The Formal Expression of Markedness – Ch.9

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Formal Expression of Markedness – Ch.9 CHAPTER 9 SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS 9.1 Introduction This chapter has two goals. The first is to discuss how the aims of this dissertation have been met (§9.2). The second is to re-evaluate the notion of markedness in the light of the results of the preceding chapters (§9.3). 9.2 Aims This dissertation started with an informal statement of two leading ideas, repeated in (1). (1) Leading Ideas (a) Markedness relations between categories may be ignored, but never reversed. (b) The more marked an element is, the greater the pressure to preserve it. Evidence for (1a) was shown to come from systems in which different sonority categories are treated the same for stress purposes (ch.3, ch.4). For example, stress placement in Gujarati is sensitive to sonority distinctions: stress deviates from its default penult position if doing so will allow it to fall on a highly sonorous low vowel: e.g. [tád=et5] ‘recently’, [sinemá] ‘movie theatre’ (ch.3§3.2). However, while stress favours low vowels over all other types, it treats mid and high vowels in exactly the same way: stress does not avoid a penult high vowel for a mid vowel (e.g. [t6hok5ío] ‘girls’, *[t6hók5io]), or avoid a mid vowel for a high vowel (e.g. [t6umÛóte5] ‘74’, *[t6úmÛote5]). In other words, the sonority categories ‘mid peripheral vowel’ and ‘high peripheral vowel’ are conflated for stress purposes in Gujarati. This contrasts with a stress system like Nganasan’s, where stress avoids the default penult position if it contains a high vowel: K ([kóntuÑa] ‘carries’, *[kontúÑa], cf Gujarati [t6 ok5ío]). Evidence for (1b) was shown to come from processes for which marked elements are exempt. For example, chapter 6 provided examples from neutralization in languages like Yamphu (Rutgers 1998). In Yamphu, a general process of Place of Articulation neutralization in syllable codas forces /t/ to surface as a glottal stop: /namÛit/ → [namÛi] ‘daughter-in-law’, cf [namÛid-æ] {instrumental}, /sitÛ-ma/ → [sima] ‘to hit’, cf [sitÛ-a] ‘hit+past’. However, the more marked PoA values ‘labial’ and ‘dorsal’ are exempt from this attrition: e.g. [khap] ‘language’, *[kha], [kep-ma] ‘to stick’; [ælik] ‘bendy’, [khaÛk- pa] ‘scrape one’s throat’. In other words, greater preservation is afforded to the marked PoA values in Yamphu, preventing them from undergoing an otherwise general process. 484 The formal expression of markedness – ch.9 Further evidence for (1b) comes from assimilation (ch.7). While coronals assimilate in Catalan, labials and dorsals do not: /son poks/ → [som poks] ‘they are few’, cf /som dos/ → [som dos] ‘we are two’, [aÚ tontu] ‘stupid year’, [ti1 pres] ‘I am in a hurry’. Again, the marked elements are preserved, exempting them from an otherwise general process. Evidence for both (1a) and (1b) was argued to be found in coalescence (ch.8). In 3aOL IRU H[DPSOH DGMDFHQW Fonsonants had to fuse to form geminates. When dorsals and coronals fused, the output element retained the more marked dorsal feature: /sak-6-ti/ → [sakÛhi], *[satÛhi], *[sacÛhi] ‘be able to {aorist+3p.sg.}. This shows that there is greater pressure to preserve the marked element in this competition, so providing evidence for (1b). ,Q FRQWUDVW WKH IXVLQJ RI ODELDOV DQG FRURQDOV LQ 3aOL SURGXFHV D VHJPHQW ZLWK WKH less marked coronal Place of Articulation: e.g. /lubh-ta/ → [ludÛha] ‘long for {participle}’, *[lubÛha]. Chapter 8 argued that the coronal PoA survives because labials and coronals are HTXDOO\ LPSRUWDQW LQ WHUPV RI SUHVHUYDWLRQ LQ 3aOL 6R XQOLNH GRUVDOV WKH RXWSXW RI Eh-t/ could be either a labial *[bÛh] or coronal [dÛh] and the preservation requirements of the language would be met. In this sense, the mappings /bh-t/ → *[bÛh] and /bh-t/ → [dÛh] are FRQIODWHGWKH\ DUH WUHDWHG LQ H[DFWO\ WKH VDPH ZD\ LQ 3 aOL %HFDXVH VXUYLYDO RI HLWKHU WKH labial or coronal PoA is countenanced, the choice between the two falls to markedness constraints; accordingly, the least marked (coronal) PoA is favoured. Now that evidence for the leading ideas has been reviewed informally, I will turn to a discussion of how the leading ideas are formally implemented. Since a detailed summary of the theoretical proposals has been provided at the end of each previous chapter, the aim of the following subsections is to provide a brief synopsis of the theoretical proposals as they relate to the leading ideas in (1). See the cross-references provided below for more detailed discussion. 