Observing Ben Wyckoff: from Basic Research to Programmed Instruction and Social Issues Rogelio Escobar and Kennon A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Observing Ben Wyckoff: from Basic Research to Programmed Instruction and Social Issues Rogelio Escobar and Kennon A The Behavior Analyst 2011, 34, 149–170 No. 2 (Fall) Observing Ben Wyckoff: From Basic Research to Programmed Instruction and Social Issues Rogelio Escobar and Kennon A. Lattal West Virginia University L. Benjamin Wyckoff’s seminal contributions to both psychological theory and application are the subject of this review. Wyckoff started his academic career as a graduate student at Indiana University, where he developed the observing-response procedure under the guidance of B. F. Skinner and C. J. Burke. At the University of Wisconsin–Madison, Wyckoff refined his mathematical theory of secondary reinforcement. This theory was the impetus for his creation of an electronic simulation of a rat running a T maze, one of the first ‘‘computer models’’ of learning. Wyckoff next went to Emory University, leaving there to help create two of the most successful companies dedicated to the advancement of programmed instruction and teaching machines: Teaching Machines, Inc. and the Human Development Institute. Wyckoff’s involvement in these companies epitomizes the application of basic behavior-analytic principles in the development of technology to improve education and human relationships. The emergent picture of Wyckoff is that of a man who, through his research, professional work in educational applications of behavioral principles, and active involvement in the civil rights movement of the 1960s, was strongly committed to applying behavioral science to positively influence human behavior change. Key words: Benjamin Wyckoff, observing responses, conditioned reinforcement, teaching machines, programmed instruction, human relationships, diversity issues, client-centered therapy The first author is now at the National Lewis Benjamin Wyckoff, Jr., Ben Autonomous University of Mexico and was to friends and colleagues, was born in supported by a postdoctoral fellowship award- 1922 in Niagara Falls, New York. He ed by the PROFIP of DGAPA-UNAM. This paper was possible thanks to the recollection died at age 84 on June 6, 2007, at St. of many contributors to whom the authors are Simons Island, Georgia. He is per- indebted: Estelle Wyckoff and Andrew Weisk- haps best remembered among behav- off (Benjamin Wyckoff’s family); John Cotton ior analysts as the creator of the and Richard Atkinson (Indiana University); observing-response procedure. In his Lewis Gollub (Harvard University); Bernard Pyron and H. Philip Zeigler (University of doctoral dissertation he noted that, Wisconsin); Stephen Kendall, Donald J. Levis, ‘‘We shall adopt the term ‘observing and William Deckner (Emory University); response’ (RO) to refer to any re- Donald Tosti, Roger Addison, Roger Stein- sponse which results in exposure to horst, Clifton Chadwick, and David Shields (Teaching Machines Inc.); Jerome Berlin, the pair of discriminative stimuli Gene Ruyle, and Schlomo Friedlander (Hu- involved’’ (Wyckoff, 1951b, p. 1). man Development Institute); Lizette Royer Many behavior analysts are famil- (Archives of the History of American Psy- iar with the above statement, because chology); Constance Carter (Library of Con- gress); and Kathleen Shoemaker (Emory Wyckoff’s observing-response proce- University Manuscript, Archives, and Rare dure is still widely used for investi- Book Library). gating conditioned or secondary re- Address correspondence to Rogelio Esco- inforcement (hereafter, conditioned bar, Laboratorio de Condicionamiento Oper- ante, Facultad de Psicologı´a, Universidad reinforcement) (e.g., DeFulio & Nacional Auto´noma de Me´xico. Av. Univer- Hackenberg, 2008; Escobar & Bru- sidad 3004, Col. Copilco-Universidad, Me´xi- ner, 2009; Fantino & Silberberg, co, D.F. C.P. 04510 (e-mail: rescobar@ 2010; Lieving, Reilly, & Lattal, servidor.unam.mx), or to Kennon A. Lattal, Department of Psychology, West Virginia 2006; Pessoa, Huziwara, Perez, En- University, Morgantown, West Virginia demann, & Tomanari, 2009; Shahan 26506 (e-mail: [email protected]). & Podlesnik, 2008). 