Increased Writing Activity Inneurological Conditions: a Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Increased Writing Activity Inneurological Conditions: a Review 51051ournal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1996;61:510-514 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.61.5.510 on 1 November 1996. Downloaded from SHORT REPORT Increased writing activity in neurological conditions: a review and clinical study P van Vugt, P Paquier, L Kees, P Cras Abstract of behaviour and that similar increased writing Increased writing activity in a 70 year old, activities have been labelled differently. We right handed man presenting with a his- would like to clear up the terminological con- tory of alcohol misuse and maturity onset fusion in the literature on increased writing diabetes is reported. Brain CT disclosed activity by reporting another patient and by corticosubcortical atrophy and 99mTc- differentiating automatic writing behaviour HMPAO SPECT disclosed severe bilat- from hypergraphia. eral frontal hypoperfusion more prominent on the right. The patient's neuropsychological symptomatology con- Case report sisted of severe (verbal) aspontaneity, A 70 year old right handed man with a long intermittent utilisation behaviour, and history of alcohol and tobacco misuse and a pronounced increased writing activity, previous medical history of well controlled which mainly consisted of a persevera- non-insulin dependent diabetes mellitus was tive, micrographic written reproduction admitted after a fall causing a left orbitomaxil- of visually or verbally perceived language lar fracture. He had been drinking several fragments. Several neurological causes of beers a day for many years and had shown increased writing activity and the equivo- progressive memory disturbances during the cal terminology met in the medical litera- year before admission. Relatives reported dys- ture are reviewed. A distinction between praxic behaviour, decreased verbal output, hypergraphia and automatic writing and two fugue episodes. Clinical examination behaviour is proposed. It is concluded on admission showed a frail, apathetic old that our patient's increased writing activ- man who was disoriented in time and space. ity may be characterised as automatic His liver was slightly enlarged. There was no writing behaviour. tremor and no asterixis. He displayed bilateral grasping, and there was a clear bilateral pal- http://jnnp.bmj.com/ (3 Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996;61:510-514) momental reflex. During his stay in hospital the patient remained aspontaneous. Only rarely did he respond, with one or two words, Keywords: frontal lobe; hypergraphia; compulsive adequately answering the question. He walked writing Unit of with a wide base and there was bilateral Neurolinguistics, reduced diadochokinesia. However, there was University of Antwerp Increased writing activity in mental illness is a no ataxia nor dysmetria. on September 25, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. (UIA), Belgium P van Vugt well accepted topic of interest.'2 On the other Laboratory tests showed slightly raised L Kees hand, abnormal writing behaviour resulting ammonia (up to 66 mg/100 ml). Hepatic Department of ENT from neurological disorder is seldom reported. enzymes were normal, except for gGT which Surgery, University of Probably the best known example is the ictal3 was slightly raised. Chest radiography, urine, Antwerp (UIA) and Department of or interictal increased writing activity seen in thyroid function, serology (for Treponema pal- Neurology, University some patients with temporal lobe epilepsy.' 4 5 6 lidum, HIV), uric acid, lipids, vitamin B 12, Hospital Erasme However, its frequency and specificity in tem- and CSF tests were negative or normal. His (ULB), Belgium poral lobe epilepsy4 have been questioned: EEG showed theta waves in the occipital P Paquier increased writing activity has also been lobes. Brain CT disclosed pronounced, gener- Department of Neurology (Born- described in mentally retarded patients alised corticosubcortical atrophy and ventricu- Bunge Foundation), (epileptic and non-epileptic)7 and in patients lomegaly more prominent on the right. As University of Antwerp with brain tumours,"9 cerebral strokes,'0 multi- normal pressure hydrocephalus was suspected (UIA), Belgium P Cras ple sclerosis,"I or frontal lobe dementia.'2 Omnipaqueg was injected intrathecally. In the past, increased has The of CSF was normal. 99mTc- Correspondence to: writing activity resorption Dr P van Vugt, Unit of been referred to as hypergraphia,' echo- HMPAO SPECT showed severe frontal Neurolinguistics, CBL-UIA, on Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 graphia, graphomania,8 anosognosic graph- hypoperfusion, more pronounced the right Wilrijk, Belgium. omimia, 4 graphorrh6e,'5 and phonographie.'6 (fig 1). Received 22 November 1995 Although diagnostic criteria have been pro- The mini mental state examination22 was and in final revised form 3 July 1996 posed,'4 '7 a comparative study of case reports difficult to evaluate due to the lack of commu- Accepted 4 July 1996 shows that one term may cover different types nication, but the patient scored 11/30 at his Increased wniting activity in neurological conditions: a review and clinical study 511 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.61.5.510 on 1 November 1996. Downloaded from L- F,:--, 4Aol TrRANst)5EF::=E -S I : n tv yq - v A.r-;i-. t.. ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~iI =1 6-- :i Figurel1 99mTc-HMPAO SPECT images show severefrontal hypoperfsion,more pronounced on the right. best. Psychiatric examination showed some care was taken to replace the ballpoint pen loss of decorum. The patient did not confabu- used the day before by a differently coloured late. There was no obsessive compulsive per- one. To observe the patient's communicative =~~~~~~~~sonality disorder and during clinical bedside reaction when confronted with unexpected examination no obsessional traits were found. written stimuli, we surreptitiously inserted On the ward he displayed a mild form of utili- questions about his youth, his family, the hos- http://jnnp.bmj.com/ sation behaviour and a pronounced increased pital food, etc in the otherwise blank note- writing activity. book. Instructions related to writing as well as During an observation period of five weeks, geometric figures alternated with these ques- the patient produced 22 925 written words. tions, as we wanted to assess the patient's abil- The writing was micrographic but legible (fig ity to adapt his behavioural pattern and the 2). Generally, the patient copied whatever he degree to which the imitation was servile set his eyes on: a newspaper headline, the (table). on September 25, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. instructions on the television set, the trade The differences in ink disclosed that during mark on his pencil. He went on copying until the observation period the patient wrote three the end of the page or until another visual to four pages a day. He tended to fill all the stimulus caught his eye (table). The copying space available on the pages, and would often was rather careless: punctuation, numbers, return to previous pages to fill up the margins and abbreviations were not always respected, horizontally and vertically. Except for one conjunctions were often added (and ... and ... instance of shadow copying, small as well as and), prepositions were substituted. The large geometric patterns were disregarded and intrusion of neologisms suggested lexical wan- simply covered with text. Horizontal lines did dering (for example, de eeuwige achtervelden, not influence the writing activity, whereas ver- the happy backing grounds). As he became tical lines seem to have been perceived and more distracted during the act of writing, he respected as margins, or at least as an element would insert questions asked to him or a sen- organising writing space. Usually, questions tence overheard in a conversation. As a result, and instructions were copied several times. the text sometimes became very awkward and Occasionally a semantically related one word difficult to understand. or one syntagma response was formulated and The patient's writing activity was closely repeated over and over again. More than once, monitored during weeks 2 and 3. The nursing the copied instruction was personalised-for staff made sure that the patient only had a example, "You must not write here" became notebook at his disposal and every morning "I must not write here". 512 van Vugt, Paquier, Kees, Cras J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.61.5.510 on 1 November 1996. Downloaded from 2 'JI a a §4W44<Cg>GA%410, t2 -4- -'~~~~.s~ -~~~~~~~~~~~~ cO a1) *_ 0 N- 0 0~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ 4-J 't -T X CC -jziDX*10 j4/4>dS<AtA44%tso1m4t1 CD o 4 Ii:~~~~~~~: http://jnnp.bmj.com/ V~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~4 on September 25, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~+-D~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~- C Q +1 9I Aj--~~. '- -4a1~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~'0-'- ViV ~ I_q a -3 44 -. 4- 0-. Increased writing activity in neurological conditions: a review and clinical study 513 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.61.5.510 on 1 November 1996. Downloaded from Some examples ofinstructions and stimuli randomly inserted in a blank DINAS notebook, and description of the patient's graphic response Instruction or stimulus Spatial display Patient's response Write the word steam engine See fig 2A The word is initially copied four times. Later on, the three times in the box patient wrote the word another five times in the box. Finally, the page is covered with the word. Copy below the following Printed at 130 mm The sentence is copied once. The next day we found sentence once from the top out that the same sentence had been used to cover the entire page as well as the next two pages. Do not write on this side of Printed in a 60 mm wide The ban is copied on the right side of the line; afterwards, the line margin, see fig 2B the patient fills up the restricted area. Do you like the food? Printed at the page centre Unrelated page covering writing, until the question is reached. Below the question is answered 64 times: "Yes, very much". Diamond 140 x 90 mm Page centred The patient keeps on writing straight across the geometric figure.
