ϫ T cells in Borna disease virus, an atypical, further support to the idea that a Th2 children with autism are inversely corre- neurotropic, noncytolytic, negative- predominant immune response may play lated with plasma levels of C4b [Warren strand RNA virus, is tropic for limbic a role in autism pathogenesis. In contrast, et al., 1995]. The DR beta 1 gene is and cerebellar circuitry. Infection causes Singh reports increased plasma IL-12 and located in close proximity to the C4b a spectrum of behavioral deficits depend- IFN-␥ levels in autism [1996], findings gene on the HLA region of chromosome ing on the age, immune status, central more consistent with Th1 than Th2- 6, and is also near to genes encoding IgA nervous system maturity, and of weighted responses. Lastly, a preliminary and 21-hydroxylase (class III region) the host. In immunocompetent adult report by Nelson and colleagues indicates [Wilton et al., 1985; Brai et al., 1994; Lewis rats, infection results in meningo- encephalitis, diffuse central nervous sys- a lower level of autoantibodies to MBP, Fiore et al., 1995; Reil et al., 1997; Schr- tem damage, dopamine neurotransmitter GFAP, and NAFP at birth in children oeder et al., 1998]. IgA deficiency, also disturbances, and disorders of movement with autism or mental retardation than in noted in autism [Warren et al., 1997], is and behavior [Solbrig et al., 1994]. Neo- normal controls [Nelson et al., 2000]. associated with the presence of autoanti- natally infected Lewis rats have only min- These apparent inconsistencies in levels bodies [Brai et al., 1994; Fiore et al., imal, transient inflammation [Hornig et of autoantibodies in children with autism 1995] and an increased incidence of overt al., 1999] but nonetheless have abnor- might be explained by the difference in autoimmune disease [Barka et al., 1995]. malities of hippocampal and cerebellar time of sampling, with later timepoints Some DR beta 1 alleles reportedly have a development [Hornig et al., 1999], reflecting a break in immune tolerance. very strong association with autism growth [Bautista et al., 1994], play be- It is conceivable that there is a link [Warren et al., 1992; Daniels et al., 1995; havior [Pletnikov et al., 1999a], emo- between susceptibility to infection with Warren et al., 1996a, b], although one tional reactivity [Hornig et al., 1999], virus and HHV-6 and an auto- larger study of multiplex sibships with socioemotional communication (prelim- immune diathesis. The observation that autism did not confirm this result [Rog- inary data, Hornig and Lipkin), spatial the CD46 receptor binds complement ers et al., 1999]. Given the presumed and aversive learning, [Dittrich et al., proteins C3b and C4b is interesting in heterogeneity of the disorder, and the 1989], locomotor activity [Hornig et al., light of reports of decreased plasma levels possibility that genetic loading in families 1999; 2000], and taste preferences [Bau- of C4b [Warren et al., 1994; 1995] and with multiple affected members may be tista et al., 1994]. These abnormalities increases in the null allele of the C4b less likely to require exposure to an en- bear obvious similarities to the impaired gene [Warren et al., 1991] in autism. The vironmental factor (e.g., virus, bacteria, social interaction and atypical responses C4b gene is located in the MHC on toxin, or other agent) in order to lead to to sensory and emotional stimuli patho- chromosome 6; partial C4 deficiency and the autistic phenotype, the absence of gnomonic of autism. the C4b null allele are associated with an linkage in this one study does not rule Neonatal rat infection presents an increased susceptibility to a variety of au- out a role for an HLA-linked immuno- intriguing model for neuropsychiatric ill- toimmune diseases [Brai et al., 1994; Ul- genetic vulnerability in a subset of chil- ness; its immunopathologic correlates are giati and Abraham, 1996; Naves et al., dren with autism. Immunogenetic stud- more subtle and the cerebellar and hip- 1998; Kawano et al., 1999]. CD46 re- ies that compare subpopulations with and pocampal dysgenesis observed is consis- ceptor, in addition to serving as the entry without evidence of immune dysfunc- tent with the neurodevelopmental ab- site for vaccine strains of measles [Tatsuo tion will be required to address this pos- normalities reported in autism [Kemper et al., 2000], is also the entry site for sibility. and Bauman, 1993], schizophrenia [Alt- HHV-6 [Santoro et al., 1999; Clark, Expression of complex neuropsy- shuler et al., 1987; Fish et al., 1992], and 2000]. Increased levels of antibodies to chiatric diseases such as autism may re- affective disorders [Soares and Mann, 1997]. Close parallels exist amongst the HHV-6 and measles are positively asso- quire the presence of specific genes, an core and associated features of these psy- ciated with peripheral autoantibodies to environmental trigger, and exposure at a chiatric disorders and the wide range of CNS antigens in children with autism particular time during brain develop- physiologic and neurobehavioral distur- [Singh et al., 1998]; studies have not yet ment; all factors, genetic and environ- bances described in neonatally infected been reported regarding such associations mental, must be reconstructed within a animals; in particular, the overlap of signs with autoantibodies in cerebrospinal neurodevelopmental context. Thus, de- of autism spectrum disorders with ele- fluid. velopmental differences in host inflam- ments of the neonatal syndrome is espe- Alterations in T cell subsets may matory and neuroendocrine capacities cially striking. also fit with the hypothesis that increased and in rates of maturation of nervous and Abnormal growth and physiologic susceptibility to specific types of viral in- immune system elements [Rubin et al., profiles have been noted, although the fections may be mediated by regulation 1999a] are likely to contribute to the mechanisms contributing to these distur- of virus-specific receptors and of Th1 vs. differential susceptibility of neuronal and bances are not known. Neonatally in- Th2 immunity, and may provide a link glial populations to pre- or postnatal in- fected animals are stunted in growth to development of CNS-directed auto- flammatory stressors (infectious, immune) compared to uninfected littermates as immune responses in autism. In conjunc- and impact the phenotypic expression of early as day 4 postinfection [Bautista et tion with reports of an increase in T cells autism and other neurodevelopmental al., 1994; Carbone et al., 1991; Hornig et expressing a “late activated” pattern (i.e., disorders [Briese et al., 1999; Hornig et al., 1999] without demonstrable alter- positive for DR or class II MHC mole- al., 1999]. ations of , growth hormone, in-

202 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN sulin-like growth factor-1 [Bautista et al., coding for IL-1␤ have been noted by bright light). Other indices of disturbed 1994], or amount of food ingested [Bau- several investigators following neonatal emotional reactivity in infected rats, in- tista et al., 1995]. They also display al- infection of Lewis rats [Hornig et al., cluding increased defecation in the tered sleep-wake cycles and heightened 1999; Plata-Salama´n et al., 1999; Sauder brightly lit open field, were consistent taste preferences for salt-containing solu- and de la Torre, 1999], suggesting the with this hypothesis of increased fearful- tions [Bautista et al., 1994]. Given re- possibility that cytokines may contribute ness and drive to avoid aversive stimuli. ported alterations in other neuropeptide to sleep-wake cycle abnormalities in this Indeed, the more traditional measures of systems following neonatal infection of model as well. The potential contribu- anxiety that were measured in this study, Lewis rats [Plata-Salama´n et al., 1999], it tion of dysregulation of autocoids and freezing and thigmotaxis, were actually is intriguing to speculate that disturbed temperature regulation to the pathogen- decreased in infected animals compared salt preferences might result from dys- esis of the neonatal syndrome remains to to controls [Pletnikov et al., 1999b]; regulation of the neuropeptide arginine be studied. Of note, many children with however, these measures are less reliable vasopressin, either through direct infec- autism demonstrate abnormal taste pref- as measures of anxiety at extremely high tion of magnocellular neurons of hypo- erences and sleep disturbances [Wing, levels, where fearfulness and flight re- thalamus or indirect effects on the mag- 1997]. sponses are induced [Davis and Shi, 1999; nocellular division through perturbations Persistent disturbances are also re- King, 1999]. of mineralocorticoid responses (including ported in the cognition, socioemotional Additionally, during acoustic star- potential effects of BDV on mineralocor- behavior and communication, and motor tle testing of neonatally infected animals, ticoid release by the adrenal glands, or on development of neonatally BDV-in- Pletnikov and colleagues found a disso- mineralocorticoid receptors in hypothal- fected animals. Cognitive deficits and ciation between the amplitude of