INFORMATION TO USERS

This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UM( films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some these and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough. substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these wilt be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversee materials (e.g.. maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographicaity in this copy. Higher quality 6’ x 9“ black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

ProQuest Information and Learning 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 800-521-0600

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. EFFECTS OF ADAPTED PLUS FEEDBACK ON THE ACQUISTION, MAINTENANCE, AND GENERALIZATION OF CONVENTIONAL SKILLS FOR CHILDREN WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION

DISSERTATION

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of

Philosophy in the Graduate School ofThe Ohio State University

By

Tammy L. Burt M.Ed.

The Ohio State University 2002

Dissertation Committee: Approved by . . Dr. David Porretta, Advisor Advisor Dr. Jacqnefme Goodwary College o f Education

Dr. Timothy Heron

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UMI Number 3049000

UMf

UMI Microform 3049000 Copyright 2002 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company. Alt rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company 300 North Zeeb Road P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of using a series of adapted

bicycles plus feedback on the acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of

conventional cycling skills by children with mild mental retardation. The children ranged

in agp from 7 to ll years old. Participants were introduced to cycling skills through a

series of four adapted bicycles and one conventional , designed to allow them to

gradually become accustomed to the dynamics o f cycling. Positive corrective and

positive specific feedback were provided. Feedback focused on three aspects of

performance; (I) pedal rate, (2) head position, and(3) steering participation. Participants

were required to ride each of the bicycles independently for a distance of 12 m, in 3 out

of 5 consecutive trials. Data analysis included visual inspection of graphical performance.

All ten participants (100%) were successful in acquiring conventional cycling skills. Six

of the 10 participants (60%) demonstrated maintenance, while 3 of the 10 participants

(30%) demonstrated generalization of conventional cycling skills. While not achieving

criterion levels for maintenance and generalization, four participants exhibited partial

maintenance levels and seven participants exhibited partial generalization levels. Results

are discussed relative to dynamic systems theory and motor skill development

n

i Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Dedicated to Llovd Thomas Burt, my brother and inspiration

m

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEGMENTS

The author coaid not have completed this manuscript without the assistance and courtesy of many individuals. First, I would like to thank Dr. David Porretta for his expertise and patience over the past few years, as he guided me along my path towards commencement In addition, I would like to extend gratitude to my committee members, Dr. Jacqueline Goodway and Dr. Timothy Heron. To Dr. Jacqueline Goodway, I appreciate your encouragement and thank you for sharing your knowledge and resources relative to dynamic systems theory and motor development To Dr. Timothy Heron, I thank you for your steadfastness, professionalism in teaching and your research design expertise in applied behavioral analysis. I am indebted to Dr. Richard Klein, who provided me with considerable insight and the generous use of his adapted bicycles. Many doctoral students and friends agreed to assist in data collection. I am grateful for your gift of time. Thanks to Ian Pena, Dr.Heather Savage, Chad Crowe, Harriet Amui, Patrick Akuffo, Ismael FIores-Marti, Lloyd L. Burt, Linda Burt, Tricia Lantz, Shea Cusick and Barbara Brenton-Sahr. Finally, I would like to give my warmest thanks to my friends and family who consistently supported my efforts and exhibited supreme patience and understanding. To Timothy Sahr, I thank you for saving me with your computer savy. To Dr. Sue Sutherland, I thank you for your friendship and for accompanying me over much o f this journey. To Heather Savage, you have been a source of many cherished memories and have served as a brilliant “star". I thank you for your warm friendship. To my family, thank you for your unconditional love.

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. VITA

June 2 ,1 9 6 2 ...... Baltimore, Maryland

1984 ...... BA. Physical Education, Andeison University

1997...... M£d. Special Education, Bowling Green State University

1985-1988 ...... Teacher/Coach, Groveport-Madison Schools Groveport, Ohio

1988-1989 ...... Teacher/Coach, Madison Christian School Groveport, Ohio

1991-1995 ...... Recreation Program Director, The Buckeye Ranch Intensive Care Center, Grove City, Ohio

1995-1997 ...... Graduate Research Assistant, Bowling Green State University

1997-2000 ...... Graduate Teaching and Research Assistant, The Ohio State University

2001-presenL ...... Leisure Education Coordinator, The Association for Individuals with Developmental Disability, Columbus, Ohio

FIELDS OF STUDY

Major Field: Education

Emphasis: Adapted Physical Education

Cognates: Research and Pedagogy

V

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page Abstract...... ii

Dedication ...... iii

Acknowledgments ...... iv

Vita...... v

List of Tables ...... x

List o f Figures ...... xi

Chapters:

I. Introduction ...... I

LI Purpose of this Study ...... 8 1.2 Research Questions ...... 8 1.3 Definition ofTerms ...... 9 13.1 Adapted Bicycles ...... 10 1.3.1.1 Bicycle A...... 10 1.3.13 Bicycle B...... 10 13.13 Bicycle C...... 10 1.3.1.4 Bicycle D...... II 1.33 Conventional Cycling...... 11 133 Conventional Bicycle...... 11

3 Review of Literature...... 12 2.1 Motor Skill Acquisition and Performance for Individuals without Mental Retardation ...... 12 23 Motor Skill Acquisition and Performance for Individuals with Mental Retardation...... 14 23 Dynamic Systems Theory ...... 21 23.1 Skill Acquisition of Individuals without MR...... 21 333 Skill Acquisition oflndividuals with MR...... 27 vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 24 Acquisition of Cycling Skills ...... 30 24.1 ...... 30 24.2 Training Aids and Devices ...... 31 25 Teaching Methods/Strategies...... 35 26 Special Olympics ...... 36 2.7 Summary...... 36

3. Methodology ...... 38

3.1 Pilot Stutfy ...... 38 3.2 Participant Selection ...... 39 3.3 Description of Independent Variable...... 42 33.1 Bike A (double-rotter) ...... 42 33.2 Bike B (rear-roller)...... 43 3.33 Bike C (rear-roller) ...... 44 3.3.4 Bike D (fat-tire)...... 44 33.5 Degrees of Freedom ...... 45 33.6 Size of Bicycles ...... 45 3.4 Description ofDependent Variable ...... 45 3.4.1 Distance Traveled Independently ...... 45 3 .4.2 Number of Trials ...... 46 3.43 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 46 3.4.4 Head Position...... 46 3.4.5 Maintenance ...... 47 3.4.6 Generalization ...... 47 3.4.7 Social Validity ...... 47 3.5 Procedures ...... 48 3.5.1 Pretest...... 50 3.5.2 Acquisition ...... 51 3.5.3 Generalization Probes ...... 53 3.5.4 Bicycle Probes ...... 54 333 Maintenance/Generalization Session...... 54 3.6 Interobserver Agreement...... 55 3.7 Treatment Integrity and Procedural Reliability ...... 56 3.8 Experimental Design ...... 57 3.9 Social Validity ...... 57 3.10 Data Analysis ...... 58

4. Results...... 59

4.1 Interobserver Agreement...... 59 42. Procedural Reliability...... 62 4.3 Participant 1 ...... 62 43.1 Distance and Trials ...... 62 4.3.2 Downstrokes per Minute...... 64 4 3 3 Head Position...... 66 vii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 4.3.4 Summary...... 68 4.4 Participant 2 ...... 68 4.4.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 68 4.4.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 70 4.4.3 Head Position ...... 70 4.4.4 Summary...... 63 4.5 Participant 3 ...... 73 4.5.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 73 4.5.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 75 4.5.3 Head Position...... 77 4.5.4 Summary...... 79 4.6 Participant 4 ...... 79 4.6.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 79 4.6.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 81 4.6.3 Head Position...... 81 4.6.4 Summary...... 84 4.7 Participont5 ...... 84 4.7.1 Distance and Trials ...... 84 4.7.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 87 4.73 Head Position ...... 87 4.7.4 Summary...... 90 4.8 Participant 6 ...... 90 4.8.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 90 4.83 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 92 4.83 Head Position ...... 94 4.8.4 Summary ...... 94 4.9 Participant 7 ...... 96 4.9.1 Distance and Trials ...... 96 4.9.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 98 4.93 Head Position ...... 100 4.9.4 Summary ...... 100 4.10 Participant 8 ...... 102 4.10.1 Distance ami Trials ...... 102 4.10.2 Downstrokes per Minute ...... 104 4.10.3 Head Position...... 106 4.10.4 Summary...... 108 4.11 Participant 9 ...... 108 4.11.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 108 4.113 Downstrokes per Minute...... 110 4.113 Head Position...... 110 4.11.4 Summary...... 113 4.12 Participant 10 ...... 114 4.12.1 DistanceandTrials ...... 114 4.12.2 Downstrokes per Minute...... 116 4.123 Head Position...... 116 4.12.4 Summary...... 119 viii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 4.13 Overall Summary of Resuhs ...... 119 4.14 Social Validity...... 121

5. Discussion...... 123 5.1 Research Questions...... 123 5.1.1 Research Question 1 ...... 123 5.1.2 Research Question 2 ...... 132 5.13 Research Question 3 ...... 133 5.1.4 Research Question 4 ...... 135 5.13 Research Question 5 ...... 138 5.2 Limitations ...... 140 53 Implications for Practice ...... 142 5.4 Future Research...... 145 53 Summary...... 145 List of References ...... 148

APPENDICES

APPENDIX A-Bicycie Photographs ...... 160 APPENDIX B-Adapted Bicycle Description...... 164 APPENDIX C-DIustrated Patents ...... 166 APPENDIX D-Pilot Data ...... 173 APPENDIX E-Behavioral and Social Sciences Human Subjects Institutional Review Board (IRB) ...... 179 APPENDIX F-Informed Consent ...... 181 APPENDIX G-Floor Diagram ...... 173 APPENDIX H-Data Collection Sheets ...... 175 APPENDIX I-Procedurai Reliability Checklist ...... 189 APPENDIX J-Social Validity Questionnaire...... 190 APPENDIX K-Interobserver Agreement Observation Schedule ...... 191

DC

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1 Participant Profiles ...... 40

2 Interobserver Agreement (Percent Agreement) for Distance by Trial ...... 60

3 Interobserver Agreement (Percent Agreement) for Downstrokes per Minute..61

4 Interobserver Agreement (Percent Agreement) for Upright Head Position ...... 61

5 Procedural reliability by participant across all sessions. The values represent the percent of number of “yes” answers on the procedural reliability checklist...... 62

6 Summary of results including participant numbers, trial per bike, total trials, ability to maintain and generalize skill, bike size, mean downstrokes per minute (dpm), and mean percentage of upright head position (uhp) during maintenance ...... 120

7 Mean elapsed time and range in seconds by bike ...... 120

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant#!).. 63

2 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #1) ...... 65

3 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant # 1) ...... 67

4 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #2).. 69

5 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #2) ...... 71

6 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #2) ...... 72

7 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #3).. 74

8 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #3) ...... 76

9 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #3) ...... 78

10 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #4).. 80

11 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #4) ...... 82

12 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #4) ...... 83

13 DistancetraveIedinmeters,bybike,triaI,session( ) (Participant #5).. 85

14 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #5) ...... 88

15 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #5) ...... 89

16 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #6)... 91

17 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #6) ...... 93

18 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #6) ...... 95 xi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 19 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant#?).. 97

20 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #7) ...... 99

21 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #7) ...... 101

22 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #8).. 103

23 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #8) ...... 105

24 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #8) ...... 107

25 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #9).. 109

26 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #9) ...... I l l

27 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #9) ...... 112

28 Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( ) (Participant #10).. 115

29 Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #10) ...... 117

30 Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #10) ...... 118

xn

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

The Surgeon General’s report (United States Department o f Health and Human

Services [USDHHS], 1996) addressing physical activity and health reports that

significant health benefits can be obtained by including a moderate amount o f physical

activity into our daily lives. The recent emphasis on the amount, rather than the intensity,

of physical activity makes it more inviting and increases activity options for people in

order to become active and healthy. Numerous physiological benefits involving the

musculoskeletal, cardiovascular, respiratory and endocrine systems can be attributed to

increased physical activity. Health benefits include, reduced risk of premature mortality

and reduced risks of coronary heart disease, hypertension, colon cancer and diabetes

mellitus. Other positive effects appearing to be related to regular physical activity are

reduced depression, reduced anxiety, improved mood, and enhanced ability to perform

daily tasks. Despite these findings, and the fact that higher levels of physical activity

result in lower mortality rates, more than 60% of American adults are not physically

active on a regular basis (USDHHS, 1996). Comparatively, people with disabilities are

less likely to report engaging in regular moderate physical activity than are people

without disabilities (USDHHS, 1996).

I

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. More specifically, youngsters classified with mild mental retardation (mild MR)

have had limited opportunities to participate in health related physical fitness practices

and are prone to obesity (Eichstaedt, Wang, Polacek, & Dohrmann. 1991). The

recognition that children are surprisingly inactive and that lifetime health patterns are

formed in childhood (Stuck-Ropp & DiLorenzo. 1993) point to the need for interventions

directed toward the enhancement of physical activity in children, particularly children

with disabilities.

One such intervention would be to introduce lifetime activities that would be

beneficial and enjoyable to all members of the population. It is proposed that cycling is

such an activity. Cycling is a popular activity among both children and adults. In a study

by Janz(1991), children and adolescents ranging in age from 6 to!7 years reported that

the largest portion of their leisure time, in rank order, was spent watching television,

bicycling, and playing video games. Considering that bicycling was the only non-

sedentary activity reported, its popularity should be seriously contemplated by health and

fitness advocates as a physical activity intervention. Bicycles are reasonably affordable

and easily attainable in today's society. Cycling offers many benefits beyond the obvious

gams in physical fitness and general health. Cycling is an avenue to increase socialization

with peers, it is an inclusive recreational activity, which all members of a family can

participate in together, it can improve self-confidence and, in terms of practicality,

cycling can serve as a means of environmentally friendly transportation. As an added

note, former President Clinton recently released a report by both the Secretary of

Education and the Secretary o f Health and Human Services (National Center for Chronic

Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, 2000), seeking to promote physical activity

2

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. through a variety of strategies such as redesigning communities to facilitate walking and

bicycling. There are currently over 700 miles o f bicycle paths and lanes in the state o f

Ohio alone (Ohio Department of Transportation, 2001).

Unfortunately, many individuals, especially those with disabilities, have never

learned to ride a bicycle (Joules, 1996). Children with MR are often characterized as

being clumsy or awkward. In addition, children with MR may lag behind typically

developing children, in tom s of developmental milestones, by two to four years

(Holland, 1987). In tests of eye/hand coordination, static balance and body coordination,

Eichstaedt, et a!.. (1991) reported that children with mild or moderate MR scored

significantly lower than children without MR. In fact, in a study o f motor proficiency of

children with moderate levels of MR, Job ling (1998) found balance to be the weakest

characteristic, followed closely by bilateral coordination and response speed. Due to

deficits in balance and coordination, children with MR experience difficulty in learning

and performing motor tasks, especially when task complexity increases (Wade, Newell,

& Wallace, 1978) and the task requires a temporal response (Kail, 1992; Newell, Wade,

& Kelly, 1979).

It is estimated (Joules, 1996) that approximately 10 % of the adult population or

28,368,954 individuals (Populations Estimates Program, 2001) have not yet acquired the

skill ofbicycle riding Ziglar and Hodapp (1986) estimate that between 2% and 2.5% of

the population are classified as possessing MR. This accounts for 5 to 7 million people

with MR in the United States. O f this figure, the number of young children with MR who

3

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. have not learned to tide a bicycle is probably significant For some children, it takes

many frustrating years to join their peers on a two-wheeled bicycle. However, many are

left behind in this “rite of passage”.

Statistics from Ohio Special Olympics, indicate that only 191 individuals with

MR compete in cycling events compared to 7,989 competitors in track & field events and

7,481 competitors in bowling events (M.S. Allen, personal communication, May, 2002).

It is difficult to pinpoint why there is such a low number of cycling participants compared

to other sports opportunities. Hypothetically, it could be attributed to lack of equipment,

finances, or programming However, perhaps a larger number of individuals with MR

would actively participate in cycling programs if learning to ride were not such a difficult

task. At this point, however, no empirical research has been found specifically addressing

the learning of cycling skills for individuals with MR

Several theories exist (Schmidt, 1982) which pertain to the learning of

movement/leisure skills, one of which is dynamic systems theory. This theory has direct

relevance to the learning of motor skills for individuals with mental retardation/

developmental disabilities (Gallahue, 2000; Thelen & Smith, 1994; Ulrich, Ulrich &

Angulo-Kinzler, 1998).

Dynamic systems theory originated through Bernstein's (1967) ideas related to the

notion of coordmative structures. In addition, the writings of Easton (1972), Greene

(1972), Kugler, Kelso and Turvey, (1982), and Turvey (1977) have contributed to the

early development o f this theory. More recently, the writings of Caldwell and Clark

(1990), Corbetta and Vereijken (1999), Kamm, Thelen, and Jensen (1990), Kelso, Ding,

and Schoner (1993), Thelen (1989), and Thelen and Smith (1994), have contributed to the

4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. theory’s refinement According to dynamic systems theory, a variety of systems interact

with our nervous system to create human movement These would include, but are not

limited to such factors as gravity, motivation, body position and context in addition to

the skeletal, muscular, and neurological systems (ail of these factors are important in

learning to ride a bicycle). All aspects of the environment performer, and the task are

constantly changing and interacting. Dynamic systems theory posits that "systems

undergoing change are complex, coordinated and somewhat self-organizing" (Clark &

WhitaiL, 1989, p. 190). In fee t dynamic systems theory has been described as an explicit

theory of change, devoted to uncovering structural and behavioral transitions that occur

in living systems (Corbetta & Vereijken, 1999).

Several complex and technical descriptions of dynamic systems theory exist

involving unfamiliar terminology and mathematical equations (Jeka & Kelso, 1989;

Kelso etaL, 1993). However, an understandable explanation of dynamic systems theory

as it applies to the study of development and learning in sport/leisure, is offered by

Corbetta and Vereijken (1999). Change in behavior is described in terms of stability and

instability. Behavioral states or identifiable forms of behavior are considered to be stable,

while transitions from one behavioral state to another are described as being unstable or

in flux. Environmental and/or internal modifications can bring about instability m a

movement system. It is during these periods of instability that the movement system

becomes more flexible, and new modes of functioning are likely to emerge. Moreover,

Kelso et ai., (1993) recognized that unstable patterns are generally considered as

predictors of change. Preferred modes of coordination, known as attractor states, that

occur before and after periods of instability or transition are comparatively stable and

5

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. resilient to change. To date, the primary focus of developmental and learning studies has

been on the behavioral states or stable periods of a system in change. Dynamic systems

theory shifts the focus from stage-oriented studies to understanding the processes of

change. From this perspective, the primary attraction focuses on periods of instability and

the elements that elicit transition from one behavioral state to another.

It is believed that both stability and instability are the result of self-organizing

processes, as opposed to prescriptive explanations involving motor programs or neural

maturation (Corbetta & Vereijken, 1999). Order emerges through the interaction of the

many components that make up a complex system. There are numerous ways in which

these many components may come together. These various combinations are referred to

as degrees of freedom. For example, an individual has several options to choose from

when the task involves moving from point A to point B. One could walk, crawl, hop, roll,

gallop, skip, run or ride a bicycle to complete the task. This is just a sampling of the

many locomotor combinations or degrees of freedom available to a person. However,

constraints related to the environment, the individual, or the task, have the power to

reduce the degrees of freedom available. If the individual was required to balance an egg

on a spoon while moving from point A to point B, this constraint would reduce the

number o f options available to complete the task successfully.

Two methods that have provided useful insights into the dynamic organization of

behaviors are (a) perturbing the system by provoking sudden changes in the environment

(Schoner, 1995), or (b) inducing a progressive change in the environment following a

steady continuum, until the system makes a shift to another mode of functioning (Kelso,

1995). Corbetta and Vereijken (1999) point out that both methods have been successful at

6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. identifying stability and predicting change in a system. In addition, these methods have

demonstrated that the system is capable o f spontaneously developing preferred modes o f

coordination to fit new environmental constraints without specific initial intent or

prescription.

From the standpoint o f dynamic systems, the use of a series of adapted bicycles

would represent a progressive change in the task at hand. In this study, the effects of an

adapted bicycle intervention on the acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of

conventional cycling skills for children with mfld MR were examined. Positive corrective

and positive specific feedback was provided across three dimensions (pedal rate, steering

participation, and head position) across five bicycles. A series o f four adapted bicycles

(see Appendix A) developed by Dr. Richard Klein were utilized by a child to gradually

progress toward successfully riding on a conventional two-wheeied bicycle. The adapted

bicycles feature an assortment of specially designed rollers, wide tires and lower

ratios, which serve to slow down the tipping rates and forward motion of the bicycles. A

detailed description of the adapted bicycles appears in Appendix B.

The presentation of a gradual continuum of change would encourage a shift to a

more advanced mode of functioning At the onset, the modification o f bicycle response is

crucial in terms o f compensating for significantly slower reacrion times (Distefano &

Brunt, 1982; Surburg 1985) and balance difficulties (Jobling 1999; Krebs, 2000 )

exhibited by children with MR. Forward speed and tipping rate are tempered to allow

children the opportunity to experience the dynamics of cycling in more forgiving and

appropriate conditions. However, as each bicycle in the series is introduced, the dynamics

change to provide increasingly less stability. Essentially, there are more degrees of

7

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. freedom available to perform the task. This brings more flexibility to the system. The

resulting instability allows fertile formation o f new behavioral patterns, in this case, the

ability to successfully tide a conventional bicycle.

Indeed, systems undergoing change are complex. All aspects of the performer, the

task and the environment must be considered in the acquisition of new skills. In

addressing the unique needs of individuals with MR, it is proposed that learning to ride a

bicycle through equipment modification will become less problematic.

Purpose of this Study

The purpose o f this study was to investigate the effect of using a series of adapted

bicycles on the acquisition, maintenance and generalization of conventional cycling skills

by children with mild mental retardation.

Research Questions

1. Does the use of adapted bicycles by individuals with mild mental retardation affect the

acquisition of conventional cycling skills?

a. How many trials to criterion are required for each bicycle during acquisition?

b. What is the overall direction, degree and extent of variability o f the trend as defined by

the relationship between distance and trial by bike during acquisition?

c. What is the total number of trials to criterion for conventional bicycle riding during

acquisition?

2. Is the riding of a conventional bicycle by individuals with mild mental retardation

maintained following the use of adapted bicycles?

3. Is the riding of a conventional bicycle by individuals with mild mental retardation

generalized to a variation o f the task?

s

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 4. Relative to select variables of dynamic systems theory,

a. What percentage o f intervals does the participant maintain an upright bead position (by

trial and by bike)?

b. What was the number of downstrokes per minute (by trial and by bike)?

5. What is the social value of learning to ride a conventional bicycle through the use o f

adapted bicycles?

Definition o fTerms

The following are operational definitions o f the terms used in this study.

Mild Mental Retardation (mild MR)

In this study, mild MR refers to children who have been designated as

"developmentally handicapped" in the state of Ohio. The current definition o f mental

retardation as established by the American Association on Mental Retardation

(AAMRJ992) states:

Mental retardation refers to substantial limitations in present functioning. It is

characterized by significantly subaverage intellectual functioning, existing

concurrently with related limitations in two or more of the following applicable

adaptive skill areas: communication, self-care, home living, social skills,

community use, self-direction, health and safety, functional academics, leisure,

and work. Mental retardation manifests before age 18.

Note that the term "developmentally handicapped” is soon to be replaced by tue

term "cognitive disability” in the state o f Ohio. Revisions are currently being made to the

handbook of rules and definitions

9

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Adapted Bicycles

In this study, adapted bicycles refer to four specially designed bicycles (see

Appendix A) created by Dr. Richard Klein, professor emeritus of mechanical engineering

at the University of Illinois. Each adapted bicycle is described below:

B icycle A . The "double roller” bicycle features two rollers in lieu of conventional

pneumatic tires. The rollers have been mounted onto a 16 in or 20 in (40.64 cm. or 50.8

cm. tire diameter), single-speed . One roller is located in the front position

and the other is located in the rear position of the bicycle. Both rollers to be used on bike

A have crowns o f36.25 arc-inches (92.075 arc-cm.) (roller #5). The drive train is

modified to convert the design to that of fixed gear as opposed to free wheel or "coaster

brake" system.

B icycle B. The rear roller bicycle is a variation on the two-roller bicycle above. It

features only one roller in the rear position; this roller has a crown o f36.25 arc-inches

(92.075 arc-cm.) (roller #5). The front fork is configured with a conventional front fork

and front tire. The rear roller has a-crown of 90.10 arch inches (228.854 arc-cm.). The

drive train is a fixed gear system.