149 150 ROGELIO ESCOBAR & KENNON A. LATTAL Wyckoff’s observing-response pro- libertarians. Wyckoff’s journey cedure offered a means of demon- through behavior analysis may be strating the acquisition and main- likened to the journey described by tenance of an operant, the only Skinner in ‘‘A Case History in programmed consequence of which Scientific Method’’ (1956): an induc- is the production of discriminative tive approach in which the scientist stimuli, without altering the rate of follows the data wherever they hap- primary reinforcement (for reviews, pen to lead. This approach in Wyck- see Dinsmoor, 1983; Fantino, 1977). off’s career is an instructive counter- The procedure typically consists of point to the numerous examples of two components alternating random- gaps among basic research, applied ly. During one component, responses research, and service delivery aspects are reinforced according to an inter- of behavior analysis (e.g., Michael, mittent schedule (e.g., a variable 1980; Pierce & Epling, 1980). The interval); during the other compo- purpose of this historical review is to nent, responses are not reinforced. A trace the course of Wyckoff’s journey. response on a second operandum briefly produces the stimulus associ- EARLY ACADEMIC LIFE AND ated with the ongoing component INDIANA UNIVERSITY (e.g., a green light during the rein- forcement component and a red light Wyckoff attended Antioch College during the extinction component). from 1941 to 1947, having completed The observing-response procedure is 5 years of premed courses. Following one of most useful methods for graduation, he applied to Harvard studying conditioned reinforcement Medical School, but he changed his (see, e.g., Williams, 1994, for a mind and pursued a PhD in psychol- review). ogy instead. His wife, Estelle, noted A year after he finished his doc- that he learned of Skinner’s work and toral studies, the theoretical Part 1 of decided to study under his supervi- his dissertation was published in sion at Indiana University (E. Wyck- the Psychological Review (Wyckoff, off, personal communication, March 1952). The Social Sciences Citation 22, 2009). Index indicates that this article has According to J. Cotton (personal been cited in over 250 other research communication, February 24, 2009), and theoretical articles in psychology. who became friends with Wyckoff at The procedural details of the observ- Indiana, he and Wyckoff took an ing-response procedure, which con- introductory course on the topic of stituted Part 2 of the dissertation, the experimental analysis of behavior were not published until 1969, how- with Skinner, circa 1947. The goal of ever, in the volume entitled Condi- the course was to familiarize gradu- tioned Reinforcement edited by Hen- ate students with Skinner’s 1938 dry (Wyckoff, 1969). book, The Behavior of Organisms. Designing the observing-response Even before Skinner arrived in procedure is alone sufficient to secure Bloomington in 1945, Indiana Uni- Wyckoff mention in any history of versity was fertile ground for behav- the experimental analysis of behav- iorism and behaviorists. Under the ior. That seminal work, however, influence of J. R. Kantor, W. N. perhaps overshadowed his less well- Kellogg, and R. C. Davis, graduate known work in behavior analysis students were well acquainted with from the mid-1960s that invites the learning theories. As part of the task attention of an even broader audi- to rebuild the Department of Psy- ence of applied behavior analysts, chology after the war, Kantor at- educational technologists, educators, tracted Skinner to Indiana University computer enthusiasts, and even civil (Hearst & Capshew, 1988). In the OBSERVING BEN WYCKOFF 151 following years, the addition to the ing with him on experiments on faculty of W. O. Verplanck, W. K. discrimination in which reinforce- Estes, S. W. Bijou, D. G. Ellson, C. J. ment was contingent on two proper- Burke, W. O. Jenkins, and I. J. ties of the stimuli. Skinner’s notes Saltzman favored the melding of (1983, p. 415) suggest that the ratio- different behavioristic approaches to nale for these experiments was to the study of learning. study concept formation. The origins of behavior analysis From his interactions with Skinner are closely tied to Bloomington, at Indiana, Wyckoff earned an invi- Indiana. The first and second Con- tation to work with Skinner at ferences on the Experimental Analy- Harvard during the summer of sis of Behavior were held there, at 1950. In Cambridge, Wyckoff imme- Indiana University, in 1947 and 1948 diately became good friends with (see Dinsmoor, 1987). Wyckoff may Charles Ferster, with whom he spent be seen in a photograph of the many evenings playing music, some- attendees of the first conference (see times joined by Skinner. Wyckoff p. 456 of Vol. 5 of the Journal of the was a fine pianist (E. Wyckoff, Experimental Analysis of Behavior personal communication, June 18, [JEAB]; see also Hearst & Capshew, 2009). 