Perception of Relative Pitch with Different References: Some Absolute
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Absolute and Relative Pitch Processing in the Human Brain: Neural and Behavioral Evidence
bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/526541; this version posted March 18, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. Absolute and relative pitch processing in the human brain: Neural and behavioral evidence Simon Leipold a, Christian Brauchli a, Marielle Greber a, Lutz Jäncke a, b, c Author Affiliations a Division Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland b University Research Priority Program (URPP), Dynamics of Healthy Aging, University of Zurich, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland c Department of Special Education, King Abdulaziz University, 21589 Jeddah, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia Corresponding Authors Simon Leipold Binzmühlestrasse 14, Box 25 CH-8050 Zürich Switzerland [email protected] Lutz Jäncke Binzmühlestrasse 14, Box 25 CH-8050 Zürich Switzerland [email protected] Keywords Absolute Pitch, Multivariate Pattern Analysis, Neural Efficiency, Pitch Processing, fMRI Acknowledgements This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF), grant no. 320030_163149 to LJ. We thank our research interns Anna Speckert, Chantal Oderbolz, Désirée Yamada, Fabian Demuth, Florence Bernays, Joëlle Albrecht, Kathrin Baur, Laura Keller, Marilena Wilding, Melek Haçan, Nicole Hedinger, Pascal Misala, Petra Meier, Sarah Appenzeller, Tenzin Dotschung, Valerie Hungerbühler, Vanessa Vallesi, -
TUNING JUDGMENTS 1 1 2 3 4 Does Tuning Influence Aesthetic
TUNING JUDGMENTS 1 1 2 3 4 5 Does Tuning Influence Aesthetic Judgments of Music? Investigating the Generalizability 6 of Absolute Intonation Ability 7 8 Stephen C. Van Hedger 1 2 and Huda Khudhair 1 9 10 11 1 Department of Psychology, Huron University College at Western 12 2 Department of Psychology & Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Author Note 21 Word Count (Main Body): 7,535 22 Number of Tables: 2 23 Number of Figures: 2 24 We have no known conflict of interests to disclose. 25 All materials and data associated with this manuscript can be accessed via Open Science 26 Framework (https://osf.io/zjcvd/) 27 Correspondences should be sent to: 28 Stephen C. Van Hedger, Department of Psychology, Huron University College at Western: 1349 29 Western Road, London, ON, N6G 1H3 Canada [email protected] 30 31 32 TUNING JUDGMENTS 2 1 Abstract 2 Listening to music is an enjoyable activity for most individuals, yet the musical factors that relate 3 to aesthetic experiences are not completely understood. In the present paper, we investigate 4 whether the absolute tuning of music implicitly influences listener evaluations of music, as well 5 as whether listeners can explicitly categorize musical sounds as “in tune” versus “out of tune” 6 based on conventional tuning standards. In Experiment 1, participants rated unfamiliar musical 7 excerpts, which were either tuned conventionally or unconventionally, in terms of liking, interest, 8 and unusualness. In Experiment 2, participants were asked to explicitly judge whether several 9 types of musical sounds (isolated notes, chords, scales, and short excerpts) were “in tune” or 10 “out of tune.” The results suggest that the absolute tuning of music has no influence on listener 11 evaluations of music (Experiment 1), and these null results are likely caused, in part, by an 12 inability for listeners to explicitly differentiate in-tune from out-of-tune musical excerpts 13 (Experiment 2). -
Philosophy of Music Education
University of New Hampshire University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository Honors Theses and Capstones Student Scholarship Spring 2017 Philosophy of Music Education Mary Elizabeth Barba Follow this and additional works at: https://scholars.unh.edu/honors Part of the Music Education Commons, and the Music Pedagogy Commons Recommended Citation Barba, Mary Elizabeth, "Philosophy of Music Education" (2017). Honors Theses and Capstones. 322. https://scholars.unh.edu/honors/322 This Senior Honors Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses and Capstones by an authorized administrator of University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Philosophy of Music Education Mary Barba Dr. David Upham December 9, 2016 Barba 1 Philosophy of Music Education A philosophy of music education refers to the value of music, the value of teaching music, and how to practically utilize those values in the music classroom. Bennet Reimer, a renowned music education philosopher, wrote the following, regarding the value of studying the philosophy of music education: “To the degree we can present a convincing explanation of the nature of the art of music and the value of music in the lives of people, to that degree we can present a convincing picture of the nature of music education and its value for human life.”1 In this thesis, I will explore the philosophies of Emile Jacques-Dalcroze, Carl Orff, Zoltán Kodály, Bennett Reimer, and David Elliott, and suggest practical applications of their philosophies in the orchestral classroom, especially in the context of ear training and improvisation. -
Effects of Harmonics on Relative Pitch Discrimination in a Musical Context