Recommended publications
  • Frontotemporal and Dopaminergic Control of Idea Generation and Creative Drive
    THE JOURNAL OF COMPARATIVE NEUROLOGY 493:147–153 (2005) Frontotemporal and Dopaminergic Control of Idea Generation and Creative Drive ALICE W. FLAHERTY* Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114 ABSTRACT This article presents a three-factor anatomical model of human idea generation and creative drive, focusing on interactions between the temporal lobes, frontal lobes, and limbic system. Evidence is drawn from functional imaging, drug studies, and lesion analysis. Temporal lobe changes, as in hypergraphia, often increase idea generation, sometimes at the expense of quality. Frontal lobe deficits may decrease idea generation, in part because of rigid judgments about an idea’s worth. These phenomena are clearest in verbal creativity, and roughly parallel the pres- sured communication of temporal lobe epilepsy, mania, and Wernicke’s aphasia—compared to the sparse speech and cognitive inflexibility of depression, Broca’s aphasia, and other frontal lobe lesions. The phenomena also shape non-linguistic creativity, as in that of frontotemporal demen- tia. The appropriate balance between frontal and temporal activity is mediated by mutually inhibitory corticocortical interactions. Mesolimbic dopamine influences novelty seeking and cre- ative drive. Dopamine agonists and antagonists have opposite effects on goal-directed behavior and hallucinations. Creative drive is not identical to skill—the latter depends more on neocortical association areas. However, drive correlates better with successful creative output than skill does. Traditional neuroscientific models of creativity, such as the left brain – right brain hemi- spheric model, emphasize skills primarily, and stress art and musical skill at the expense of language and mathematics. The three-factor model proposed here predicts findings in a broad range of normal and pathological states and can be tested in many experimental paradigms.
    [Show full text]
  • The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders Diagnostic Criteria for Research
    The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders Diagnostic criteria for research World Health Organization Geneva The World Health Organization is a specialized agency of the United Nations with primary responsibility for international health matters and public health. Through this organization, which was created in 1948, the health professions of some 180 countries exchange their knowledge and experience with the aim of making possible the attainment by all citizens of the world by the year 2000 of a level of health that will permit them to lead a socially and economically productive life. By means of direct technical cooperation with its Member States, and by stimulating such cooperation among them, WHO promotes the development of comprehensive health services, the prevention and control of diseases, the improvement of environmental conditions, the development of human resources for health, the coordination and development of biomedical and health services research, and the planning and implementation of health programmes. These broad fields of endeavour encompass a wide variety of activities, such as developing systems of primary health care that reach the whole population of Member countries; promoting the health of mothers and children; combating malnutrition; controlling malaria and other communicable diseases including tuberculosis and leprosy; coordinating the global strategy for the prevention and control of AIDS; having achieved the eradication of smallpox, promoting mass immunization against a number of other
    [Show full text]
  • A Critical and Descriptive Approach to Interictal Behavior with the Neurobehavior Inventory (NBI)
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Repositório Institucional UNIFESP Epilepsy & Behavior 25 (2012) 334–340 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Epilepsy & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh A critical and descriptive approach to interictal behavior with the Neurobehavior Inventory (NBI) Guilherme Nogueira M. de Oliveira a,b,⁎, Arthur Kummer a, Renato Luiz Marchetti c, Gerardo Maria de Araújo Filho d, João Vinícius Salgado a, Anthony S. David e, Antônio Lúcio Teixeira a a Neuropsychiatric Unit, Neurology Division, School of Medicine, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil b Epilepsy Treatment Advanced Centre (NATE), Felicio Rocho Hospital, Belo Horizonte, Brazil c Department and Institute of Psychiatry, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo (FMUSP), São Paulo, Brazil d Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (UNIFESP), São Paulo, Brazil e Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, DeCrespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK article info abstract Article history: Purpose: The purpose of this study was to test the psychometric properties of the Neurobehavior Inventory Received 23 July 2012 (NBI) in a group of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) patients from a tertiary care center, correlating its scores Revised 21 August 2012 with the presence of psychiatric symptoms. Accepted 23 August 2012 Methods: Clinical and sociodemographic data from ninety-six TLE outpatients were collected, and a neuro- Available online 24 October 2012 psychiatric evaluation was performed with the following instruments: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), structured psychiatric interview (MINI-PLUS), Neurobehavior Inventory (NBI), and Hamilton Keywords: Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D).