1988; Skinner, 1979). Wyckoff also According to Skinner (1983, pp. 37, attended the third conference at 415), during his time at Harvard, Columbia University, where a pho- Wyckoff continued their research on tograph (Dinsmoor, 1990) shows him concept formation that was started at standing next to another graduate Indiana. It was supposed to employ a student, Lloyd Homme, who also single-key procedure in which rein- was one of his closest friends in forcement would be contingent on Bloomington. In the ‘‘Conference two properties of the stimuli. Skinner on the Experimental Analysis of wanted
Recommended publications
  • New Directions in Behavioral Activation
    Clinical Psychology Review 79 (2020) 101860 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Psychology Review journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinpsychrev Review New directions in behavioral activation: Using findings from basic science T and translational neuroscience to inform the exploration of potential mechanisms of change Courtney N. Forbes Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Mail Stop 948, 2801 West Bancroft Street, Toledo, OH 43606, USA HIGHLIGHTS • Understanding mechanisms of change can facilitate improvements in BA treatments. • BA treatments may work by targeting (low) reward responsiveness directly. • Basic science findings can inform hypotheses about potential mechanisms of change. ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Interest in behavioral activation treatments for depression has increased over the past two decades. Behavioral Behavioral activation activation treatments have been shown to be effective in treating depression across a variety of populations and Reward responsiveness settings. However, little is known about the mechanisms of change that may bring about symptom improvement Depression in behavioral activation treatments. Recent developments in the theoretical and empirical literature on beha- Mechanisms of change vioral activation treatments have coincided with advances in basic science and translational neuroscience re- Translational neuroscience garding the mechanisms underlying individual differences in responsiveness to reward. Attenuated reward re- sponsiveness has been associated with depression and related clinical outcomes at the self-report, behavioral, and neural levels of analysis. Given that behavioral activation treatments are focused on increasing individuals' contact and engagement with sustainable sources of reward in their environment, it is plausible that behavioral activation treatments bring about improvements in depression symptoms by targeting (low) reward respon- siveness directly.
    [Show full text]
  • General Psychology
    mathematics HEALTH ENGINEERING DESIGN MEDIA management GEOGRAPHY EDUCA E MUSIC C PHYSICS law O ART L agriculture O BIOTECHNOLOGY G Y LANGU CHEMISTRY TION history AGE M E C H A N I C S psychology General Psychology Subject: GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY Credits: 4 SYLLABUS A definition of Psychology Practical problems, Methods of Psychology, Work of Psychologists, Schools of psychology, Attention & Perception - Conscious clarity, determinants of Attention, Distraction, Sensory deprivation, Perceptual constancies, perception of fundamental physical dimensions, Illusions, Organizational factors of perception. Principles of learning Classical conditioning, Operant Conditioning, Principles of reinforcement, Cognitive Learning, Individualized learning, Learner & learning memory - kinds of memory, processes of memory, stages of memory, forgetting. Thinking and language - Thinking process, Concepts. Intelligence & Motivation Theories - Measurement of Intelligence; Determinants; Testing for special aptitudes, Motivation - Motives as inferences, Explanations and predictors, Biological motivation, Social motives, Motives to know and to be effective. Emotions Physiology of emotion, Expression of emotions, Theories of emotions; Frustration and conflict, Personality - Determinants of Personality, Theories of personality Psychodynamic, Trait, Type, Learning, Behavioural & Self: Measurement of personality Suggested Readings: 1. Morgan, Clifford. T., King, Richard. A., Weisz, John.R., Schopler, John, Introduction to Psychology, TataMcGraw Hill. 2. Marx, Melvin H.