B icycle C. The second rear roller bicycle is a variation on Bicycle B, in that the

in the front is larger and the sprocket in the rear position is smaller, allowing the

bicycle to move forward at a quicker pace relative to pedal cadence. In addition, the rear

roller used on Bicycle C has a crown o f26.35 arc-inches (66.929 arc-cm.) (roller #7).

to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. B icycle ZX The "fat tire" bicycle is a standard 16 in or 20 m (40.64 cm or SO.S cm)

sxzs frame, which features a wide, 16x8x5 in (40.64x20.32x12.7 cm) inflatable garden

tractor style the located in the front position. The rear the is a conventional ,

with a conventional drive train.

Conventional Cycling

Conventional cycling refers to the act of riding a typical 16-inch or 20-inch (40.64

cm or 50.8 cm) single speed bicycle (see Appendix A) with pneumatic tires and a free

wheel or coaster brake system, without the use o f training wheels or other assistive

devices.

Conventional Bicycle

The conventional bicycle used in this study is a single speed, 16 in or 20 in (40.64

cm or 50.8 cm) bicycle with pneumatic tires and coaster brakes. This is a child’s size

bicycle and will appropriately accommodate children in the 7 to 11 age range.

it

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER!

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

For the purpose of reviewing the extant literature relative to this research study,

Chapter Two is divided into five sections. The first section examines a variety of

strategies and practices related to the acquisition and performance of motor skills for

individuals without mental retardation. The second section provides information

pertaining to the acquisition and performance of motor skills for individuals with mental

retardation. The third section addresses dynamic systems theory as it relates to motor skin

acquisition and performance. The fourth section focuses on devices and strategies iitili7prf

in the acquisition of cycling skills. The fifth section provides a summary of the related

literature.

Motor Skfll Acquisition and Performance for Individuals

Without Mental Retardation

Experts in the fields o f physical education, kinesiology, and exercise physiology

have long been interested in understanding the acquisition and performance of motor

skills, in addition to an increased interest in comprehending the underlying processes

involved in learning and demonstrating motor skill proficiency. Early motor skill research

focused on applied tasks such as the learning of telegraphy and Morse code (Bryan &

Harter, 1897,1899X typing skills (Book, 1908X and athletic performance (McCIoy, 1934,

1937). However, motor learning and control research during the 1960rs and I970’s

12

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. directed much attention to novel tasks, not directly related to practical or sport and leisure

skills. During this era, there was a shift to a more process-oriented approach, which led to

an increase in the examination of simple motor tasks (e.g., Iinear-positioning) (Bilodeau,

Bilodeau, & Schumsky, 1959). Currently, however, the emphasis is directed towards an

ecological approach to studying human movement through applied sport and leisure skills

in applied settings. For example, study of motor skills such as juggling (Beek & van

Santvoord, 1992), dart throwing (McDonald, van Emmerik, & Newell, 1989; Yang &

Porretta, 1999), and riding a ski-simulator (van Emmerik, Brinker, Vereijken, & Witing,

1989) is returning. The emphasis in the preceding studies is on the "qualitative changes in

the dynamics o f the movement form in addition to the traditional quantitative or scaling

changes that occur with practice." (Newell & Rovegno, 1990, p. 188).

Although human movement seems somewhat effortless on the surface, tasks such

as running, swimming, playing tennis or riding a bicycle, actually require rather complex

actions. A number of research studies have been conducted (Singer, Lidor, & Cauraugh,

1993; Wulf, McNevin, Fuchs, Ritter, & Toole, 2000) which focused on the principles

related to the acquisition and performance of motor skills and the application of these

principles to instruction. These principles included practice composition (Adams, 1971;

Brown, 1928; Henry, 1960; Knapp & Dixon, 1952; MagilL, 1989; Moxely, 1979; Naylor

& Briggs, 1963; Schmidt, 1975; Wickstrom, 1958; Wrisberg & Ragsdale, 1979); practice

schedules (Baddeley & Longman, 1978; Lee & Genovese, 1988; Porretta & O’Brien,

1991; Shea & Kohl, 1990; Shea, Shebflske & Worchell, 1993); knowledge of

performance/results (Shea et al., 1993; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990); and cognitive

strategies (Adams, 1971; Anshel & Singer, 1980; Epstein, 1980; Gallagher & Thomas,

13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1986; Magfll, 1989; Payne & Issacs. 1999; Schmidt, 1975; Shea et aL. 1993; Singer,

1978; Singer, Lidor & Cauraugh, 1994; Weis, Ebbeck & Rose, 1992; Winther & Thomas,

1981).

Motor Skill Acquisition and Performance for Individuals with Mental Retardation

Most research on motor skill acquisition and performance for individuals with

mental retardation has drawn directly from motor learning research with the non-disabled

population. The emphasis in this section will be on motor skill acquisition and

performance for individuals with mental retardation.

It is generally known that children with mental retardation experience difficulty in

learning motor skills. Moreover, performance on a variety of motor skills tends to be

lower and more variable as levels of mental functioning decrease (Bruininks, 1974). As a

result, coordination, balance, and reaction times of individuals with MR have been

investigated over the years. Studies have established that persons with mental retardation

exhibit slower reaction times and movement tunes than do people without MR (Eason &

Surburg, 1993; BCrupski, 1977; Nettelbeck & Brewer, 1981; Surburg, Porretta, & Sutlive,

1995). Furthermore, assessments utilizing the Bnrininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor

Proficiency (BOTMP) revealed that children with Down syndrome performed poorly on

subtests o f balance, response speed, and bilateral coordination (Jobling, 1998). The most

significant and enduring problems children demonstrated were with balancing skills. In a

study ofjumping coordination patterns o f children with mild MR. differences in

coordination and equilibrium control were offered as possible explanations for the

performance deficits displayed (DiRocco, Clark. & Phillips. 1987). The previous studies

14

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. concur with the lower test scores involving eye/hand coordination, static balance, and

body coordination o f students with MR and Down syndrome reported by Eichstaedt et aL

(1991).

One explanation for the apparent difficulty in learning motor skills comes from a

cognitive perspective. Bouffard (1990) suggests that the following factors contribute to

the difficulties in problem-solving as it relates to movement: "a) deficiencies in the

knowledge base or lack of access to it, b) lack of spontaneous use o f strategies, c)

inadequate metacognitive knowledge and understanding, d) lack of executive control or

self-regulation, and inadequate motivation.1* (p. 186). Along the same lines, Belmont and

Mitchell (1987) report that children with MR have problems with learning due to

inadequate nervous activity at the most fundamental level of information processing.

They have termed this difficulty in learning the strategy deficiency hypothesis.

From a practical standpoint, Bouffard (1990) suggests that retention and transfer

o f skills should be emphasized many teaming situation. Therefore, it is important to

teach in appropriate contexts (as close to a natural setting as possible). In addition, ample

practice time should be provided to promote mastery and automaticity. ft is also

recommended that in order to increase movement competence, skills should be taught

through a skill-upgrading program. Initially matching the learner to the appropriate task

will provide successful experiences. As a person experiences success he/she will be more

motivated to continue participation. The principle of optimum perceived difficulty

(Belmont & Mitchell, 1987) applies here as welL It states that"... any variable that

contributes to the task's perceived difficulty will bear a curvilinear relationship to task-

targeted activity" (p. 100). Hence, if a child with MR believes that the task before

15

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. him/her is too easy or too difficult, little energy will be put forth. However, if it is

perceived as being moderately difficult, more effort is evident It is imperative that

teachers carefully gage activities based on students’ abilities. Positive attitudes to

physical activity depend on the development of movement confidence (Griffin & Keogh,

1982). hi turn, positive experiences have an effect on confidence and motivation as well

as self-concept (Harter, 1978).

The problem-solving behavior o f individuals with MR was examined while

performing in a simulated racket sport (Bouffard & Wall, 1991). The object was to

systematically manipulate the amount of information available to the performer to

determine if decision-making rules were utilized. The knowledge about where the ball

would land on the table was manipulated The position the players selected to return the

ball, the type of stroke used and the number of balls hit were all affected by this

knowledge o f ball placement Therefore, the selection of responses was context

dependent Results demonstrated a relationship between knowledge, decision-making and

performance for individuals with mild MR.

As was mentioned earlier, the structure o f practice can have an effect on the

acquisition, retention, and generalization of motor skills. In a study designed to examine

the retention and transfer o f skins in children with mental retardation, input was

organized by utilizing three different practice schedules (Del Rey & Stewart, 1989). The

task involved pressing a button when the last light on a runway was illuminated Blocked,

serial, and random practice schedules were used during the acquisition phase. The results

provided support for both random and serial practice schedules in terms o f retention.

There were no significant findings for transfer. This offers some support to the idea of

16

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. contextual interference. Serial practice and to a larger extent, random practice conditions,

force the learner to differentiate the critical features among tasks (Del Rey & Stewart,

1989). Although support for transfer was not evident in the Del Rey and Stewart study,

Edwards, Elliot, and Lee (1986) and Porretta (1982) did observe transfer findings in

individuals with MR as a result of manipulating the structure of practice sessions.

Additional research studies (Porretta, 1988; Porretta & O'Brien, 1991) examining

contextual interference effects in children with mild MR incorporated the use o f gross

motor skills. In the earlier study (Porretta, 1988) children with mild MR participated in a

beanbag tossing task. Children were assigned to either random, serial, or blocked practice

trials for one day (48 total trials). Although less error was exhibited for both transfer and

retention for children assigned to the random practice group, the difference was not

significant Therefore, results provided marginal support for the use of random practice

for better transfer and retention than other practice schedules. In a similar study, Porretta

and O'Brien (1991) compared three different practice schedules (random, serial and

blocked) relative to a coincidence anticipation task. Participants were asked to swing a

plastic bat at a taut nylon string. The swing was to occur just as the last light in a series of

lights was illuminated. However, in this study, twice the amount o f practice time was

allotted. Practice occurred over two days with 48 trials each day. Results indicated that

significantly less error occurred during both retention and transfer phases for those

practicing in random conditions as compared to blocked conditions. Significant

differences were not found during the practice phase. This study provides more support

for the use of contextual interference effects through random practice for children with

mild MR. It also supports the use of additional practice trials across multiple sessions.

17

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Utilizing cognitive strategies for individuals with mental retardation has also been

examined as a means to enhancing skill acquisition and performance. The use o f imagery

or mental practice was studied in conjunction with physical practice for a striking task

(Porretta & Surburg, 1995). The physical practice plus imagery group performed

significantly better in terms of both accuracy and variability than did the physical practice

only group. This supports the use of imagery practice combined with physical practice for

individuals with mild MR. In a related study, the use of imagery practice was utilized as a

supplement to physical practice in the performance of a throwing task (Surburg et aL,

1995). High and low cognitive demands were also examined in relation to the use of

imagery and physical practice. Forty youngsters with mild MR were randomly assigned

into four groups: low cognitive loading-physical practice, low cognitive loading-imagery

and physical practice, high cognitive loading-physical practice, high cognitive loading-

imagery and physical practice. Results indicated that participants supplied with imagery

practice performed much better than the non-imagery groups. However, the cognitive

loading o f a task did not affect the use o f imagery practice. Overall, the use of imagery

techniques for individuals with mild MR may prove useful in teaching motor skills.

Yang and Porretta (1999) modified Singer's (1986) five-step cognitive strategy for

use with individuals with mild MR. Their four-step strategy is less complicated than

Singer’s and consists o f ready, look, do and score. In their study, three closed gross motor

skills were targeted; the basketball free throw, the overhand softball throw and the dart

throw. Results suggest that the four-step approach was a useful strategy for individuals

is

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. with mild MR in that performance improved during the acquisition phase. Although

performance did not continue to improve throughout the retention phase; participants

were able to successfully transfer performance of skills to different settings.

A study by Porretta (1987) examined the effects o f organized and random

presentation of a movement series by adolescents with and without mild MR. Participants

were asked to move a linear slide to distances of 10-20-30-40- and 50-cm., by grasping

the handle of the slide and moving it from right to left Movements were presented in two

different conditions, organized and random. In the organized condition, participants were

presented with movement distances in sequential order. In the random condition,

movement distances were presented in random order. Although the adolescents with MR

exhibited greater absolute error than individuals without MR, they benefited significantly

from organized presentation of movement distances. Results support the use of

experimenter-presented organization to enhance the recall of movement distances by

adolescents with mild MR Porretta (1987) also examined the effect of response

organization on reaction tune and movement time of children with mild MR Participants

included thirty children with mild MR and thirty children without disability, matched for

chronological age. The task was to touch a button as stimulus lights came on. The three

precued conditions involved hand, direction or midline. In the precue hand condition,

participants were told to use either the right or left hand to depress the button. In the

direction condition, the participants were informed as to which side o f the body the

response would take place. The midline condition, involved knowledge about whether or

not to cross the midline in response to the stimulus. Finding suggest that children with

mild MR had significantly slower reaction times and movement times in comparison to

19

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. the children without disabilities. However, both groups performed similarly in that they

were able to utilize precued hand information as opposed to other precued information.

The relationship between internal attributional statements and perceived physical

competence of children with MR was investigated by Kozub and Porretta (1999). Eighty-

six children with MR, between the ages of 8 and 15, served as participants in this study.

Each child was surveyed, and results indicated that internal attributional statements were

chosen more often by older children compared to younger children. This mirrors the

developmental trend found in children without disabilities. Internal attributional

statements are reflective of a mastery-oriented profile, in that success is linked to ability.

However, in general, both male and female children with MR exhibited an external

attributional profile. This is demonstrated by the tendency to select external rationales for

competent outcomes. This type of profile may have a negative impact on self-esteem and

hinder potential achievement in physical activity. It is suggested that practitioners take

this into consideration when providing feedback and teaching children new motor skills.

Feedback should reinforce the use o f appropriate strategies in the successful completion

of a task. In this way, the child will be encouraged to be internally responsible for skill

acquisition. Reinforcing strategy-based effort will assist in generating a mastery-oriented

profile.

Motor task persistence of children with and without MR was investigated by

Kozub, Porretta, and Hodge (2000). Participants for this study included thirty-one

children with and without MR, matched for chronological age. Each participant was

asked to engage in two novel motor tasks. A persistence score was calculated based upon

the total number of trials and seconds engaged. Results indicated that children with MR

2 0

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. persisted less and attempted fewer trials than the children without MR. It is suggested

that tower persistence scores may be related to external attributional constructs o f

children w ith MR.

Dynamic Systems Theory

The emergence o f dynamic systems theory in the field o f motor studies occurred

approximately twenty years agp (Kelso, Holt, Kugler, & Turvey, 1980; Kugier, Kelso, &

Turvey, 1980; Turvey, 1977). Early human movement research studies that utilized

dynamic systems theory involved skilled adult performance and the coordination of

limited body parts. For example, researchers examined skilled coordination patterns in

tasks such as repetitive flexion and extension o f the index fingers (Haken, Kelso, &

Bunz, 1985; Schoner & Kelso, 1988), bimanual manipulation o f hand-held pendulums

(Kugler & Turvey, 1987; Schmidt, Beek. Trefiner, & Turvey, 1991; Turvey, Rosenblum,

Kugier, & Schmidt, 1986) and the motion o f leg swinging between individuals (Schmidt,

Carello, & Turvey, 1990). Through this body of research, it became evident that dynamic

systems theory could aid in the understanding of movement behavior and coordination.

However, only a limited number o f studies involving the learning of new skills or whole

body coordination have been conducted utilizing dynamic systems theory.

Skill Acquisition of Individuals without MR

There is a growing database supporting the dynamic systems principles in human

motor development and the acquisition of motor skills. In particular, there are a variety of

studies involving infants. Researchers have examined treadmill-elicited stepping o f

infants at various ages in the first year of life (Davis, 1991; Thelen, 1986;

21

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Thelen & Ulrich, 1991), the transition from rocking to crawling (Goldfield, 1989), leg

movements o f pre-term and full-term infants (Heriza, 1988), the relationship between

physical growth and a newborn reflex (Thelen, Fisher & Ridley-Johnson, 1984) and

newborn stepping (Thelen & Cooke, 1987; Thelen & Fisher, 1982). Highlights from

selected studies rooted in dynamic systems theory, involving both infants and adults in

the acquisition of motor skills are detailed below.

In a study involving interlimb coordination, Thelen (1994) demonstrated that

three-month-old infants can modify their current "intrinsic dynamics" (Zanone & BCelso,

1991) into new kicking patterns, when presented with a novel task. Intrinsic dynamics are

preferred modes o f coordination that exist at the start of the learning process (Corbetta &

Vereijken, 1999). Three-month-old infants usually kick with either both legs in alteration

or with single leg kicks. Simultaneous kicking o fboth legs at the same time is less

common (Thelen, 1985). In this study, infants controlled the movements of an overhead

mobile by means o f a string attached to their left ankles. Some infants had their legs

yoked together at the ankle with soft elastic. The elastic allowed kicks to be coordinated

as single, alternating, or simultaneous, but the simultaneous kicks activated the mobile

more vigorously. Results revealed that all infants kicked more and faster when their kicks

activated the mobile, than when they did not Interestingly, only the infants in the yoked

condition increasingly produced simultaneous kicking patterns. Infants were able to shift

their patterns o f interlimb coordination to accomplish a novel, experimentally imposed

task. These findings suggest that an adapted pattern o f movement was assembled in

22

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. reaction to a specific task constraint. Therefore, learning processes, rather than

autonomous brain maturation, may underlie the acquisition of motor skills (Thelen,

1994).

Clark, Whitail, and Phillips (1988) analyzed the organization of interlimb

coordination in newly walking infants with and without support and infants who had been

walking for op to 6 months. Results indicated that the coordinative structure for interlimb

coordination in newly walking infonts is similar to that found in the adult walker. Both

temporal phasing (Le., proportion of tune footstrike to footstrike) and distance phasing

(i.e., proportional distance covered in a single step) were examined. While the average

temporal phasing of the newly walking infants was the same (50% out-of-phase) as that

found in older infants and adults, it was much more variable. Over the first three months

of walking, the variability decreases. Nonetheless, the pattern o f interlimb coordination is

in place at the very onset of walking In terms of distance phasing, the new walkers

exhibited more variability. However, the infants had attained a phasing relationship

similar in variability to adults by three months of age. Interestingly, providing support to

the newly walking infonts brought about significant gams for both temporal and distance

phasing. Thus, reducing the postural demands of the infant reduced the variability of

interlimb coordination. This key observation lends support to the argument that the

dynamical requirements of the upright posture may well contribute to the form of

interlimb coordination in infants.

Utilizing the Philippson phases to represent the walking cycle, Clark and Phillips

(1987) analyzed the step cycle organization of infant and adult walkers at various speeds.

The Philippson cycle consists of four phases involving the components of heel strike,

23

i Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. deep knee flexion and toeoff occurring during swing and stance movements. They

discovered that step cycle organization for infants and adults was almost identical across

all four phases o f the step cycle. Changes in speed also resulted in similar methods of

adjustment between infonts and adults. From a dynamical systems perspective, the

Philippson step cycle is not specified by the central nervous system, rather, it emerges

from the constraints of the task and is based on the physical laws that govern biological

system s.

In a longitudinal study, five children were observed over a two-year period

following the onset of walking (Bril & Breniere, 1992). Investigators were interested in

describing the developmental changes in gait velocity. These changes were then related

to components o f postural stability and velocity. Results indicated that in the first six

months of walking, there was a three-fold increase in the range of speed After the first

six months, range of speed leveled out and remained constant. Step width, on the other

hand, decreased two-fold Initially, the increased velocity of infant walkers is primarily

due to increased step length. After about five months, however, increased velocity is

attributed to increased cadence. Developmentally, there appear to be two phases of

independent walking. During the earliest phase (first five months), infants are integrating

postural constraints into the dynamic necessities o f walking. The second phase is

characterized by fine-tuning o f the gait parameters.

Whitall and Getchell (1995) applied a dynamical systems approach to examine

the development of locomotor skills, specifically, the transition from walking to running.

In a dynamic systems analysis it is important to identify variables that represent the

essence o f a particular behavioral pattern. Kelso and Schoner (1988) refer to these as

24

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. collective variables. The collective variables observed in this study were relative stance,

estimated pathway o f center o f mass, and segmental/joint action, la a comparison o f

walking and running in newly running infants, these variables revealed a relatively

continuous change between the two gait forms over the first few months o f running.

Coordination o f the knee joint was similar between walking and running However,

coordination of the ankle joint was less consistent Data from relative stance and stride

length indicated that vertical and horizontal displacement was not evident These findings

suggest that possible control parameters for newly running infants are strength and

balance. These control parameters are necessary to shift a child into more advanced forms

o f running

Several studies involving adult participants have also examined the acquisition of

motor skills from a dynamic systems perspective (Beek & van Santvoord, 1992;

McDonald et aL, 1989; Newell & van Emmerik, 1989; Schmidt, Treffner, Shaw &

Turvey, 1992; van Emmerik, 1992). Following are descriptions of research studies

involving adults participants and the examination o f skill acquisition such as ski-

simulator movements, drawing novel rhythmic tasks between limbs and juggling.

In a study by Vereijken, van Emmerik, W hiting and Newell (1992), five adult

males practiced slalom-like movements on a ski apparatus. The purpose o f this study was

to test Bernstein’s notions of “freezing” degrees of freedom in skill acquisition

(Bernstein, 1967). Bernstein defined the problem of movement coordination as “the

process of mastering redundant degrees o f freedom” (Bernstein, 1967, p. 127). In learning

a novel task, the student is faced with the enigma of reorganizing the control o f an

overwhelming number of degrees of freedom in order to perform the task successfully. It

25

Ii Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. is suggested by Bernstein (1967) that coordination is brought about by an initial “breezing

out" o f the degrees o f freedom, followed by the subsequent release o f these degrees o f

freedom info the skillful movements o f the performer. In this study, data were collected

across seven consecutive days, via the placement of LED markers on major joints o f the

upper body and legs. Results indicated that in the early phases o f learning, the joint

angles of the torso and lower limbs displayed minimal movement, based on range of

movement and standard deviations. In addition, there was evidence of high joint

. With practice, these findings were reversed, illustrating a significant increase

in angular movement and a decrease in the degree ofjoint couplings. These findings lend

empirical support to Bernstein’s notion o f freezing and releasing degrees o f freedom in

the acquisition of a motor skill. Likewise, in a study of involving drawing movements

(van Emmerik, 1992), comparisons between dominant and non-dominant limbs indicated

significantly higher between joints of the non-dominant limbs. This finding

signifies a comparable freezing out of the degrees of freedom in solving the coordination

problem, in that rigid coupling was evident in the earlier stages o f practice.

In a study involving adult participants, Beck and van Santvoord (1992) conducted

a dynamical systems analysis on learning to juggle. By focusing on the temporal

constraints ofjuggling, they managed to create a mathematical model, or description of

the process of learning the cascade juggle. As opposed to a purely intellectually

constructed motor program, Beek and van Santvoord (1992) have drawn attention

towards the dynamical principles at work

26

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. in skill acquisition. Ultimately, what they discovered was that learning a new motor skill

involves the discovery o f fixed points in the perceptual-motor workspace associated with

that skill, from which deviations can be made in an attempt to further refine the skill

Skill Acquisition of Individuals with MR

Based on the principles o f dynamic systems theory, behavior that is unstable is

more easily shifted into new patterns. It is during these transitional periods as opposed to

more stable periods, that perturbations to the system are more likely to cause change.

Ulrich et aL, (1998) put this principle to the test in a study that examined the impact of

context manipulations on the stepping response of infants with Down syndrome during a

transitional period- Twelve infants with Down syndrome (mean age of 13.3 months) were

tested To examine the type and number of steps that would be produced, each infant was

held upright on a small treadmill. The four context manipulations were referred to as

velcro, nubbly, girdle and weights. It was thought that these manipulations would

enhance the production o f alternate stepping over other stepping patterns, in addition to

an overall increase in the total number of steps taken.

The velcro condition consisted of a carpeted treadmill surface in combination

with the use of socks with velcro strips sewn onto the bottom. It was predicted that the

velcro would keep the foot in contact with the carpeted longer and in turn strengthen

the interaction between the extensor muscle forces of the infant and the moving belt The

nubbly condition featured a rubber belt with small projections in combination with bare

feet The resulting ticklish sensation was thought to encourage the infants to step mote

quickly. The girdle condition involved a smooth treadmill belt and bare feet similar to

baseline conditions. In addition, the infant wore a belt the covered the hiprtower back

27

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. area. This manipulation was considered due to the fact that infants with Down syndrome

tend to have greater hip and pelvic instability. It was felt that by stabilizing the lower

lumbar-sacral area, thigh abduction would be minimized. The weights condition was also

performed on a smooth treadmill belt and bare feet However, terrycloth cuffs holding a

small weight were attached to each ankle. The prediction was that by adding mass to the

ankle, more forward movement would be produced.