Perception & Psychophysics 1996, 58 (5), 704-712 Effects of harmonics on relative pitch discrimination in a musical context LAUREL J. TRAINOR McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada The contribution of different harmonics to pitch salience in a musical context was examined by re quiring subjects to discriminate a small (% semitone) pitch change in one note of a melody that re peated in transposition. In Experiment 1,performance was superior when more harmonics were pres ent (first five vs. fundamental alone) and when the second harmonic (of tones consisting of the first two harmonics) was in tune compared with when it was out of tune. In Experiment 2, the effects ofhar monies 6 and 8, which stand in octave-equivalent simple ratios to the fundamental (2:3 and 1:2, re spectively) were compared with harmonics 5 and 7, which stand in more complex ratios (4:5 and 4:7, respectively). When the harmonics fused into a single percept (tones consisting of harmonics 1,2, and one of 5, 6, 7, or 8), performance was higher when harmonics 6 or 8 were present than when harmon ics 5 or 7 were present. When the harmonics did not fuse into a single percept (tones consisting of the fundamental and one of 5, 6, 7, or 8), there was no effect of ratio simplicity. This paper examines the contribution of different har gion depended to some extent on the fundamental fre monics to relative pitch discrimination in a musical con quency: The fourth and higher harmonics dominated for text. Relative pitch discrimination refers to the ability to fundamental frequencies up to 350 Hz, the third and higher compare the pitch interval (i.e., distance on a log frequency for fundamentals between 350 and 700 Hz, the second and scale) between one set oftwo tones and another, where the higher for fundamental frequencies between 700 and fundamentals ofthe tones are at different absolute frequen 1400 Hz, and the first for frequencies above 1400 Hz. -
The Perception of Melodic Consonance: an Acoustical And
The perception of melodic consonance: an acoustical and neurophysiological explanation based on the overtone series Jared E. Anderson University of Pittsburgh Department of Mathematics Pittsburgh, PA, USA Abstract The melodic consonance of a sequence of tones is explained using the overtone series: the overtones form “flow lines” that link the tones melodically; the strength of these flow lines determines the melodic consonance. This hypothesis admits of psychoacoustical and neurophysiological interpretations that fit well with the place theory of pitch perception. The hypothesis is used to create a model for how the auditory system judges melodic consonance, which is used to algorithmically construct melodic sequences of tones. Keywords: auditory cortex, auditory system, algorithmic composition, automated com- position, consonance, dissonance, harmonics, Helmholtz, melodic consonance, melody, musical acoustics, neuroacoustics, neurophysiology, overtones, pitch perception, psy- choacoustics, tonotopy. 1. Introduction Consonance and dissonance are a basic aspect of the perception of tones, commonly de- scribed by words such as ‘pleasant/unpleasant’, ‘smooth/rough’, ‘euphonious/cacophonous’, or ‘stable/unstable’. This is just as for other aspects of the perception of tones: pitch is described by ‘high/low’; timbre by ‘brassy/reedy/percussive/etc.’; loudness by ‘loud/soft’. But consonance is a trickier concept than pitch, timbre, or loudness for three reasons: First, the single term consonance has been used to refer to different perceptions. The usual convention for distinguishing between these is to add an adjective specifying what sort arXiv:q-bio/0403031v1 [q-bio.NC] 22 Mar 2004 is being discussed. But there is not widespread agreement as to which adjectives should be used or exactly which perceptions they are supposed to refer to, because it is difficult to put complex perceptions into unambiguous language. -
A Definition of Song from Human Music Universals Observed in Primate Calls
bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/649459; this version posted May 24, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. This article is a US Government work. It is not subject to copyright under 17 USC 105 and is also made available for use under a CC0 license. A definition of song from human music universals observed in primate calls David Schruth1*, Christopher N. Templeton2, Darryl J. Holman1 1 Department of Anthropology, University of Washington 2 Department of Biology, Pacific University *Corresponding author, [email protected] bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/649459; this version posted May 24, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. This article is a US Government work. It is not subject to copyright under 17 USC 105 and is also made available for use under a CC0 license. Abstract Musical behavior is likely as old as our species with song originating as early as 60 million years ago in the primate order. Early singing likely evolved into the music of modern humans via multiple selective events, but efforts to disentangle these influences have been stifled by challenges to precisely define this behavior in a broadly applicable way. Detailed here is a method to quantify the elaborateness of acoustic displays using published spectrograms (n=832 calls) culled from the literature on primate vocalizations. Each spectrogram was scored by five trained analysts via visual assessments along six musically relevant acoustic parameters: tone, interval, transposition, repetition, rhythm, and syllabic variation. -
Absolute Pitch (AP)