    [Show full text]
  • Reinforcement
    Reinforcement This article is about the psychological concept. For behavior but this term may also refer to an enhancement the construction materials reinforcement, see Rebar. of memory. One example of this effect is called post- For reinforcement learning in computer science, see training reinforcement where a stimulus (e.g. food) given Reinforcement learning. For beam stiffening, see shortly after a training session enhances the learning.[2] Stiffening. This stimulus can also be an emotional one. A good ex- In behavioral psychology, reinforcement is a ample is that many people can explain in detail where they were when they found out the World Trade Center was attacked.[3][4] Reinforcement is an important part of operant or instrumental conditioning. 1 Introduction B.F. Skinner was a well-known and influential researcher who articulated many of the theoretical constructs of reinforcement and behaviorism. Skinner defined rein- forcers according to the change in response strength (re- sponse rate) rather than to more subjective criteria, such as what is pleasurable or valuable to someone. Accord- Diagram of operant conditioning ingly, activities, foods or items considered pleasant or en- joyable may not necessarily be reinforcing (because they consequence that will strengthen an organism’s future be- produce no increase in the response preceding them). havior whenever that behavior is preceded by a specific Stimuli, settings, and activities only fit the definition of antecedent stimulus. This strengthening effect may be reinforcers if the behavior that immediately precedes the measured as a higher frequency of behavior (e.g., pulling potential reinforcer increases in similar situations in the a lever more frequently), longer duration (e.g., pulling future; for example, a child who receives a cookie when a lever for longer periods of time), greater magnitude he or she asks for one.
    [Show full text]
  • Operantsissue III, 2017 from the President
    ISSN 2476-0293 OperantsISSUE III, 2017 from the president was recently talking with a researcher in child development. Naturally, I mentioned Skinner. The researcher said, “of course his science works in the lab, but I have to work in the real world.” Scientific principles, I pointed Iout, are universal. Gravity does not work just in a lab. Nor do the laws of operant conditioning. Skinner made his discovery in a lab, but selection by consequences operates with all organisms and in all settings. This issue of Operants illustrates places around the world where behaviorology is “working.” Julie S. Vargas, Ph.D. President, B. F. Skinner Foundation Chinese Traditional Translated by Kiwiya Zhang 最近我与一位儿童发展领域的研究者聊天。很自然地,我提起了Skinner。这位研究者说:“当然他的科学在实验室里说得通,但我却是要在真实 世界里工作。”我指出,科学的原则是具有普遍性的。万有引力不仅仅在实验室里有用,操作制约也是。Skinner在实验室里发现了后效对行为的 作用,但该作用在所有场合、对所有生命体都有效。本期Operants就阐释了行为学在哪些地方“有用”。 Czech Translated by Helena Vadurova Nedávno jsem mluvila s jedním výzkumníkem, který se zabývá vývojem dítěte. Přirozeně jsem zmínila Skinnera. Ten výzkumník mi řekl: „jeho práce samozřejmě funguje v laboratoři, ale já musím pracovat ve skutečném světě.“ Vědecké principy, poznamenala jsem, jsou uni- verzální. Gravitace funguje nejen v laboratoři. Stejné je to se zákony operantního podmiňování. Skinner svůj objev učinil v laboratoři, ale výběr na základě následků funguje u všech organizmů a ve všech prostředích. Toto vydání časopisu Operants představuje různá místa na světě, kde behaviorální věda „funguje“. French Translated by MarieCeline Clemenceau J’ai récemment échangé avec un chercheur dans le développement de l’enfant. Naturellement, j’ai mentionné Skinner. Le chercheur a déclaré: «Bien sûr, sa science fonctionne dans un laboratoire, mais je dois travailler dans le monde réel».