    [Show full text]
  • NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Neurol
    NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Neurol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 October 22. NIH-PA Author ManuscriptPublished NIH-PA Author Manuscript in final edited NIH-PA Author Manuscript form as: J Comp Neurol. 2005 December 5; 493(1): 147±153. doi:10.1002/cne.20768. FRONTOTEMPORAL AND DOPAMINERGIC CONTROL OF IDEA GENERATION AND CREATIVE DRIVE Alice W. Flaherty1 1Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02114, U.S.A. Abstract This paper presents a three-factor anatomical model of human idea generation and creative drive, focusing on interactions between the temporal lobes, frontal lobes, and limbic system. Evidence is drawn from functional imaging, drug studies, and lesion analysis. Temporal lobe changes, as in hypergraphia, often increase idea generation, sometimes at the expense of quality. Frontal lobe deficits may decrease idea generation, in part because of rigid judgments about an idea's worth. These phenomena are clearest in verbal creativity, and roughly parallel the pressured communication of temporal lobe epilepsy, mania, and Wernicke's aphasia--compared to the sparse speech and cognitive inflexibility of depression, Broca's aphasia, and other frontal lobe lesions. The phenomena also shape non-linguistic creativity, as in that of frontotemporal dementia. The appropriate balance between frontal and temporal activity is mediated by mutually inhibitory corticocortical interactions. Mesolimbic dopamine influences novelty seeking and creative drive. Dopamine agonists and antagonists have opposite effects on goal-directed behavior and hallucinations. Creative drive is not identical to skill—the latter depends more on neocortical association areas. However, drive correlates better with successful creative output than skill does.
    [Show full text]
  • Geschwind Syndrome in Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration: Neuroanatomical and Neuropsychological Features Over 9 Years
    cortex 94 (2017) 27e38 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Behavioural Neurology Geschwind Syndrome in frontotemporal lobar degeneration: Neuroanatomical and neuropsychological features over 9 years Laura Veronelli a,b, Sara J. Makaretz a, Megan Quimby a, * Bradford C. Dickerson a and Jessica A. Collins a, a Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA b Department of Neurorehabilitation Sciences, Casa di Cura Del Policlinico, Milan, Italy article info abstract Article history: Geschwind Syndrome, a characteristic behavioral syndrome frequently described in pa- Received 17 January 2017 tients affected by temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), consists of the following features: hyper- Reviewed 7 April 2017 religiosity, hypergraphia, hyposexuality, and irritability. Here we report the 9-year-clin- Revised 31 May 2017 ical course of a case of Geschwind Syndrome that developed as a first and salient clinical Accepted 6 June 2017 expression of right temporal lobe variant of frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD). Action editor Stefano Cappa Only one patient affected by frontotemporal dementia has previously been shown to Published online 27 June 2017 present with Geschwind Syndrome. MS presented at age 73 with 3 years of personality and behavioral symptoms. Her early Keywords: symptoms primarily included hyper-religiosity, hypergraphia, and poor emotional regu- Right temporal lobe lation (irritability, impulsivity, disinhibition, egocentric behavior). Over nine years, other Frontotemporal lobar degeneration cognitive functions (word retrieval, memory coding and recall, set-shifting, famous face Geschwind Syndrome and building recognition) became affected; however, hyper-religiosity, hypergraphia, and Hypergraphia scarce emotional control remained her most prominent deficits.