9.2.1 Markedness constraints Scale-referring markedness constraints have two tasks: (i) to formally encode hierarchical relations between scale elements and (ii) to allow category conflation. Chapter 3 argued that both (i) and (ii) could be achieved if constraints refer stringently to sets of elements and are freely rankable. An example of the type of constraints advocated here is provided in (3); the constraints refer to the partial vowel sonority scale in (2). The element ∆Ft refers to the head of a foot. (2) (Partial) vowel sonority scale | 〉 i, u 〉 e, o 〉 a | (3) *∆Ft/{}, *∆Ft/{,i/u}, *∆Ft/{,i/u, e/o}, *∆Ft/{, i/u, e/o, a} As an example, the constraint *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} is violated by a stressed syllable that contains a vowel of equal or less sonority than the mid vowels [e o]. For example, [pítk] incurs two violations of this constraint. 485 Paul de Lacy The constraints implement the hierarchy expressed by the scale in (2). More precisely, the constraints prevent reversal of the scale in (2). For example, low vowels are more desirable than mid vowels for stress purposes in Gujarati, as discussed above. This was implemented by ranking the constraint *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} above the constraint that requires penult stress (ALIGNFTR) in ch.3. (4) /tad=et5/ *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} ALIGNFTR (a) ta(d=ét5) *! / (b) (tád=e)t5) * However, mid vowels can never attract stress away from low vowels. For this to happen, there would have to be some constraint that assigned a violation to stressed low vowels but not to stressed mid vowels. As shown in quasi-tableau (5), there is no such constraint. While there is a constraint that favours [á] over [é] (i.e. *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o}), there is no constraint that does the opposite. (5) *∆Ft/{} *∆Ft/{,i/u} *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o,a} (a) # * * * * (b) í * * * (c) é * * (d) á * Because [é] incurs a proper subset of the violations of [á], the ranking of the constraints will make no difference to the relative markedness of [é] and [á] – the former can never be preferred over the latter in terms of these constraints. However, there are constraints that treat the two categories – i.e. mid and low vowels – in the same way. This is crucial for the competition between mid and high vowels in Gujarati. As discussed above, the two categories are conflated for stress purposes. This follows if all constraints that distinguish the two – i.e. *∆Ft/{,i/u} – are ‘inactive’ for stress, which in this case means ‘ranked below ALIGNFTR’. This situation is illustrated in tableau (6). (6) h /t6 ok5io/ *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} ALIGNFTR *∆Ft/{,i/u} (a) (t6hók5i)o * *! / (b) t6hok(5ío) * * The only constraint that favours stressed mid vowels over stressed high vowels is *∆Ft/{,i/u}. Because it is ranked below ALIGNFTR, its violations are inconsequential in determining the winner for stress. 486 The formal expression of markedness – ch.9 In this ranking, it is crucial that *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} assigns exactly the same violations to both (a) and (b). This is how conflation is formally implemented – through the assignment of equal violations. By doing so, *∆Ft/{,i/u,e/o} makes no decision between the two candidates; this allows ALIGNFTR to make the crucial decision, favouring the candidate with stress in the default position. Chapter 3 provided an in-depth discussion of why stringent form is crucial in producing conflation. • Structural elements The other major markedness-related theoretical proposal is that certain scales can combine with structural elements to form constraints. For example, the constraints in (3) are combined with the structural element ‘stressed syllable’. This was argued to be only one of many possible structural elements. In fact, scales like the sonority hierarchy were argued to combine with both heads and non-heads of all prosodic levels. Detailed arguments for this proposal were provided in chapter 4. 