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Models of Depression: a Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement
    International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy Volume 4, No. 2, 2008 Behavioral Models of Depression: A Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement Paulo Roberto Abreu & Carlos E. Santos Abstract This paper provides a review of behavioral models of depression highlighting the problems associated with its historical emphasis on lowered frequencies of positive reinforcement. We analyzed the models of Ferster and Lewinsohn in their theoretical approach, methodology and application. We conducted a review of the suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, by the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation and by operant extinction. A number of recommendations are made concerning how these processes can inform interventions. We conclude that interventions geared towards identifying aversive control play a promising role in clinical change in the treatment of depression. Keywords: Aversive Control, Response-Contingent Positive Reinforcement, Depression, Clinical Behavior Analysis, Functional Assessment, Behavioral Activation. The present article aims to revisit the principles of behavioral models of depression by emphasizing advancements in the field, as well as providing a critique of the dominant models. With this goal in mind, we aim to re-direct the conceptual analysis towards an understanding of the environmental variables frequently observed in the behaviors of depressed individuals. Drawing from the existing literature, we propose possible variables associated with behavioral suppression and its mechanisms during the development and course of depressive behavior repertories. Three particular behavioral processes are considered: (1) suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, (2) the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation, and (3) operant extinction. We begin with a discussion of the dominant models of depression, followed by a critique, and we conclude with a discussion of how each of these processes can inform clinical interventions.
    [Show full text]
  • B. F. Skinner's Analysis of Verbal Behavior: a Chronicle
    ISSN 1982-3541 Belo Horizonte-MG, Brazil 2007, Vol. IX, nº 2, 21-38 B. F. Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior: a chronicle Ernst A. Vargas Julie S. Vargas B. F. Skinner Foundation Terry J. Knapp University of Nevada, Las Vegas Abstract A time line of Skinner’s work on verbal behavior must start at the very beginning of his scientific career. Skinner laid down the conceptual framework of his Theory of Behavior as early as his thesis. He developed his experimental work within this framework, and depended on research results to adjust and modify his theory. At the same time, he initiated efforts to interpret verbal behavior within his theoretical formulations and upon the operant relations he discovered in the laboratory. Work on both mediated and nonmediated relations entwined throughout his career. He discovered the basic verbal relations slowly and inductively over a twenty-five year span of effort. Key words: Skinner, Verbal behavior, History, Theory. Introduction ior derived from an experimental analysis of a more rigorous sort” (Skinner, 957, p. ). Skinner’s writing of Verbal Behavior From the beginning, it was an effort about took place over many years, in many settings, which he was quite explicit as he stated in a and with continuous revisions. More impor- letter to Fred Keller, “What I am doing is ap- tantly, it took place within the implicit frame- plying the concepts I’ve worked out experi- work of his theory of behavior, primarily mentally to this non-experimental (but Em- based on the process of behavioral selection. pirical) field” (Skinner, July 2, 1934).
    [Show full text]
  • Some Historic Roots of Education Reform
    Some Historic Roots of Education Reform Francis Mechner The Mechner Foundation Queens Paideia School 1 INTRODUCTION Those of us who are old enough can remember the big splashes that educational technology made in the 1960s and 1970s with involvement of the national media, the White House, Congress, state agencies, and corporate America, and international reverberations via UNESCO and the OECD. What follows is my attempt to weave the connecting threads of these achievements into a story that reveals them as precursors of current work in school reconfiguration and education reform. THE COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY PSYCHOLOGY DEPARTMENT OF THE 1950s That’s where the story begins. It was a time of unabashed idealism, of gradu- ate students giddily discussing the ways in which the emerging science of behavior would transform society. We were on fire with a sense of mission. Each of us had reached it by a different path. My own featured a war- whipped childhood with multiple close calls in the Holocaust. Survivor’s guilt? Maybe. What I know for sure is that I was left with a strong sense of obligation. Perhaps it was my Viennese upbringing that led me to assume that I would fulfill it as a painter or pianist, but at age 20, I discovered what seemed like a more impactful way—the science of behavior. In any case, I adopted a monastic existence in which personal comforts had no standing. I spent 14-hour days, 7-day weeks in the lab, feeling dedicated to the advancement of behavioral science and its application to human affairs.