Results indicated that with the altered conditions, infants produced more total

steps and more alternating steps as compared to baseline conditions. The velcro condition

had the greatest impact of all the context manipulations. In addition, the changes in

context also had a significant effect on the trajectory o f foot displacement in swing. This

research demonstrates the utility o f extrinsic factors on the development of motor skills.

In addition, it provides an increased understanding of behavior during transitional

periods, since transitional periods are an optimal time to effect movement changes.

In an earlier study, Thelen (1986) demonstrated that infants without disability

could produce alternating stepping patterns while supported on a moving treadmill prior

to independent walking or standing. In a similar vein, Ulrich, Ulrich, and Collier (1992)

investigated the ability of infants with Down syndrome to produce organized stepping

patterns on a treadmill. Results show that six o f the seven infants responded to the

treadmill stimulus by producing alternating steps. These findings support those ofThelen

(1986) in that the maturation of the central nervous system is not the only component

necessary in the development of upright locomotion. While leg strength and balance are

factors that may prevent children without disability to walk at earlier ages, for children

28

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. with Down syndrome additional factors may add to the delay. For example delayed

postural reactions (ShumwayCook & WooDacott, 1985), joint laxity (Livingston &

{first, 1986), difficulty in regulating muscle stiffness (Davis & Kelso, 1982) or being

overweight (Chumlea & Cronk, 1982).

In an examination of the mechanical properties underlying movement control of

children with and without Down syndrome, 13 participants took part in a finger

positioning task (Davis & Kelso, 1982). In this two-part study o f a simple, discrete

movment, it was revealed that the gross organization of movement was very similar

among children with and without Down syndrome. However, important differences were

noted, relative to damping and stiffness. Damping refers to the oscillatory behavior about

a newly established equilibrium position. Participants with Down syndrome were less

accurate in controlling these movements, and there was a tendency towards

underdamping. In addition, individuals with Down syndrome were less able to regulate

stiffness with increasing levels of torque, than were participants without Down syndrome.

In summary, although there appears to be overall similarities in the gross organization of

motor control for individuals with and without Down syndrome, movement patterns were

qualitatively different.

Shumway-Cook and Woollacot (1985) investigated the dynamics of postural

control in children with and without Down syndrome This study examined six children

with and 11 children without Down syndrome. Participants stood on a hydraulically

controlled platform and were monitored through the use of surface electromyograms.

Findings suggest that both groups of children produced similar directional responses to

2 9

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. platform movements. However, children with Down syndrome exhibited increased

variability in patterns, slower reactions and increased body sway in comparison to

children without disability.

Acquisition o f Cycling Skills

Training Wheels

A very popular method for introducing cycling skills to young children has been

the use o ftraining wheels. Training wheels assist in keeping the novice rider in an upright

position, however, training wheel use is actually counterproductive in learning how to

balance and ride a conventional bicycle (RX. Idem, personal communication, October,

1998). The use of training wheels promotes inappropriate feedback responses. As a child

rides with training wheels, the tendency is to lean to one side or the other as the bike

moves forward on three wheels (two cycle wheels and one training wheel). This in turn,

results in a preoccupation with the upper torso as a balancing . This tendency

is very noticeable, as upper-body articulation is quite pronounced when observing a child

attempting to balance a two-wheel bicycle after many years on training wheels. Upper-

body articulation can be described as the shifting o f the shoulders to one side or another.

Finally, the use of training wheels tends to promote a false sense of security. The

bicycle and rider maintain an upright position, regardless o f the technique or movements

employed. If the rider of a bicycle with training wheels does fail, the injuries tend to be

more serious when compared to falls from a bicycle without training wheels (Joules,

1996). This is due to the wide position o f training wheels. When the bike does tip, the

30

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. child is propelled off and upper body injuries are more common. On the other hand, when

falling from a tw o-w heeled bicycle, the legs can slow the fall and injuries to the lower

body are mote common (Joules, 19%).

In terms o f achieving balance on a conventional bicycle without training wheels,

the appropriate technique requires one to steer in the direction o f the fall or lean. Roberts

(1995) explains that the track of a bicycle is not a straight line, but one of constant lateral

undulations. The reason for these oscillations is that any tendency to fall to one side or

the other is promptly arrested by a corresponding horizontal movement in the same

direction. With this in mind, balancing a bicycle is much easier and more productive

when the arms are loose and participating in the steering process. Again, the use o f

training wheels may tend to inhibit this necessary action. The arms are often observed as

being rigid and inflexible as a result of a preoccupation with the upper-body as an

alternate balancing mechanism.

Training Aids and Devices

A complex array of training aids and devices have been developed and patented

over the years, for the discreet purpose of teaching children how to ride bicycles (Egley,

1994; Geller, 1991; Harrison, 1995; Henrichs, 1996; Kalmus, 1994; Krauss, 1993;

Nanassi, 1995; Pearson, 1993; Rieber, 1992; Saunders, 1989) (see Appendix C). Despite

the efforts o f these entrepreneurs, the problem remains unsolved. In each case, the basic

needs o f the learner have not been properly addressed. Rather, in many cases, the

instructor is required to maintain and control balance o fthe bicycle. In addition, an

emphasis is placed on the comfort and upright posture o fthe instructor. Other devices are

simply variations o n training wheels.

31

| Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Patented training devices fall into one o f three categories: (a) harness^ loop or

handle which attaches to the child (Heinrichs, 1996; Nanassi, 1995; Pearson, 1993); (b)

handles which attach to the bicycle (Geller, 1991; Harrison, 1995; Kalmus, 1994; Krauss,

1993) and; (c) alternative training wheels (Egley, 1994; Rieber, 1992; Saunders, 1989). A

brief description outlining the major features o f each training device follows, however,

there is no empirical evidence to support any of the devices described below.

The first category consists of devices that attach directly to the child. The balance

harness (Heinrichs, 1996) is proposed to be useful in teaching cycling, skating, and

mobility skills. The harness consists of two long hoops. One is placed over the child's

head, around the back, and under the arms. The second hoop is then placed under the

arms from the front and across the chest The opposite ends of the loop are held together

above the child's head by the supervising adult Henrichs (1996) suggested that the adult

provide support as the child attempts to ride the bicycle slowly. The adult's role is to

maintain the child’s balance. If the child should start to lose balance, the adult is to lift up

on the harness to bring the rider back to a balanced position.

The Pearson (1993) training device consists of a loop, fitting snugly around the

child’s waist with an attached handle. The device extends behind the child. With the loop

and handle system, the adult supports and controls the balance o f the child as the child

rides the bicycle. Pearson (1993) claims three advantages of the loop and handle system

over conventional bicycle training. First, the adult does not have to stoop over in an

uncomfortable position to hold onto the seat o fthe bike. Second, when holding the

32

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. handle as opposed to the seat, the adult will not overcompensate and control too much of

the child’s weight Third, the loop and handle system is much less expensive to

manufacture than training wheels.

The strap and handle training aid patented by Nanassi (1995) is similar to

Pearson’s (1993) patent with one exception. Nanassi (1995) has added a quick release

system to the handle attachment Otherwise, the two devices are similar in that they

propose to help the cyclist maintain balance.

The next category of training aids can be described as handles, which attach to the

bicycle. Geller (1991) introduced a handle with along shaft that attaches to the lower seat

post and brake bridge. For safety purposes, a braking mechanism has been installed in the

extended handle. Geller (1991) claims that this training device offers several advantages.

First, it provides a comfortable grip for the instructor (no stooping over). Second, the

instructor has full control o f the bicycle because the handle is connected close to the

bicycle’s center of gravity. Third, the instructor is able to provide an immediate response

to correct an abrupt tilt o fthe bicycle. Fourth, the braking system provides a safety

feature to control the bicycle when going downhill.

The Krauss (1993) training device is comprised of a handle that attaches to the

rear wheel fork. The unique features o f this handle include a pivoting head and a

telescopically extendable shaft The handle can pivot up, down and sideways relative to

the bicycle frame. This feature has been included to accommodate the instructor's

preference of running to the left or right side of the bicycle. In addition, the extendable

shaft is adjustable according to the height o f the instructor, thus allowing the instructor to

stand comfortable in an upright position.

33

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The Kalmus (1994) training device is a handle that attaches to the bicycle seat

assembly. This handle is adjustable in both length and angle. The major contribution o f

this invention is to offer a handle that is comfortable for the instructor to grip.

The Harrison (1995) guidance device consists of an U-shaped handle that attaches

to the rear axle and is pivotally adjustable. Harrison (1995) suggested that due to the

location o f the attachment at the axle bolts, it is possible for the instructor to be involved

in the steering to some extent. The objectives, once again, are to provide a comfortable

stance and ultimate control of the bicycle by the instructor.

The last category of training aids consists of alternative training wheel designs.

Saunders (1989) submitted a device that can be described as responsive training wheels.

The training wheels are linked to the front wheel by a cable, causing the training wheels

to raise and lower in response to steering. The advantage is that a more normal leaning

action is simulated than that offered by conventional training wheels.

Reiber (1992) introduced an inwardly adjustable set o f training wheels.

Conventional training wheels are adjusted upwards as the child develops riding skills.

Inwardly adjustable training wheels offer the advantage o f reducing the amount of

support provided by training wheels while at the same time providing stability. With this

system, the bicycle will not tend to wobble from side to side as when the wheels are

adjusted in an upward fashion.

Egley (1994) patented an adjustable training wheel apparatus that incorporated a

compressed spring assembly to dampen axial movement Egley (1994) claimed that tins

device more closely approximates the actual degree o f balancing necessary to ride a

bicycle without training wheels.

3 4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. In addition to training devices or aids that attach to the learner or to the bicycle,

Cudmore (1972) patented a "velocipede" (see Appendix C), which is a two-wheel bicycle

with a barrel-shaped, inflatable, wide rear tire. The velocipede is stable like a , yet

rides like a bicycle.

Teaching Methods/Strategies

Joules (1996) has actually patented a teaching method for the acquisition of

cycling skills. This method consists o f two phases, an operant conditioning phase, and a

launch phase. During the operant conditioning phase, the instructor straddles the back tire

of the bicycle, while the child sits on the bicycle with feet on pedals and hands on handle

bars. This is a static balance drilL When the instructor causes the bicycle to lean to the

right, the child is instructed to react by turning the handlebars in the same direction as the

lean. When the bicycle leans to the left, the child is instructed to react by turning the

handlebars to the left During the launch phase, the instructor pushes the bicycle away,

and allows the child to practice dynamic balance on a moving bicycle. This method

maintains a prerequisite o f a one second reaction time when a bail is tossed to one side or

the other of a child The child is not required to catch the ball, but merely to react to it by

turning in the appropriate direction. However, there have been no empirical studies to

determine whether or not this method is better than other twanhmg methods/strategies.

Bicycling Magazine (1991) has made available a pamphlet providing instruction

on how to teach a child to ride a bicycle. This method has two phases. In the first phase,

the seat is lowered so that the child can touch the ground. The child is to coast down a

slight incline with feet extended and off the pedals. During this time, the child is to

practice balancing. The instructor is not to run along beside the bicycle. In the second

35

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. phase, the seat is raised to the appropriate height and the child is instructed to pedal as the

instractorholds the back ofthe seat and provides an initial push. However, there is no

empirical evidence to support this teaching method.

Special Olympics

Special Olympics International has compiled several training manuals intended to

guide coaches in the instruction of various sport skills. The Special Olympics

International Cycling Skills Manual (1988) is based on the premise that people entering

the program have previously mastered the primary perceptual and motor skills required to

ride a bicycle. Rather than teaching individuals how to initially ride a bicycle, the

program addresses basic fundamentals such as mounting, steering, stopping, bicycle

selection, and maintenance. The short term objectives are to warm-up properly before

cycling, to demonstrate cycling skills in three levels, to adhere to the modified rules of

competitive cycling, to ride the bicycle in a safe manner and to exhibit sportsmanship.

The three levels of cycling skills consist of: a) riding a bicycle with training wheels, b)

riding a bicycle without training wheels, and c) riding a bicycle competitively, hi

addition, the manual offers specific drills, task analysis and tips for conducting a

successful practice session. However, as with previously mentioned training methods, no

empirical evidence masts to determine whether or not the Special Olympics approach is

better than other approaches.

Summary

The review of literature encompassed several topical areas related to the

acquisition o f cycling skills for individuals with nrild MR. The first section examined

strategies and practices utilized by individuals without MR for the purpose o f acquiring

36

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. and performing motor skills. Historically, research in this area has evolved in its focus

from early applied tasks, to a process-oriented approach examining novel tasks, and

finally towards an ecological approach through applied sport and leisure skills in applied

settings. Over the years, these studies have focused on principles including practice

composition, practice schedules, knowledge o f performance/results, and cognitive

strategies. The second section provided information pertaining to the acquisition and

performance o f motor skills for individuals with MR. Many o f the studies in this area

have examined the coordination, balance, and reaction/movement times o f individuals

with MR. In addition, studies have been conducted with emphasis on the structure of

practice, cognitive strategies, and motor task persistence. General recommendations for

improving acquisition and performance of motor skills for individuals with MR point to

an emphasis on retention and transfer, and the provision of ample practice time in natural

settings. In the third section, dynamic systems theory was presented relative to the

acquisition and performance of motor skills. There is a growing database supporting the

dynamic systems principles in motor development and skill acquisition. Numerous

studies are highlighted involving both individuals with and without MR. The fourth

section introduced several devices and strategies, which were developed for the purpose

of cycling acquisition. In addition to training wheels, several alternative training devices

have been patented. These include various combinations of harnesses and handles that

attach either to the bicycle or to the learner. Various teaching methods and strategies are

outlined originating from sources such as Special Olympics and Bicycling Magazine. The

information and research outlined in the previous sections sets the groundwork for a

study of the acquisition o f cycling skills for parsons with MR.

2 7

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTERS

METHODOLOGY

This chapter describes the procedures used for the purpose of investigating the

effect o f using adapted bicycles on the acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of

conventional cycling skills by children with mild MR. It contains the following sections:

pilot study, participant selection, description of the independent variable, description of

the dependent variables, procedures, inter-observer agreement, treatment integrity and

procedural reliability, experimental design, data analysis, and social validity.

Pilot Study

A pilot study was conducted. Three females and two males (age range 4-41 years)

participated in the pilot Two participants had a disability (Cerebral Palsy and Autism)

and three participants had no disabilities. All five participants acquired the ability to ride

a conventional bicycle (see Appendix D). Mean trials to acquisition was 153 (range 64-

182) across an average o f 5.2 sessions (range 3-8). All five participants demonstrated

maintenance of the skill at criterion level. However, no participants were able to

generalize the skilL Several changes in methodology took place based upon pilot study

results. In the pilot study a series o f six adapted bicycles were utilized. Essentially, Bike

A was fit with three different configurations o f rollers, Bike B was fit with 2 different

configurations of rollers, Bike C did not exist, rather Bike D was the next bike in the

series, followed by the conventional bicycle. Results indicated that participants did not

38

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. require three versions of Bike A, nor two versions of Bike B. Additionally, it became

obvious that there was a very large gap between Bike B and Bike D. At this point, Dr.

Klein created Bike C as a better transition. Bike C was equipped with a large hoot

sprocket, to increase forward speed in relation to cadence. The original series A -l, A-2,

A-3, B -l, B-2, D and the conventional bicycle utilized in the pilot, were reduced to A, B,

C, D, E (conventional bike). Furthermore, the placement of cones for the generalization

session was reorganized to better accommodate the cognitive level of potential

participants. In the pilot, the cones were arranged in a straight vertical line, and

participants were asked to weave in and out o f the cones. One of the participants did not

understand the task. Therefore, the cones were placed in pairs, so that a zig-zag path

could be more easily visualized Additionally, the verbal command that the investigator

used at the start of each trial was changed from “1,2, 3, Go”, to “Ready, here we go”.

The “1 ,2 ,3 ” signal made some participants more aware ofthe fact that the investigator

was releasing the bicycle. Therefore, a less direct command was utilized Finally, data

were collected via both video-tape and live coders in the pilot study. In the current study

only video-tape coding was necessary as the quality o f the video was sufficient for

coding.

Participant Selection

Ten elementary school students with m ild MR, ranging in age from 7 to II years,

served as participants (Table 1). All participants received special education services and

were identified as developmentally handicapped (DH) by the state of Ohio. In accordance

with the definition o f DH, all participants exhibited deficits in adaptive behaviors.

Eichstaedt and Lavay (1992) define deficits in adaptive behaviors as “outstanding 39

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. limitations in effectively meeting general standards o f maturation, learning, personal

dependence, and/or social responsibility for youngsters o f similar age and cultural group”

(p. 6). Participants had no known physical or sensory impairments that would inhibit

performance o f cycling skills. Participants were purposefully selected based on age and

disability. None of the participants were able to independently ride a conventional bicycle

at the beginning of the study. Participants were identified by identification number and a

first name. The names used were not the actual first names o f the participants.

The research protocol was approved by the Behavioral and Social Sciences

Human Subjects Review Committee of the Ohio State University (see Appendix E).

Consent forms were mailed to the parents or guardians o f the 10 participants (see

Appendix F). The consent form consisted of an explanation o f the proposed study and

required a signature from the parent or guardian prior to the participant's involvement

All consent forms were signed and returned prior to inclusion in this study. In addition,

all participants were asked to give verbal assent o f their participation in the study. Verbal

assent was videotaped in the presence o f the parent/guardian and two witnesses.

I.D. Participant G ender Age H eight C ondition j

1 Jim M ale 10 58 in Asperger syndrome 2 B illy M ale 7 49 in Mild Autism 3 M ary Fem ale 7 48 in Down syndrom e 4 Jack M ale II 59 in Autism 5 Jerem y M ale 9 48 in Down syndrome 6 Bob M ale 8 54 in Down syndrom e 7 Pete M ale 9 51.75 in Developmental Delay S Danny M ale 10 4 8 in Developmental Delay/Cerebral Palsy 9 E rin Fem ale 9 56.5 in Verbal Dyspraxia 10 A llen M ale 9 57 in Mild Autism/ADD Table 1. Participant Profiles

40

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Following, is a profile o feach ofthe participants relative to siblings and prior

experience with bicycles. Jim has one younger sibling, a 4-year-oid sister. Jim has access

to a bicycle at home, and his past experience with bicycles primarily involved riding with

training wheels. He had attempted riding a bicycle without training wheels, but was

unsuccessful. His mother describes him as being very rigid and fearful in his attempts at

riding a conventional bicycle. Billy has three siblings. He is a triplet hi addition to an 11-

year-old brother, he has a 7-year-old brother and a 7-year-old sister. Billy has access to

bicycles at home. His past experience includes riding a tricycle (two years ago). He then

advanced to riding a bicycle with training wheels. Bike riding was slow and without a

particular focus on reaching a destination. He has had limited experience riding a scooter

for short distances. Mary has one older sibling, a 13-year-old brother. She has access to a

bicycle at home. Her past experience involves riding a bicycle with training wheels and

riding a scooter. Jack has two brothers, ages 9 and 12. Jack has access to a bicycle at

home. Jackrs past experience includes riding a tricycle, a “big wheel”, a bicycle with “big

fat” training wheels, and a scooter. He rode the scooter very slowly. Jeremy has two

siblings, a brother age 7 and a sister age 12. He has access to bicycles at home. His

experience includes riding a bicycle with training wheels and previous to that, he rode a

Fisher Price “hot wheels” trike. Bob has two sisters ages 23 and 25. Bob has access to a

bicycle at home. His past experience includes riding a “big wheel”. He was not able to

ride a tricycle or a conventional bicycle. Pete has 3 brothers and I sister who range in age

from 5-14. He has access to bicycles at home. His past experience includes riding a

tricycle and for the past 2 years he rode a bicycle with training wheels. Danny has two

younger brothers, ages 4 and 8. He has access to bicycles at home. His has had numerous

41

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. experiences with bicycles. His mother was very motivated to include Danny on a family

bike ride. At first, he practiced riding on a therapeutic bicycle. Following this, he rode on

a “tag-a-long” bike. The “tag-a-Iong” is an attachment to a conventional bicycle. This set­

up is very similar to riding in the second position on a tandem bike. Erin has no sibling.

She does have access to a bicycle at home and experience includes riding a bicycle with

training wheels. Allen has no siblings. He has access to a bicycle at home and his past

experience involved riding a bicycle with training wheels. However, his mother reported

that riding with training wheels was still difficult for him.

Description of Independent Variable

The independent variable for this study was a bicycle intervention (BI) plus

feedback. Participants were introduced to cycling skills through a series of adapted

bicycles (see Appendix A) designed to allow participants to gradually become

accustomed to the dynamics of cycling. The investigator provided both positive

corrective and positive specific feedback based upon individual performance. Feedback

was provided hierarchically on three aspects of performance; 1) pedal rate, 2) steering

participation, and 3) head position. Feedback was given following each trial, while the

participant was not in the act o f riding the bicycle.

Bike A (double-roller)

Bike A featured two rollers in lieu o f conventional pneumatic tires. The rollers

were mounted onto a single-speed bicycle frame. One roller is located in the front

position and the other is located in the rear position of the bicycle. The configuration

consisted of #5 rollers in the front and back positions. The #5 rollers represented the most

stable rollers in that they had they least amount of contour. Bike A had a direct drive or

42

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. fixed gear drive train as opposed to a free wheel or "coaster brake" system. With a direct

drive bicycle; forward motion occurs only when the pedals are moving (this does not

allow for coasting). With a free wheel system, the bicycle can coast forward while the

pedals are stationary. Bike A was the most stable bicycle in the series doe to the weight

and contour of the two rollers. In addition, the gearing was altered sufficiently to slow

forward speed (town relative to pedaling cadence. The bicycle's gearing was about one-

third that o f a conventional similarly sized bicycle. Therefore, participants could

concentrate on pedaling the bicycle without the fear of foiling or the fear of forward

speed.

Bike B (rear-roller)

Bike B is a variation on the two-roller bicycle. It featured only one roller in the

rear position. The front fork was configured with a conventional front fork and front tire.

Roller #5 was used with Bike B. This bike had a direct drive or fixed gear drive train as

opposed to a free wheel or "coaster brake" system.

Bike B was the second bike in the sequence. It was less stable than Bike A in that

it only utilized one roller in the rear position Therefore, the participant had to begin

actively participate in the steering o f the bicycle to maintain balance. Again, the gearing

was altered sufficiently to slow forward speed down relative to pedaling cadence. This

allowed the participant to concentrate on steering participation, without the fear of

forward speed.

43

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Bike C (rear-roHer)

Bike C was the second, rear-roller bicycle. There were two differences between

Bike B and Bike C. First, a #7 roller was used with Bike C. The #7 roller was less stable

than the previous (#5 roller) in that it cut with more contour. Second, the sprocket size on

Bike C was altered, so that the forward speed o f the bicycle was increased relative to

pedal cadence.

Bike O (fat-tire)

Bike O consisted of a standard bicycle frame, which featured a 16 x 6.50-8

inflatable garden tractor tire located in the front position. The rear tire was a conventional

bicycle tire. Bike D operated with a conventional free wheel drive train.

Bike D was the least stable of the four adapted bikes. The width of the fat tire was

about half that of the rollers, although they were similar in weight The gyroscopic action

of the fat tire assisted with stabilization. In addition, Bike D maintained a conventional

drive train. This meant that the forward speed of the bicycle relative to pedaling cadence

was similar to that of a conventional bicycle. The participant was required to actively

participate in the steering while moving at a typical forward speed.

Degrees o f Freedom

As a participant progresses from Bike A to Bike E, the available degrees of

freedom are gradually expanded. This increase in the degrees o f freedom is due to

specific design features o f each bicycle in the progression. For example, from Bike A to

Bike E, the tire configuration changes from very stable rollers, to rollers with more

contour and less stability, to a wide tractor tire, and finally to conventional bicycle tires.

With the change in tires, comes an increase in the degrees of freedom, relative to balance

44

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. and steering participation. The same principle applies in terms of forward speed

allowances along the bicycle progression. Changes in the front crank size and a reduction

in tire friction allow for increasing speed from bike to bike. In essence, are increasing

opportunities in response to the changing dynamics o f each bicycle, which can give rise

to new and adaptive movement forms.

Size of Bicycles

There were two sizes of bicycles available, 16” (40.64 cm) and 20” (50.8 cm).