    [Show full text]
  • Religious and Mystical Experiences As Artifacts of Temporal Lobe Function: a General Hypothesis
    Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1983, 57, 1255-1262. @ Perceptual and Motor Skills 1983 RELIGIOUS AND MYSTICAL EXPERIENCES AS ARTIFACTS OF TEMPORAL LOBE FUNCTION: A GENERAL HYPOTHESIS MICHAEL A. PERSINGER Summary.-Mystical and religious experiences are hypothesized to be evoked by transient, electrical microseizures within deep structures of the temporal lobe. Although experiential details are affected by context and rein- forcement history, basic themes reflect the inclusion of different amygdaloid- hippocampal structures and adjacent cortices. Whereas the unusual electrical coherence allows access to infantile memories of parents, a source of god expec- tations, specific stimulation evokes out-of-body experiences, space-time dis- tortions, intense meaningfulness, and dreamy scenes. The species-specific simi- larities in temporal lobe properties enhance the homogeneity of cross-cultural experiences. They exist along a continuum that ranges from "early morning highs" to recurrent bouts of conversion and dominating religiosity. Predispos- ing factors include any biochemical or genetic factors that produce temporal lobe lability. A variety of precipitating stimuli provoke these experiences, but personal (life) crises and death bed conditions are optimal. These temporal lobe microseizures can be learned as responses to existenrial trauma because stimulation is of powerful intrinsic reward regions and reduction of death anxiety occurs. The implications of these transients as potent modifiers of human behavior are considered. The neuropsychological basis of religious experiences and God beliefs has been avoided by behavioral scientists. Yet these experiences, in conjunction with the confrontation and attenuation of death anxiety, constitute a major class of human behaviors whose frequency is rivaled only by sex and aggression. This paper briefly describes a general hypothesis that religious and mystical experiences ate normal consequences of spontaneous biogenic stimulation of temporal lobe structures.
    [Show full text]
  • The Lateralising Significance of Hypergraphia in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy
    Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on April 18, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com Joutrnial of Neurology, Nelurosurgery, an?d Psychiatry 1982 ;45 :131-138 The lateralising significance of hypergraphia in temporal lobe epilepsy JKA ROBERTS, MM ROBERTSON, MR TRIMBLE From the Department of Psychological Medicinie, The National Hospital for Nervous Diseases, London SUMMARY Six patients with hypergraphia and epilepsy are presented and their clinical features compared with other patients reported in the literature. It is suggested that hypergraphia occurs more frequently in patients with right-sided non-dominant temporal lobe lesions, in contrast for example to the schizophreniform presentation of left-sided lesions. Other features of psychopathology possibly associated with non-dominant lesions, including elation, hyperreligiosity and dej"a vu experiences, are also discussed. The study of inter-ictal behaviour in patients with functions. In addition Dimond and Farrington18 temporal lobe epilepsy has improved our under- made the observation that split-brain patients tend to standing of the different roles of the temporo-limbic give an impression of euphoric mood and show structures in the dominant and non-dominant cere- emotional responses to unpleasant visual material bral hemispheres of the brain. It has been suggested mainly if this material is directed to the non-domin- that an ictal focus in the dominant temporal lobe is ant hemisphere. Finally, Lishman19 studying patients associated with aggressive behaviourl-3 and schizo- with focal brain damage has shown an association phrenia-like psychoses,3-10 although in the latter between affective disorders and damage to the case only small samples have been used and others non-dominant cerebral hemisphere.