    [Show full text]
  • AUTHOR Emotionally Disturbed. Volume 1: Identification And
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 229 948 EC 151 957 AUTHOR Noel, Margaret M., Ed.; Haring, Norris G., Ed. TITLE Progress or Change: Issues in Educating the Emotionally Disturbed. Volume 1: Identification and Program Planning. INSTITUTION Washington Univ., Seattle. Program Development Assistance System. SPONS AGENCY Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services (ED), Washington, DC. Div. of Innovation and Development. PUB DATE Sep 82 CONTRACT 300-79-0062 NOTE 172p.; For a related document, see EC 151 958. PUB TYPE Books (010) -- Viewpoints (120) Information Analyses (070) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC07 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Administrative Problems; Agency Cooperation; *Behavior Disorders; *Definitions; Delivery Systems; Educational History; Elementary Secondary Education; *Emotional Disturbances; Evaluation Methods; *Handicap Identification; Program Development; Student Evaluation; Student Placement ABSTRACT Seven papers focus on issues in identification and educational program planning for children with emotional disturbances. "Social Policy Issues in Special Education and Related Services for 'Emotionally Disturbed Children and Youth" (J. Kauffman), touches on problems with the federal definition of and terminology about the emotionally disturbed and suggests possible reasons for inadequate services to this population. Two major areas (conceptual and procedural) relating to the assessment of mild behavior disorders in children are examined in "Assessment of Behavior Disorgaers in the School Setting: Issues, Problems, and Strategies" by H. Walker. Biophysical, psychodynamic, behavioral, sociological, and ecological perspectives in assessment are considered, and a 9-step model is proposed by B. Algozzine ("Assessment of Severe Behavior Disorders"). S. Braaten presents "A Model for the Differential Assessmet and Placement of Emotionally Disturbed Students in Special Education Programs," in which data are assigned priority ratings to convey the extent of the student's disturbing behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Hypnotically Catalyzing Experiential Learning Across Treatments For
    Intl. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58(2): 186–201, 2010 Copyright © International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis ISSN: 0020-7144 print / 1744-5183 online DOI: 10.1080/00207140903523228 NHYP0020-71441744-5183Intl. Journal of Clinical and ExperimentalHYPNOTICALLY Hypnosis,Hypnosis Vol. 58, No. 2, Dec 2009: pp. 0–0 CATALYZING EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING ACROSS TREATMENTS FOR DEPRESSION: Actions Can Speak Louder Than Moods CatalyzingMICHAEL BehavioralD. YAPKO Activation with Hypnosis MICHAEL D. YAPKO1 Private Practice, Fallbrook, California, USA Abstract: A number of psychotherapeutic approaches for the treat- ment of major depression have received empirical support in the liter- ature, most notably cognitive-behavioral and interpersonal therapies. Recent studies have shown the therapeutic value of the behavioral activation component of such interventions. Depressed individuals actively learning and applying new skills on their own behalf is widely considered a critical component of recovery. This article describes the use of hypnosis to catalyze experiential learning and to encourage behavioral activation in the depressed client by directly addressing and transforming cognitive and perceptual patterns that can impede such behavioral activation, especially global thinking and ruminative coping styles. Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) has become the most widely studied psychotherapeutic treatment for depression and has many outcome studies supporting its therapeutic efficacy (Hollon, Thase, & Markowitz, 2002). However, new psychotherapy treatments that focus on aspects of depression other than cognition have been emerging in recent years (Lau, 2008). The original behavioral perspectives of Charles Ferster’s functional analysis of depression and Peter Lewinsohn’s behavioral model (Ferster, 1973; Lewinsohn, 1974) have been supported by current research. In 1996, Neil Jacobson and his colleagues conducted a study of the effective ingredients of CBT for depression (Jacobson et al., 1996).
    [Show full text]
  • Report of the HEW Work Group on Behavior Analysis in Education
    MICROCOPY RESOLUTION TEST CHART NA1141k_BUREAUJOrzSTANDARDS41963:A DOCUMENT RESUME 'ED 079 233 SP 006 614- AUTHOR Stern, Joyce D. TITLE Report of the HEW Work,Group on Behavior Analysis in Education. Final Draft. INSTITUTION Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Washington, D.C...Office of the AssiStant Secretary for Planning. and Evaluation. PUB DATE Jan 73 NOTE 178p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$6.58 DESCRIPTORS -*Behavior; *Behavioral Science Research; *Behavior Change; Behavior Patterns; *Behavior Theories; Research Projects; Student Behavior; Teacher Behavior IDENTIFIERS Department of Health Education and Welfare 'ABSTRACT The Work Group on Behavior Modification in Education, which issued this report, was authorized by Secretary Elliot Richardson'on November 21, 1970._The body of this report includes an analysis of the benefits- of behavior modification techniques in education, examples of their successful appliCation with a variety of large student populations, an examination of the problems raised by the approach,- an assessment of its readiness to receive widespread promotion, and recommendations of possible next steps that could be taken. by U:S. Meliartment of Health,-Education and Welfare and the Office of Education relative to further-research.and dissemination. There are'four-appendixes; Appendix A'contains a list of'general references of basic literature in the discipline; Appendix B provides an overview of behavior_modification_activitieS in the range of. educational settings; Appendix C lists selected materials available for instructing parents and teachers in behavior analysis techniques; and Appendix D consists of detailed reports of 15 major centers of activity in behavior modification, (Author/JA3 FILMED FROM-BEST AVAILABLECOPY FINAL DRAFT U S DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH.