The dimensions (16”, 20”) refer to the circumference of the tires. Each participant was fit

to a bicycle so that his/her feet could comfortably contact the floor flat-footed. This was

accomplished by adjusting the height of the seat

Description ofthe Dependent Variables

Distance Traveled Independently

The distance traveled independently to the nearest meter on each of the four

adapted bicycles and on the conventional bicycle served as one o fthe dependent

variables in this study. Criterion for each o f the bicycles was riding independently for a

distance of 12 m, in 3 out of 5 consecutive trials. An independent trial was scored when

the participant maintained balance of the bicycle. On bicycles with a conventional drive

train, coasting was permitted. The trial ended when a) the investigator touched the bike or

the participant, b) a foot touched the floor, or c) the front tire of the bicycle touched a

boundary line (see Appendix G). The instructor would intervene to ensure the safety of

the participants. When the bicycle appeared to be tipping over, the instructor would

intervene prior to a fell either by placing her hands on the participants sides and under

4 5

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. the arms, or by grabbing the underside o f the bicycle seat (safety was a priority). Data

involving distance traveled independently were collected through video-tape coding.

Num ber o f Trials

The number of trials to criterion on each o f the four adapted bicycles and on the

conventional bicycle was another dependent variable. Criterion for each o f the bicycles

was 12 m, in 3 out of 5 consecutive trials o f independent riding. Data involving number

o f trials to criterion were collected through video-tape coding.

Downstrokes per Minute

Downstrokes per minute was another dependent variable. The pedal rate

measured in downstrokes per minute (dpm) was another dependent variable. Pedal rate

was reported as downstrokes per minute based on the number of downstrokes per trial,

and the elapsed time for that trial. A downstroke consisted of movement of the foot and

pedal from the top position to the bottom position beginning at the 12 o'clock to 2 o'clock

position and ending at the 6 o'clock position on the crank. When the foot and pedal began

at the 3 o'clock position or lower, a downstroke was not counted.

Head Position

Head position was another dependent variable. Data on head position were

collected by partial interval recording with two-second intervals. If the head was

observed in an upright position anytime during the two-second interval, the interval was

recorded as positive for upright head position. Upright head position was reported as the

percentage o f intervals in which the participant's head was in an upright position. An

example o f upright head position was when the nose was pointed forward (8 o’clock to

10 o'clock position). A non-example occurred when the nose was pointed m the direction

46

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ofthe floor (6 o'clock to 7 o’clock) or toward the sideline or ceiling (II o’clock to 12

o’clock). These data were collected through videotape observation. Data were collected

during pretest probes and acquisition. Various combinations of head position and pedal

rate have been observed in proficient cyclists as well as in individuals who are initially

developing cycling skills. lit this study, data were collected relative to the stability and/or

instability of the hypothesized attractors involved in the complex skill of cycling.

Maintenance

Maintenance of conventional cycling skills was another dependent variable.

Maintenance is defined as the extent to which a learner continues to perform a target

behavior upon completion of the intervention (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 1987). In this

study, a maintenance session was conducted to determine the degree to which

participants maintained performance on the conventional bicycle:

Generalization

Generalization was another dependent variable in this study. Response

generalization required the participant to perform a variation of the task. In this case, the

participant was required to navigate through a series of cones (Appendix G).

Social Validity

Social validity data was another dependent variable. At the completion o f the

cycling sessions, a social validity questionnaire was completed by each o f the

parents/guardians of the study participants. Opinions regarding the efficacy of utilizing

adapted bicycles as a learning tool in acquiring conventional cycling skills were obtained.

In addition, opinions regarding the benefits o f acquiring conventional cycling skills

relative to community recreation and leisure were included.

47

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Procedures

Participants rode four adapted bicycles and a conventional bicycle. Participants

attended three, 45 min sessions a week, for a maximum o f five weeks. All sessions took

place inside a gymnasium. Participants were instructed not to practice cycling skills

outside o f study sessions. Parent/guardians were not present in the gymnasium once the

session began. The researcher, two research assistants, and the participant, were the only

persons in the gym at the time of data collection. The research assistants were responsible

for operating the two video-cameras. Participants received individualized instruction.

Initially, a pretest probe was taken to determine the participant's level of performance on

each bicycle progression. Following this, the first bicycle in the series (Bike A) was

introduced. The investigator provided either positive, positive/corrective or

positive/specific feedback relative to the participant's performance on each trial.

Feedback was provided only after the participant had completed a trial and was not in the

act of riding the bicycle. After criterion level performance was reached on a bicycle, one

generalization probe was taken on that bicycle. Following this, one probe of each bicycle

in the progression was taken to determine whether performance changes had occurred in

any other steps. During acquisition, participants rode the bicycles in sequential order (I)

Bike A, (2) Bike B, (3) Bike C, (4) Bike D, and (5) Bike E (conventional bike).

Participants did not advance to the next bicycle in the series untO a criterion level of

performance was reached on the current bicycle. Criterion was set at 12 m of independent

riding on 3 out of 5 consecutive trials. Once a participant met criterion levels on the

conventional bicycle (Bike E), a single generalization probe was taken and the session

48

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ended. On a separate (by, a maintenance/generalization session took: place. A maximum

o f 15 maintenance and 15 generalization trials occurred during the

maintenance/generalization session. All sessions were video-taped from two angles

using RCA Model CC4362 camcorders. A stationary video-camera was placed at the end

o f the data collection area to capture a front view of each participant. A second video­

camera (on rollers) was moved along the sideline to capture a sideview of the participant.

Each trial began at the ready line. The instructor pushed the bicycle forward 13 m

to the starting line. As the front the o f the bicycle touched the starting line, the instructor

released the seat. This is the point were measurement of the trial began. The trial ended

when: (a) the investigator touched the participant or the bicycle, (to intervene prior to a

fall) (b) the participant touched the floor with a foot, or (c) the front tire ofthe bicycle

touched the side boundary line. Upon completion o f each trial, the investigator

announced “stop” and the distance was measured from the starting line to the contact

point o f the lead tire. Gym tape was placed on the floor, (see Appendix G for floor

diagram) in addition to numbered markers, to assist in the measurement of the distance

traveled. At the end of each trial, the investigator asked the child to dismount the bicycle

and walk back to the starting position. Prior to the start of the next trial, the investigator

provided either positive, positive/corrective or positive/specific feedback to the

participant. Feedback addressed the hypothetical attractors of pedal rate, steering

participation or head position. Following is a detailed description of each phase in the

study including scripts.

49

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The pretest was conducted on a one-to-one (researcher and participant) basis.

When a participant entered the gymnasium, the instructor introduced herself and then

described the purpose of the study. After a brief explanation, the investigator asked for

verbal assent from the participant The investigator said:

“Hi, my name is Tammy. [ teach people how to ride bicycles. I have four special

bicycles that help people learn how to ride a regular bicycle.”

The investigator then showed each of the bicycles to the participant

“Would it be o.k. with you if I taught you how to ride a bicycle?”

Following this, procedures for practice sessions were explained. The investigator said:

“You will get to practice with all of these bicycles. Before you get on a bicycle,

you will have to put your helmet on. We will start by adjusting the seats, so that

they are in the right position for you.”

The seat of each bicycle was adjusted so that the participant could sit on the seat

while his/her feet comfortably maintained contact with the floor.

“First, you will get to try riding each bicycle one time. Then, we will practice on

Bike A. You will start with the bike behind the black tine. You will sit on the bike

and put your feet on the pedals and hands on the handlebars while I hold the seat

from behind. When you are ready, I will help get you started. I will say Ready,

here we go! You just ride the bicycle the best you can. Ride as far as you can.

When you stop, you will get off of the bike and we will walk it back to the

starting line to try again. Do you have any questions?"

50

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The investigator held the bicycle steady and upright behind the black "ready" line

as the participant mounted the bicycle. On the "Ready, here we go" signal, the

investigator poshed die bicycle forward for a distance of 1.5 m. When the front tire

touched the starting line, the investigator released the bicycle seat The trial ended when

(a) the instructor touched the bicycle or the participant, (b) the participant's foot touched

the floor, (c) the front the o f the bicycle crossed one of the side boundary lines. If a

bicycle began to tip and the participant failed to react appropriately, the investigator

immediately intervened to prevent a fall. Positive feedback was provided during the

pretest probes.

Acquisition

The acquisition phase took place on a one-to-one basis, three days a week, 45

minutes per session. During acquisition, the participant was introduced to a series of five

bicycles (4 adapted, I conventional). The order of progression was Bike A, Bike B, Bike

C, Bike D and Bike E (conventional bike). Instructions were as follows:

“You will start with the bike behind the black line. You will sit on the bike and

put your feet on the pedals and hands on the handlebars while I hold the seat from

behind. When you are ready, I will help get you started. I will say 'Ready, here

we go!’ You ride the bicycle the best you can. Go as for as you can go. When you

stop, you will get off of the bike and we will walk it back to the starting line to try

again Do you have any questions?”

The investigator held the bicycle steady and upright behind the black "ready” line

as the participant mounted the bicycle. Prior to each trial, the instructor reminded the

participant to ride as for as he/she could. On the "Ready, here we go” signal, the

51

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. investigator pushed the bicycle forward for a distance of 1.5 m. When the front tire

touched the starting line; the investigator released the bicycle seat The trial ended when

(a) the instructor touched the bicycle or the participant, (b) the participant's foot touched

the floor, (c) the front tire o f the bicycle crossed one o f the side boundary lines. When a

bicycle began to tip and the participant failed to react appropriately, the investigator

immediately intervened to prevent a fall.

The performance of each participant was closely observed as he/she rode the

bicycle. At the end o f each trial either positive, positive/corrective or positive specific

feedback addressing one of three performance issues (a) pedal rate, (b) steering

participation, or (c) head position was provided. Positive/corrective and positive specific

feedback hierachically addressed pedal rate, steering participation and head position. For

example, if the child was pedaling very slowly, the investigator said, "Good work! This

time, use your feet to pedal faster". In addition to providing verbal feedback on pedal

rate, the participant would occasionally practice pedaling on a stationary bike stand.

When addressing steering participation, a static balance drill was administered in

addition to verbal feedback. With the bike at the starting position and the participant on

the bike (hands on handlebars and feet on pedals), the instructor held the bike seat from

behind with two hands and slightly tipped the bike to the left and to the right while

instructing the child to react by moving the handlebars in the same direction o f the lean.

The investigator said.

“You need to move your arms. When the bike leans this way, you move the

handlebars in the same direction.”

After repeating the static drill three times, the investigator said:

52

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. “Good work! Remember to move your arms!”

If the participant rode with his/her head down or looking towards the sidelines;

the investigator provided feedback related to bead position. The investigator said

“Keep your head up, so you can see where you are going! Look at Curious

G eorge.”

(A sheet with a painting of Curious George was hung at the opposite end of the gym.)

Prior to each trial, the instructor reminded the participant to keep his/her eyes on Curious

George. Once criterion level was met on a bicycle, a single generalization probe was

administered on that bicycle. Following this, one probe was taken on each of the

remaining bicycles in the series and the participant advanced to the next bicycle in the

series.

Generalization Probes

A generalization probe occurred at the completion of criterion level on each

bicycle. Response generalization consisted o f a probe in which the participant was

instructed to navigate the bicycle through a series of cones placed at 1,4,8 and 12 m (see

Appendix G). Prior to the first generalization probe, the instructor demonstrated to the

participant the path that they should take by modeling a single trial. Prior to subsequent

probes, the instructorand participant would walk through the cones to remind the

participant of the correct path to follow. The investigator said:

“This time, you will ride the bike between each set of cones, like this (model one trial).

Now it’s your turn.”

53

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. B icycle Probes

Throughout the acquisition phase, a single probe was taken with each successive

bicycle in the series to determine the relationship between the independent variable and

the successive approximation of the skill o f cycling. For example, once a participant

reached the criterion level for Bike A, a generalization probe was taken using Bike A,

then a single bicycle probe was taken with Bike B, Bike C, Bike D and Bike E

(conventional bicycle) prior to practice on Bike B. In addition, intermittent bicycle probes

were taken throughout acquisition after approximately every 10 trials.

Maintenance/Generalization Session

A maintenance/generalization session took place in the session following

termination of the intervention. This occurred one to two days following acquisition of

conventional cycling skills at criterion level. During maintenance, participants performed

a maximum of 15 trials with the conventional bicycle. If the participant reached criterion

levels after 10 trials, the maintenance session ended. Cooper, Heron, and Heward (1987)

define maintenance as the extent to which a learner continues to perform a target

behavior upon completion o f the intervention. Within the present study, a maintenance

session was conducted to determine the degree to which participants maintained

performance on the conventional bicycle. The instructorexplained the maintenance phase

as follows:

Today, you will be practicing only on the regular bicycle. We will start with the

bike behind the black line. You will sit on the bike and put your feet on the pedals

and hands on the handlebars while I hold the seat from behind When you are

ready, I will help get you started and say ready, here we go? Ride the bicycle as

54

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. far as you can. When you stop, you will get off of the bike and we will walk it

hack to the starting line to try again. Do you have any questions?”

No specific or corrective feedback was provided during maintenance. At the completion

of each trial, the instructor said:

“Good job, let's do it again f”

Prior to each trial, the instructor reminded the participant to ride the bicycle as far as

possible.

After the maintenance trials were completed, the generalization trials began. Each

participant was given a maximum of 15 generalization trials. Again, if the participant

reached criterion levels after 10 trials, the generalization session ended. Response

generalization consisted of trials in which the participant was instructed to navigate the

bicycle through a series of cones placed at 1,4,8, and 12m (see Appendix G). Prior to the

generalization trials, the instructor demonstrated to the participant the path that they

should take by modeling a single trial.

Inter-observer Agreement

The investigator and a physical education doctoral student independently coded

data from the video-tapes. The coding consisted of. (a) number of trials, (b) distance

traveled independently on each o f the five bicycles (Bike A, Bike B, Bike C, Bike D and

Bike E (conventional bicycle), and (c) hypothesized attractors (pedal rate and head

position). The investigator discussed data collection procedures with the second coder

and distributed th e data, sheets (see Appendix H). The second coder attended a practice

session on how to accurately record data on the (feta sheets for video-taped coding.

During this practice session, the investigator and second coder independently coded data

55

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. from the video-tape, then compared and discussed resuits. Coding did not continue until a

minimum o f 90% agreement was reached between the two codecs. The investigator and

second coder discussed any concerns from the practice session. The investigator coded all

bicycle sessions from videotape and the second coder coded one full session for each

participant This inchided sessions across all bikes throughout acquisition, maintenance

and generalization. To ensure accuracy between the observers, the investigator

announced “stop” at the end o f each trial during data collection. Trial by trial agreement

between the two coders was calculated by using the formula:

(Agreement Agreement +■ Disagreement) x (100) = % Agreement).

Treatment Integrity and Procedural Reliability

Treatment integrity refers to the extent to which the independent variable is

applied or implemented as intended (Cooper et al., 1987). To ensure that the independent

variable was presented in an accurate and consistent manner, a procedural reliability

checklist (see Appendix I) was utilized across all session and pertaining to all trials.

Individuals present during the taping o f each session were trained to collect data

regarding all treatment components on the reliability checklist Observer training was

conducted in a live, mock session with the investigator and observers present. During the

training session, the investigator first explained how each trial would progress, step-by-

step. The treatment integrity checklist was introduced and the instructor provided a live

demonstration on the bicycles. Following this, the investigator addressed any questions or

concerns related to treatment integrity and procedural reliability.

56

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Experimental Design

A multiple probe design was used for this study (Cooper et aL, 1987). The

multiple probe design was appropriate for evaluating the relationship between the

independent variables (adapted bicycle intervention plus feedback) and the acquisition of

successive approximations o f a skill (cycling). Specifically, distance traveled

independently, number of trials, pedal rate, head position, maintenance and

generalization. In this study the participants gradually acquired the skill o f cycling as they

practiced on progressively more challenging adapted bicycles. Four adapted bicycles and

a conventional two-wheeled bicycle comprised the instructional series. Rather than

collecting prolonged baseline data on skills that were not yet developed, intermittent

measurements (probes) provided the information necessary to determine whether or not a

change had occurred prior to intervention.

Social Validity

At the conclusion of data collection, a social validity questionnaire was mailed to

the parents/guardians of all study participants. The questionnaire consisted of nine Likert-

scale statements. Opinions regarding the efficacy of utilizing adapted bicycles as a

learning tool in acquiring conventional cycling skills was obtained. In addition, opinions

regarding the benefits o f acquiring conventional cycling skills relative to community

recreation and leisure were included in the questionnaire (see Appendix J). There was a

100% return rate. This required the investigator to provide two parents with a prompt by

phone.

57

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Data Analysis

Visual inspection of the graphs was used to interpret data. Both witiun-phase and

between-phase analyses were conducted Within-phase visual analysis involved a

determination of the number and variability of data points, in addition to the level of

performance and the direction and degree of trends. Between-phase visual analysis, was

conducted to determine changes in level, trend, and degree of variability. Individual

graphics were displayed for each study participant Data were presented descriptively

using means, ranges, and percentages.

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

This chapter presents the results of the effects o f using adapted bicycles on the

acquisition, maintenance, and generalization o f conventional cycling skills by children

with mild MR. Fourteen major sections are included in this chapter. Interobserver

agreement results are addressed in the first section. The second section provides

information regarding procedural reliability results. Sections three through twelve include

data for all 10 participants. Section thirteen presents a summary o f results. Finally, social

validity results are discussed in the last section of this chapter.

Interobserver Agreement

The interobserver agreement percentage for accuracy o f bicycling acquisition

utilizing a series of adapted bicycles and a conventional bicycle was taken across all 10

participants. Session-by-session interobserver agreement data o f distance by trial were

taken from ten sessions across all five bicycles (Bikes A, B, C, D, and E). Sessions were

selected based upon a predetermined schedule to allow for observation of acquisition

across all five bicycles (see Appendix K). Session-by-session interobserver agreement

data of downstrokes per minute and upright head position were taken from ten sessions

across all 10 participants, examining the first session of each participant Table 2 contains

the interobserver agreement percentage for accuracy o f distance by trial for each of the

ten participants- The mean interobserver agreement for cycling distance by trial was

96.4% across ten participants (range 90% to 100%). 59

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Participant IOA fo r D istance

I 100% 2 100% 3 100% 4 100% 5 90% 6 94% 7 93% 8 95% 9 100% 10 92% Mean 96.4%

Table 2. Interobserver agreement (percent agreement) for distance by trial.

Interobserver agreement percentages for accuracy of downstrokes per minute and

upright head position by trial for each of the ten participants are displayed in Tables 3 and

4 respectively. The mean interobserver agreement for downstrokes per minute by trial

was 91.6% across ten participants (range 83 to 98%). The mean interobserver agreement

for upright head position by trial was 78% across 10 participants (range 67 to 88%).

Although and IOA of 90% or better are generally considered adequate for permanent

products, this percentage is arbitrary and must be considered in light of the complexity

and quantity o f observations. Interobserver agreement with an average of 75% is

considered to be adequate for data collected across intervals of 5 seconds or less (Cooper,

Heron, & Heward, 1987).

Reproduced with permission ofthe copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Participant IOA fo r Downstrokes p er M inute I 94% 2 83% 3 83% 4 94% 5 91% 6 97% 7 98% 8 87% 9 93% 10 96% M ean 91.6%

Table 3. Intecobserver Agreement (Percent Agreement) for Downstrokes Per Minute

Participant IOA for Upright Head Position 1 82% 2 86% 3 70% 4 84% 5 70% 6 88% 7 82% 8 67% 9 78% 10 73% Mean 78%

Table 4. Interobserver Agreement (Percent Agreement) for Upright Head Position

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Procedural Reliability

Procedural reliability for this study was established by the use o fa procedural

reliability checklist consisting o f seven questions (see Appendix I). Procedural reliability

data were obtained for every session in the study (total o f 58 sessions). The video-tape

operator completed the procedural reliability checklist at the end o f each session. Across

all sessions for all participants, procedural reliability was calculated at 98.7% (range 94

to 100%). Procedural reliability questions pertained to all trials in a session.

Participant Procedural Reliability Percentage t 100% 2 100% 3 100% 4 100% 5 100% 6 94% 7 100% 8 98% 9 97% 10 98% Mean 98.7%

Table S. Procedural reliability by participant across all sessions. The values represent the percent of number of “yes” answers on the procedural reliability checklist

Participant I

Distance and Trials

Jim rode the 20 in (50.8 cm) size bicycles (A-E) and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle (Bike E) in one session (29 trials). Figure 1 is a graphic display of

Jim’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization o f conventional cycling skills across

Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Jim rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B, a

distance o f4 m; Bike C, a distance o f I m; Bike D, a distance o f 12 m and Bike E, a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

DISTANCE/METERS 12 O -i©t Figure I . D istance traveled in m eters, by bike, trial, session ( | ) (Participant #1) (Participant ) | ( session trial, bike, by eters, m in traveled I istance . D Figure I ___ TRIALS 63 distance of 3 m. Following the pretest, Jim immediately met criterion levels on Bike A.

He was also able to generalize this skill at a distance o f 12 m. Likewise, on Bikes B and

C he immediately met criterion levels (12 m). He was also able to generalize this skill on

Bikes B and C (12 m). On Bike D he immediately met criterion levels. The generalization

probe elicited a distance of 1 m. Again, on Bike E he immediately met criterion levels (12

m). Jim was moderately successful at generalization with a trial distance of 8 m. Jim

demonstrated skill maintenance for 100% of the trials, (distance of 12 m). He

demonstrated skill generalization for 50% of the trials, (distance o f 12 m).

Downstrokes per Minute

Downstrokes per minute was another dependent variable that was recorded

Figure 2 is a graphic display of the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike and by

trial. The number of acquisition downstrokes per minute (dpm) on Bike A range from

60-200. There was a sharp increase in dpm during acquisition, with a drop to 126 dpm for

the generalization probe. Bike B acquisition elicited a rather sharp increase in dpm (from

70-200dpm). During the generalization probe, there was a decrease to 140 dpm. On Bike

C, Jim demonstrated a sharp increase in dpm (from 30-180), followed by a decrease from

180-105 dpm. This decrease followed instructor feedback asking Jim to slow down. Jim

was intentionally speeding up as he approached the wall at the end of the gym and the

instructor felt that it was necessary to intervene to ensure his safety. During the Bike C

generalization probe, Jim rode at 113 dpm. During acquisition on Bike D, he exhibited a

range o f44-155 dpm. Jim again demonstrated a rapidly increasingly data

path, followed by a sharp decrease when reminded to slow down. The Bike D

generalization probe elicited 40 dpm. On Bike E, Jim’s initial probe was 30 dpm. This

64

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

DOWNSTROKES PER MINUTE •Z' A Figure 2. D ow nstrokes p er M inute, by bike and tria l (P articipant #1) articipant (P l tria and bike by inute, M er p nstrokes ow D 2. Figure * * * * * * * TRIALS 65 * S 0 GENERALIZATION 0 • PCACriCE * FEEDBACK * FEEDBACK PCACriCE • MAINTENANCE e d o k was followed by an increase as well as a mildly variable data path during acquisition,

(range o f 69-84 dpm). During maintenance, be demonstrated a mildly variable data path

(range o f69-96 dpm). During generalization, the pattern was also mildly variable, (range

o f75-96 dpm).

Head Position

Figure 3 is a graphic display of upright head position (uhp) data based on the

percentage o f intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position On Bike A

acquisition, Jim demonstrated a sharp increase (0 to 50%) in uhp. Dining the Bike A

generalization probe, upright head position was recorded at 33%. On Bike B, Jim

demonstrated 0% uhp throughout acquisition and the generalization probe. On Bike C

acquisition, Jim initially demonstrated a steady data path of 0% uhp, followed by a rapid

increase to 100%. On the Bike C generalization probe, he also displayed 100% uhp. On

Bike D acquisition, he initially demonstrated 0% uhp followed by a rapid increase of

100%. On Bike D generalization probe, a rapid decrease to 0% uhp was observed. On

Bike E acquisition, Jim demonstrated a highly variable data path o f upright head position

(range of 0-100%). During the Bike E maintenance session, upright head position

increased from 0-50%, followed by a decrease and then a consistent pattern of 0%.

During the generalization session, upright head position was highly variable, (range o f 0-

100%).

66

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

% UPRIGHT HEAD POSITION Figure 3. Percent U pright Head Position* by bike and tria l (P articipant #1) articipant (P l tria and bike by Position* Head pright U Percent 3. Figure tees- 67 TRIALS m E s o a r O A CEMEKAUZA.TWN Q • PRACTICE * FEEDBACK * PRACTICE • ntenance c n a n e t in a m A Summary

Jim successfully acquired the skill o f riding a conventional bicycle after 29 trials,

in o ik session. He maintained this skill at 100%. He was able to generalize this skill 50%

of the time. He demonstrated a mean o f 85 dpm and 10% upright head position during

maintenance. During the generalization session, he demonstrated a mean o f 82 dpm and

25% uhp.