9.2.2 Faithfulness constraints Faithfulness constraints must (i) encode the proposal that more marked elements can be subject to greater preservation and (ii) allow for faithfulness conflation. Chapters 6- 8 argued that both (i) and (ii) could be achieved if faithfulness constraints referred to contiguous sets of scale elements and always preserved the most marked element. For example, the faithfulness constraints for the Place of Articulation scale in (7) are provided in (8). (7) Place of Articulation scale | dorsal 〉 labial 〉 coronal 〉 glottal | (8) Place of Articulation (PoA) faithfulness constraints • x corresponds to x' IDENT{dorsal} If x is dorsal, then x' has the same PoA as x. IDENT{dors,lab} If x is dorsal or labial, then x' has the same PoA as x. IDENT{dors,lab,cor} If x is dorsal, labial, or coronal, then x' has the same PoA as x. IDENT{dors,lab,cor,gl} If x has any PoA, then x' has the same PoA as x. 7KH H[DPSOH RI 3aOL FRDOHVFHQFH PHQWLRQHG LQ WKH SUHYLRXV VHFWLRQ ZLOO EH XVHG WR illustrate the two major properties of the ‘marked-faithfulness’ constraints in (8). • Marked-faithfulness When dorsals and coronals coalesce in 3aOL WKH UHVXOW LV D GRUVDOHJ VD k-6-ti/ → [sakÛhi], *[sathÛi].
Recommended publications
  • Plural Words in Austronesian Languages: Typology and History
    Plural Words in Austronesian Languages: Typology and History A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Research Master of Arts in Linguistics by Jiang Wu Student ID: s1609785 Supervisor: Prof. dr. M.A.F. Klamer Second reader: Dr. E.I. Crevels Date: 10th January, 2017 Faculty of Humanities, Leiden University Table of contents Abstract ........................................................................................................................ iii Acknowledgements ....................................................................................................... iv List of tables ................................................................................................................... v List of figures ................................................................................................................ vi List of maps ................................................................................................................. vii List of abbreviations .................................................................................................. viii Chapter 1. Introduction .................................................................................................. 1 Chapter 2. Background literature ................................................................................... 3 2.1. Plural words as nominal plurality marking ....................................................... 3 2.2. Plural words in Austronesian languages ..........................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics of Contrastive Phonation in Gujarati$
    Journal of Phonetics ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Phonetics journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phonetics The phonetics of contrastive phonation in Gujarati$ Sameer ud Dowla Khan n Department of Linguistics, Reed College, 3203 SE Woodstock Boulevard, Portland, OR 97202-8199, USA article info abstract Article history: The current study examines (near-)minimal pairs of breathy and modal phonation produced by ten Received 20 December 2011 native speakers of Gujarati in connected speech, across different vowel qualities and separated by nine Received in revised form equal timepoints of vowel duration. The results identify five spectral measures (i.e. H1–H2, H2–H4, 3 July 2012 H1–A1, H1–A2, H1–A3), four noise measures (i.e. cepstral peak prominence and three measures of Accepted 9 July 2012 harmonics-to-noise ratio), and one electroglottographic measure (i.e. CQ) as reliable indicators of breathy phonation, revealing a considerably larger inventory of cues to breathy phonation than what had previously been reported for the language. Furthermore, while the spectral measures are consistently distinct for breathy and modal vowels when averaging across timepoints, the efficacy of the four noise measures in distinguishing phonation categories is localized to the midpoint of the vowel’s duration. This indicates that the magnitude of breathiness, especially in terms of aperiodicity, changes as a function of time. The current study supports that breathy voice in Gujarati is a dynamic, multidimensional
    [Show full text]
  • LCSH Section K