Absolute Pitch (AP) • A.k.a. ‘perfect pitch’ • The ability to name or produce a tone without a reference tone • Very rare: 1 in 10,000 Vs. Relative pitch (RP) • Most people use relative pitch: • Recognizing tones relative to other tones • Remember and produce intervals abstracted from specific pitch, or given a reference pitch AP: how it works • Thought to be a labeling process: – AP possessors associate names/ meaning with pitches or pitch classes – Retain this association over time • AP is not ‘perfect’; i.e., auditory perception/ pitch discrimination not more accurate than RP Imaging evidence • When making judgments using AP: • possessors compared to non- possessors show more activation in frontal naming/labeling areas • Anatomically, AP possessors show greater planum temporale asymmetry – Apparently due to reduced RH PT size AP ‘flavors’ • AP not purely ‘have’ or ‘have-not; ability level varies along continuum • Some possessors make more accurate judgments with certain instruments – e.g. piano vs. pure sine wave tones – Sometimes called ‘absolute piano’ AP ‘flavors’ cont’d • Other possessors may perform more accurately with white-key notes than black-key notes – E.g. C,D,E vs. C#, D# • May be due to early learning influence – Early musical training on keyboard usually starts with white-key notes only • So, is AP learned? Learnable? Nature vs. Nurture, of course • The debate continues: – Some researchers ascribe genetic origins to AP, suspecting that early musical training is neither sufficient nor necessary – Others find most possessors -
Frequency Ratios and the Perception of Tone Patterns
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1994, 1 (2), 191-201 Frequency ratios and the perception of tone patterns E. GLENN SCHELLENBERG University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada and SANDRA E. TREHUB University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada We quantified the relative simplicity of frequency ratios and reanalyzed data from several studies on the perception of simultaneous and sequential tones. Simplicity offrequency ratios accounted for judgments of consonance and dissonance and for judgments of similarity across a wide range of tasks and listeners. It also accounted for the relative ease of discriminating tone patterns by musically experienced and inexperienced listeners. These findings confirm the generality ofpre vious suggestions of perceptual processing advantages for pairs of tones related by simple fre quency ratios. Since the time of Pythagoras, the relative simplicity of monics of a single complex tone. Currently, the degree the frequency relations between tones has been consid of perceived consonance is believed to result from both ered fundamental to consonance (pleasantness) and dis sensory and experiential factors. Whereas sensory con sonance (unpleasantness) in music. Most naturally OCCUf sonance is constant across musical styles and cultures, mu ring tones (e.g., the sounds of speech or music) are sical consonance presumably results from learning what complex, consisting of multiple pure-tone (sine wave) sounds pleasant in a particular musical style. components. Terhardt (1974, 1978, 1984) has suggested Helmholtz (1885/1954) proposed that the consonance that relations between different tones may be influenced of two simultaneous complex tones is a function of the by relations between components of a single complex tone. ratio between their fundamental frequencies-the simpler For single complex tones, ineluding those of speech and the ratio, the more harmonics the tones have in common. -
Absolute Pitch
EXPLORING THE SELF-CONCEPT AND IDENTITY OF SYDNEY CONSERVATORIUM STUDENTS WITH AND WITHOUT ABSOLUTE PITCH. Julie O’Connor A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Music (Music Education) (Honours), Sydney Conservatorium of Music, University of Sydney. 2006 ii Abstract Absolute Pitch (AP) is the ability to identify pitches without external references (Parncutt & Levitin, 2001). It is a rare ability that is more prevalent among musicians. This qualitative study explored the perceptions of Sydney Conservatorium of Music students through interviews, focusing on the value of AP possession, and implications for music self-concept. The study involved 12 Conservatorium University and High School students; six participants were self- nominated absolute pitch possessors, and the remaining six were categorised as relative pitch (RP) users. Through discussions of the value, prevalence and practicality of AP, the data suggested that AP is a highly desirable ability among Conservatorium students, and particularly valued by those who possess it. The results also suggested that RP students tend to have less positive self-concepts in aural perception and music theory, while having more positive self-concepts in other musical arenas. The majority of the AP participants had a desire to become a solo performer, and the RP participants’ tended to plan broader musical goals such as combining teaching and ensemble performance. These results suggested that the possession of AP has had a significant effect on the identity of these individuals. iii Acknowledgements First and foremost, I would like to thank my supervisor, James Renwick, whose insightful advice (and subtle pushing) inspired and motivated me throughout the study. -
Relative Pitch and the Song of Black-Capped Chickadees