Participant 2

D istance and T rials

Billy rode the 16 in (40.64 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle in two sessions (50 trials total). Figure 4 is a graphic display of

Billy’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of conventional cycling skills across

Bikes A-E. During the pretest tnals, Billy rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B, a

distance of 0 m; Bike C, a distance of 9 m; Bike D, a distance of 1 m and Bike E, a

distance of 3 m. Billy immediately met criterion levels on Bikes A, B and C. hi addition,

he was also able to generalize this skill for a distance o f 12 m on Bikes A, B and C. Up to

this point, bicycle probes for Bike D ranged horn 0 to I m in distance. The data path for

Bike D was extremely variable, ranging from 0 to 12 m. After 17 trials, Billy successfully

met criterion level (12 m) m 3 out o f 5 trials. He was also able to generalize this skill at a

distance o f 12 m on Bike D. Up to this point, bicycle probes o f Bike E ranged from 0-12

m. Baseline measures for Bike E were extremely variable, ranging from 4-12 m. After 10

trials, Billy successfully met criterion levels o f 3 out o f 5 trials at 12 m. He was also able

to generalize this skill at a distance o f 12 m on Bike E. During the maintenance and

68

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

DISTANCE/METERS Figure 4. D istance traveled in m eters, by bike, tria l, session ( | ) (P articipant #2) articipant (P ) | ( session l, tria bike, by eters, m in traveled istance D 4. Figure *T®|—0 B, 5 |~ (J |~ 5 B, 1 •r > • . a i ® 1 1 1 l \ t 1 ------! * i L i I#’ B ~ si ------. ------° T r TR/ACS 4HMMKMMX Q(Mwmg>now Y^TVY ------* B C A generalization session, Billy demonstrated maintenance o f the skill doting 20% o f the

time, at 12 m. He demonstrated generalization o fthe skill during 80% o f the

generalization trials, at 12 m.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 5 is a graphic display ofthe number o f downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by triaL The number o f acquisition downstrokes per minute (djm ) on Bike A range

from 49-92. Billy demonstrated a gradual increase in dpm during acquisition. The

generalization probe was recorded at IB dpm. On Bike B, the data path initially revealed

a rapid increase, followed by a gradual increase in downstrokes per minute. The range

was 30-120 dpm on Bike B. The generalization probe was recorded at 133 dpm. On Bike

C, the range was 64-106 dpm during acquisition. Billy exhibited a gradual increase in

dpm. The generalization probe for Bike C was recorded at 113 dpm. On Bike D, the

range was 0-80 dpm during acquisition. Downstrokes per minute were variable during

acquisition. The generalization probe was recorded as 72 dpm on Bike D. On Bike E, the

data path was variable, with a range of 0-86 dpm. The generalization probe was 75 dpm

on Bike E. During Billy's maintenance session, he demonstrated a variable data path,

with a range o f20-90 dpm. During generalization, the data path was variable, with a

range o f30-90 dpm.

Head Position

Figured is a graphic display of head position data based on the percentage of

intervals that Billy's head was in an upright position. On Bike A, Billy demonstrated a

higdily variable data path, with a range of 0-78% during acquisition. Upright head

7 0

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. * PRACTICE* FEEDBACK. 0 PROBE Q oenerauzation MAINTENANCE ------»A

r & fc*o ■? €

Cj r § *5v*

^ - D

■ f t v A A 0

TRt/HS

Figure 5. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant # 2 )

71

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. • PRACTICE * FEEDBACK 0 PROBE Q GENERALIZATION A MAINTENANCE f r

IV &

o

Figure 6. Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #2)

72

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. position was 0% for the generalization probe on Bike A. On Bike B, the data path was

extremely variable, with a range from 20-100% uhp. The generalization probe was

recorded at 50%. On Bike C, the data path was extremely variable, with a range of 25-

100% uhp. The generalization probe on Bike C was recorded at 67%. On Bike D, the data

path was extremely variable, with a range of 0-100%. The generalization probe was

recorded as 67% on Bike D. On Bike E, the majority of uhp data points (12 out of 14)

were at 100%. The range on Bike E was 0-100%. The generalization probe was recorded

at 0% uhp. During Billy’s maintenance session, the data path was extremely variable,

with a range o f 0-100%. During the generalization session, the data path was extremely

variable, with a range o f 0-100%.

Summary

Billy successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle after a total

o f 50 trials, across two sessions. He maintained this at 20%. He was able to generalize

this skill 80% of the tnne. He demonstrated a mean of 51 dpm and 67% upright head

position during maintenance. During the generalization session, he demonstrated a mean

of 65 dpm and 70% upright head position.

Participant 3

D istance and T rials

Mary rode the 16 in (40.64 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle in four sessions (82 trials total). Figure 7 is a graphic demonstration

ofMary’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of conventional cycling skills

across Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Mary rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike

B, a distance o f 3 m; Bike C, a distance of 12 m; Bike D, a distance of 3 m; and Bike E, a

73 j

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced Figure 7 . D istance traveled in m eters, by bike, tria l, session ( ! ) (Participant #3) (Participant ) ! ( session l, tria bike, by eters, m in traveled istance D . 7 Figure a-|«,— 0 a-|«,— "I

Pi'STAHCe/AiimtS L^r i * » ej- !*-»• » • ■» a-l • ------* i • • * _ • 1 — 1 • —~- - ~ t— t i l ------( 1 1_ ' ' 0 . " W J U w m i n — H ^ b M^ M T B M M C X . i .♦ 1 ------TRIALS 74 ----- * 1 ------T W/“ ------« a A & B c distance of I m. Following the pretest, Mary immediately met criterion levels on Bike A.

She was also able to generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m on Bike A. On Bike B,

Mary had one bike probe at 11 meters. During acquisition, she met criterion after 6 trials.

She was also able to generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m on Bike B. On Bike C,

Mary had two probes, one at 5m and one at 12 m. She immediately met criterion levels

on Bike C in three trials. She was able to generalize this skill at a distance of 8 m on Bike

C. On Bike D, Mary had three probes at 0 m. The data path for Bike D was extremely

variable and increasing. After 40 trials, Mary met criterion levels on Bike D. She was

able to generalize this skill at a distance of 4 m. On Bike E, Mary had seven bike probes

ranging from 0-5 m in distance. Mary met criterion levels during acquisition after 12

trials. The data path was extremely variable. Two generalization probes were recorded at

0 and I m in distance. During the maintenance and generalization session, Mary

demonstrated maintenance o f the skill 70% of the time, at 12 m. Mary demonstrated

generalization of the skill at 1-4 in, on Bike E.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 8 is a graphic display of the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by trial. The number of downstrokes per minute on Bike A range from 78-112, with a

variable data path. The generalization probe on Bike A was recorded at 100 dpm. On

Bike B, the data path was extremely variable, with a range o f30-124. The generalization

probe on Bike B was recorded at 96 dpm. On Bike C, the data path was gradually

increasing, with a range o f 58-115 dpm. The generalization probe on Bike C was

recorded at 110 dpm. On Bike D, the data path was variable, with a range of 0-120 dpm.

The generalization probe on Bike D was recorded at 48 dpm. On Bike E, the data path

75

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. • rucnce * FZEDOAOt | Book QoENOAUZAnON V o 1 4 MMNTBMMCS C------‘A

-V“” b

C X**~

n D8HB®B p 7TCiAlS

Figure S. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #3)

76

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. over the first half of trials was extremely variable, with a range o f 0-150 dpm. During the

remaining trials, the data path became more stable, with a range o f70-96 dpm. The

generalization probe was recorded at 60 dpm on Bike E. During Mary’s maintenance

session, the data path was stable, with a range o f69-96 dpm. During generalization trials,

the data path was stable, with a range o f60-72 dpm.

Head Position

Figure 9 is a graphic display o f upright head position data based on the percentage

of intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position. On Bike A, the data

path was extremely variable, with a range o f 0-50%. The generalization probe was

recorded at 60% uhp. On Bike B, the data path was extremely variable, with a range o f 0-

100%. The Bike B generalization probe was recorded at 0% uhp. On Bike C, the data

path was variable (range o f 0-33%) uhp. The generalization probe was recorded at 0%

uhp. On Bike D, the majority (27 out of 28) of data points over the first half of

acquisition, were at 0% uhp. During the second half of Bike D acquisition, the data path

became extremely variable, (range o f 0-100%). The generalization probe for Bike D was

recorded at 100% uhp. On Bike E, the first half of the data path was stable at 0% uhp.

The second half of the data path was extremely variable, with a range of 0-100% uhp.

The generalization probe on Bike E was recorded at 100% uhp. During the maintenance

session, Mary demonstrated an extremely variable data path, with a range of 0-100% uhp.

During the generalization session, Mary exhibited an extremely data path, with a range of

0-100% uhp.

77

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. * FftACTICE « FEEDBACK O p I O I E

Q g o e x m e a t v k A. KABREKANCE •L

-* c \? l.nA

TBialS

Figure 9. Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #3)

78

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Summary

Mary successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in five

sessions (82 trials total). She maintained this skill 70% of the time at a distance of 12 m.

She generalized this skill at 1 to 4 m. Mary demonstrated a mean of 88 dpm and 20%

upright head position during maintenance. During the generalization session, she

demonstrated a mean o f 63 dpm and 63% upright head position.

Participant4

D istance and Trials

Jack rode the 20 in (50.8 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle in 4 sessions (156 trials total). Figure 10 is a graphic display of

Jack’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization o f conventional cycling skills across

Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Jack rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B, a

distance of 0 m; Bike C, a distance o f 0 m; Bike D, a distance of 0 m; and Bike E, a

distance of 0 m. Jack immediately met criterion levels of 3 out of 5 trials at 12 m on Bike

A. He was also able to generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m. Jack rode 0 m on Bike B

during a bicycle probe. Dining acquisition, the data path was rapidly increasing He met

criterion levels after 6 trials. The generalization probe elicited a distance of 4 m. On Bike

C, two probes elicited distances o f 0 and 1 m respectively. During acquisition, he was

able to meet criterion levels following 6 trials. The generalization probe elicited a 1 m

distance. On Bike D, three probes elicited distances ranging from 0-1 m. Throughout the

first 77 intervention trials (two-thirds of all intervention trials), Jack demonstrated a

variable data path, (range of 0-7 m in distance). For the remaining 27 intervention trials,

the data path became extremely variable, (range of 2-12 m). During tins period, Jack met

7 9

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. i 1i i -— 1------L - h nr

y . (___

f t b m------^r i

* 1 1

t r

• • ------a ------a ------S-,------,------

Figure 10. Distance traveled in meters, by bike, tria

I 80 i

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. A

8

c

1 - - »

1 • • • • » " J ] /rm

stance traveled in meters, fay bike, trial, session (I) (Participant #4)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. criterion levels on Bike D. On Bike E, Jack experienced 11 probes, with a range of 0-5 m.

Daring acquisition, Jack met criterion levels on Bike E after 15 trials. The data path was

extremely variable, with a range of 3-12 m. During the maintenance and generalization

sessions, Jack demonstrated maintenance o fthe skill daring 90% o fthe trials, at 12 d e l He

demonstrated generalization o f the skill during 40% of the trials at a distance of 12 m.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 11 is a graphic display of the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by triaL The number or downstrokes per minute (dpm) on Bike A increased rapidly,

with a range of46-105. There was a decrease in dpm with the generalization probe,

which elicited a cadence o f 86 dpm. On Bike B, there was a rapid increase in the data

path, with a range of 0-105 dpm. The generalization probe was recorded at 68 dpm. On

Bike C, the date path was variable, with a range of 0-60 dpm. The generalization probe

was recorded at 45 dpm. On Bike D, the data path was variable, with a range of 0-80

dpm. The generalization probe was recorded at 60 dpm. On Bike E, the data path

variable with a range of 0-60 dpm. During Jack’s maintenance session, he demonstrated a

steady data path, with a range o f53-70 dpm. During generalization, the data path was

steady, with a range o f40-70 dpm.

Head Position

figure 12 is a graphic display of head position data based on the percentage of

intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position. On Bike A, the data path

was extremely variable, with a range o f33-100%. The generalization probe was recorded

at 75% uhp. On Bike B, the data path was extremely variable, with a range of 0-100%.

During the generalization probe, uhp was recorded at 100%. On Bike C, the data path

s i

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

Pew»tncc»*< ftH aimvTV i U ' S Figure 11. D ow nstrokes p er M inute, by bike and tria l (P articipant #4) articipant (P l tria and bike by inute, M er p nstrokes ow D 11. Figure 82 TPtALC* tuamuKS £ 0 B Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

% UPfi\iXT «£/*0 A * /7 * W IV TV r i ■ V | Figure 12. Percent U pright H ead P osition, by bike and tria l (P articipant #4) articipant (P l tria and bike by osition, P ead H pright U Percent 12. Figure 1 IV BBH^p ^ H B tB y W H W S M P T 85 QcoOTMrtoncw e ------"V " •

...... 0 E 6 c 1) A was extremely variable, with a range of 0-100%. The generalization probe was recorded

at 100% uhp. On Bike D, Jack demonstrated 100% uhp during a majority o f the trials (94

of 107), with a range o f 0-100%. The generalization probe was recorded at 100% uhp. On

Bike E, Jack demonstrated 100% uhp during a majority o f the trials (22 o f 28), with a

range o f 0-100%. During Jack's maintenance session, a stable data path o f 100% uhp in 9

out of 10 trials was observed. One trial was observed at 0%. During the generalization

session, uhp was stable at 100% in 13 o f IS trials. Two trials were observed at 50% uhp.

Summary

Jack successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in four

sessions (156 total trials). He maintained this skill 90% o f the time at a distance of 12 m.

He was able to generalize this skill 40% of the time at a distance of 12 m. He

demonstrated a mean of 60 dpm and 90% upright head position during maintenance.

During the generalization session, he demonstrated a mean o f 53 dpm and 87% upright

head position.

Participant 5

D istance and T rials

Jeremy initially rode the 20 in (50.8 cm) bikes. He easily met criterion on the 20

in Bike A, but experienced difficulty with the 20 in version o f Bike B. At this point, he

switched to the 16 in (40.6^ cm) bicycles and remained on the 16 in bicycles throu^iout

the study. Jeremy acquired the skill of riding a conventional bike in four sessions (195

total trials). Figure 13 is a graphic display of Jeremy's acquisition, maintenance, and

generalization of conventional cycling skills across Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials,

Jeremy rode: Bike A, a distance o f 12 m; Bike B, a distance o f I m, Bike C, a distance of

84

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. rr-i

I_____

Figure 13. Distance traveled in meters

85

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. c

D

o

.AWA f W e 7*vtS stance traveled in meters, by bike; trial, session (1) (Participant #5)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. I m; Bike D, a distance of I m and Bike E, a distance of I m. Jeremy immediately met

criterion levels (3 out o f5 trials at 12 m) on Bike A. He was able to generalize this skill

at a distance o f 12 m. On Bike B, he had one probe at a distance o f 1 m. After 32 trials

Jeremy experienced very limited success, (distance range of 0-5 m). At this point, a

decision was made to put Jeremy on the 16 in (40.64 cm) bike. After 13 trials on the

smaller bike, Jeremy met criterion levels. He was able to generalize this skill at a distance

of 12 m. On Bike C, Jeremy experienced two probes at 0 m (20 in bike) and I m (16 in

bike). On Bike C, Jeremy immediately met criterion IeveL He was able to generalize this

skill at a distance of 12 m. On Bike D, Jeremy had three probes. The first probe was on a

20 in bike at 1 m, and the next two probes were on the 16 in bike, both at I m. During the

first 35 trials (first third) of intervention, Jeremy demonstrated a variable data path in the

range of 0-5 m. Over the remaining 70 trials (last two-thirds) of intervention Jeremy

demonstrated an extremely variable data path, with a range of 0-12 m. During this time,

he did not reach the criterion level o f 3 out of 5 trials at 12 m. Rather, it was during a

probe of Bike E, that he met criterion levels on Bike E, effectively skipping Bike D.

Jeremy experience a total of 27 probes o f Bike E throughout the study. The first 23

probes were in the range of 1-8 m. In the final four consecutive probes, Jeremy rode Bike

E a distance o f 12 m each time. During the generalization pro be o f Bike E, he rode a

distance of 4 m. During the maintenance and generalization session, Jeremy

demonstrated maintenance of the skill during 40% of the time at 12 m. He demonstrated

generalization ofthe skill in the range o f 1-4 m.

86

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 14 is a graphic display of the number o f downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by triaL The number o f downstrokes per minute (dpm) on Bike A ranged from 33-46.

The generalization probe was recorded at 34 dpm for Bike A On Bike B, Jeremy

demonstrated a variable data path, with a range of 0-62. The generalization probe was

recorded at 66 dpm on Bike B. On Bike C, the data path was rapidly increasing, with a

range o f 0-73 dpm. The generalization probe was recorded at 66 dpm on Bike C. On Bike

D, Jeremy demonstrated an extremely variable data path, with a range of 0-180 dpm. On

Bike E, the data path was initially extremely variable and increasing, this was followed

by a variable data path. The range for Bike E was 0-150 dpm. The generalization probe

elicited a cadence of 150 dpm on Bike E. During the maintenance session, the data path

was extremely variable, with a range of60-140 dpm. During the generalization session,

the pattern was extremely variable, with a range o f60-120 dpm.

Head Position

Figure 15 is a graphic display of head position data, based on the percentage of

intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position. On Bike A, Jeremy

demonstrated an extremely variable data path (range o f56-100%). The generalization

probe elicited 67% uhp on Bike A On Bike B, the data path was extremely variable,

with a range o f 0-100%. The generalization probe elicited 71% uhp on Bike B. On Bike

C, the data path was extremely variable. The generalization probe elicited 60% uhp.

On Bike D, the data path was extremely variable across the first 72 trials (first two-thirds)

of acquisition, (range of 0-100%). The final 36 Bike D acquisition trials (last third) were

variable, with 32 trials at 100% and 4 trials at 0% uhp. On Bike E, the majority of the

87

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. **-1 * rucna * nnaMX

{ omiaumon £ MMffOlAKS o ^ B *

X £ ±

TKuttS

Figure 14. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #5)

88

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. #nucm *ntnua n 0NflN BaBWutfrmw A

1 UAM r r

c DC to

Figure 15. Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #5)

89

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. first half of the trials were at 0% uhp (15 of 17). During second half of trials were stable

at 100% for 11 consecutive trials. The generalization probe was elicited 100% uhp.

During the maintenance session, Jeremy demonstrated a variable data path, with a range

of 0-100%. During the generalization session, the data path was stable, with 13 out of 15

trials at 100%.

Summary

Jeremy successfully acquired the skill o f riding a conventional bicycle in four

sessions (195 total trials). He maintained this skill 29% o f the time at 12 m. He was able

to generalize this skill for a distance of 1-4 m_ Jeremy demonstrated a mean of 109 dpm

and 83% upright head position during maintenance. During the generalization session, he

demonstrated a mean of 96 dpm and 87% upright head position.

Participant 6

Distance and Trials

Bob rode the 20 in (50.8 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle in 6 sessions (162 total trials). Figure 16 is a graphic display o f

Bob’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization o f conventional cycling skills across

Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Bob rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B, a

distanceof I m; Bike C, a distance of I m; Bike D, a distance o f 0 m; Bike E, a distance

o f 0 m. Bob immediately met the criterion levels on Bike A. He was also able to

generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m on Bike A. During acquisition, Bob

demonstrated an extremely variable data path. He meet criterion levels after 26 trials on

Bike B. The generalization probe was recorded at 4 m. On Bike C, Bob had one probe at

0 m. During acquisition, Bob had a higher level o f responding, but with more variability,

90

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. L.

I k s M 1 i r( I I $ I j r -ei-

I_____

Figure 16. Distance tra\

91

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. *ruao>«ciDucx.

Puiai«tg>noii A MMNIDMtKl

A

S t. AT, c

4 } 0

l " “ , . ■ } • * • « .------1------r----- «k . . 1 v TKtats

I fi. Distance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( |) (Participant #6)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. (range o f 5-12 m). Bob met criterion levels after S trials. The generalization probe elicited

a distance of 4 m. On Bike D, Bob had two probes at 1 and 2 m_ During acquisition, Bob

demonstrated an extremely variable data path. He met criterion levels after 82 trials on

Bike D. The generalization probe elicited a distance of 1 m. Bob experienced 11 probes

on Bike E, with a range of 0-6 m. The data path during acquisition was extremely

variable, with a range of 4-12 m. Bob met criterion levels on Bike E after 15 trials. The

generalization probe on Bike E elicited a distance of I m. During the maintenance and

generalization session, Bob demonstrated maintenance of the skill during 20% of the

trials at 12 m. The data path during maintenance was extremely variable, with a range of

2-12 m. He demonstrated generalization o f the skill in the range o f 1-4 m after five trials.

He refused to complete the remaining generalization trials.

Downstrokes per minute

Figure 17 is a graphic display o f the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by trial. The number of downstrokes per minute (dpm) on Bike A ranged from 60-80.

The generalization probe was measured at 55 dpm. On Bike B, the data path was

extremely variable, with a range of 0-120 dpm. The generalization probe elicited a

cadence o f 60 dpm on Bike B. On Bike C, the data path was initially extremely variable,

with a range of 12-120 dpm. In the remaining acquisition trials, the data path was stable

and gradually increasing, with a range o f28-75 dpm. Bob demonstrated a decrease

during the generalization probe with 35 dpm. On Bike D, the data path was variable, with

a range o f 0-96 dpm. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 30 dpm. On Bike E,

the data path was variable, with a range of 0-90 dpm. The generalization probe elicited a

92

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. JB • RACm* fBDtAOC OfMSK

A munhmmck *

%2>

' A / ^ V V - ' a g

c

ju5

It D •

A u V pcnnn e TIKIS

Figure 17. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #6)

95

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. cadence o f 60 dpm. During the maintenance session, Bob demonstrated a variable data

path, (range o f60-90 dpm) on BikeE. During generalization, the data path was steady a t

60 dpm.

Head Position

Figure IS is a graphic demonstration of head position data based on the

percentage o f intervals that the participant's head was in an upright position. On Bike A,

Bob demonstrated an extremely variable data path, with a range o f20-60% uhp. The

generalization probe elicited 20% uhp. On Bike B, Bob demonstrated an initially low and

stable data path at 0% uhp, with one trial of 50%. This was followed by an extremely

variable pattern ranging from 0-100%. The final third ofBob’s trials on Bike B returned

to a low and stable data path o f 0% uhp. The generalization probe elicited 0% uhp.

Upright head position on Bike C was extremely variable. On Bike D, Bob initially

demonstrated a low and stable data path o f 0% uhp. This was followed by an extremely

variable data path (range o f 0-100%) uhp on Bike D. The generalization probe elicited

0% uhp. On Bike E, Bob initially demonstrated a low and stable data path of 0% uhp.

This was followed by an extremely variable data path ranging from 0-100%. The Bike E

generalization probe elicited 0% uhp. During the maintenance session, Bob demonstrated

a low and stable data path at 0% uhp. During generalization, upright head position was

extremely variable with a range o f 0-100%.

Summary

Bob successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in 6 sessions

(162 total trials). He maintained this skill 50% o f the time at 12 m. He was able to

94

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure IS. Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #6)

95

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. generalize this skill in the range o f 1-4 m. He demonstrated a mean o f 73 dpm and 0%

upright head position during maintenance. During the generalization session, he

demonstrated a mean o f 60 dpm and 20% upright head position.

Participant?

D istance and T rials

Pete rode the 20 in (50.8 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle in 6 sessions (273 total trials). Figure 19 is a graphic demonstration

ofPete’s acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of conventional cycling skills

across Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Pete rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B,

a distance of I m; Bike C, a distance o f I m; Bike D, a distance o f 0 m; and Bike E, a

distance of 0 m. Following the pretest, Pete immediately met criterion levels on Bike A.

He was also able to generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m. On Bike B, there was one

probe for a distance of I tn. During the first 33 trials of intervention, the data path was

variable, with a range of 0-3 m. It became evident, that Pete was not making any progress

on Bike B. At this point, a roller change was made on Bike B. The #7 roller was replaced

by the #5 roller. Pete immediately met criterion levels on Bike B with the #5 roller and

was able to generalize this skill at 4 m. The #7 roller was then reinserted onto Bike B.

Pete then demonstrated an extremely variable data path with a range o f 0-12 m. He met

criterion levels on Bike B after 62 trials with the #7 roller in place. He was able to

generalize this skill at a distance o f 4 m. Pete experienced 5 probes on Bike C (2-12 m).