    K., Rupert (Fictitious character) Motion of K stars in line of sight Ka-đai language USE Rupert (Fictitious character : Laporte) Radial velocity of K stars USE Kadai languages K-4 PRR 1361 (Steam locomotive) — Orbits Ka’do Herdé language USE 1361 K4 (Steam locomotive) UF Galactic orbits of K stars USE Herdé language K-9 (Fictitious character) (Not Subd Geog) K stars—Galactic orbits Ka’do Pévé language UF K-Nine (Fictitious character) BT Orbits USE Pévé language K9 (Fictitious character) — Radial velocity Ka Dwo (Asian people) K 37 (Military aircraft) USE K stars—Motion in line of sight USE Kadu (Asian people) USE Junkers K 37 (Military aircraft) — Spectra Ka-Ga-Nga script (May Subd Geog) K 98 k (Rifle) K Street (Sacramento, Calif.) UF Script, Ka-Ga-Nga USE Mauser K98k rifle This heading is not valid for use as a geographic BT Inscriptions, Malayan K.A.L. Flight 007 Incident, 1983 subdivision. Ka-houk (Wash.) USE Korean Air Lines Incident, 1983 BT Streets—California USE Ozette Lake (Wash.) K.A. Lind Honorary Award K-T boundary Ka Iwi National Scenic Shoreline (Hawaii) USE Moderna museets vänners skulpturpris USE Cretaceous-Paleogene boundary UF Ka Iwi Scenic Shoreline Park (Hawaii) K.A. Linds hederspris K-T Extinction Ka Iwi Shoreline (Hawaii) USE Moderna museets vänners skulpturpris USE Cretaceous-Paleogene Extinction BT National parks and reserves—Hawaii K-ABC (Intelligence test) K-T Mass Extinction Ka Iwi Scenic Shoreline Park (Hawaii) USE Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children USE Cretaceous-Paleogene Extinction USE Ka Iwi National Scenic Shoreline (Hawaii) K-B Bridge (Palau) K-TEA (Achievement test) Ka Iwi Shoreline (Hawaii) USE Koro-Babeldaod Bridge (Palau) USE Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement USE Ka Iwi National Scenic Shoreline (Hawaii) K-BIT (Intelligence test) K-theory Ka-ju-ken-bo USE Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test [QA612.33] USE Kajukenbo K.
    [Show full text]
  • Using 'North Wind and the Sun' Texts to Sample Phoneme Inventories
    Blowing in the wind: Using ‘North Wind and the Sun’ texts to sample phoneme inventories Louise Baird ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Nicholas Evans ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Simon J. Greenhill ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University & Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History [email protected] Language documentation faces a persistent and pervasive problem: How much material is enough to represent a language fully? How much text would we need to sample the full phoneme inventory of a language? In the phonetic/phonemic domain, what proportion of the phoneme inventory can we expect to sample in a text of a given length? Answering these questions in a quantifiable way is tricky, but asking them is necessary. The cumulative col- lection of Illustrative Texts published in the Illustration series in this journal over more than four decades (mostly renditions of the ‘North Wind and the Sun’) gives us an ideal dataset for pursuing these questions. Here we investigate a tractable subset of the above questions, namely: What proportion of a language’s phoneme inventory do these texts enable us to recover, in the minimal sense of having at least one allophone of each phoneme? We find that, even with this low bar, only three languages (Modern Greek, Shipibo and the Treger dialect of Breton) attest all phonemes in these texts.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Groningen Phonological Grammar and Frequency Sloos
    University of Groningen Phonological grammar and frequency Sloos, Marjoleine IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2013 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Sloos, M. (2013). Phonological grammar and frequency: an integrated approach. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 28-09-2021 Summary Phonological grammar and frequency: an integrated approach Language consists of two internal components: the mental lexicon, all memorized morphemes, and the grammar, a set of language rules. In phonology, these two parts are almost always studied independently, viz. usage-based phonology typically investigates the lexicon and generative phonology usually focuses on the grammar. During the last decades, the call for a combined model has gradually become stronger (Ernestus & Baayen (2011), Pierrehumbert (2002), Smolensky & Legendre (2006), van de Weijer (2012)).