Relative Pitch and the Song of Black-Capped Chickadees Chickadees, like people, have a strong sense of relative pitch. These birds use skillful, precise pitch changes to advertise their quality and attract mates Ronald Weisman and Laurene Ratcliffe ore than 2,000 years ago, the acer- descriptions of birdsongs. Donald Bor- Music and Relative Pitch Mbic philosopher Marcus Tullius ror, a master bioacoustician and field Relative pitch is the ability to produce Cicero observed that Roman songbirds biologist, found many of the song de- or recognize a note based on a refer- compose more excellent melodies than scriptions inadequate by modern stan- ence note. The ability to hum a simple any musician. He certainly doesn’t dards, as he wrote in his foreword to tune when given a set note with which stand alone in history on that count; it the 1967 reprinting of Mathews’s book. to start, as most people can, shows is a nearly universal human experience Borror acknowledged that Mathews relative pitch in action. A sense of to find joy and wonder in birdsong— lacked modern electronic equipment relative pitch is common among non- and to compare the songs to human and that his primary interest was in musicians even if they never use the music. People have been transcribing the musical content of birdsongs. To- term, but musical training develops melodies of birds into the notation of day, however, Mathews’s approach this knack. Trained singers have an ad- music since at least the 18th century; seems dated and quaint. Musical no- vanced appreciation of pitch change: Vivaldi’s Goldfinch concerto and Han- tation is simply unable to provide the They can produce precise relative fre- del’s Cuckoo and the Nightingale organ detailed, accurate and reproducible quency changes from one note to the concerto include musical notation for descriptions required in modern bio- next in a melody. -
Universal and Non-Universal Features of Musical Pitch Perception Revealed by Singing
Article Universal and Non-universal Features of Musical Pitch Perception Revealed by Singing Highlights Authors d Pitch perception was probed cross-culturally with sung Nori Jacoby, Eduardo A. Undurraga, reproduction of tone sequences Malinda J. McPherson, Joaquı´n Valdes, Toma´ s Ossando´ n, d Mental scaling of pitch was approximately logarithmic for US Josh H. McDermott and Amazonian listeners Correspondence d Pitch perception deteriorated similarly in both groups for very high-frequency tones [email protected] (N.J.), [email protected] (J.H.M.) d Sung correlates of octave equivalence varied across cultures and musical expertise In Brief Jacoby et al. use sung reproduction of tones to explore pitch perception cross- culturally. US and native Amazonian listeners exhibit deterioration of pitch at high frequencies and logarithmic mental scaling of pitch. However, sung correlates of octave equivalence are undetectable in Amazonians, suggesting effects of culture-specific experience. Jacoby et al., 2019, Current Biology 29, 3229–3243 October 7, 2019 ª 2019 Elsevier Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.020 Current Biology Article Universal and Non-universal Features of Musical Pitch Perception Revealed by Singing Nori Jacoby,1,2,9,* Eduardo A. Undurraga,3,4 Malinda J. McPherson,5,6 Joaquı´n Valdes, 7 Toma´ s Ossando´ n,7 and Josh H. McDermott5,6,8,* 1Computational Auditory Perception Group, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt 60322, Germany 2The Center for Science and Society, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, -
Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report: Music and Cognition Margaret Tennant
Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report M. Tennant 1 Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report: Music and Cognition Margaret Tennant, Ph. D. May 26, 2012 Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report M. Tennant 2 Table of Contents Prologue 3 Sabbatical Report 4 Interactive Lecture, Multicultural Day April 10, 2012 18 Suggested Activities for Applying to Introductory Psychology 24 Conference Schedule SMPC 27 Bibliography 33 Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report M. Tennant 3 Prologue I would like to thank the Board of Trustees of the Ventura Community College District; Dr. James Meznek, Chancellor; Dr. Pam Eddinger, Moorpark College President; and the Moorpark College Academic Senate; for this opportunity for scholarly study during the Fall 2011 semester. The contents of this report include the following: 1) The Sabbatical Report: A brief overview of content researched during the sabbatical 2) Applications: Ideas for incorporating music into an introductory psychology course and the outline of the interactive lecture presented to students on Multicultural Day April 10, 2012. 3) Conference Schedule: The schedule of the Society of Perception and Music Cognition, held August 11-14, 2011. 4) Bibliography: A complete list of readings referenced or read during my Fall 2011 sabbatical. Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report M. Tennant 4 Music and Cognition: Fall 2011 Sabbatical Report Music is an integral part of the human experience, yet it largely omitted from introductory psychology texts (Sloboda & Juslin, 2010). It is not, however, omitted from students' lives. Music accompanies them as they socialize, shop, work, exercise, and worship. Music directs their thoughts and emotions in the films they see and the songs that they hear. How does this occur and what gives music such power? This sabbatical project was designed to better understand the underlying mechanisms of experiencing music and thus apply psychology to one of the passions of students’ lives.