He immediately met criterion levels on Bike C. He was able to generalize this skill at a

distance of 12 m on Bike C. Pete experienced 6 probes on Bike D (range of 1-5 m).

During acquisition, the data path was extremely variable. Pete met criterion levels after

9 6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. n A n ^ tA a /—^ A/S=

Figure 19. Distance traveled in m

97

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. £

H f w ,

■51- B t 1 ft1 C

• .'luvW W W i".

• . * • ' . • • •• • 1 r ------ft------1------*------1------*------L A / <

istance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( | ) (Participant #7)

t I . Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 116 trials on Bike D. He was able to generalize this skill at a distance of 4 m on Bike D.

On Bike E, Pete experienced 26 probes, with a range o f 0-12 m. Pete immediately met

criterion levels on Bike E. The generalization probe elicited a distance o f 1 m. Pete

demonstrated maintenance o f the skill 43 % o f the time at a distance o f 12 m. He

demonstrated generalization in the range of 1-8 m on Bike E.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 20 is a graphic display of the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by triaL The number of downstrokes per minute on Bike A, range from 57-84, with a

gradually increasing data path. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 67 dpm. On

Bike B, the data path was initially extremely variable, (range o f 0-180 dpm). At this

point, a roller change from #7 to #5 was made. On Bike B (roller #5) the data path was

gradually increasing (range o f66-94 dpm). Roller #7 was reinserted and the data path

was variable, (range of60-140 dpm). The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 110

dpm. On Bike C, the data path was variable and gradually increasing, (range of 30-100

dpm). The generalization probe elicited a cadence o f 100 dpm. On Bike D, the data path

was initially variable, (range o f 0-120 dpm) across the first 49 trials. The data path for the

remaining 40 trials was fairly stable, (range o f45-80 dpm). The generalization probe

elicited a cadence o f 60 dpm on Bike D. On Bike E, the data path was variable, (range of

0-90 dpm). The Bike E generalization probe elicited a cadence o f 60 dpm. During Pete’s

maintenance session, he demonstrated a variable data path, (range o f38-70 dpm). During

the generalization session, the datapath was variable, (range o f40-90 dpm).

98

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

fKM /♦>***-'rs Figure 20. D ow nstrokes per M inute, by b ike and tria l (Participant #7) (Participant l tria and ike b by inute, M per nstrokes ow D 20. Figure nruK-s gffllBBtfpHBB 99 Head Position

Figure 21 is a graphic display o f head position data, based on the percentage of

intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright head position. On Bike A, Pete

demonstrated a stable data path of 0% uhp. The generalization probe elicited 67% uhp.

On Bike B, the data path was initially low and extremely variable, (range of 0-100%).

The majority of trials (29 out o f 36), w oe at 0% uhp. The rollers were then changed on

Bike B, horn a #7 to a #5. At this point, the data path was rapidly decreasing, with a

range of 0-100% uhp. The #7 roller was reinserted, and the data path became extremely

variable, (range o f 0-100%). The generalization probe elicited 100% uhp. On Bike C, the

data path was initially steady at 0% uhp, followed by extreme variability, (range ofO-

100%). The generalization probe elicited 50% uhp. On Bike D, the data path was

extremely variable (range of 0-100%). The majority of data points (35 out of 46) were at

100% uhp, in the final half of trials on Bike D. The generalization probe elicited 100%

uhp. On Bike E, the data path was extremely variable, (range of 0-100%). The

generalization probe elicited 100% uhp. During Pete's maintenance session, the data path

was extremely variable (range o f 0-100%) uhp. During the generalization session, the

data path was extremely variable (range of 0-100%) uhp.

Summary

Pete successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in six sessions

(273 total trials). He maintained this skill 43% of the time at a distance o f 12 m. He was

able to generalize this skill with a range of 1-8 m in distance. He demonstrated a mean of

56 dpmand47% upright head position during maintenance During the generalization

session, he demonstrated a mean of 58 dpm and 64% upright head position.

too

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. m * n w n > m u x - Q______t 4. MAOrmiAMCK a-

l ...... l lot:Jr— :3l A Mffr^TW . 1

1 A I nmmiT 8 K S

Figure 21. Percent Uprigbt Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #7)

101

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Participants

Distance and Trials

Danny rode the 20 in bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a conventional

bicycle in 7 sessions (230 total trials). He did experience 5 trials on the 16 in version of

Bike B. Figure 22 is a graphic demonstration ofDanny’s acquisition, maintenance, and

generalization of conventional cycling skills across Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials,

Danny rode: Bike A, a distance of4m ; Bike B, a distance of 1 m; Bike C, a distance o f 0

m; Bike D, a distance of 1 m; and Bike E, a distance of 1 m. Following the pretest, Danny

met criterion levels on Bike A after a total of 5 trials. The generalization probe elicited a

distance of 1 m. During acquisition demonstrated a steady data path at 0 m. At this point,

the 16 in bicycle was utilized. After 5 trials on the 16 in Bike B, the data path remained

steady at 0 m. The 20 in bicycle was reintroduced and over the next 13 trials, Danny

demonstrated a steady data path (range o f 0-1 m). It was determined that in order for

Danny to experience some degree o f success, the rollers would need to be changed. At

this point, the #7 roller was replaced by the #5 roller. The data path became extremely

variable, and after 20 trials Danny met criterion levels on Bike B with the #5 roller. He

was able to generalize this skill at a distance of 12 m. The #7 roller was reinserted on

B ike B a t this point Danny again demonstrated an extremely variable data path. Criterion

levels were met after 63 trials on Bike B with the original #7 roller in place. The

generalization probe elicited a distance of I m. Danny experienced three bicycle probes

on Bike C, each probe was recorded at a distance o f 1 m. Danny immediately met

criterion levels on Bike C. The (fata path was extremely variable. The Bike C

102

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ' A

!■

Figure 22. Distance traveled in meters,

103

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. A

v w w \ f * 1 t- I <1 ---- * ------i t ___ w

n w w 1------.s

,WI • *• 1 Y*. ptlHi

i i stance traveled in meters, by bike, trial, session ( |) (Participant #8)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. generalization probe elicited a distance o f 8 m. Danny experience four probes on Bike D

(range o f 0-1 m in distance). Dining acquisition, the data path was initially extremely

variable at low levels, followed by extreme variability at higher levels. Danny met

criterion levels on Bike D after 68 trials. The Bike D generalization probe elicited a

distance o f I meter. Danny experienced 10 probes on Bike E, (range o f 12 m in distance).

During acquisition, data path was extremely variable across 10 trials. He met criterion

during this time. The Bike E generalization probe elicited a distance o f4 m. Danny

demonstrated maintenance of the skill during 45% o f the time at 12 m. He demonstrated

generalization o f the skill in a range of 0-8 m.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 23 is a graphic display of the number of downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by trial. The number of downstrokes per minute on Bike A, ranged from 5-47, with a

gradually increasing data path. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 47 dpm. On

Bike B, the data path was initially extremely variable (range of 0-120 dpm). After the

first 23 trials, roller #7 was replaced by roller #5. The data path with Bike B (roller #5)

was extremely variable (range of 0-120 dpm). After 21 trials, criterion levels were m et

Roller #5 was replaced by the original roller #7. The data path with Bike B (roller #7)

was extremely variable (range o f40-180 dpm). The Bike B generalization probe elicited

a cadence of 120 dpm. On Bike C, the data path was initially variable and gradually

increasing, this was followed by extreme variability. Downstrokes per minute were in the

range o f20-150 on Bike C. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 87 dpm.

On Bike D, the data path was variable (range of 0-90 dpm). The generalization probe

elicited a cadence o f 60 dpm. On Bike E, the (fata path was variable (range o f0-70 dpm).

104

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ts*»-

3 1*1 $ -

l^pjK r^rV^iBV 181 TKlALS.

Figure 23. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #8)

105

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The generalization probe elicited a cadence o f 30 dpm. During Danny7 s maintenance

session, he demonstrated a variable data path (range o f20-80 dpm). During

generalization, the data path was variable, with a range o f30-60 dpm.

Head Position

Figure 24 is a graphic display of head position data based on the percentage of

intervals that the participant head was in an upright position. On Bike A, Danny

demonstrated an extremely variable data path, (range o f 0-50%) uhp. The generalization

probe elicited 0% uhp. Danny performed a total o f six trials on Bike A during acquisition.

On Bike B, Danny initially demonstrated a low level o f upright head position. The first

20 out o f 23 trials were recorded at 0% uhp (range 0-100%). Following this, a ro ller was

change from #7 to #5 was made. During this period, the data path was extremely variable

(range of 0-100%) uhp. The roller was then switched back to a #7, and the data path

remained extremely variable (range o f 0-100%) uhp. Fifteen of the final 18 trials were

recorded at 100% uhp. The generalization probe elicited 100% uhp on Bike B. On Bike

C, the data path was initially low, with 11 out o f 12 trials at 0% uhp. This was followed

by an extremely variable data path, with the majority of trials at 100% uhp (51 out of 61).

The range on Bike D was 0-100% uhp. The generalization probe elicited 100% uhp. On

Bike E, the data path was extremely variable (range o f 0-100%). The majority o f trials

were at 100% uhp (15 out o f 22). The generalization probe elicited 0% uhp. During

Danny’s maintenance session, the data path was extremely variable (range o f 0-100%)

uhp. The majority (9 out o f 11) of trials were at 100%. During the generalization session,

the data path was extremely variable (range o f0-100%). The majority (7 out o f 10) of

trials were at 0% uhp.

106

hL Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. without prohibited reproduction Further owner. copyright the of permission with Reproduced

yt UfWAfT rt>Si npA) Figure 24. Percent U pright Head Position, by bik e and tria l (Participant #8) (Participant l tria and e bik by Position, Head pright U Percent 24. Figure A 107 t j m lw A O M O a g Q - Summary

D anny successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in seven

session (230 total trials). He maintained this skill 45% of the time at a distance of 12 del

He was able to generalize this skill with a range o f 0-8 m distance. He demonstrated a

mean of 57 dpm and 81% upright head position during maintenance. Dining the

generalization session, he demonstrated a mean of 51 dpm and 30% upright head

position.

Participant 9

D istance and T rials

Erin rode the 20 in bicycles and acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle

in seven sessions (381 total trials). Figure 25 is a graphic demonstration o f Erin’s

acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of conventional cycling skills across Bikes

A -E During the pretest trials, Erin rode: Bike A, a distance of 12 m; Bike B, a distance of

0 m; Bike C, a distance o f 3 m; Bike D, a distance of 0 m; Bike E, a distance of 0 m.

Following the pretest, Erin immediately met criterion levels on Bike A. The

generalization probe elicited a distance of 4 m. Erin experienced one probe on Bike B at

9 m. Erin immediately met criterion levels on Bike B. The generalization probe elicited a

/Kgtamv* o f 12 m. Erin experienced two probes on Bike C, both at a distance o f 12 m. On

Bike C, she immediately met criterion level. The generalization probe elicited a distance

of 12 m. Erin experienced three probes on Bike D, with a range of 0-1 m. During the first

168 tn>k (first two-thirds) o f intervention, the data path was variable, (range o f 0-7 m).

During the remaining 108 trials of intervention, the data path became increasingly

variable, (range o f 0-12 m in distance). The Bike D generalization probe elicited a

108

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure 25. Distance traveled in

* f*»

109

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 5

>

i IK. ' - • -** - *. - ' " •; ' ' * '• v l U L n e

mce trav eled in meters, by bike, trial, session (j) (Participant #9)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. distance of 1 m. Erin experienced 56 probes o f Bike E, (range o f 0-5 m). Daring

acquisition, the data path was extremely variable (range o f 0-12 m). After 29 trials, Erin

met criterion levels on Bike E. The generalization probe elicited a distance of 1 m.

D uring th e maintenance and generalization session, Erin demonstrated maintenance of

the skill 7% of the time at 12 m. She demonstrated generalization of the skill with a range

o f 1-4 m.

Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 26 is a graphic display of the number o f downstrokes per minute, by bike

and by trial. The number of downstrokes per minute (dpm) on Bike A ranged from 126-

150. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 120 dpm. On Bike B, there was a

rapid increase in downstrokes per minute (range o f60-147). The generalization probe

elicited a cadence of 115 dpm. On Bike C, there was a rapid increase in downstrokes per

minute (range of 51-140). The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 105 dpm. On

Bike D, the data path was variable (range o f 0-120 dpm). The generalization probe

elicited a cadence of 30 dpm. On Bike E, the data path was again variable (range of 0-120

dpm). The Bike E generalization probe elicited a cadence o f 60 dpm. During Erin’s

maintenance session, she demonstrated a variable data path (range o f60-120 dpm).

During generalization, the data path was extremely variable (range of 0-120 dpm).

Head Position

Figure 27 is a graphic demonstration of bead position data based on the

percentage of intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position. On Bike A,

Erin demonstrated variable data path (range of 0-33%) uhp. The generalization probe

elicited 50% uhpt On Bike B, Erin demonstrated a rapid increase in upright head position

n o

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. iO

S'

‘V A .A .’/ '

Figure 26. Downstrokes per Mini

111

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 6mm Q

rzoL'. kes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #9)

.,s. .1 '.a-* t’:• •TfikSffll Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ii Miy U: \ mu i i l L i m m r t m i

Figure 27. Percent Upright Head Posil

112

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1

mnmmmmn^pn

ight Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #9)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. (range of 0-100%). The generalization probe elicited 100% uhp. On Bike C, Erin initially

demonstrated a stable data path at 100% uhp. This was followed by a rapid decrease to

50% uhp. The generalization probe elicited 67% nhp. On Bike D, she demonstrated

extreme variability in upright head position during the first half o f acquisition. During the

majority of the trials in the first half (86 out of 110) upright head position was at 0%. The

range was from 0-100%. During the second half the majority of the trials (60 out of 112)

upright head position was at 100%. The data path was extremely variable. The Bike D

generalization probe elicited 0% uhp. On Bike E, the data path was mostly stable over

the first 32 trials, with 30 out of 32 trials at 0% uhp. The range was 0-100% During the

remaining trials on Bike E, the data path was extremely variable, however, the majority

o f the trials (35 out of 54), she exhibited 100% uhp (range was 0-100%). During Erin’s

maintenance session, she demonstrated a highly variable data path (range of 0-100%)

uhp. During the generalization session, upright head position was extremely variable

(range of 0-100%).

Summary

Erin successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in seven

sessions (381 total trials). She maintained the skill 7% o f the time at 12 m. While Erin did

not meet criterion for skill generalization, she did perform generalization trials (range of

1-4 m). Erin demonstrated a mean of 76 dpm and 67% uhp during maintenance During

generalization, she demonstrated a mean of 52 dpm and 47% uhp.

113

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Participant 10

Distance and Trials

Alien code the 20 in (50.8 cm) bicycles and acquired the skill o f riding a

conventional bicycle in seven sessions (227 total trials). Figure 28 is a graphic

demonstration o f Allen’s acquisition, maintenance^ and generalization of conventional

cycling skills across Bikes A-E. During the pretest trials, Allen rode: Bike A, a distance

o f 12 m; Bike B, a distance of I m; Bike C, a distance o f 3 m; Bike D, a distance o f 0 m;

and Bike E, a distance of 1 m. Following the pretest, Allen immediately met criterion on

Bike A_ The generalization probe on Bike A was recorded at 1 m. On Bike B, Allen

experienced one probe, for a distance o f I m. During acquisition, the data path was

extremely variable and gradually increasing. He met criterion on Bike B after 39 trials

(range of 0-12). The generalization probe elicited a distance of I m. On Bike C, two

probes elicited distances of 2 and 4 m. During acquisition, the data path was extremely

variable (range of 1-12 m). He met criterion after!9 trials. The generalization probe

elicited a distance o f 1 m. On Bike D, Allen experienced two probes at I and 2 m. During

acquisition, the data path was initially variable (range o f 2-5 m) this was followed by an

extremely variable data path (range o f 2-12 m). Allen met criterion after 66 trials on Bike

D. The generalization probe elicited a distance o f I m. On Bike E, Allen experienced 16

probes (range o f 0-10 m). During acquisition, the data path was extremely variable (range

o f 2-12 m). Allen met criterion after 38 intervention trials. While Allen did not meet

criterion levels for maintenance; he did exhibit partial maintenance with several trials a t

12 m. While Allen did not meet criterion for skill generalization, he (fid perform several

trials (range 1-4 m).

114

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Figure 28. Distance traveled in mete

IIS

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. »MlS

i iveled in meters, by bike, trial, session (I) (Participant #10)

Reproducedwith permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Downstrokes per Minute

Figure 29 is a graphic display of the number o fdownstrokes per minute, by bike

and by trial. The number of downstrokes per minute on Bike A, range from 30-80, with a

gradually increasing data path. The generalization probe elicited a cadence of 86 dpm. On

Bike B, the data path was initially extremely variable (range of 0-120 dpm), and was

followed by a gradually increasing and variable data path (range of 7S-lS7dpm). The

generalization probe elicited a cadence of 60 dpm. On Bike C, the data path was

extremely variable (range of26-120 dpm). The generalization probe elicited a cadence of

120 dpm. On Bike D, the data path was variable (range o f 0-90 dpm). The generalization

probe elicited a cadence o f 60 dpm. On Bike E, the data path was variable (range o f 0-

100 dpm). During Allen’s maintenance session, he demonstrated a somewhat variable

data path, (range o f60-80 dpm). During generalization, the data path was somewhat

variable (range o f40-60 dpm).

Head Position

Figure 30 is a graphic display of head position data based on the percentage of

intervals that the participant’s head was in an upright position. On Bike A, Allen

demonstrated an extremely variable data path (range of 0-80%) uhp. The generalization

probe elicited 0% uhp. Allen performed a total of six trials on Bike A during acquisition.

On Bike B, Allen demonstrated an extremely variable data path (range o f 0-100%) uhp.

The Bike B generalization probe elicited 100% upright head position. Allen performed a

total of 53 trials on Bike A during acquisition. On Bike C, Allen demonstrated an

extremely variable (fata path (range of 0-100%) uhp. The Bike C generalization probe

elicited 100% uhp. Allen performed a total o f 37 trials on Bike C during acquisition. On

116

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. *3 ^ _ _ Braoac ^ 8 QtWaKAUZATMH 0 0“ A

Figure 29. Downstrokes per Minute, by bike and trial (Participant #10)

117

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Bmai ■ QaMMUZAnON •I\ A. MMOSUMI — L n ------_ A

Figure 30. Percent Upright Head Position, by bike and trial (Participant #10)

its

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Bike D, Alien demonstrated an extremely variable data path (range of 0-100%) uhp.

Initially, the majority o fdata points (24 out o f 30) were recorded at 0% uhp. During the

remaining trials, the majority o f data points (22 out o f 38) were at 100% uhp. The Bike D

generalization probe elicited 100% uhp. Allen performed a total of 68 trials on Bike D

during acquisition. On Bike E, Allen demonstrated an extremely variable data path

(range of 0-100%) uhp. Initially, the majority ofdatapom ts( 13 out of 19) were at 0%

uhp. Following this, the majority o f data points (30 out o f44) were at 100% uhp. He

performed a total of 63 trials on Bike E. During Allen’s maintenance session, the data

path was initially extremely variable (range o f 0-100%) uhp. Following this, the data path

became more stable, with 11 out o f 12 trials recorded at 100% uhp. During the

generalization session, upright head position was initially extremely variable (range of 0-

100%) followed by a stable data path, with 7 out of 7 trials at 100% uhp.

Summary

Allen successfully acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle in 7 sessions

(227 total trials). He maintained this skill 47% of the time at 12 m distance. While Allen

did not meet criterion for skill generalization, he did perform several trials (range of 1-4

m). He demonstrated a mean of 73 dpm and 87% uhp during maintenance. During the

generalization session, he demonstrated a mean of 54 dpm and 60% uhp.

Overall Summary ofResuIts

Table 6 summarizes the results o f each participant, including number o f trials^

maintenance/generalization, bike size, downstrokes and head position. Table 7 is a

summary ofmean elapsed time and range in seconds by bike.

119

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Summary o f Results

P a rt T rials: Total M ain. Gen. S ize dpm uhp

A B C D E

t 4 5 6 7 7 29 YY 20 in 85 10%

2 4 5 6 21 14 50 N Y 16 in 51 67%

3 4 8 6 44 20 82 Y N 16 in 88 20%

4 4 8 9 108 27 156 Y Y 20 in 60 90%

5 4 47 6 no 28 195 N N 16 in 109 83%

6 4 31 16 85 26 162 N N 20 in 73 0%

7 4 101 9 127 32 273 YN 20 in 56 47%

8 6 106 15 76 21 230 Y N 20 in 57 81%

9 4 5 7 271 94 381 NN 20 in 76 67%

10 5 55 36 69 62 227 YN 20 in 73 87%

Table 6: Summary of results including participant numbers, trial per bike, total trials, ability to maintain and generalize skill, bike size, mean downstrokes per minute (dpm), and mean percentage o f upright head position (uhp) during maintenance

B ike A B ikeB B ikeC B ikeD B ikeE Mean 19.9 sec 8.3 sec 9.8 sec 3.5 sec 3.8 sec Range 8.8-30 sec 4.6-14.2 sec 6.4-14.7 sec 2 .l-6 .5 se c 2.Q-6.9 sec

Table 7: Mean elapsed time and range in seconds by bike

120

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Social Validity

A four-point Likert scale questionnaire was utilized fo r obtaining the opinions o f

participants’ parents, regarding the value of conventional cycling skills (Appendix J). The

questionnaire was completed fay 10 of the participants1 mothers. The scale was composed

o f the following responses: strongly agree, agree, disagree, ami strongly disagree:

Mine of the 10 respondents (90%) strongly agreed and one respondent (10%)

agreed with statement one, that a gradual introduction to bicycle-riding is less threatening

than the traditional approach of trial and error. Nine of the 10 respondents (90%) strongly

agreed and one respondent (10%) agreed with statement two, that their children looked

forward to attending cycling sessions. Eight o f the 10 respondents (80%) strongly agreed

and two respondents (20%) agreed with statement three, that the use o f adapted bicycles

made it possible for their children to ride a conventional bicycle. Six of the 10

respondents (60%) strongly agreed, two agreed (20%), one disagreed (10%) and one

strongly disagreed (10%) with statement four, that their children will bicycle ride with

friends and/or family. The respondent who strongly disagreed, went on to explain that her

child would not attempt to ride the conventional bicycle in an outdoor setting. Four of the

10 respondents (40%) strongly agreed, five respondents (50%) agreed and one respondent

(10%) disagreed with statement five, that the ability to ride a conventional bicycle would

increase their child1 s level o f physical activity. Eight o f 10 respondents (80%) strongly

agreed and two respondents (20%) agreed, with statement six, that the ability to ride a

conventional bicycle will increase their child’s feeling o f independence. Six o f 10

respondents (60%) strongly agreed and four respondents (40%) agreed, with statement

seven, that learning to ride a conventional bicycle had boosted their child’s self-esteem.

121

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Five of 10 (50%) respondents strongly agreed, four respondents (40%) agreed and one

respondent (10%) disagreed, with statement eight, that bicycle riding is a popular

recreational activity in my neighborhood Seven of 10 respondents (70%) strongly agreed

and three respondents (30%) agreed, with statement ten, that bicycle riding is a lifelong

sport/leisure activity.

In summary, results of the social validity questionnaire provide strong support for

the use o f adapted bicycles in developing conventional cycling skills. This method was

perceived as being less threatening than traditional approaches and children anticipated

participation in cycling sessions. Strong support was also evident relative to the social,

physical, and emotional affects o f learning how to ride a conventional bicycle. Finally,

there was strong agreement in terms of the popularity and perception ofbicycling as a

lifelong leisure activity.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTERS

DISCUSSION

This chapter provides 2 discussion of the results of using 2 series o f adapted

bicycles on the acquisition, maintenance, and generalization o f conventional cycling

skills by children with mild MR The discussion focuses on each of the five research

questions, perceived limitations of the study, implications for practice, and suggestions

for future research. Finally, a summary of the study is presented.

Research Questions

Research Question I: Does the use of adapted bicycles by individuals with mild MR

affect the acquisition o f conventional cycling skills?

Initial probes on Bike E (conventional bike), revealed that none o f the participants

in this study were capable of riding a conventional bicycle prior to intervention. The

mean distance traveled (hiring initials probes on the conventional bicycle was Im, (range

of 0-3 m). O f the 10 participants in this study, all 10 acquired the skill of riding a

conventional bicycle at criterion level (3 out of 5 consecutive trials at 12 m). These

results represent a 100% success in terms o f acquiring conventional cycling skills.