the amus). Alternatively, BDV could influ- unusual emotional reactivity were first startle response and autonomic reactivity: ence salt taste preferences through effects described in Wistar rats, in which spatial whereas infected animals had a signifi- on gustatory neurocircuitry, either at the and aversive learning impairments were cantly lower startle response amplitude level of primary gustatory transduction, found along with increased motor activ- compared to sham-inoculated rats, and sensory transmission via the chorda tym- ity and decreased anxiety responses in both habituation and footshock sensitiza- pani branch of cranial nerve VII, or pro- open field testing under bright light con- tion of their startle responses demon- cessing of taste signals in the rostral por- ditions from 15 weeks postinfection [Dit- strated a normal pattern, autonomic re- tions of the nucleus of the solitary tract, trich et al., 1989]. Similar deficits in spa- sponses (defecation) were higher where primary gustatory nerves termi- tial learning and memory were more [Pletnikov et al., 1999b]. These findings nate for salt sensation [King et al., 1999]. recently confirmed in neonatally infected point to a possible disturbance in integra- A role for a higher order sensory process- Lewis rats 43 to 72 days postinfection, in tion of signals from neural circuits in- ing defect may be a more likely explana- association with the peak of dentate gyrus volved in higher order sensory processing tion than salt depletion or aberrant taste granule cell loss [Rubin et al., 1999b]. (glutamatergic afferents to or efferents discrimination capacity given that neona- Emotional responses of adult Lewis rats from a critical site in primary startle re- tally infected animals do not differ in the infected as neonates appear to depend sponse neurocircuitry, the caudal pontine amount of salt solution consumed in sin- both on testing conditions and age at reticular nucleus), anxiety/fear responses, gle bottle taste acceptance experiments testing. Dittrich et al. [1989] found loco- and autonomic system outputs. Although [Bautista et al., 1994]; nonetheless, the motor hyperactivity and absence of freez- the role of amygdala in fear-potentiation alternative hypotheses have yet to be ex- ing behavior in brightly lit open field of the classic motor aspects of startle re- amined by neurohormonal and serum os- testing of 4 month old Wistar rats in- sponses of infected animals appears to be molality assessments and detailed neuro- fected with BDV as neonates, a finding intact, exaggeration of responses of the pathologic analysis of neural systems which was interpreted as indicative of central nucleus of the amygdala to nor- regulating salt intake, including the nu- abnormally low anxiety responses. How- adrenergic, autonomic signals from locus cleus of the solitary tract. ever, based on a comparison of the aver- coeruleus may be faulty [Koch, 1999], Whether disturbances of circadian siveness of bright and dim illumination possibly accounting for the opposite ef- rhythms relate to direct or indirect effects conditions in 3 to 4 month old, neona- fects of neonatal infection on the motor of BDV infection is similarly unclear; tally infected and sham-inoculated ani- and autonomic components of the startle however, a wide variety of inflammatory mals, Pletnikov et al. [1996b] noted that response. The relationship of these ab- stressors are known to induce sleep-wake hyperactivity of infected animals was normalities of anxiety and fear responses disturbances through shifts in cytokines, greater in bright light conditions than in to developmental maturity and unfolding prostaglandins, and body temperature dim light, whereas the opposite pattern of CNS damage following neonatal in- [Dunn, 1993; Dunn and Swiergiel, 1998; pertained in the control animals. Al- fection was not reported. In this context, Wong et al., 1997]. In studies of the though freezing and thigmotaxic (“wall- we have reported a transient, abnormal retroviruses HIV [Opp et al., 1996] and seeking” behavior consistent with an response to novelty (inhibition of loco- feline immunodeficiency virus [Pros- attempt to escape from the testing ap- motor activity responses upon introduc- pero-Garcia et al., 1994] in rats, sleep paratus) behaviors were more evident in tion to novel environment) at 4 weeks architecture changes are associated with neonatally infected animals in bright light postinfection, when brain levels of the envelope glycoprotein (gp); in the as opposed to dim light open field testing, mRNAs for cytokines associated with case of HIV, intracerebroventricular ad- the mean time spent in freezing behavior decreased exploratory responses, such as ministration of gp 120 also increases during the testing period was consistently IL-1, [Dantzer et al., 1998], were at their mRNA expression for the cytokines, in- lower in infected than in control animals. peak [Hornig et al., 1999]; this observa- terleukin (IL)-1␤ and IL-10 in hypothal- The authors concluded that the increased tion is also consistent with abnormal amus, a brain region crucial for sleep motor activity in bright light represented amygdaloid responses. regulation [Opp et al., 1996]. Intrigu- hyperreactivity of neonatally infected an- Disturbances of complex socio- ingly, increased brain levels of mRNA imals to aversive stimulation (i.e., the emotional behaviors and communication

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN 203 known to be altered in conditions such as natally infected rats show asymmetric est functional abnormalities stand in con- autism have also been reported. Play be- protoambulatory (“pivoting”) responses, trast to the reportedly dramatic losses in havior is decreased with respect to both with an increased frequency of falls into a Purkinje cells by 6 weeks postinfection infected animals’ initiation of nondomi- supine position between days 4 and 9 [Eisenman et al., 1999; Hornig et al., nance-related play interactions and re- postinfection—an unusual movement 1999; Weissenbo¨ck et al., 2000] and im- sponse to initiation of play by nonin- never observed in control animals. In ad- ply the possibility of alternate, compen- fected, age- and gender-matched control dition, they exhibit significant delay in satory circuits for maintainence of gait animals or by infected littermates [Plet- righting themselves on a flat surface, and and balance. nikov et al., 1999a]. Given the selective explore the open-field chamber less As seen in adult rats infected with effects of BDV on D2 receptors in the widely through the 9th postnatal day BDV, locomotor activity is increased in adult infection model [Solbrig et al., [Hornig et al., 1999]. Such sensorimotor neonatally infected rats beginning as early 1994; 1996a,b], it is interesting to note processing disturbances suggest early as 4 weeks of age. Interestingly, the de- that this receptor subtype is thought to be functional damage to motor circuitry, in- gree of baseline motor activity in neona- the site of dopamine’s effects on social cluding cerebellum and striatum, and to tally infected rats far exceeds the baseline play behavior [Vanderschuren et al., acetylcholine systems; in addition to levels seen in the adult system. As noted 1997]. Other neuromodulators thought known infection of cerebellum and stri- above, exploratory behavior patterns and to be disrupted in adult BDV infection of atum in the neonatal system, adult infec- adaptation to novel environment are ab- Lewis rats, including monoamines, ace- tions have been associated with losses of normal at 4 weeks after neonatal infec- tylcholine, and opioids [Solbrig et al., choline acetyltransferase-positive fibers tion, as evidenced by prolonged locomo- 1995], also play a role in regulating social before the onset of encephalitis. Thus, it tor inhibition upon introduction to a play [Vanderschuren et al., 1997]. Studies is conceivable that similar damage may novel environment, and suggest dysfunc- of regional monoamine shifts following occur to cholinergic neurons in the neo- tion of the amygdaloid nuclei. This resis- neonatal infections are underway to de- natal system, where immune cell infiltra- tance to novel stimuli is not seen at any termine whether neurochemical aberra- tion also occurs later and is only fleeting. other timepoint through to 12 weeks; tions similar to those in adult-infected Given a recent report by Teitelbaum and indeed, this behavioral inhibition at 4 animals may contribute to these social colleagues indicating early, often tran- weeks is all the more striking given that it play abnormalities. Nucleus accumbens sient, locomotor abnormalities in chil- is only apparent in the first 30 minutes of and striatum, two brain areas affected in dren with autism, including abnormali- testing, following which infected animals both the adult and neonatal rat models, ties of the righting response, crawling, progress to marked hyperactivity for the also impact initiation of play behaviors. and ambulation, [Teitelbaum et al., remainder of the 90 minute test session. Furthermore, these sites receive signals 1998], further examination of the patho- Additionally, infected animals fail