    [Show full text]
  • What 50 Years of Research Tell Us About Pausing Under Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement Henry D
    The Behavior Analyst 2008, 31, 39–60 No. 1 (Spring) What 50 Years of Research Tell Us About Pausing Under Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement Henry D. Schlinger California State University, Los Angeles Adam Derenne University of North Dakota Alan Baron University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee Textbooks in learning and behavior commonly describe performance on fixed-ratio schedules as ‘‘break and run,’’ indicating that after reinforcement subjects typically pause and then respond quickly to the next reinforcement. Performance on variable-ratio schedules, on the other hand, is described as steady and fast, with few long pauses. Beginning with Ferster and Skinner’s magnum opus, Schedules of Reinforcement (1957), the literature on pausing under ratio schedules has identified the influences on pausing of numerous important variables, in particular ratio size and reinforcement magnitude. As a result, some previously held assumptions have been called into question. For example, research has shown that the length of the pause is controlled not only by the preceding ratio, as Ferster and Skinner and others had assumed (and as implied by the phrase postreinforcement pause), but by the upcoming ratio as well. Similarly, despite the commonly held belief that ratio pausing is unique to the fixed-ratio schedule, there is evidence that pausing also occurs under variable-ratio schedules. If such widely held beliefs are incorrect, then what about other assumptions? This article selectively examines the literature on pausing under ratio schedules over the past 50 years and concludes that although there may indeed be some common patterns, there are also inconsistencies that await future resolution. Several accounts of pausing under ratio schedules are discussed along with the implications of the literature for human performances, most notably the behaviors termed procrastination.
    [Show full text]
  • THE NEW BEHAVIORISM Mind, Mechanism and Society
    THE NEW BEHAVIORISM Mind, Mechanism and Society John Staddon CONTENTS Part 1: History Chapter 1: Early Behaviorism Chapter 2: Behaviorism and Learning Psychology Part 2: Radical Behaviorism Chapter 3: Radical Behaviorism, I: Method Chapter 4: Radical Behaviorism, II: Explanation Chapter 5: Skinner and Theory Chapter 6: Variation and Selection Chapter 7: Behavior-Evolution Parallels Chapter 8: Rationality Chapter 9: Truth, Science and Behaviorism Chapter 10: Free Will and Utopia Chapter 11: Values Chapter 12: Skinner and Mental Life Part 3: The New Behaviorism Chapter 13: Cognitivism and The New Behaviorism i Chapter 14: Internal States: The Logic of Historical Systems Chapter 15: Consciousness and Theoretical Behaviorism Postscript to Parts 1-3: Alchemy of the Mind Part 4: Behaviorism and Society Chapter 16: Law, Punishment and Behaviorism Chapter 17: The Health-Care Schedule Chapter 18: Reinforcement and ‘Socialized’ Medicine Chapter 19: Teaching ii Preface to the Second Edition This edition is almost completely rewritten. It is about 45% longer than the first. I cover two new social issues and also devote more space to the philosophy of cognitivism and the science behind theoretical behaviorism. B. F. Skinner figures less prominently in this edition than the last, but his work is a theme that still runs through many chapters – because his influence has been so great and his writings raise so many provocative issues that are identified with behavior- ism. But, as readers of the first edition detected, my position is far from Skinnerian. Many reviewers thought well of the first edition but, naturally, I want to attend to those who did not.
    [Show full text]