    [Show full text]
  • Information Structure in Sembiran Balinese I Wayan Arka Australian National University / Universitas Udayana I Nyoman Sedeng Universitas Udayana
    Chapter 5 Information structure in Sembiran Balinese I Wayan Arka Australian National University / Universitas Udayana I Nyoman Sedeng Universitas Udayana This paper discusses the information structure in Sembiran Balinese, an endangered, con- servative mountain dialect of Balinese. It presents the first detailed description of the ways topic, focus and frame setter in this language interact with each other and with other ele- ments in grammar. It is demonstrated that Sembiran Balinese employs combined strategies that exploit structural positions, morpho-lexical and syntactic resources in grammar. The description is based on a well-defined set of categories of information structure using three semantic-discourse/pragmatic features of [+/−salient], [+/−given] and [+/−contrast]. This novel approach allows for the in-depth exploration of the information structure space in Sembiran Balinese. The paper also highlights the empirical-theoretical contributions ofthe findings in terms of the significance of local socio-cultural context, and the conception of information structural prominence in grammatical theory. 1 Introduction Sembiran Balinese is one of the endangered conservative dialects of Balinese (i.e. Bali Aga, or Mountain Balinese). It is spoken by around 4,500 speakers in the mountainous village of Sembiran in northern Bali.1 Sembiran Balinese has a similar morphosyntax to Plains, or Dataran Balinese, but it is different in that it lacks the speech level system characteristics of Plains Balinese.2 The noticeable difference is therefore related tothe lexical stock, including the pronominal system, which is discussed in §2. 1SBD should be distinguished from the Plains Balinese dialect, which lexically has been influenced by many other languages, namely, Javanese, Sanskrit, English, Arabic and Indonesian.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonology: Analysis and Theory
    This page intentionally left blank Clear and concise, this textbook is an introduction to phonology for students which assumes no prior knowledge of this area of linguistics and provides an overall view of the field which can be covered within one year. The book does not confine itself to any specific theoretical approach and can therefore be used for study within any framework and also to prepare students for work in more specialised frameworks such as Optimality Theory, Government, Dependency and Declarative Phonology. Each chapter focuses on a particular set of theoretical issues including segments, syllables, feet and phonological processing. Gussmann explores these areas using data drawn from a variety of languages including English, Icelandic, Russian, Irish, Finnish, Turkish and others. Suggestions for further reading and summaries at the end of each chapter enable students to find their way to more advanced phonological work. EDMUND GUSSMANN is Professor of General Linguistics at the University of Gda´nsk,Poland. His books include Introduction to Phonological Analysis (1980), Studies in Abstract Phonology (1980), Phono-Morphology (ed., 1985), Licensing in Syntax and Phonology (ed., 1995) and (with A. Doyle) Reverse Dictionary of Modern Irish (1996). Phonology Analysis and Theory EDMUND GUSSMANN University of Gdansk´ Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo Cambridge University Press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge , United Kingdom Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York www.cambridge.org Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521574099 © Edmund Gussmann 2002 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provision of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press.