to from the amygdaloid nuclei, areas in- genesis of neurodevelopmental distur- show the normal patterns of attenuation volved in the selection of socially appro- bances in this infection-based animal in exploratory activity at 60 and 90 min- priate responses [Vanderschuren et al., model is warranted. utes, consistent with spatial memory def- 1997] and also observed to be heavily Because the cerebellum undergoes icits and hippocampal dysfunction. Sub- infected after adult and neonatal BDV substantial postnatal development in tle increases are noted in mild stereotypic infection. In another intriguing parallel many mammals, it is particularly vulner- behaviors, such as sniffing and rearing, with a core feature of autistic disorder, able to injury from perinatal infection actions which can also serve to assist that of impaired social communication, with any of several including exploration of the environment [Ike- infant-maternal communication appears Borna disease virus, mumps virus, and moto and Panksepp, 1994; Poltyrev and altered following neonatal infections lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus Weinstock, 1997], but not in more se- [Hornig et al., 1999]. Ultrasonic vocal- [Monjan et al., 1971, 1973; Oster-Gran- vere, self-mutilatory repetitive behaviors ization distress calls induced by maternal ite and Herdon, 1985; Rubin et al., [Hornig et al., 1999]. separation are one of the earliest and most 1998]. The observation that many agents Our understanding of the mecha- universal social communication responses may induce similar patterns of damage nisms mediating functional damage to to develop in mammals [Hofer, 1996; suggests that a nonspecific mechanism the Lewis rat host following neonatal Winslow and Insel, 1990], and are nor- may be implicated, such as induction of BDV infection is beginning to expand. mally reduced by social signals (e.g., pres- soluble factors triggered by viral replica- Consistent with previous reports ence of an anesthetized mother or litter- tion. [Narayan et al., 1983; Carbone et al., mate in the testing chamber) and by There is substantial evidence for 1991], we found morphologic alterations serotonergic agents, but increased fol- the role of the cerebellum in motor be- in brains of rats infected as newborns, lowing administration of noradrenergic havior and motor learning (for reviews including loss of dentate gyrus granule reuptake inhibitors [Hofer, 1996]. In see [Llinas and Welsh, 1993; Roland, cells and disorganization of cerebellar neonatally infected Lewis rats, ultrasonic 1993; Thompson and Kim, 1996]). In granule cell layer. Cerebellar abnormali- vocalizations are remarkable for increased neonatal BDV infection, overt cerebellar ties included decreased overall size and call frequency and abnormal waveforms dysfunction appears late and is mild to foliation of cerebellum, and thinning of (Hornig and Lipkin, unpublished data). moderate in severity [Hatalski, 1996; cerebellar granule cell layers. Hippocam- The efficacy of these calls in eliciting Hornig et al., 1999]. Only 5% of neona- pal changes in neonatally and adult in- appropriate maternal responses and their tally infected Lewis rats had mild gait fected animals included distinct loss of responsivity to social cues are currently ataxia between 12 and 24 weeks postin- granule cells in dentate gyrus. Previous being explored. fection, but all were impaired in skills investigators reported an absence of cel- Even before neuronal subsets are requiring more complex coordination lular inflammatory response to BDV fol- observed to be depleted in cerebellum, and balance, such as that required to lowing neonatal infection [Narayan et al., the developmental progression of motor maintain balance while walking across a 1983; Herzog et al., 1985; Stitz et al., skills and activity levels is distorted. Neo- dowel [Hornig et al., 1999]. These mod- 1989; Carbone et al., 1991; Sierra-

204 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN Honigmann et al., 1993; Bautista et al., neonatally infected and sham-inoculated brain to be even more striking than as- 1994; 1995; Gosztonyi and Ludwig, animals, suggest they cannot be impli- trocytosis. Interestingly, microglia re- 1995; Plata-Salama´n et al., 1999; Rott cated. Staining for PECAM on endothe- main activated even at 76 week postin- and Becht, 1995; Rubin et al., 1999a], a lial cells does not differ between infected fection, despite the absence of any phenomenon ascribed to the immaturity and uninfected groups, and although lev- evidence of infection; in contrast, a small of the rat immune system in the postnatal els of ICAM-1 increase on endothelial proportion of astrocytes do become in- period. Humoral immune responses to and other vascular and perivascular cells fected, although astrocytic activation BDV in neonatally-infected animals are of neonatal BD rats at about 4 weeks subsides between 4 and 24 week postin- also reported to be restricted, with anti- postinfection, the most prominent stain- fection. Cerebellar size has been reported BDV antibody titers remaining below ing appears in hippocampus and cerebel- to be reduced, with evidence of reactive 1:10 through 133 days postinfection. lum in addition to cerebral cortex. Thus, astrocytosis as demonstrated by glial [Carbone et al., 1991]. In contrast to the the regional distribution of ICAM-1 does fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP) reactivity many published reports of persistent in- not explain the apparent exclusion of in- as early as 3 days postinoculation, preced- fection without an inflammatory re- flammatory infiltrates from hippocampus ing the identification of BDV proteins in sponse, close serial analysis has revealed and cerebellum at 4 weeks postinfection. the cerebellum. Furthermore, reactivity transient, regionally restricted inflamma- Developmental maturity of the im- of cerebellar astrocytes and loss of cere- tion [Hornig et al., 1999; Sauder and de mune and nervous systems at the time of bellar granule cells occur without signs of la Torre, 1999]. These inflammatory in- exposure to the viral agent are critical to BDV infection in those cell populations filtrates emerge about 4 weeks postinfec- determining inflammatory responses and at all timepoints through to 30 days tion in perivascular areas of motor and ultimate neurodevelopmental outcomes postinfection. Curiously, Purkinje cells parietal cortex, with less marked infil- [Hornig et al., 1999; Rubin et al., appear to be the predominant cerebellar trates in nonhippocampal portions of 1999a]. Animals infected within the first cell population demonstrating BDV an- temporal cortex, thalamus, and basal gan- 12 hours of life demonstrate tolerance tigens, although these cells were previ- glia, and disappear completely by 6 and do not have a robust immune re- ously reported to be retained through day weeks postinfection. Despite nearly com- sponse to the virus [Hornig et al., 1999]. 30 postinfection [Bautista et al., 1995]. plete loss of the dentate gyrus, marked In contrast, animals infected with low However, our investigations indicate that loss of cerebellar Purkinje cells, and thin- titer stocks or administered virus after the by day 42 postinfection, Purkinje cell ning of granular cell layers in cerebellum first 12 hours of life may have marked populations are selectively depleted. Sim- by this timepoint, no such infiltrates are inflammatory cell infiltrates and en- ilarly, at 7 months after neonatal infec- present in these areas at 2, 4, or 6 weeks hanced morbidity and mortality. tion, Eisenman and colleagues report postinfection. Areas of inflammation are The manner in which infection re- 75% depletion of Purkinje cells [Eisen- also distinct from those areas showing the sults in loss of select neuronal subsets in man et al., 1999]. greatest amount of (periven- this model system is not yet known. Astrocytosis and microgliosis occur tricular germinal layer, dentate gyrus, Mechanisms may be both direct and in- in all brain regions by 3 weeks postinfec- granular layer of cerebellum). Immuno- direct. Clearly, infection alone is an in- tion. The onset and distribution of glial histochemical analysis of infiltrates indi- sufficient signal, as many neuronal pop- cell proliferation coincides with neuronal cates that they are comprised primarily of ulations remain persistently infected in loss yet is sustained through 24 weeks T cells with approximately equal num- neonatal BD without evident reduction postinfection, after neuronal cell bers of CD4ϩ and CD8ϩ cells [Weis- of cell numbers. Cells that are lost due to wanes. Astrocyte and microglia mor- senbo¨ck et al., 2000]. neonatal infection, predominantly found phology is consistent with activation (in- Curiously, brain lymphocytes from in cerebral cortex, dentate gyrus, and the creased cytoplasm; short, thickened pro- 4 week old, neonatally infected rats do Purkinje cell layer of cerebellum, appear cesses; additionally, in microglia, more not demonstrate cytotoxic activity to be lost exclusively through apoptosis intense staining with OX42). In hip- against BDV antigens [Hornig, Briese, [Hornig et al., 1999; Weissenbo¨ck et al., pocampus, gliosis