Despite the common knowledge that individuals with MR have difficulty in learning and

performing motor tasks, especially tasks that require a temporal response (Kail, 1992;

Newell, Wade, & Kelly, 1979), the children in this study experienced success in a short

period of time. In addition, findings support the notion that through a progressive change

in the task (adapted bicycles), the learner can shift to other modes (e.g. upright head 123

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. position, pedal rate) of functioning (Kelso, 1995). The movement system (learner)

spontaneously develops a preferred mode o f coordination to fit the new task (adapted

bicycle) constraints (Corbetta & Vereijken, 1999). This was evident as each child

progressed from bike to bike.

Although there have been no previous empirical studies on the acquisition of

conventional cycling skills, the data from this study supports anecdotal evidence from

several bicycle camps conducted in Wisconsin and California. Children with a variety of

disabilities participated in these bicycle camps and utilized similar adapted bicycles.

Overall, the one-week bicycle camps were successful in teaching children how to ride

conventional bicycles. However, this is the only empirical study to demonstrate that the

use of adapted bicycles has a positive affect on the acquisition o f conventional cycling

skills for children with mild MR. cl How many trials to criterion are requiredfor each bicycle during acquisition? On Bike A, the mean trials to criterion was 4.3 (range 4-6). All 10 participants

required no positive corrective or positive specific feedback on Bike A. And participants

experienced immediate success. This is due to the contour o f the #5 rollers, which make

Bike A extremely stable, even when the bike is motionless. On Bike B, the mean trials to

criterion was 37.1 (range 5-106). Two participants (Bob- #6 and Allen- #10) required a

more than average practice with feedback. Bob (Down syndrome) had difficulty

following directions and staying on task. Allen (mild autism/ADD) was very cooperative,

however he was generally not focused on the task at hand. He sang songs during most of

the trials. It is interesting to note that although most children with MR exhibit

developmental motor delays, these seem to be more related to cognitive factors such as

124

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. attention and comprehension rather than to physiological or motor deficits (Krebs, 2000).

One participant (Jeremy- #5) experienced initial difficulty on Bike B doe to an

inappropriate fit He was 48 in/121.92 cm tall and was attempting to leant on a 20 in/50.8

cm bicycle. Once this was corrected and Jeremy was placed on the 16 in./40.64 cm

version of Bike B, he quickly met criterion levels. The remaining two participants

(Danny- #8 and Pete- WT) experienced difficulty on Bike B, with the original #7 roller in

place. After a period of time, with very limited success, the #7 roller was exchanged for

the less contoured (more stable) #5 roller. Both participants experienced success with the

£5 roller and quickly met criterion levels. At this point, the #7 roller was reinserted and

both participants eventually met criterion levels on Bike B, with instructor intervention.

When Danny (developmental delay/cerebral palsy) initially began riding Bike B, he

would over-react to any tipping sensation by tightening the muscles in his legs so, that his

knees would touch. This reaction would cause the bike to tip. Once he began to

participate in the steering to (counter tipping), he began to relax and was less likely to

tighten his leg muscles. Postural deviations and cerebral palsy are common in children

with MR and can pose problems relative to body mechanics and balance (Krebs, 2000).

This was certainly true for Danny. However, taking these factors into consideration and

adapting the task by providing him with additional practice on less contoured rollers

enabled Danny to experience success and continue to improve. Paul (developmental

delay) also had some difficulties with Bike B. He tended to be extremely cautious and

reacted to the tipping sensation by taking his feet off of the pedals and placing them on

the floor. Again, once he began to participate in the steering, he experienced more

success. On Bike C, the mean trials to criterion was 11.6 (range 6-36). Bike C was

125

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. designed to move along with increased forward speed in relation, to pedal cadence. This

appeared to be extremely heipfid in assisting participants to maintain balance and meet

criterion levels quite rapidly on Bike C. Many o f the participants noticed and commented

on the increase in speed. On Bike D, the mean trials to criterion was 91.8 (range 7-271).

Only two participants (Joe- #1 and Billy- #2) immediately met criterion levels on Bike D.

Joe (Asperger syndrome) has a very high functioning form o f autism. He was extremely

attentive and followed directions explicitly. He was very inquisitive and asked a lot of

questions about the bikes. Billy (mild Autism) was a risk-taker. He was very relaxed on

the bikes from the start. The remaining participants required a additional practice and

feedback prior to meeting criterion levels. A few o f the participants seemed to have

difficulty with the weight of Bike D. They exhibited this by rocking back and forth as

they were riding, in an attempt to gain forward momentum. However, Bike D also proved

to be crucial, in that once criterion levels were finally met on this bike, the transition to

the conventional bike generally occurred over a short period of time. One participant

(Jeremy- #3) effectively skipped Bike D by meeting criterion levels on Bike E during one

of the probes. Jeremy was very small and lightweight On Bike E, the mean trials to

criterion was 33.1 (range 7-94). Seven of 10 participants immediately met criterion

levels on the conventional bicycle. The remaining three participants required very little

practice and feedback prior to meeting criterion levels on Bike E. The limited amount of

practice and feedback on Bikes A, B, C and E point to the effectiveness o f the adapted

bicycle design, in that the bicycles do most o f the teaching From a dynamic systems

perspective, the task constraints were amenable to the current abilities of the performer.

126

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The previous results point to the importance of activity modification. Individuals

with MR can participate in challenging sports skills when appropriate modifications are

made to enable successful experiences (Krebs, 2000). Modifications were made for the

task of cycling by initially reducing the speed of execution and the requirements in terms

of reaction time to tipping. Griffin & Keogh (1982) stress the importance of successful

experiences in movement situations, because it is these experiences that lead to

movement confidence. In this sense, it is essential to know the learner and to provide

more simple tasks at first, to provide sufficient practice for mastery and automaticity

(Bouffard, 1990). Following the provision o f less demanding tasks, more challenging

tasks can be presented. This can be related to the instructional approach o f task analysis,

which is commonly utilized with individuals with MR. Since many children with MR

have difficulty attending to multiple task cues as typically developing children, breaking

the skills down into sequential tasks is a valuable approach (Krebs, 2000). On Bike A. the

participant could concentrate on pedaling due to the fact that the bike was so stable. On

Bike B, stability became more of an issue, however, the bike was still geared to move

forward at a slow speed. On Bike C, the gearing was modified to increase forward speed

relative to cadence, however, the stability remained the same. On Bike D, the stability

was decreased and the forward speed remained the same. Finally, on Bike E. the

participant was ready to tackle the dynamics of a conventional bicycle. The initial

successful experiences on the first bikes in the series may have attributed to continued

success as the progression became more challenging

127

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. b. What is the overall direction, degree and extent o fvariability o f the trend as

defined by the relationship between distance and tried by bike during acquisition?

On Bike A, 9 of the 10 participants exhibited a trend that was high and stable,

with zero trend. Presumably, this is due to the contour of the #5 toilers, which provide

extreme stability, even when the bicycle is motionless. One participant (Danny-#8)

demonstrated a trend that was initially tow, but rapidly increased to a high and stable

level. Initially, Danny (Cerebral Palsy) exhibited hip and knee flexion with internal

rotation. This action was counterproductive to pedaling.

On Bike B, 3 of the 10 participants (#’s 1,2 & 9) exhibited a trend that was high

and stable, with a zero trend. One participant (#4) demonstrated a trend on Bike B, that

was initially low, but rapidly increased to a high and stable level Another participant (#3)

demonstrated a trend that was initially extremely variable. However, the variability

decreased and performance became more stable. One participant (#6) exhibited a trend

that was initially low and variable, followed by extreme variability and increasing levels.

One participant (#10) demonstrated extreme variability with a gradually increasing level

of performance. One participant (#5) initially exhibited a low and variable trend with the

20 in (50.8 cm) bicycle. After switching to the 16” (40.64 cm) bicycle, the trend rapidly

increased to a high and stable leveL Two of the participants (#Ts 7 & 8) required a roller

change on Bike B. Both of these participants started out with roller #7 on bike B. Both

exhibited trends that were low and variable with zero trend. Upon switching to roller #5

(less contour/more stable at 36.25 arc in) on Bike B, one o f the two participants (Pete-#7)

exhibited an immediate increase to a high and stable level. When the #7 (more

contour/less stable at 26.35 arc in) roller was reinserted, Pete demonstrated an extremely

128

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. variable trend that gradually increased to a high level. When Danny (#8) experienced a

roller change from #7 to #5, he exhibited and extremely variable trend that gradually

increased to a high level. When the #7 roller was reinserted, Danny exhibited a highly

variable trend that increased to a high level.

On Bike C, 6 o f the 10 participants (#’s 1, 2, X 5,7, & 9) immediately exhibited

trends that were high and stable with zero trend. The remaining four participants (#Ts

4,6,8, & 10) demonstrated trends that were extremely variable on Bike C.

On Bike D, one of the 10 participants (#1) exhibited a trend that was immediately

high and stable. Two participants (#’s 2 & 7) demonstrated trends that were extremely

variable on Bike D. The remaining seven participants (#Ts 3 ,4 ,5 ,6 ,8 ,9,& 10)

demonstrated trends that were initially low and variable, followed by gradually increasing

performance levels with extreme variability.

On Bike E, one o f the 10 participants (#1) exhibited a trend that was immediately

high and stable. One participant (#7) demonstrated a trend that was initially low,

followed by a rapid increase to a high level with stability. Seven o f 10 participants (#’s 2,

3 ,4 ,6 ,8 ,9,& 10) exhibited trends with extreme variability. One participant (#5) met

criterion level on Bike E during a probe. These probes were stable at a high level on

B ikeE .

These data concur with descriptions of change as described by dynamic systems

theory. For example, Corbetta and Vereijken (1999) describe behavior changes in terms

of stability and instability. They note that environmental and/or internal modifications

can bring about instability in a movement system. The increasing level o f difficulty from

bike to bike and roller to roller represented a change in the task that consequently

129

J I Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. perturbed the movement system towards instability. This was evident in the extreme

variability that was observed prior to reaching criterion levels on each bicycle. It is

daring these periods of instability that the movement system becomes more flexible; and

new modes of functioning are likely to emerge (Corbetta & Vereijken, 1999). Moreover,

Kelso et aL, (1993) recognized that unstable patterns are generally considered as

predictors of change.

Preferred modes of coordination (attractor states) that occur before and after

periods o f instability or transition are comparatively stable and resilient to change (Kelso

et at., 1993). Traditionally, motor skill acquisition has been described as the process of

reducing variability relative to outcome measures. However, in contrast to this Vereijken,

van Emmerik, Whiting and Newell (1991) describe the acquisition of coordination as the

search for optimal movement strategies. Increased variability prior to the acquisition of a

motor skill is supported by Thelen, Corbetta and Spencer (1996), in which the

development of reaching in infants was examined. During what is referred to as an

"active period”, infants displayed a high level o f variability and enhanced exploration in

speed parameters prior to discovering more stable and appropriate speeds for reaching

movements. This phenomenon is also evident in research studies involving skill

acquisition such as independent walking (Clark, Whitail & Phillips, 1988), a bimanual

rhythmic task (Schmidt, Treffiter, Shaw & Turvey, 1992), drawing movemnts (van

Emmerick. 1992) and stepping patterns (Vereijken & Thelen, 1997). Therefore, the

observation of an increase in the variability of performance is a positive indication that

the participant is in the process of exploring increased degrees o f freedom and

demonstrating selforganization prior to acquiring and refining new skills.

130

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Visual examination of the graphic display o f data supports the notion that unstable

patterns are predictors o f change (Kelso et aL, 1993). Extreme variability in the d ata

paths was followed by an increase in performance. The shift from bike to bike

represented a change in the task that consequently perturbed the movement system

towards instability.

c. What is the total number oftrials to criterion fo r conventional bicycle riding

during acquisition?

The mean trials to criterion for conventional cycling, across Bikes A, B, C, D and

E was 179 (range o f29-381). The majority of trials occured on Bike D with a mean of

91.8 trials (51%). Comparatively, the mean number of trials per bike were; Bike A, 4.3

(3%), Bike B, 37.1 (21%), Bike C, 11.6 (7%) and Bike E, 33.1 (18%). The numerous

trials on Bike D points to the need for either the creation of an additional bike between

Bikes C and D or an alteration in the design o f Bike D. This might assist in filling the gap

and providing for a smoother transition from bike to bike. In addition, less instructor

intervention would be required From a dynamic systems perspective, if the task

dynamics correspond to the existing intrinsic dynamics of a participant, learning the new

pattern will be easier (Zanzone & Kelso, 1991).

The wide range of total trials (29-381) could be attributed to the personality,

disability, and/or behavior of each participant Learner constraints such as attentiveness,

flexibility, cooperativeness, and/or motivation could easily attribute to the length o f time

required to Ieam a particular skill. Several o f the participants were easily distracted (#’s

2, 3 ,4 ,6 & 10). Again, cognitive factors such as attention and distractibility may be

influential in developmental motor delays evident in children with MR (Krebs, 2000).

131

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. One participant (#5) was uncooperative and (fid not follow directions well. When he

became frustrated, he would remove his helmet and sit down, refusing to participate. Four

participants (#’s 2 ,3 ,5 & 6) could be categorized as being risk-takers. For example, these

participants would continue with rapid pedaling despite the fact that they were about to

fall. The inclination to take risks was advantageous to learning. It allowed for longer

trials and more practice time. They would generally pedal faster and trust that the

investigator would catch them if they did fall. In terms o f disability, participant #8

(cerebral palsy) did struggle at times in large part to spasticity in his legs. Three of the

participants with autism (#’s 2,4 & 10) were not very focused and were easily distracted.

Ail three had a tendency to talk or sing to themselves as they rode the bikes. Perhaps the

extreme stability of the adapted bicycles made it possible for participants with an

inclination to distraction and inattention to succeed regardless o f their ability to focus on

the task. From a dynamic systems perspective, the constraints of the task (stability, slow

speed) made allowances for subsystems (such as attentiveness) to gradually fall into

place. Newell (1984) supports the notion that movement or skill development such as

bicycle riding develops from an interaction among the individual, the task and the

environment. However, once participants advanced to less stable variations of the bicycle

and the conventional bicycle, some degree of attention became necessary in order to

afford safe participation. It may be the case that participants with different ages and

behavior characteristics or disabilities might respond differently to the bicycle

intervention.

132

i

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Research Question 2: Is the riding o f a conventional bicycle by individuals with mild

mortal retardation maintained following the use o fadapted bicycles?

Six out of 10 participants (#’s 1,3 , 4 , 7 , 8 & 1 0 ) maintained the skill of

conventional cycling at criterion levels. The mean percentage for conventional cycling

maintenance was 47% for all participants (range 7-100%). For several participants, a

strong attractor well bad not yet developed. Attractors are considered to be preferred

modes of behavior (Thelen & Smith, 1994). The attractors for conventional cycling were

still in a state of instability. Therefore, they continued to exhibit a highly variable

performance. To increase the level o f maintenance by children with mild MR it may be

necessary to provide additional practice time on the conventional bicycle, once the skill

has been acquired This would result in more stable attractors relative to the skill of

cycling. In the present study, as soon as each participant met the 3 out of 5 criterion,

practice ceased until the generalization session. A study by Porretta and O’Brien (1991)

supports the use of additional practice time relative to increasing retention and transfer of

motor skills by children with mild MR. Presenting multiple contexts of related or similar

skills can also facilitate retention and transfer o f motor skills (Brooks & McCauley, 1984;

Liberty, Haring, White, & Billingsley, 1988). In fact, Salmoni, Schmidt, and W alter

(1984) suggest that the acquisition o f motor skills can only be accurately evaluated by

examining retention and transfer data.

133

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Research Question 3: Is the riding of a conventional bicycle by individuals with mild MR

generalized to a variation of the task?

In this study, response generalization was observed. Three o f the 10 participants

(#’s 1 ,2 & 4) were able to generalize the skill o f conventional cycling at criterion levels

(3 out of 5 consecutive trials at 12 m), through cones set up in a zig-zag pattern. It is

interesting to note, that the presence of the cones on the floor were particularly helpful to

participant #2 (autism). Although he generally had difficulty focusing on the task, the

cones seemed to give him a focal point and his performance greatly improved during

generalization trials. Two participants (#’s 7 & 8) were able to generalize the skill up to a

distance of 8 m. The remaining five participants (#Ts 3 ,5 , 6,9 & 10) were able to

generalize the skill up to a distance of 4 m. Perhaps the low level of generalization

performance was due to the limited space in the gym or the spacing of the cones

(environmental and task constraints). Moreover, additional practice time on the

conventional bicycle may have led to improved generalization. Additionally, more

generalization trials could have led to better performance. From a practical standpoint,

Bouffard (1990) suggests that retention and transfer of skills should be emphasized in any

learning situation. Although generalization probes occurred throughout the acquisition

phase, the minimal level of success with generalization in this study points to a need for

an increased emphasis in this area, particularly for children with mild MR. In addition to

response generalization, practice in natural outdoor environments should also be

considered. Bouffard (1990) suggests that individuals with MR be taught in contexts that

are as similar as possible to the target task. Consideration o f the natural environment in

which the task will eventually be performed should be emphasized (Brown & Campione,

134

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1986). This is particularly applicable to participant #5, whose mother reported that

following the study he refused to ride his bicycle outside. Rather, he wanted to revert to

riding his bicycle with training wheels around the neighborhood. This scenario supports

the observation by Salmoni, Schmidt, and Waiter (1984) highlighting the need for an

examination o f both retention and generalization o f motor skills as part o f a complete

evaluation of motor skill acquisition.

Research Question 4: Relative to select variables o f dyna m ic systems theory;

a. What percentage o f intervals did participants maintain an upright head

p o sitio n ?

On Bike A, the mean percentage for the upright head position was 35% (range 13-

85%) for all 10 participants. On Bike B, the mean percentage for the upright head

position was 40% (range 0-80%) for all 10 participants. On Bike C, the mean percentage

for the upright head position was 48% (range 16-83%) for all 10 participants. On Bike D,

the mean percentage for the upright head position was 49% (range 14-94%) for all 10

participants. On Bike E, the mean percentage for the upright head position was 49%

(range 14-81%) for all ten participants.

The interobserver reliability for percentage of upright head position was reported

in Table 4. Although and IOA o f 90% or better are generally considered adequate for

permanent products, this percentage is arbitrary and must be considered in light o f the

complexity and quantity of observations. Interobserver agreement with an average of

75% is considered to be adequate for data collected across intervals o f 5 seconds or less

(Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 1987). In this study, the intervals were two seconds in

duration.

135

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. In order to contextualize the percentage of upright head position and the number

of downstrokes per minute, refer to Table 7 fora summary o faverage elapsed tune per

trial by bike in addition to the range of elapsed time by bike. The average time per trial

on Bike A was 19.9 sec (range of 8.5-30.0), Bike B was 8.3 sec (range of 4.6-14.2), Bike

C was 9.8 sec (range o f 6.4-14.7), Bike D was 3.5 sec (range of 2 .1-6.5), and Bike E was

3.8 sec (range of2.0-6.9). Trials on Bike A lasted the longest and trials on Bike D were

the shortest in duration. In general, there was a decrease in elapsed time from Bike A to

Bike E, with slight variability. Therefore, an upright head percentage of0% or 100% on

Bike A would be more significant than an upright head percentage of 100% on Bike E.

The same logic applies to do wnstrokes per minute on Bikes A-E.

b. What was the member o f downstrokes per minute durmg acquisition?

This data is presented based on two different bike sizes. Due to the different

circumference of the tires (16 in and 2 0 in), dpm must be compared with the same bike

size. On the 16 in (40.64 cm) Bike A, three participants exhibited a mean of 8 8 dpm

(range 77-99). On the 20 in (50.8 cm) Bike A, seven participants exhibited a mean o f 81

dpm (range 23-134). On the 16 in (40.64 cm) Bike B, three participants exhibited a mean

of76 dpm (range 41-100). On the 20 in (50.8 cm) Bike B, seven participants exhibited a

mean of 92 dpm (range 42-141). On the 16 in (40.64 cm) Bike C, three participants

exhibited a mean of 115 dpm (range 62-92). On the 20 in (50.8 cm) Bike C, seven

participants exhibited a mean o f 76 dpm (37-120). On the 16 in (40.64 cm) Bike O, three

participants exhibited a mean o f 71 dpm (range 44-103). On the 20 in (50.8 cm) Bike D,

seven participants exhibited a mean of 54 dpm (range 40-73). On the 16 in (40.64 cm)

Bike E, three participants exhibited a mean of 82 dpm (range 58-109). On the 20 in (50.8

136

j

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. cm) Bike E, seven participants exhibited a mean of 58 dpm (43-71). Due to the difference

in tire circumference between the 16 in and 2 0 in bicycle tires, the expectation would be

for a slightly higher rate of dpm on the 16 m bicycle. This was true for all bicycles in the

series with the exception of Bike B. A reasonable explanation for this would be the

extreme dpm scores for participant # 1. The ideal dpm fora 16 in and 20 in conventional

bicycles respectively was 82 and 58. This estimation is based on the average dpm across

all ten participants during maintenance sessions. However, the environmental constraints

o f the gymnasium should be taken into consideration. In an outdoor setting or on an

uneven surface, the dpm data might be different Consideration should be given to the

amount o f force applied to the pedal for participants pedaling in an aggressive/passive

manner or on a surface with an incline/decline. In addition, in a more natural setting,

there would likely be more space to travel longer distances such as on a bike path or large

parking lo t All of the trials in the gymnasium were limited to a distance of 12 m. In

addition, due to the fact that participants were in the early stages of learning a new skill,

many o f the trials were very short in length, perhaps only one or two meters per trial. The

combination o f these circumstances (constraints) made it impractical for participants to

settle into a comfortable cadence over an extended period o f time. From a theoretical

standpoint, the environmental constraints o f an indoor setting could have an effect of the

pedal rate. As Newell (1984) suggests, all elements of the environment, the task, and the

individual must be taken into consideration when interpreting these data.

From a dynamic systems perspective, systems undergoing change are

characterized as being complex, coordinated and somewhat self-organizing (Clark &

Whitall, 1989). Through this process order emerges through the interaction o f the many

137

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. components that make op such a complex system. In a dynamic systems analysis, it is

important to identify variables that represent the essence of a particular behavior pattern.

Kelso and Schoner (1988) refer to these as collective variables. The collective bicycle

tiding variables that were examined in this study were head position and dOwnstrokes per

minute. These are just two examples of the many potential variables that could be

considered in the developmental landscape of bicycle tiding. Over time and horn bike to

bike the percentage of upright head position increased from 35-49%. Perhaps the reason

that participants did not demonstrate a significant increase in uhp could be that they were

attending to other variables, such as steering participation or pedal cadence. Another

explanation might involve eye position. It is possible that the head could have be down

while the eyes were looking forward These reasons are in alignment with (Jobling, 1999:

Sugden & Keogh, 1990) who suggest that children with MR have enduring problems

with postural control and balance. Difficulties with postural control can have direct

relevance to head position. Overall, there was evidence of extreme variability,

particularly on Bikes A, B and C. However, the percentage of upright head position had a

tendency to increase as participaiits reached criterion levels on Bikes D and E.

In terms o f cadence, as measured by downstrokes per minute, participants had a

tendency to increase the number o f downstrokes per minute, bike by bike, and then level

out to a sufficient rate to maintain balance o f the bicycle. These findings are supported by

Thelen (1994) in her study involving interlimb coordination of infants. When participants

are presented with a novel task, intrinsic dynamics can be modified The preferred mode

13S

I Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. of coordination at the start o fthe teaming process for cycling acquisition was a tendency

to lo o k down and to pedal slowly. As the intrinsic dynamics began to change, participants

becam e m ore proficient w ith cycling skills-

Research Question 5: What is the social value o f learning to ride a conventional bicycle

through the use o f adapted bicycles?

The results obtained from the social validity questionnaire support the notion that

a gradual introduction to bicycle-riding is less threatening than the traditional approach of

trial and error (100%). Furthermore, parents indicated that children looked forward to

attending cycling sessions for this study (100%). Results illustrate that the use of adapted

bicycles made it possible for each child to ride a conventional bicycie( 100% agreement).

Overall, results support the notion that children will ride bicycles with friends and/or

family once they learn to ride.

However, one parent disagreed and another parent strongly disagreed that their

children would ride with others. The parent who strongly disagreed, explained that

although her son teamed how to ride a conventional bicycle in an indoor setting, he

refused to ride his bike in an outdoor setting near his home. This highlights the

importance of skill generalization. Porretta (1988) explains that the lack of skill

generalization may be the most pervasive and difficult educational problem for

individuals with MR. Teaching skills in the environment where the skills will ultimately

be used is preferable to artificial environments (Krebs, 2000). The negative response by

the other parent may be due to the foct that the participant has only one 4 year old

sibling. Other possible explanations would be that the participant does not have many

neighborhood friends and/or his parents do not ride bicycles themselves. Social value

139

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. results support the idea that the ability to ride a conventional bicycle would increase the

child’s physical activity level (9 out of 10). One parent disagreed with this statement.