    [Show full text]
  • Considerations About Semitic Etyma in De Vaan's Latin Etymological Dictionary
    applyparastyle “fig//caption/p[1]” parastyle “FigCapt” Philology, vol. 4/2018/2019, pp. 35–156 © 2019 Ephraim Nissan - DOI https://doi.org/10.3726/PHIL042019.2 2019 Considerations about Semitic Etyma in de Vaan’s Latin Etymological Dictionary: Terms for Plants, 4 Domestic Animals, Tools or Vessels Ephraim Nissan 00 35 Abstract In this long study, our point of departure is particular entries in Michiel de Vaan’s Latin Etymological Dictionary (2008). We are interested in possibly Semitic etyma. Among 156 the other things, we consider controversies not just concerning individual etymologies, but also concerning approaches. We provide a detailed discussion of names for plants, but we also consider names for domestic animals. 2018/2019 Keywords Latin etymologies, Historical linguistics, Semitic loanwords in antiquity, Botany, Zoonyms, Controversies. Contents Considerations about Semitic Etyma in de Vaan’s 1. Introduction Latin Etymological Dictionary: Terms for Plants, Domestic Animals, Tools or Vessels 35 In his article “Il problema dei semitismi antichi nel latino”, Paolo Martino Ephraim Nissan 35 (1993) at the very beginning lamented the neglect of Semitic etymolo- gies for Archaic and Classical Latin; as opposed to survivals from a sub- strate and to terms of Etruscan, Italic, Greek, Celtic origin, when it comes to loanwords of certain direct Semitic origin in Latin, Martino remarked, such loanwords have been only admitted in a surprisingly exiguous num- ber of cases, when they were not met with outright rejection, as though they merely were fanciful constructs:1 In seguito alle recenti acquisizioni archeologiche ed epigrafiche che hanno documen- tato una densità finora insospettata di contatti tra Semiti (soprattutto Fenici, Aramei e 1 If one thinks what one could come across in the 1890s (see below), fanciful constructs were not a rarity.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonology of Quantity in Icelandic and Norwegian
    Dysertacje Wydziału Neofilologii UAM w Poznaniu 25 Przemysław Czarnecki The Phonology of Quantity in Icelandic and Norwegian Wydział Neofilologii UAM w Poznaniu Poznań 2016 The Phonology of Quantity in Icelandic and Norwegian Dysertacje Wydziału Neofilologii UAM w Poznaniu 25 Przemysław Czarnecki The Phonology of Quantity in Icelandic and Norwegian Wydział Neofilologii UAM w Poznaniu Poznań 2016 Projekt okładki: Przemysław Czarnecki Recenzja: prof. dr hab. Stanisław Puppel Copyright by: Przemysław Czarnecki Wydanie I, Poznań 2016 ISBN 978-83-947398-6-7 *DOI: 10.14746/9788394739867* Wydanie: Wydział Neofilologii UAM w Poznaniu al. Niepodległości 4, 61-874 Poznań e-mail: [email protected] www.wn.amu.edu.pl The most remarkable fact about phonological phenomena is that they exist at all Jonathan Kaye (1989): Phonology: a cognitive view. Hillsdale: Erlbaum Table of Contents Introduction ...................................................................... 9 Chapter 1. Sounds of Icelandic and Norwegian ............. 14 1.1 Vowels ................................................................... 15 1.2. Consonants ........................................................... 18 1.3. A note on stress and its relation to vowel length ..... 21 Chapter 2. The main assumptions of the theoretical framework .................................................... 24 Chapter 3. Quantity in Icelandic and Norwegian – “open syllable lengthening” ......................... 32 Chapter 4. Branching onsets in Icelandic and Norwegian ... 38 4.1. Branching onsets
    [Show full text]
  • Fra Sabba Da Castiglione: the Self-Fashioning of a Renaissance Knight Hospitaller”
    “Fra Sabba da Castiglione: The Self-Fashioning of a Renaissance Knight Hospitaller” by Ranieri Moore Cavaceppi B.A., University of Pennsylvania 1988 M.A., University of North Carolina 1996 Thesis Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Italian Studies at Brown University May 2011 © Copyright 2011 by Ranieri Moore Cavaceppi This dissertation by Ranieri Moore Cavaceppi is accepted in its present form by the Department of Italian Studies as satisfying the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Date Ronald L. Martinez, Advisor Recommended to the Graduate Council Date Evelyn Lincoln, Reader Date Ennio Rao, Reader Approved by the Graduate Council Date Peter M. Weber, Dean of the Graduate School iii CURRICULUM VITAE Ranieri Moore Cavaceppi was born in Rome, Italy on October 11, 1965, and moved to Washington, DC at the age of ten. A Fulbright Fellow and a graduate of the University of Pennsylvania, Ranieri received an M.A. in Italian literature from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in 1996, whereupon he began his doctoral studies at Brown University with an emphasis on medieval and Renaissance Italian literature. Returning home to Washington in the fall of 2000, Ranieri became the father of three children, commenced his dissertation research on Knights Hospitaller, and was appointed the primary full-time instructor at American University, acting as language coordinator for the Italian program. iv PREFACE AND ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I deeply appreciate the generous help that I received from each member of my dissertation committee: my advisor Ronald Martinez took a keen interest in this project since its inception in 2004 and suggested many of its leading insights; my readers Evelyn Lincoln and Ennio Rao contributed numerous observations and suggestions.