is most evident in den- Planz, and Lipkin, unpublished data], 2000]. tate gyrus. In cerebellum, gliosis is suggesting that these T cells are not spe- Apoptotic cells, characterized by equally prominent in white and grey cific. Furthermore, neonatally thymecto- shrinkage, hypereosinophilic cytoplasm, matter. Curiously, microglia express mized, infected animals do not differ nuclear and , and MHC Class I and II, CD4 and CD8 from sham thymectomized, infected an- signal on transferase dUTP-biotin nick (OX8 and CD8b) molecules on their imals in degree of dentate gyrus damage, end labeling (TUNEL) assay, peaked at surface [Weissenbo¨ck et al. 2000]. Purkinje cell loss, or cortical , four weeks p.i., with most marked pa- Astrocytes and microglia are acti- despite successful ablation of inflamma- thology in dentate gyrus and cortical lay- vated in the absence of infection, be it tion at the 4 week timepoint by the neo- ers 5 and 6 of retrosplenial and cingulate directly by BDV or indirectly through natal thymectomy procedure [Hornig, cortex. Although apoptosis is described elaboration of soluble factors by other Stitz, and Lipkin, unpublished data]. in hippocampus of developing animals as cell types. Given the role of astrocytes in How these presumably activated, but late as day 7 to 10 of postnatal life, it is guiding migration of granule cells during nonspecific T cells are attracted to, and not found at later timepoints [Toth et al., cerebellar development, an assessment of transiently retained in selected regions of 1998]. It is intriguing to speculate that the frequency of astrocyte reactivity CNS and not in others is an intriguing, BDV may influence the expression of without viral infection in conjunction but unanswered question. Likely candi- age-related programs associated with with studies of apoptosis in limbic struc- dates for direct viral effects include cell normal, development-associated apopto- tures would aid our understanding of the adhesion molecules and chemokines. sis through the direct or indirect elabo- relative contributions of migrational fail- Levels and distribution of the cell adhe- ration of soluble factors. ure and programmed in the sion molecules PECAM and ICAM-1, as We observe reactivity and prolifer- evolution of BD neuropathogenesis. measured by in ation of microglial cells throughout the Even more intriguing may be the mech-

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN 205 anism by which microglia are activated in factor family, interferons), the Recent studies concerning cyto- the absence of infection. In neonatal TGF-␤ superfamily factors (including kine expression during neonatal infection HIV-1 infection, for instance, diffuse mi- TGF-␤1, 2, 3; GDNF), and the classic provide a converging view of the poten- croglial activation and reactive astroglio- neurotrophic factors (NGF, BDNF, NT3, tial importance of cytokines as mediators sis occur along with impaired brain NT4/5). A large subset of the neu- of BDV-related CNS injury in neonatal- growth and developmental delays [Ep- ronotrophic, hematolymphoietic cyto- ly-infected rats [Hornig et al., 1999; stein and Gelbard, 1999]. However, in kines may be roughly categorized ac- Plata-Salama´n et al., 1999; Sauder and de contrast to neonatal BD, microglia inter- cording to their origin from one of two la Torre, 1999]. Cytokine expression nalize virus in neonatal HIV-1 infection types of T-helper cells: Th1 (cell-medi- changes over time in different brain re- and replication can occur upon chronic ated immunity and stimulation of anti- gions, with maximal alteration occurring exposure to proinflammatory cytokines gen-presenting cells) or Th2 (humoral or at 4 weeks. Higher levels of mRNAs for [Janabi et al., 1998]. Two other findings B-cell mediated immunity). The poten- cytokine products of CNS macrophages/ regarding microglia-related injury in tial mechanisms of cytokine-mediated microglia (IL-1␣, IL-1␤, IL-6, TNF-␣) HIV-1 infection may be relevant to an damage in the context of the developing are noted in hippocampus, amygdala, understanding of mechanisms in BD brain include: direct effects on neuronal cerebellum, prefrontal cortex, and nu- pathogenesis: 1) production of the neu- elements; activation or suppression of cleus accumbens [Hornig et al., 1999]. rotoxin, quinolinic acid, is substantially second messenger/intracellular signaling Elevated levels of these proinflammatory greater in uninfected than in HIV-1 in- pathways; induction of shifts in excito- cytokines were first apparent at 2 weeks, fected monocytes following lipopolysac- toxic elements such as quinolinic acid or peaked at 4 weeks, and then declined at 6 charide or interferon-␥ stimulation [Not- acute phase proteins such as neopterin or and 12 weeks. No alterations in other tet et al., 1996]; and 2) the viral protein, ␤-2-microglobulin; direct alterations of proinflammatory cytokines, including gp41, may act indirectly to induce neu- neuronal function (e.g., inhibition of IL-2, IL-3, TNF-␤, and IFN-␥, were rotoxicity by triggering IL1 ␤ production long-term potentiation in hippocampus); observed. Given that production of sev- in microglia, thereby stimulating iNOS activation or suppression of glial cells; or eral of these proinflammatory cytokines production and NO generation by astro- alteration of glial cell proliferation or dif- is unique to T cells, B cells, mast cells, cytes [Hu et al., 1999]. General mecha- ferentiation (including expression of ad- and bone marrow stromal cells, and not nisms of CNS injury following microglial hesion molecules such as the integrins) to macrophages or microglia, these data activation also appear to relate to differ- [Benveniste, 1997; Mehler et al., 1996]. suggest that BDV may exert a selective ential regulation of MHC and other mol- Given that the postnatal expression of effect on cells of microglial or macro- ecules on microglia [Stoll and Jander, neuronotrophic cytokine and cytokine phage lineage. 1999]. Whether the unusual pattern of receptor mRNAs in brain differs for each Following neonatal infection, upregulation of MHC, CD4, and CD8 cytokine [Benveniste, 1997], and that the BDV influences the expression of apop- molecules observed on uninfected, acti- sensitivity of neuronal populations to the tosis-related products. Increased levels of vated microglia in neonatal BDV infec- trophic or apoptosis-inducing effects of mRNAs coding for FAS and ICE tion might explain the pattern of damage cytokines changes during development, (caspase-1), two promotors of apoptosis, observed is not known. wide variation in the patterns of virus- and decreased mRNA for bcl-x, a factor Higher levels of message for tissue induced, cytokine-related damage would that inhibits apoptosis, were identified in factor (TF) have been found in infected be expected, depending on the relative hippocampus, amygdala, prefrontal cor- hippocampus [Gonzalez-Dunia et al., maturity of the evolving nervous system tex, nucleus accumbens, and cerebellum 1996]. TF is a member of the class II at the time of infection. In addition, cell [Hornig et al., 1999]. These findings are cytokine receptor family primarily pro- loss induced by either BDV or develop- consistent with promotion of apoptosis duced by astrocytes that plays important mentally-programmed changes may alter throughout the brains of rats neonatally roles in cellular signal transduction, brain the capacity of resident CNS cells to both infected with BDV by at least two strat- function, and neural development produce and respond to neuronotrophic egies. A host of excitants or neurotoxins through its effects on pro- cytokines. including arachidonic acid, platelet-acti- Ϫ tease cascades. Although this may be one One of the primary mechanisms of vating factor, free radicals (NO, O2 ), mechanism by which BDV may alter host defense following viral infection be- glutamate, quinolinate, cysteine, cyto- CNS development [Gonzalez-Dunia et gins with the induction of interferon-␥ kines (TNF-␣, IL1-␤, IL-6), amines, and al., 1996], cerebellar changes cannot be (IFN-␥) and other cytokines, which in as yet unidentified factors arising from explained by this mechanism as TF up- turn initiate a cascade of host responses in stimulated macrophages and possibly re- regulation is not observed in cerebellum a wide variety of cell types. In the CNS, active astrocytes may also influence apo- despite prominent astrocytosis. Further- IFN-␥ modulates oligodendrocyte, neu- ptosis by excessive activation of N- more, BDV infection of astrocytes ap- ronal and glial cell functions, and is im- methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors pears to be required for TF upregulation portant in activating glial cells to produce [Lipton, 1996]. Interestingly, Gosztonyi [Gonzalez-Dunia et al., 1996], and cere- mediators of or death, in- and Ludwig [1995] have proposed that bellar astrocytes are rarely infected cluding toxic intermediates of nitrogen the targeted of BDV for two [Hornig et al., 1999]. and oxygen, and complement compo- hippocampal cell layers, stratum oriens One means by which a virus might nents [St. Pierre et al., 1996]. Viral dam- and stratum radiatum, may be due to disrupt neural function and development age to neurodevelopmental circuitry may their rich concentration of glutamate and in the absence of inflammation is through thus parallel the production of these aspartate receptors, and preliminary re- the induction of neuronotrophic cyto- downstream mediators following IFN-␥ sults from our laboratory indicate re- kines. Neuronotrophic cytokines comprise induction, and provide a means by which gional upregulation at 4 weeks postinfec- a burgeoning set of immunoregulatory BDV might disrupt brain cell differenti- tion of a non-NMDA glutamate molecules, including the hematolym- ation and function without inflammatory receptor, the calcium-permeable AMPA phoietic factors (e.g., interleukins, tumor cell infiltration. receptor, GluR1 (Hornig and Lipkin,