    [Show full text]
  • Temporal Lobe Epilepsy and Dostoyevsky Seizures: Neuropathology and Spirituality
    Temporal lobe epilepsy and Dostoyevsky seizures: Neuropathology and Spirituality Dr Alasdair Coles Religious Seizures The fullest description of what have been termed ‘religious seizures’, comes from the writing of Dostoyevsky (hence the sobriquet ‘Dostoyevsky seizures’), particularly in the character of Prince Myshkin in ‘The Idiot,’ whose epilepsy is a key motif throughout the book. For instance: He [Myshkin] remembered that during his epileptic fits, or rather immediately preceding them, he had always experienced a moment or two when his whole heart and mind, and body seemed to wake up to vigour and light; when he became filled with joy and hope, and all his anxieties seemed to be swept away for ever; these moments were but presentiments, as it were, of the one final second (it was never more than a second) in which the fit came upon him. (page 139) (Dostoyevsky, 1869). The seizures described by Dostoyevsky do not have explicit religious content, so the term ‘religious seizure’ is unhelpful. More accurate perhaps is James Leuba’s ‘ecstatic seizures’. In his 1925 classic, he describes some cases: Among the dread diseases that afflict humanity there is one that interests us quite particularly; that disease is epilepsy. Its main manifestation is often preceded by curious signs, varying greatly from person to person, but fairly constant in the same person. In some instances, the ‘aura,’ as these premonitory symptoms are called, is in the nature of an ecstasy. In Modern Medicine, Dr Spratling reports the case of a priest under his care whose epileptic attacks were preceded by a rapturous moment.
    [Show full text]
  • – the Neuroscience of Religious & Spiritual
    FEATURE “NEUROTHEOLOGY” – THE NEUROSCIENCE OF RELIGIOUS & SPIRITUAL EXPERIENCES Ian Westmore This article is based on a presentation given by Dr Westmore at the Sanofi In- Focus academic weekend in Somerset West on 8th March 2019. ost psychiatrists would have been trained which people live,” incorporating to work with their patients according to personal growth or transformation, the biopsychosocial model that was first usually in a context separate proposed by George Engel in 1977. This from organized religious Mmodel was an important step in acknowledging the institutions, such as a belief in a fact that in any illness there are in addition to the supernatural (beyond the known biological, also psychosocial determinants. and observable) realm, personal growth, a quest for an ultimate As psychiatrists however, we have come to appreciate or sacred meaning, religious that there is probably another dimension that should experience, or an encounter with Ian Westmore be added to this model – the spiritual. So for many one’s own “inner dimension”.2 nowadays, the model has changed to a “bio-psycho- social-spiritual” model. NEUROTHEOLOGY FEW OF US NOWADAYS WOULD DISPUTE THIS COULD BE SIMPLY DEFINED AS THE FACT THAT THE ABILITY TO EXPERIENCE “THE NEUROSCIENCE OF THEOLOGICAL A SPIRITUAL DIMENSION AND PRACTICE BELIEF”3, BUT OVER THE YEARS THE RELIGION IS A BRAIN FUNCTION. THESE UNDERSTANDING OF NEUROTHEOLOGY ALSO SEEM TO BE EXPERIENCES THAT HAS EVOLVED AND ONE COULD ARE UNIQUELY HUMAN. SO, WE MIGHT NOW SAY THAT, “NEUROTHEOLOGY IS ASK, WHAT IS IT IN, OR ABOUT OUR MULTIDISCIPLINARY IN NATURE AND BRAINS THAT ENABLES US TO EXPERIENCE INCLUDES THE FIELDS OF THEOLOGY, SPIRITUALITY AND PRACTICE RELIGION? RELIGIOUS STUDIES, RELIGIOUS EXPERIENCE AND PRACTICE, PHILOSOPHY, [It needs mentioning that whilst the terms “religion” and “spirituality” are often used interchangeably, COGNITIVE SCIENCE, NEUROSCIENCE, they are not the same.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychologising and Neurologising About Religion: Facts, Fallacies and the Future Transcript
    Psychologising and Neurologising about Religion: Facts, Fallacies and the Future Transcript Date: Wednesday, 23 January 2008 - 12:00AM PSYCHOLOGISING AND NEUROLOGISING ABOUT RELIGION Professor Malcolm Jeeves CBE FRSE Welcome George R. Bush The Revd George R. Bushhas been Rector of St Mary-le-Bowsince 2002. Before coming to St Mary’s he was Vicar of St Anne’s, Hoxton, having held earlier appointments as a curate in inner-city Leeds and a chaplain at Cambridge. He has studied history, history of art, canon law and theology and has published on certain developments in canon law involving a previous Bishop of London, Edmund Gibson (1669–1748). He is a past President of Sion College. In 2007 he co-edited (with Michael Byrne) a 380-page volume of essays - St Mary-le- Bow: A History. When I was a chaplain to students, it fell to me to interview all new entrants individually as part of their initiation. I had a carefully crafted and rehearsed conversation which involved explaining the life of the chapel and the role of the chaplain as an independent non-academic resource. One student replied to this explanation, boldly I thought, ‘Oh you’re a sort of social worker’. Although that may well have amply described what the authorities - for whom my likely pastoral competence was the touchstone - hoped for from me, I felt it as a trivialisation and an offence. I wrote at the time that ‘the display of being approachable and unshockable may be read as giving the nod to unbelief or the absence of personal moral seriousness’.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neurobiological Basis of Hyper-Religiosity
    The neurobiological basis of hyper-religiosity Bachelor thesis in Cognitive Neuroscience Department Psychology and Health Section Cognitive Neuroscience Tilburg University Author: Daniëlle Bouman Supervisor: A. van Boxtel Date: June 2011 2 Abstract The neurobiological basis of hyper-religiosity is discussed by comparing the neurobiological substrates of the four disorders in which hyper-religiosity usually occurs. These disorders are obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), schizophrenia, temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), and mania. After an introduction on hyper-religiosity, the four disorders and their neurobiological basis are discussed in four separate chapters. An integrating chapter compares all brain areas involved in the four disorders and through this comparison, a general neurobiological basis of hyper-religiosity is found. The main areas involved in hyper- religiosity are the frontal lobes, the temporal lobes, and the limbic system. In the discussion, the limitations and validity of the thesis are discussed, and hyper-religiosity is compared to the regular expression of religiosity. Keywords: Hyper-religiosity, obsessive-compulsive disorder, schizophrenia, temporal lobe epilepsy, mania. 3 Table of contents 1. Introduction ............................................................................................................................ 4 2. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and hyper-religiosity ............................................................ 6 Brain areas ............................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 10. Delirium, Dementia, and Amnestic and Other Cognitive Disorders
    CHAPTER 10. DELIRIUM, DEMENTIA, AND AMNESTIC AND OTHER COGNITIVE DISORDERS Kaplan & Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry CHAPTER 10. DELIRIUM, DEMENTIA, AND AMNESTIC AND OTHER COGNITIVE DISORDERS ERIC D. CAINE, M.D. AND JEFFREY M. LYNESS, M.D. Definition History Comparative Nosology Diagnosis Pathology and Laboratory Examination Etiology and Differential Diagnosis Cognitive Disorders Diagnosis and Clinical Features Diagnosis and Clinical Features Psychiatry is in the midst of a profound transformation, at once struggling to incorporate a dynamic understanding of neuroscience and molecular biology while maintaining a view of unique persons or individuals as the central focus of therapeutic intervention. To date it has been beyond the scope of knowledge to effectively integrate research data regarding individual differences with more abstract findings regarding fundamental aspects of brain development or aging-related neurodegeneration. Discovering the bases for the major neuropsychiatric diseases can be expected to provide powerful clues for defining the nature of how neurobiological processes are expressed as emotions, thoughts, or actions, or how life events and daily experiences alter and shape brain growth and development. Since the late 1980s, a major conceptual transition has occurred in the way clinicians and researchers view the relation between mental disorders and brain function. For much of the past century psychiatry was trapped in an either-or dilemma—either a condition was viewed as a symptomatic manifestation of structural cerebral or systemic pathology (organic), or it was considered psychological or emotional in nature (functional). However, clinicians recognized that there are no behaviors that do not involve the brain, and that the transmission of culturally derived processes from individual to individual is influenced by each person's central nervous system (CNS).
    [Show full text]