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Prefix
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Prefix independence: typology and theory A thesis submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Master of Arts in Linguistics by Noah Eli Elkins 2020 © Copyright by Noah Eli Elkins 2020 ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS Prefix independence: typology and theory by Noah Eli Elkins Master of Arts in Linguistics University of California, Los Angeles, 2020 Professor Bruce Hayes, Co-Chair Professor Kie Zuraw, Co-Chair The prefix-suffix asymmetry is an imbalance in the application of phonological processes whereby prefixes are less phonologically cohering to their roots than suffixes. This thesis presents a large- scale typological survey of processes which are sensitive to this asymmetry. Results suggest that prefixes’ relative phonological aloofness (independence) constitutes a widespread and robust generalization, perhaps more so than previously realized. In terms of analysis, I argue that the key concept is the special prominence of initial syllables, supported by much evidence from phonetics, psycholinguistics, and phonology itself. My formal treatment consists of constraint families that serve to support such prominence. I propose that a highly-ranked CRISPEDGE constraint (Itô & Mester 1999) relativized to the left edge of root-initial syllables can account for much of the typological data. This proposal rests on the fact that root-initial syllables constitute a privileged position in phonological grammars (e.g. ii Beckman 1998, Becker et al. 2012), and so to maximize the efficacy of the root-initial percept, segments are hesitant to share their features leftward to target prefixes – and vice versa – as this would blur the strong root-initial boundary.
    [Show full text]
  • Attitudes Towards the Safeguarding of Minority Languages and Dialects in Modern Italy
    ATTITUDES TOWARDS THE SAFEGUARDING OF MINORITY LANGUAGES AND DIALECTS IN MODERN ITALY: The Cases of Sardinia and Sicily Maria Chiara La Sala Submitted in accordance with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Leeds Department of Italian September 2004 This copy has been supplied on the understanding that it is copyright material and that no quotation from the thesis may be published without proper acknowledgement. The candidate confirms that the work submitted is her own and that appropriate credit has been given where reference has been made to the work of others. ABSTRACT The aim of this thesis is to assess attitudes of speakers towards their local or regional variety. Research in the field of sociolinguistics has shown that factors such as gender, age, place of residence, and social status affect linguistic behaviour and perception of local and regional varieties. This thesis consists of three main parts. In the first part the concept of language, minority language, and dialect is discussed; in the second part the official position towards local or regional varieties in Europe and in Italy is considered; in the third part attitudes of speakers towards actions aimed at safeguarding their local or regional varieties are analyzed. The conclusion offers a comparison of the results of the surveys and a discussion on how things may develop in the future. This thesis is carried out within the framework of the discipline of sociolinguistics. ii DEDICATION Ai miei figli Youcef e Amil che mi hanno distolto
    [Show full text]