206 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN unpublished data). Consistent with re- cific neural cells and circuits. Future Bolton P, Pickles A, Harrington, R, et al. 1992. gion-specific findings of apoptosis by work should focus on dissecting the Season of birth: issues, approaches and find- ings for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry TUNEL analysis, this upregulation is mechanisms of developmental neuropa- 33:509–530. most evident on granule cells in dentate thology in animal models and using clues Brai M, Accardo P, Bellavia D. 1994. Polymor- gyrus and at synapses of cells in molecular derived from these more simple systems to phism of the complement components in hu- cell layer of cerebellum that terminate on target infectious disease investigation. f man pathology. Ann Ital Med Int 9:167–172. Purkinje cells (presumably, Bergmann Briese TB, Hornig M, Lipkin WI. 1999. Bornavi- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS rus immunopathogenesis in rodents: models glia or basket cells). for human neurological diseases. J Neurovirol Withdrawal of selected neurotro- Research in the Emerging Diseases 5:604–612. phic factors can also contribute to apo- Laboratory is supported by grants from the Burd L. 1988. Month of birth of non-speaking ptotic losses [Takei et al., 1999]. In con- National Institutes of Health (NS29425, children. Dev Med Child Neurol 30:685– trast to findings of diffuse alterations in MH57467, HD37546, MH01608), the 686. MIND Institute of UC Davis, the CAN Burger RA, Warren RP. 1998. Possible immuno- gene expression for cytokines described genetic basis for autism. Mental Retardation above, changes in neurotrophic factor Foundation, and the Tourette Syndrome Dev Disabil Res Rev 4:137–141. mRNAs are restricted to hippocampus. Association. Burmester GR, Jahn B, Gramatzki M, et al. 1984. Decreased mRNA coding for BDNF and Activated T cells in vivo and in vitro: diver- NT3 is prominent in hippocampus by 4 REFERENCES gence in expression of Tac and Ia antigens in Altshuler LL, Conrad A, Kovelman JA, et al. 1987. the nonblastoid small T cells of inflammation weeks, but is still evident by 12 weeks Hippocampal pyramidal cell orientation in and normal T cells activated in vitro. J Im- postinfection. Although decreased NT3 schizophrenia: a controlled neurohistologic munol 133:1230–1234. mRNA may reflect loss of the granule study of the Yakovlev Collection. Arch Gen Carbone KM, Park SW, Rubin SA, et al. 1991. cell population in dentate gyrus, the role Psychiatry 44:1094–1098. Borna disease: association with a maturation of BDNF in maintaining viability of cells Anderson GM. 1994. Studies on the neurochemis- defect in the cellular immune response. J Vi- try of autism. In Bauman ML, Kemper TL, rol 65:6154–6164. suggests that its downregulation may be a editors. The Neurobiology of Autism. Balti- Chess S. 1971. Autism in children with congenital more essential step in neonatal BDV more: Johns Hopkins University Press. 227– rubella. J Autism Child Schizophr 1:33–47. pathogenesis. Nonetheless, if BDNF 242. Chess S. 1977. Follow-up report on autism in con- withdrawal is a potent influence on apop- Anonymous. 1999. A research-oriented framework genital rubella. J Autism Child Schizophr tosis, it is difficult to explain the abrupt for risk assessment and prevention of chil- 7:69–81. dren’s exposure to environmental toxicants. Clark DA. 2000. Human herpesvirus 6. Rev Med dropoff in apoptotic losses in dentate gy- Environ Health Perspect 107:510. Virol 10:155–173. rus after 5 to 6 weeks postinfection. Fur- Atlas JA. Birth seasonality in developmentally dis- Comi AM, Zimmerman AW, Frye VH, et al. 1999. thermore, this mechanism is unable to abled children. 1989. Psychol Rep 64:1213– Familial clustering of autoimmune disorders account for cell losses in cerebellum at 1214. and evaluation of medical risk factors in au- Barak Y, Ring A, Sulkes J, et al. 1995. Season of any timepoint as BDNF is not expressed tism. J Child Neurol 14:388–394. birth and autistic disorder in Israel. Am J Connolly AM, Chez MG, Pestronk A, et al. 1999. at substantive levels in normal cerebel- Psychiatry 152:798–800. Serum autoantibodies to brain in Landau- lum. Barak Y, Kimhi R, Stein D, et al. 1999. Autistic Kleffner variant, autism, and other neurologic The epidemiology of Borna disease subjects with comorbid : a possible disorders. J Pediatrics 134:607–613. association with viral infections. Child Psy- Cook EH. 1990. Autism: review of neurochemical virus and its role in human disease remain chiatry Human Dev 29:245–251. controversial [Hatalski et al., 1997; Stae- investigation. Synapse 6:292–308. Barka N, Shen GQ, Shoenfeld Y, et al. 1995. Cook EH, Leventhal BL. 1996. The serotonin sys- heli et al., 2000]. Similarities between Multireactive pattern of serum autoantibodies tem in autism. Curr Opin Pediatr 8:348–354. in asymptomatic individuals with immuno- some behaviors observed in neonatally Cook EH, Courchesne R, Lord C, et al. 1997. globulin A deficiency. Clin Diagn Lab Im- infected rats and in autistic children led to Evidence of linkage between the serotonin munol 2:469–472. transporter and autistic disorder. Mol Psychi- the hypothesis that the virus might be Baron-Cohen S, Saunders K, Chakrabarti S. 1999. atry 2:247–250. implicated in pathogenesis of autism. Al- Does autism cluster geographically? A re- Daniels WW, Warren RP, Odell JD, et al. 1995. search note. Autism 3:39–43. though neither serologic nor molecular Increased frequency of the extended or ances- data support a role for Borna disease virus Bartlik BD. 1981. Monthly variation in births of autistic children in North America. J Amer tral haplotype B44-SC30-DR4 in autism. as an etiologic agent in autism [Hornig et Med Women’s Assoc 36:363–368. Neuropsychobiology 32:120–123. al., 1999], multicenter studies are under- Bauman ML, Kemper TL. 1994. Neuroanatomic Dantzer R, Bluthe´ R-M, Laye´ S, et al. 1998. Cy- way to assess whether it may be impli- observations of the brain in autism. In Bau- tokines and sickness behavior. Ann NY Acad man ML, Kemper TL, editors. The Neuro- Sci 840:586–590. cated in other neuropsychiatric disorders. Davis M, Shi C. 1999. The extended amygdala: are Whatever the outcome of these surveys, biology of Autism. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press: 119–145. the central nucleus of the amygdala and the the BDV infected neonatal rat is a pow- Bautista JR, Schwartz GJ, de la Torre JC, et al. bed nucleus of the stria terminalis differen- erful model system for exploring the 1994. Early and persistent abnormalities in tially involved in fear versus anxiety? Ann NY mechanisms by which early life infection rats with neonatally acquired Borna disease Acad Sci 877:281–291. and/or immune activation can result in virus infection. Brain Res Bull 34:31–40. De Carli M, D’Elios MM, Zancuoghi G, et al. Bautista JR, Rubin SA, Moran TH, et al. 1995. 1994. Human Th1 and Th2 cells: functional neurodevelopmental pathology. Developmental injury to the cerebellum fol- properties, regulation of development and lowing perinatal Borna disease virus infection. role in autoimmunity. Autoimmunity SUMMARY Brain Res Dev Brain Res 90:45–53. 18:301–308. A comprehensive approach to in- Benveniste EN. 1997. Cytokine expression in the Delgiudice-Asch G, Hollander E. 1997. Altered vestigating the pathogenesis of neurode- nervous system. In: Keane RW, Hickey WF, immune function in autism. CNS Spectrums editors. Immunology of the Nervous System. 2:61–68. velopmental disorders must consider the New York: Oxford University Press: 419– DeLong GR, Bean SC, Brown FR. 1981. Ac- interaction of both host and environ- 459. quired reversible autistic syndrome in acute mental factors. Evidence from epidemi- Bergroth V, Konttinen YT, Pelkonen P, et al. encephalopathic illness in children. Arch ology and animal models suggest that 1988. Synovial fluid lymphocytes in different Neurol 38:191–194. subtypes of juvenile rheumatoid arthritis. Ar- Denney DR, Frei BW, Gaffney GR. 1996. Lym- early infection, possibly with a variety of thritis Rheum 31:780–783. phocyte subsets and interleukin-2 receptors in agents, may trigger complex behavioral Blattner RJ. 1974. The role of viruses in congenital autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord 26:87– disorders by impacting viability of spe- defects. Am J Dis Child 128:781–786. 97.

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN 207 Department of Developmental Services. 1999. Gupta S, Aggarwal S, Rashanravan B, et al. 1998. neurotransmitter mRNA levels without his- Changes in the Population of Persons with Th1- and Th2-like cytokines in CD4ϩ and tologic injury. Brain Res 451:333–339. Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disor- CD8ϩ T cells in autism. J Neuroimmunol Lipkin WI, Carbone KM, Wilson MC, et al. ders in California’s Developmental Services 85:106–109. 1988b. Neurotransmitter abnormalities in System, 1987 through 1998: A Report to the Hafler DA, Hemler ME, Christenson L, et al. 1985. Borna disease. Brain Res 1988;475;366–370. Legislature. Sacramento, Department of De- Investigation of in vivo activated T cells in Lipkin WI, Hornig M. 1998. Microbes and the velopmental Services, California Health and multiple sclerosis and inflammatory central brain. Lancet 352:SIV21. Human Services Agency: 19. nervous system diseases. Clin Immunol Im- Lipton SA. 1996. Similarity of neuronal cell injury Desmond MM, Wilson GS, Melnick JL, et al. munopathol 37:163–171. and death in AIDS dementia and focal cere- 1967. Congenital rubella encephalitis. Course Hatalski CG. 1996. Alterations in the immune re- bral : potential treatment with and early sequelae. J Pediatrics 71:311–331. sponse within the central nervous system of NMDA open-channel blockers and nitric ox- Deykin EY, MacMahon B. 1979. Viral exposure rats infected with Borna disease virus: poten- ide-related species. Brain Pathol 6:507–517. and autism. Am J Epidemiol 109:628–638. tial mechanisms for viral persistence. Ph.D. Llinas R, Welsh JP. 1993. On the cerebellum and Dittrich W, Bode L, Ludwig H, et al. 1989. Learn- Thesis, University of California Irvine. Irvine, motor learning. Current Opin Neurobiol ing deficiencies in Borna disease virus-in- CA: University of California Irvine. 3:958–965. fected but clinically healthy rats. Biol Psychi- Hatalski CG, Lewis AJ, Lipkin WI. 1997. Borna Markowitz PI. 1983. Autism in a child with con- atry 26:818–828. disease. Emerg Infect Dis. 3:129–135. genital cytomegalovirus infection. J Autism Dunn AJ. 1993. Infection as a stressor: a cytokine- Herzog S, Wonigeit K, Frese K, et al. 1985. Effect Dev Disord 13:249–253. mediated activation of the hypothalamo-pitu- of Borna disease virus infection on athymic Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, et al. itary-adrenal axis? Ciba Found Symp 172: rats. J Gen Virol 66:503–508. 1990. The UCLA-University of Utah epide- 226–239. Hofer MA. 1996. Multiple regulators of ultrasonic miologic survey of autism: prenatal, perinatal, Dunn AJ, Swiergiel AH. 1998. The role of cyto- vocalization in the infant rat. Psychoneuroen- and postnatal factors. Pediatrics 86:514–519. kines in infection-related behavior. Ann NY docrinology 21:203–217. Mehler MF, Goldstein H, Kessler JA. 1996. Effects Acad Sci 840:51–58. Hoon AH, Reiss AL. 1992. The mesial temporal of cytokines on CNS cells: neurons. In: Ran- Eisenman LM, Brother R, Tran MH, et al. 1999. lobe and autism: case report and review. Dev sohoff RM, Benveniste EN, editors. Cyto- Neonatal Borna disease virus infection in the Med Child Neurol 34:252–265. kines and the CNS. Boca Raton: CRC Press: rat causes a loss of Purkinje cells in the cere- Hornig M, Weissenbo¨ck H, Horscroft N, et al. 115–150. bellum. J Neurovirol 5:181–189. 1999. An infection-based model of neurode- Money J, Bobrow NA, Clarke FC. 1971. Autism Epstein LG, Gelbard HA. 1999. HIV-1-induced velopmental damage. Proc Natl Acad Sci and autoimmune disease: a family study. J neuronal injury in the developing brain. J USA 96:12102–12107. Autism Child Schizophr 1:146–160. Leukoc Biol 65:453–457. Hu S, Ali H, Sheng WS, et al. 1999. gp-41 medi- Monjan AA, Gilden DH, Cole GA, et al. 1971. Ferrari P, Marescot MR, Moulias R, et al. 1998. ated astrocyte inducible nitric oxide synthase Cerebellar hypoplasia in neonatal rats caused [Immune status in infantile autism. Correla- mRNA expression: involvement of interleu- by lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus. Sci- kin-1␤ production by microglia. J Neurosci tion between the immune status, autistic ence 171:194–196. 19:6468–6474. symptoms and levels of serotonin] (French). Monjan AA, Cole GA, Gilden DH, et al. 1973. Ikemoto S, Panksepp J. 1994. The relationship be- Encephale 14:339–344. Pathogenesis of cerebellar hypoplasia pro- tween self-stimulation and sniffing in rats: Fiore M, Pera C, Delfino L, et al. 1995. DNA duced by lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus does a common brian system mediate these typing of DQ and DR alleles in IgA-deficient infection of neonatal rats: evolution of disease behaviors? Behav Brain Res 61:143–162. subjects. Eur Immunogenet 22:403–411. following infection at 4 days of age. J Neu- Ivarsson SA, Bjerre I, Vegfors P, et al. 1990. Autism Fish B, Marcus J, Hans SL, et al. 1992. Infants at ropathol Exp Neurol 32:110–124. as one of several disabilities in two children risk for schizophrenia: sequelae of a genetic Mouridsen SE, Nielsen S, Rich B, et al. 1994. with congenital cytomegalovirus infection. neurointegrative defect: a review and replica- Season of birth in infantile autism and other Neuropediatrics 21:102–103. tion analysis of pandysmaturation in the types of childhood psychoses. Child Psychia- Janabi N, DiStefano M, Wallon C, et al. 1998. Jerusalem Infant Development Study. Arch Induction of human immunodeficiency virus try Hum Dev 25:31–43. Gen Psychiatry 49:221–235. type 1 replication in human glial cells after Narayan O, Herzog S, Frese K, et al. 1983. Behav- Gillberg C. 1990. Do children with autism have proinflammatory cytokines stimulation: effect ioral disease in rats caused by immunopatho- March birthdays? Acta Psychiatr Scand 82: of IFN␥, IL1␤, and TNF␣ on differentiation logical responses to persistent borna virus in 152–156. and chemokine production in glial cells. Glia the brain. Science 220:1401–1403. Gillberg C, Coleman M. 1992. Infectious diseases. 23:304–315. Naves M, Hajeer AH, Teh LS, et al. 1998. Com- In Gillberg C, Coleman M, editors. The Bi- Kawano M, Seya T, Koni I, et al. 1999. Elevated plement C4B null allele status confers risk for ology of the Autistic Syndromes—2nd Edi- serum levels of soluble membrane cofactor systemic lupus erythematosus in a Spanish tion. London, Mac Keith Press: 218–225. protein (CD46, MCP) in patients with sys- population. Eur J Immunogenet 25:317–320. Gillberg C, Gillberg IC. 1983. Infantile autism: a temic lupus erythematosus (SLE). Clin Exp Nelson KB, Grether JK, Dambrosia JM, et al. 2000. total population study of nonoptimal pre-, Immunol 116:542–546. Neuropeptides and neurotrophins in neonatal peri-, and neonatal conditions. J Autism Dev Kemper TL, Bauman ML. 1993. The contribution blood of children with autism, mental retar- Disord 13:153–166. of neuropathologic studies to the understand- dation, or cerebral palsy. Annual Meeting of Gillberg C, Wing L. 1999. Autism: not an ex- ing of autism. Neurol Clin North Am 11: the American Academy of Neurology, San tremely rare disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 175–187. Diego, May 3, 2000. 99:399–406. King CT, Travers SP, Rowland NE, et al. 1999. Nottet HS, Flanagan EM, Flanagan CR, et al. Gillberg C, Steffenburg S, Schaumann H. 1991. Is Glossopharyngeal nerve transection eliminates 1996. The regulation of quinolinic acid in autism more common now than ten years quinine-stimulated fos-like immunoreactivity human immunodeficiency virus-infected ago? Brit J Psychiatry 158:403–409. in the nucleus of the solitary tract: implica- monocytes. J Neurovirol 2:111–117. Gillberg IC, Gillberg C, Kopp S. 1992. Hypothy- tions for a functional topography of gustatory Oldstone MB. 1989a. Molecular mimicry as a roidism and autism spectrum disorders. J nerve input in rats. J Neurosci 19:3107–3121. mechanism for the cause and a probe uncov- Child Psychol Psychiatry 33:531–542. King SM. 1999. Escape-related behaviours in an ering etiologic agent(s) of autoimmune dis- Gonzalez-Dunia D, Eddleston M, Mackman N, et unstable elevated and exposed environment. ease. Curr Top Microbiol Immunol 145:127– al. 1996. Expression of tissue factor is in- I. A new behavioural model of extreme anx- 135. creased in astrocytes within the central ner- iety. Behav Brain Res 98:113–126. Oldstone MB. 1989b. Viral alteration of cell func- vous system during persistent infection with Koch M. 1999. The neurobiology of startle. Prog tion. Sci Am 261:42–48. borna disease virus. J Virol 70:5812–5820. Neurobiol 59:107–128. Oldstone MB. 1989c. Viruses can cause disease in Gosztonyi G, Ludwig H. 1995. Borna disease: neu- Kostantareas MW, Hauser P, Lennox C, et al. the absence of morphologic evidence of cell ropathology and pathogenesis. Curr Top Mi- 1986. Season of birth in infantile autism. injury: implication for uncovering new dis- crobiol Immunol 190:39–73. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 17:53–65. eases in the future. J Infect Dis 159:384–389. Gupta S, Aggarwal S, Heads C. 1996. Dysregulated Landau EC, Cicchetti DV, Klin A, et al. 1999. Opp MR, Rady PL, Hughes TKJ, et al. 1996. immune system in children with autism: ben- Season of birth in autism: a fiction revisited. J Human immunodeficiency virus envelope eficial effects of intravenous immune globulin Autism Dev Disord 29:385–393. glycoprotein 120 alters sleep and induces cy- on autistic characteristics. J Autism Dev Dis- Lipkin WI, Battenberg ELF, Bloom FE, et al. tokine mRNA expression in rats. Am J ord 26:439–452. 1988a. Viral infection of neurons can depress Physiol 270:R963–R970.

208 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN Oster-Granite ML, Herdon RM. 1985. The patho- infection. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 112:237– ease: changes in brain dopamine receptors. genesis of parvovirus-induced cerebellar hyp- 244. Virology 222:332–338. oplasia in the Syrian hamster, Mesociretus Rubin SA, Sylves P, Vogel M, et al. 1999b. Borna Staeheli P, Sauder C, Hausmann J, et al. 2000. auratus: fluorescent antibody, foliation, cyto- disease virus-induced hippocampal dentate Epidemiology of borna disease virus. J Gen architectonic, golgi and electron microscopic gyrus damage is associated with spatial learn- Virol 81:2123–2135. studies. J Compar Neurol 169:481–522. ing and memory deficits. Brain Res Bull 48: Stevens MC, Fein DH, Waterhouse LH. 2000. Peterson MR, Torrey EF. 1976. Viruses and other 23–30. Season of birth effects in autism. J Clin Exp infectious agents as behavioural teratogens. In Santoro F, Kennedy PE, Locatelli G, et al. 1999. Neuropsychol 22:399–407. Coleman M, editor. The Autistic Syndromes. CD46 is a cellular receptor for human her- St. Pierre BA, Merrill JE, Dopp JM. 1996. Effects of New York: Elsevier: 23–42. pesvirus 6. Cell 99:817–827. cytokines on CNS cells: glia. In: Ransohoff Plata-Salama´n CR, Ilyin SE, Gayle D, et al. 1999. Sauder C, de la Torre JC. 1999. Cytokine expres- RM, Benveniste EN, editors. Cytokines and Persistent Borna disease virus infection of sion in the rat central nervous system follow- the CNS. Boca Raton: CRC Press: 151–168. neonatal rats causes brain regional changes of ing perinatal Borna disease virus infection. J Stitz L, Soeder D, Deschl, et al. 1989. Inhibition of mRNAs for cytokines, cytokine receptor Neuroimmunol 96:29–45. immune-mediated meningoencephalitis in components and neuropeptides. Brain Res Schroeder HWJ, Zhu AB, March RE, et al. 1998. persistently Borna disease virus-infected rats Bull 49:441–451. Susceptibility locus for IgA deficiency and by cyclosporine A. J Immunol 143:4250– Pletnikov MV, Rubin SA, Vasudevan K, et al. common variable immunodeficiency in the 4256. 1999a. Developmental brain injury associated HLA-DR3, -B8, -A1 haplotypes. Mol Med Stoll G, Jander S. 1999. The role of microglia and with abnormal play behavior in neonatally 4:72–86. macrophages in the pathophysiology of the Borna disease virus-infected Lewis rats: a Schwab JJ. 1982. Psychiatric aspects of infectious CNS. Prog Neurobiol 58:233–247. model of autism. Behav Brain Res 100:43– diseases. Curr Psychiatr Ther 21:225–239. Stubbs EG, Crawford ML. 1977. Depressed lym- 50. Sells CJ, Carpenter RL, Ray CG. 1975. Sequelae of phocyte responsiveness in autistic children. J Pletnikov MV, Rubin SA, Schwartz GJ, et al. central nervous system enterovirus infection. Autism Child Schizophr 7:49–55. 1999b. Persistent neonatal Borna disease virus N Engl J Med 293:1–4. Stubbs EG, Ash E, Williams CPS. 1984. Autism (BDV) infection of the brain causes chronic Sierra-Honigmann AM, Rubin SA, Esafanous MG, and congenital cytomegalovirus. J Autism emotional abnormalities in adult rats. Physiol et al. 1993. Borna disease virus in peripheral Dev Disord 14:183–189. Behav 66:823–831. blood mononuclear and bone marrow cells of Takei N, Tanaka O, Endo Y, et al. 1999. BDNF Plioplys AV, Greaves A, Yoshida W. 1989a. Anti- neonatally and chronically infected rats. J and NT-3 but not CNTF counteract the CNS antibodies in childhood neurologic dis- Neuroimmunol 45:31–36. Ca2ϩ ionophore-induced apoptosis of cul- eases. Neuropediatrics 20:92–102. Singh VK. 1996. Plasma increase of interleukin-12 tured cortical neurons: involvement of dual Plioplys AV, Greaves A, Kazemi K, et al. 1989b. and interferon-gamma: pathological signifi- pathways. Neuropharmacol 38:283–288. Autism: anti-210K neurofilament immuno- cance in autism. J Neuroimmunol 66:143– Tanoue Y, Oda S, Asano F, et al. 1988. Epidemi- globulin reactivity. Neurology 39:187. 145. ology of infantile autism in southern Ibaraki, Plioplys AV, Greaves A, Kazemi K, et al. 1994. Singh VK. 1997. Immunotherapy for brain diseases Japan: differences in prevalence in birth co- Lymphocyte function in autism and Rett syn- and mental illnesses. Prog Drug Res 48:129– horts. J Autism Dev Disord 18:155–166. drome. Neuropsychobiol 29:12–16. 146. Tatsuo H, Ono N, Tanaka K, et al. 2000. SLAM Poltyrev T, Weinstock M. 1997. Effect of prenatal Singh VK, Warren RP, Odell JD, et al. 1991. (CDw150) is a cellular receptor for measles stress on opioid component of exploration in Changes of soluble interleukin-2, interleu- virus. Nature 406:893–897. different experimental situations. Pharmacol kin-2 receptor, T8 antigen, and interleukin-1 Teitelbaum P, Teitelbaum O, Nye J, et al. 1998. Biochem Behav 58:387–393. in the serum of autistic children. Clin Immu- Movement analysis in infancy may be useful Prospero-Garcia O, Herold N, Waters AK, et al. nol Immunopathol 61:448–455. for early diagnosis of autism. Proc Natl Acad 1994. Intraventricular administration of a Singh VK, Warren RP, Odell JD, et al. 1993. Sci USA 95:13982–13987. FIV-envelope protein induces sleep architec- Antibodies to myelin basic protein in children Tepper VJ, Farley JJ, Rothman MI, et al. 1998. ture changes in rats. Brain Res 659:254–258. with autistic behavior. Brain Behav Immunity Neurodevelopmental/neuroradiologic recov- Raiten DJ, Massaro T. 1986. Perspectives on the 7:93–103. ery of a child infected with HIV after treat- nutritional ecology of autistic children. J Au- Singh VK, Singh EA, Warren RP. 1997a. Hyper- ment with combination antiretroviral therapy tism Dev Disord 16:133–144. serotoninemia and serotonin receptor anti- using the HIV-specific protease inhibitor Reil A, Bein G, Machulla HK, et al. 1997. High- bodies in children with autism but not mental ritonavir. Pediatrics 101:E7. resolution DNA typing in immunoglobulin A retardation. Biol Psychiatry 41:753–755. Thompson RF, Kim JJ. 1996. Memory systems in deficiency confirms a positive association Singh VK, Warren R, Averett R, et al. 1997b. the brain and localization of memory. Proc with DRB1*0301, DQB1*02 haplotypes. Circulating autoantibodies to neuronal and Natl Acad Sci USA 93:13438–13444. Tissue Antigens 50:501–506. glial filament proteins in autism. Pediatr Neu- Ticher A, Ring A, Barak Y, et al. 1996. Circannual Rice D, Barone SJ. 2000. Critical Periods of Vul- rol 17:88–90. pattern of autistic births: reanalysis in three nerability for the Developing Nervous Sys- Singh VK, Lin SX, Yang VC. 1998. Serological ethnic groups. Human Biol 68:585–592. tem: Evidence from Humans and Animal association of measles virus and human her- Todd R, Ciaranello R. 1985. Demonstration of Models. Environ Health Perspect 108:511– pesvirus-6 with brain autoantibodies in au- inter- and intraspecies differences in serotonin 533. tism. Clin Immunol Immunopathol 89:105– binding sites by antibodies from an autistic Ritvo ER, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, et al. 108. child. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 82:612–616. 1990. The UCLA-University of Utah epide- Singh VK, Mehrotra S, Agarwal SS. 1999. The Todd RD, Hickok M, Anderson GM, et al. 1988. miologic survey of autism: the etiologic role paradigm of Th1 and Th2 cytokines: its rel- Antibrain antibodies in infantile autism. Biol of rare diseases. Am J Psychiatry 147:1614– evance to autoimmunity and allergy. Immu- Psychiatry 23:644–647. 1621. nol Res 20:147–161. Toth Z, Yan XX, Haftoglou S, et al. 1998. Seizure- Rogers T, Kalaydjieva L, Hallmayer J, et al. 1999. Soares JC, Mann JJ. 1997. The anatomy of mood induced neuronal injury: vulnerability to fe- Exclusion of linkage to the HLA region in disorders—review of structural neuroimaging brile seizures in an immature rat model. J ninety multiplex sibships with autism. J Au- studies. Biol Psychiatry 41:86–106. Neurosci 18:4285–4294. tism Dev Disord 29:195–201. Solbrig MV, Fallon JH, Lipkin WI. 1995. Behav- Torrey EF, Miller J, Rawlings R, et al. 1997. Roland PE. 1993. Partition of the human cerebel- ioral disturbances and pharmacology of Borna Seasonality of births in schizophrenia and bi- lum in sensory-motor activities, learning and disease. Curr Top Microbiol Immunol 190: polar disorder: a review of the literature. cognition. Canad J Neurol Sci 20:S57–S77. 93–101. Schizophr Res 28:1–38. Rott R, Becht H. 1995. Natural and experimental Solbrig MV, Koob GF, Fallon JH, et al. 1994. Trottier G, Srivastava L, Walker CD. 1999. Etiol- Borna disease in animals. Curr Top Microbiol Tardive dyskinetic syndrome in rats infected ogy of infantile autism: a review of recent Immunol 190:17–30. with Borna disease virus. Neurobiol Dis advances in genetic and neurobiological re- Rubin SA, Pletnikov M, Carbone KM. 1998. 1:111–119. search. J Psychiatry Neurosci 24:103–115. Comparison of the neurovirulence of a vac- Solbrig MV, Koob GF, Fallon JH, et al. 1996a. Ulgiati D, Abraham LJ. 1996. Comparative analysis cine and a wild-type mumps virus strain in the Prefrontal cortex dysfunction in Borna disease of the disease-associated complement C4 developing rat brain. J Virol 72:8037–8042. virus (BDV)—infected rats. Biol Psychiatry gene from the HLA-A1, B8, DR3 haplotype. Rubin SA, Bautista JR, Moran TH, et al. 1999a. 40:629–636. Exp Clin Immunogenet 13:43–54. Viral teratogenesis: brain developmental dam- Solbrig MV, Koob GF, Joyce JN, et al. 1996b. A Vanderschuren LJ, Niesink RJ, Van Ree JM. age associated with maturation state at time of neural substrate of hyperactivity in Borna dis- 1997. The neurobiology of social play be-

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN 209 havior in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21: Warren RP, Yonk J, Burger RW, et al. 1995. Wing L. 1997. The autistic spectrum. Lancet 350: 309–326. DR-positive T cells in autism: association 761–1766. Warren RP. 1998. An immunologic theory for the with decreased plasma levels of the comple- Winslow JT, Insel TR. 1990. Serotonergic and development of some cases of autism. CNS ment C4B protein. Neuropsychobiol 1995; catecholaminergic reuptake inhibitors have Spectrums 3:71–79. 31:53–57. opposite effects on the ultrasonic isolation Warren RP, Singh VK. 1996. Elevated serotonin Warren RP, Odell JD, Warren WL, et al. 1996a. calls of rat pups. Neuropsychopharmacol levels in autism: association with the major Strong association of the third hypervariable 3:51–59. histocompatibility complex. Neuropsycho- region of HLA-DR ␤ 1 with autism. J Neu- Wong ML, Bongiorno PB, Rettori V, et al. 1997. biol 34:72–75. roimmunol 67:97–102. Interleukin (IL) 1␤, IL-1 receptor antagonist, Warren RP, Foster A, Margaretten NC, Pace Warren RP, Singh VK, Averett RE, et al. 1996b. IL-10, and IL-13 gene expression in the cen- NC. 1986. Immune abnormalities in pa- Immunogenetic studies in autism and related tral nervous system and anterior pituitary dur- tients with autism. J Autism Dev Disord disorders. Mol Chem Neuropathol 28:77–81. ing systemic inflammation: pathophysiologi- 16:189–197. Warren RP, Odell JD, Warren WL, et al. 1997. cal implications. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA Warren RP, Foster A, Margaretten NC. 1987. Re- Brief report: immunoglobulin A deficiency in 94:227–232. duced natural killer cell activity in autism. J a subset of autistic subjects. J Autism Dev Wright HH, Abramson RK, Self S, et al. 1990. Am Acad Adolesc Psychiatry 26:333–335. Disord 27:187–192. Serotonin may affect lymphocyte cell surface Warren RP, Cole P, Odell JD, et al. 1990. Detection Weiss B, Landrigan PJ. 2000. The developing brain markers in autistic probands. American Acad- of maternal antibodies in infantile autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29:873–877. and the environment: an introduction. Envi- emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, San Warren RP, Singh VK, Cole P, et al. 1991. In- ron Health Perspect 108:373–374. Francisco, CA. creased frequency of the null allele at the Weissenbo¨ck H, Hornig M, Hickey WF, et al. Yonk LJ, Warren RP, Burger RA, et al. 1990. ϩ complement C4B locus in autism. Clin Exp 2000. Microglial activation and neuronal ap- CD4 helper T cell depression in autism. Immunol 83:438–440. optosis in Bornavirus infected neonatal Lewis Immunol Lett 25:341–345. Warren RP, Singh VK, Cole P, et al. 1992. Possible rats. Brain Pathol 10:260–272. Yuwiler A, Shih JS, Chen CH, et al. 1992. Hyper- association of the extended MHC haplotype Weizman A, Weizman R, Szekely GA, et al. 1982. serotoninemia and antiserotonin antibodies in B44-SC30-DR4 with autism. Immunogenet- Abnormal immune response to brain tissue autism and other disorders. J Autism Dev ics 36:203–207. antigen in the syndrome of autism. Am J Disord 22:33–45. Warren RP, Burger RA, Odell D, et al. 1994. Psychiatry 7:1462–1465. Zhao Z-S, Granucci F, Yeh L, et al. 1998. Molec- Decreased plasma concentrations of the C4B Wilton AN, Cobain TJ, Dawkins RL. 1985. Fam- ular mimicry by herpes simplex virus-type 1: complement protein in autism. Arch Pediatr ily studies of IgA deficiency. Immunogenetics autoimmune disease after viral infection. Sci- Adolescent Med 148:180–183. 21:333–342. ence 279:1344–1347.

210 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS ● PATHOGENESIS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ● HORNIG AND LIPKIN