This was the same parent, whose child refused to tide his bicycle outside. Furthermore,

there is strong support relative to parental perceptions for the notion that learning to ride

a bicycle increases self-esteem and a feeling o f independence (100%). Studies involving

psychosocial outcomes of Special Olympic athletes report increased self-concept (Wright

& Cowden, 1986) and increased levels of perceived competence and perceived social

acceptance (Gibbons & Busbakra, 1989). Results also illustrate that bicycling is a popular

activity inm ost neighborhoods (9 out of 10). Finally, all respondents ( 10) agreed that

bicycle tid in g is a lifelong sport/leisure a c tiv ity .Krebs (2000) concurs, stating that

cycling is a useful skill and can be considered as a lifelong leisure pursuit

Limitations

1. This research study was limited to two female and eight male participants (7-11 years

of age) with mild MR.

2. The setting for this study was inside a gymnasium on the campus o f a large mid-west

university. The gym was small (18.7 m by 11.2 m). Only 5.2 meters o f free space was

available past the finish line. It is possible that the outcomes might have differed if the

study took place in a larger gymnasium, or an outdoor setting. In addition, the gym had a

tendency to be warm. As a result, room temperature may have affected the performance

of participants.

3. Sessions for this study were conducted in the late afternoon and evening, (4-8 pm).

Some participants came to the study directly from school and prior to eating dinner.

Other participants came following mealtime. As a result, the time o f day may have

140

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. affected performance. In addition, the study took place in the winter months between

November and January. As a result, the cold outdoor temperatures may have affected

motivation, due to the fact that this newly acquired skill could not be implemented in a

natural setting.

4. Exposure to training wheels or negative experiences on bicycles without training

wheels could have possibly impacted participants’ performance. Children who were

accustomed to riding with training wheels may have developed a tendency towards

increased upper-body articulation. This can be described as leaning the upper torso to the

right or left, in an attempt to balance the bicycle. This habit is contraindicatory to

conventional bicycle riding. Those who have experienced falling while attempting to ride

a conventional bicycle may have been fearful. In other instances, participants who

experienced repeated failure, may have had low expectations.

5. Distance traveled, head position, and downstrokes per minute were the dependent

variables for this study. Fear of contacting the wail may have prevented some participants

from crossing the 12 m line. The same may be true for downstrokes per minute, in that

the pedal rate may have been altered due to the proximity of the wall at the end of the

gymnasium. Head position may have been affected due to various distractions in the

environment Although participants were encourage to look at the Curious George banner

at the end of the gym, many participants were visually attracted to the meter markers

placed along the sidelines. Several participants counted the markers as they rode the

bikes. Likewise, the rolling video-camera along the opposite sideline was a focal point

for certain individuals. Although family members were not permitted inside the

gymnasium, they were often just outside the doorway. At times, this caused participants

141

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. to look backward as they were riding the bicycle: Perhaps more stringent guidelines for

individuals accompanying participants would have affected performance.

6. Braking and independently starting are skills that were outside the scope o f this study.

However, once each child completed the study, the investigator spent 10-15 minutes

working with the child on braking and independent starts. The parent was asked to come

into the gym at this time, in order to receive instructions relative to practicing braking and

independent starts with the child

7. The data collected relative to upright head position was based only on the position of

the head Consideration was not given to eye position. In many instances, the head could

have been down, when the eyes were looking forward or vice-versa.

8. Due to the multiple probe design, there may have been a practice effect for participants

who received multiple probes on the conventional bike (Bike E).

Implications for Practice

This study shows, that children with mild MR can benefit from an adapted bicycle

intervention. Ail 10 participants acquired the skill of riding a conventional bicycle. In

terms of skill maintenance, 6 o u t o f 10 participants were able to maintain this level of

performance. Four of the 10 participants revealed a slight decrease in performance during

the maintenance session following acquisition. Three out o f 10 participants were able

generalize the skill o f conventional bicycle riding at criterion levels ( 12 m , 3 out o f 5

trials). Furthermore, two participants were able to generalize this skill up to 8 m in

distance T he fiv eremaining participants had limited success w ith response

generalization, in that they were only able to generalize this skill up to 4 m. Based on

these findings, it is important for teachers and coaches to provide ample practice time and

142

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. to emphasize maintenance and generalization of cycling skills in teaching children with

mild MR. With regard to generalization, it is recommended that natural environments be

utilized. Although the adapted bicycles axe designed for indoor use, after a child has

progressed to riding a conventional bicycle, it is important to provide opportunities to

practice in an outdoor environment Initially, it is recommended that children practice in

an outdoor setting such as a parking lot, with free space and limited obstacles. Following

this, learners can advance to bicycle trails and other venues that require more refined

movements and reactions.

It has been demonstrated that it is practical for physical educators and coaches to

utilize adapted bicycles in their respective cycling programs. From a dynamic systems

perspective, the use of a series of adapted bicycles represents a progressive task change.

The presentation of a gradual continuum of more challenging bikes encouraged a shift by

participants to more advanced modes o f functioning. Moreover, by initially providing the

participants an appropriate task, participants were provided with successful experiences

from the start In addition to increasing movement competence, participants were

motivated to continue participation and meet new challenges as they progressed from

bike to bike.

From a practical standpoint, although the adapted bicycles are not currently

available to the general public, it may be possible to obtain them in the foreseeable

future. This purchase would be for adapted physical education teachers and therapeutic

recreation specialists who teach a large number o f children. In this way, hundreds and

thousands of children could learn to ride conventional bicycles and the cost for parents

would be mhrimal.

143

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Relative to bicycle design, it is important to have access to various roller

contours. Each child is different and the task must be adapted to fit the needs o feach

individual. For the participants in this study, the #5 and #7 rollers were sufficient for

adapting the task to individual needs. In addition, as the design becomes more

streamlined in terms o f roller exchange, it would be less labor intensive if a quick release

design were implemented as opposed to the current method of roller exchange. This

would also decrease the amount of time that children would have to wait while

transitioning between bicycles and/or rollers. The addition o f Bike C to the series was

essential in bridging the gap between Bikes B and D. The increased forward motion of

Bike C provided an invaluable experience prior to transitioning onto Bike D. However,

the majority of trials and time continued to be concentrated on Bike D, the final bicycle

in the adapted series. Perhaps a variation o f Bike D could be effectively utilized to make

the transition even smoother. In terms o f bike size, both the 16 in (40.64 cm) and 20 in

(50.8 cm) series were used in this study. Participants who were 49 in (124.46 cm) tall or

less benefited from the use of the 16 in (40.64 cm) series, while participants 52 in (132.08

cm) and taller benefited from the use o f the 20 in (50.8 cm) series. There was one

exception. Participant # 8 (height 48 in/121.92 cm) had more success on the 2 0 in (50.8

cm) bicycles. He was heavier and exhibited spasticity in his legs. The majority (7 out of

10) of children learned on the 20 in (50.8 cm) bicycle series. Therefore it is

recommended that both sizes are made available for optimum use and success

144

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Future Research

L Replicate the study with an additional number o f participants o f other ages and

disabilities.

2 . Compare the use of the adapted bicycle series with other methods such as with existing

patented devices/methods, or more traditional methods o f trial and error.

3. Increase the number o f trials to criterion on the conventional bicycle, in order for

participants to more fully acquire the skill o f bicycle riding.

4. Utilize markers and biomechanical software to obtain precise data in re^rd to upper-

body articulation and steering participation, head position and cadence.

3. Examine the effects of an adapted bicycle intervention on generalization to an outdoor

setting.

6. Conduct studies examining variations of conventional bicycling skills such as the use

o f geared bikes, riding with one hand, retrieving a water bottle, etc.

Summary

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of using a series o f adapted

bicycles and feedback on the acquisition, maintenance^ and generalization of

conventional cycling skiUs by children with mild MR. An applied behavioral analysis

multiple probe design was utilized The adapted bicycle series consisted of four bikes;

Bike A, Bike B, Bike C, and Bike D. The goal was to gradually progress to riding Bike E

(conventional bike).

145

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. In the first session, a probe was taken with each bike to determine each

participant's current a b ility level. Prior to each probe, the instructor to ld the participant to

ride the bicycle as far as possible to the end o fthe gym. Next was the acquisition phase

which included feedback. Feedback was in the form o f positive, positive/corrective or

positive/specific feedback, relative to bead position, pedal rate and steering participation.

Criterion for each bicycle was set at 3 out of 5 consecutive trials at a distance of 12 m.

Once criterion was met on a particular bike, a generalization probe was administered.

Upon advancement, another probe was taken on each bicycle in the series.

Generalization was tested in the form of response generalization. Cones were

placed by pairs in a zig-zag pattern across the gymnasium The participant was instructed

to ride the bicycle through the cones to the end of the gym. The instructor provided a

demonstration of the correct pathway. No feedback was given during generalization

trials.

Following acquisition of cycling skills on Bike E (conventional bicycle) the

session ended. Maintenance and generalization phases were then initiated respectively.

During the maintenance phase, the participant was instructed to ride the bike as far as

possible. Each participant received a maximum of 15 maintenance trials. During the

generalization phase, the cones were set up and the participant was instructed to ride

through the cones in a weaving pattern as far as possible. Each participant received a

maximum of 15 trials.

Acquisition results indicate that all ten participants acquired the skin of riding a

conventional bicycle at criterion level (3 out of 5 consecutive trials at 12 m). These

results represent a 100% success rate in terms o f acquisition of conventional cycling

146

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. skills. The majority of participants ( 6 oat o f 10) demonstrated maintenance of

conventional cycling skills at criterion levels. Across ail ten participants, maintenance

was evident an average o f47% o f the time. Three often participants were able to

generalize this skill at criterion levels. The remaining participants demonstrated lower

levels of generalization.

Visual examination ofthe graphic display of data supports the notion o f dynamic

systems, that unstable patterns are predictors o f change (Kelso et aL, 1993). Extreme

variability in the data paths was followed by an increase in performance. The shift from

bike to bike represented a change in the task that consequently perturbed the movement

system towards instability. Additionally, the data collected relative to head position and

pedal rate revealed that the preferred mode of cycling at the beginning of the learning

process was capable of being altered. Initially, upright head position was extremely

variable. As upright head position became more consistent, cycling proficiency increased.

As upright head position became more consistent, cycling proficiency increased.

Likewise, participants initially proceeded with caution in terms of pedal rate. As the

number o f downstrokes per minute increased and stabilized, cycling proficiency

increased. This demonstrates that as instrumental variables fall into place across the

developmental landscape, performance is affected in a positive manner.

147

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UST OF REFERENCES

Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory o f motor learning Journal o f Motor B eh a vio r, J, 111-150.

American Association on Mental Retardation. (1992). M ental retardation: Definition, classification, and systems ofsupports (9th ed.). Washington DC: A uthor.

AnsheL, M. H. & Singer, R. N. (1980). Effect o f learner strategies with modular versus traditional instruction on motor skill learning and retention. Research Quarterly For Exercise and Sport, 51, 451-462.

Baddeley, A. D. & Longman, D. J. A. (1978). The influence o f length and frequency o f training session on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21, 627-635.

Beek. P. J. & van Santvoord, A. A. M. (1992). learning the cascade juggle: A dynamical systems analysis. Journal o f Motor Behavior, 2 4 , 85-94.

Belm ont, J. M , & Mitchell, D. W. (1987). The general strategies hypothesis as applied to cognitive theory in mental retardation. Intelligence, 11, 91-105.

Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation ofmovements, Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Bicycling Magazine (Eds.). (1991). Teaching your child to ride, [pamphlet].

Bilodeau, E. A., Bilodeau, L M , & Schumsky, D. A. (1959). Some effects of introducing and withdrawing knowledge of results early and late in practice. J o u rn a l o f Experimental Psychology, 5 8 , 142-144.

Book, W. F. (1908). The psychology o f skill, with special reference to its acquisition in typewriting. Missoula: University ofMontana.

Bouffard, M. (1990). Movement problem solution by educable mentally handicapped individuals. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 7,183-197.

Bouffard, M. & Wall, A. E. (1991). Knowledge, derision making, and performance in table tennis by educable mentally handicapped adolescents. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 8, 57-90.

148

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Bril, B. & Breniere, Y. (1992). Postural requirements and progression velocity in young w alkers. Journal o f Motor Behavior : 2 4 ,105-116.

Brooks, PJrL & McCauley, C. (1984). Cognitive research in mental retardation. A m erica n Journal o f Mental Deficiency, 88,479-486.

Brown, R. W. (1928). A comparison of the whole, part, and combination methods for teaming piano music. Journal o f Experimental Psychology, I /, 235-247.

Brown, A. L. & Campione, J. C. (1986). Training for transfer Guidelines for promoting flexible use of trained skills. In M.G. Wade (Ed.), Motor skill acquisition o f the mentally handicapped: Issues in research and training (pp.257-271. New York: North Holland.

Bruininks, R. H. (1974). Physical and motor development of retarded persons. InN.R. Ellis (Ed.), International review o f research in mental retardation, 7. New York: Academic Press.

Bryan, W., & Harter, N. (1897). Studies in the physiology and psychology o f telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 4 , 27-53.

Bryan, W., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6 , 115-124.

CaidwelL, G. E., & Clark, J. E. (1990). The measurement and evaluation of skill within the dynamical system perspective. In J.E. Clark & JTL Humphrey (Eds.), Advances in motor development research. New York: AMS Press.

Chumlea, W. C. & Cronk, C. E. (1982). Overweight among children with trisomy 21. Journal o f Mental Deficiency Research, 25,275-279.

Clark, J. E. & Phillips, S. J. (1987). The step cycle organization of infant walkers. Joum al o f Motor Behavior, 19,421-433.

Clark J. E. & Whithall, J. (1989). What is motor development: The lessons o f history. Q u est, 41, 183-202.

Clark, J. E., Whitall, J. & Phillips, S. J. (1988). Human interhmb coordination: The first 6 months of independent walking Developmental Psychobiology, 21, 445-456.

Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E , Heward, W. L. (1987). Applied behavior analysis, Columbus, OH: Merrill Publishing Company.

Corbetta, D. & Vereijken, B. (1999). Understanding development and learning o f motor coordination in sport The contribution o fdynamic systems theory. International Journal o f Sport Psychology, 30, 507-530. 149

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Cudmore, P. (1972). Velocipede. Patent No. 246,727.

Davis, D. (1991). Treadm ill-elicited stepping in premature infants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University.

Davis, W. E. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1982). Analysis of “invariant characteristics” in the motor control o f down’s syndrome and normal subjects. Journal ofM otor Behavior. 14, 194-212.

Del Rey, P., & Stewart, D. (1989). Organizing input for mentally retarded subjects to enhance memory and transfer. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 6247-2S4. ,

DiRocco, P. J., Clark, J. E., & Phillips, S. J. (1987). Jumping coordination patterns of mildly mentally reta rd ed children. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 4178- , 191.

Distenfano, E. A. & Brunt, D. (1982). Mentally retarded and normal children’s performance on gross motor reaction- and movement-time tasks with varying degrees o f uncertainty o f movement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 5 5 ,1235-1238.

Eason, B. L. & Surburg, P. R. (1993). Effects of midline crossing on reaction time and movement time with adolescents classified as mildly mentally retarded. A d a p ted Physical Activity Quarterly, 10,269-280.

Easton, T. A. (1972). On the normal use o f reflexes. American Scientist, 60. 591-599.

Edwards, J. M., Elliot, D., & Lee, T. D. (1986). Contextual interference effects during skill acquisition and transfer in Down’s syndrome adolescents. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 3250-258. ,

Egley, L. P. (1994). Adjustable training wheel apparatus. Patent No. 5,352,403.

Eichstaedt, C. B., Wang, P.Y., Polacek, J. J., & Dohrmann, P. F. ( 1991). Physicalfitness and motor skill levels o f individuals with mental retardationz Mild, moderate, and individuals with Down syndrome, ages 6-21. Normal: Illinois State University Printing Services.

Epstein, M. L. (1980). The relationship o f mental imagery and mental rehearsal to performance o f a motor task. Journal o f Sport Psychology. 2 , 211-220.

Gallagher, J. D. & Thomas, J. R. (1986). Developmental effects o f grouping and recoding on learning a movement series. Research Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport. 57, 117-127.

Gallahue, D. L. (2000). Motor Development fit J.P. Winnick (Ed.), Adapted Physical A c tiv ity a n d S p o rt (pp.265-279). Champaign. EL: Human Kinetics. 150

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Geller, R. (1991). Guidance apparatus for bicycle training. Patent No. 5,154,096.

Gibbons, S. L.,& Bushakra, F. B. (1989). Effects o f Special Olympics participation on the perceived competence and social acceptance of mentally retarded children. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 640-51. ,

Goldfield, E. (1989). Transition from rocking to crawling; Postural constraints on infant m ovem ent Developmental Psychology, 25, 913-919.

Green, P. H. (1972). Problems of organization of motor systems. In R-Rosen & F. M. Snell (Eds.), Progress in theoretical biology (Vol 2). New York: Academic Press.

Griffin, N. S., & Keogh, J. F. (1982). A model for movement confidence, hi J. A. S. Kelso & J. E. Clark (Eds.), The development o f movement control and coordination (pp.213-236). New York: Wiley.

Haken, H., Kelso, J.A.S., & Bunz. H. (1985). A theoretical model o f phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347-356.

Harrison, J. A. (1995). Bicycle guidance device. Patent No. 5 ,4 0 7 ,2 7 ?

Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model Human Development, 21,34-64.

Henrichs, S. (1996). The balance harness, [pamphlet]. Patent No. 5,540,188. Dayton, OH: M.A.S.S. Inc.

Henry, F. ML (I960). Increased response latency for complicated movements and a “memoty-drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly, 31,448-458.

Heriza, C. (1988). Comparison of leg movements in pre-term infants with healthy full- term infants. Physical Therapy, 6 8 ,1687-1693.

Holland, B. V. (1987). Fundamental motor skill performance o f nonhandicapped and educable mentally impaired students. Education and Training in M ental Retardation, 2 2197-204. ,

Janz, K. F. (1991). Physical activity levels and profiles and their relation to physical fitness in children and adolescents: The muscat ine study. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, (10), 4803. (Publication No. AAT 9107359)

Jeka, J. J., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). The dynamic pattern approach to coordinated behavionA tutorial review. In S.A. Wallace (Ed.), Perspectives on the coordination o f movement (pp.3-45). Amsterdam: North-Hofland.

151

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Jobimg, A. (1998). Motor development in school-aged children with Down syndrome: a longitudinal perspective. International Journal o f disability. Development and Education, 45,283-293.

Jobling, A. (1999). Attainment of motor proficiency in school-aged children with (town syndrom e. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16,344-361.

Joules, R. (1996). Pedal magic [On-line]. Available: http://home.earthlink.net/ 130ules/index html.

Kail, R. (1992). General slowing o f information-processing by persons with mental retardation. American Journal on M ental Retardation, 97,333-341.

Kalmus, A. L. (1994). Bicycle riding training device. Patent No. 5,303,944.

Kamm, K_, Thelen, E., & Jensen, J. L. (1990). A dynamical systems approach to motor Development Physical Therapy, 70 (12), 763-774.

Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns; The self-organization o f brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: A Bradford Book, MTF Press.

Kelso, J. A. S., Ding, M., & Schoner, G. (1993). Dynamic pattern formation: A primer. In L. B. Smith & E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 13-50). Cambridge: A Bradford Book, MFF Press.

Kelso, J. A. S., Holt, K. G., Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: Q. Empirical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Ed.), Tutorial in motor behavior (pp.49-70). Amsterdam- North-Holland.

Kelso, J. A. S. & Schoner, G. (1988). Self-organization o f coordinative movement patterns. Human Movement Science, 7 , 27-46.

Knapp, C. G. & Dixon, W. R. (1952). Learning to juggle: A study o f whole and part m ethods. Research Quarterly, 23,38940 1.

Kozub, F. M. & Porretta, D. L. (1999). Internal and external attributions for levels of perceived physical competence in children with mental retardation. Education and Training in M ental Retardation and Developmental D isabilities, 34,35-42.

Kozub, F. M., Porretta, D. L., & Hodge, S. R. (2000). Motor task persistence of children with and without mental retardation. M ental Retardation, 38,42-49.

Krauss, M. (1993). Device for facilitating teaching youngsters to ride bicycles. Patent No. 5,259,638.

152

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Krebs, P. (2000). Mental Retardation. In J.P. Winnick (EdX Adapted Physical Education A n d S p o rt (pp. 111-126). Champaign, EL: Human Kinetics.

Krupski, A. (1977). Role of attention in the reaction-thne performance of mentally retarded adolescents. American Journal o f Mental Deficiency. 82.74-83.

Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: L Theorectical lines o f convergence. InG . E. Stelm ach & J. Requin (E ds.), Tutorials in motor behavior {pp. 3-47). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). On coordination and control in naturally developing systems. In J. A. S. Kelso & J .E. Clark (Eds.), The development o f movement control and coordination (pp. 5-78). New York: Wiley.

Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly c f rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

Lee, T. D., & Genovese, E. D. (1988). Distribution of practice in motor skill acquisition: Learning and performance effects reconsidered. Research Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 59, 277-287.

Liberty, K. A., Haring,N. G., White, O. R., & Billingsley, F. (1988). A technology for the fixture: Decision rules for generalization. Education and Training in M ental Retardation, 23, 315-325.

Livingston, B. & Hirst, P. (1986). Orthopedic disorders in school children with down syndrome with special reference to the incidence o fjoint laxity. C lin ic a l Orthopaedics and Related Research, 207,74-76.

MagilL, R. A. (1989). Motor learning concepts and applications. Dubuque, Iowa: Wm. C. Brown Publishers.

McCloy, C. H. (1934). The measurement o f general motor capacity and general motor ability. Research Quarterly, 546-61. ,

McCloy, C. H. (1937). An analytical study of the stunt type test as a measure of motor educability. Research Quarterly. 846-55. ,

McDonald, P. V., van Emmerik, R. E. A., & Newell, K. M. (1989). The effects of Practice on limb kinematics in a throwing task. Journal o f Motor Behavior, 21, 245-264.

Moxley, S. E. (1979). Schema: The variability o f practice hypothesis. Journal o f M otor Behavior. II, 65-70.

153

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Nanassi, J. (1995). Bicycle training aid. Patent No. 5,382,040.

National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. (2000). Promoting better health for young people through physical activity and sports [On-line]. Available: http.// www.cdc. gov/nccdphp/dash/presohvsactrpt/

Naylor, J., & Briggs, G. (1963). Effects of task complexity and task organization on the relative efficiency o f part and whole training methods. Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 65, 217-244.

Nettlebeck, T., & Brewer, N. (1981). Studies of mild mental retardation and times performance. In N. EL Ellis (Ed.), International review in mental retardation (pp. 61-106). New York: Academic Press.

Newell, fCM. (1984). Physical constraints to development of motor skills. In J.R. Thomas (Ed.), Motor development during adulthood and adolescence. Minneapolis, MN: Burgess

Newell, K. M , & Rovegno, I. (1990). Motor learning: Theory and practice. Q uest, 42, 184-192.

Newell, K. M , Wade, M. G. & Kelly, J. M (1979). Temporal anticipation o f response initiation by mentally retarded persons. American Journal o f Mental Deficiency, 84,219-295.

Ohio Department of Transportation. (2001). ODOTs biking ohio home page [On-line]. Available: http://www.dot-State.oh.us/bike/defaurt.btm

Payne, V. G. & Isaacs, L. D. (1999). Human development a lifespan approach. M ountain View, California: Mayfield Publishing Company.

Pearson, L. C. (1993). Bicycle riding training device. Patent No. 5,133,820.

Population Estimates Program. (2001). Population Division, U.S. Census Bureau, Washington, D C. [On-line]. Available: http://www.census.gov/popuIation/estimafes/nation/intfilel-l.txt

Porretta, D. L. (1982). Motor schema formation by EMR boys. A m erica n J o u rn a l o f Mental Deficiency, 8 7 ,164-172.

Porretta, D. L. (1987). Selected parameters of response organization in mildly mentally retarded children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 64,95-100.

Porretta, D. L. (1988). Contextual interference effects on the transfer and retention ofa gross motor skill by mildly mentally handicapped children. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 5332-339. , 154

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Porretta, D. L. & O’Brien, K (1991). The use of contextual interference trials by mildly mentally handicapped cbildrerL Research Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 62, 240-244.

Porretta, D. L. & Surburg, P. R. (1995). Imagery and physical practice in the acquisition o f gross motor riming of coincidence by adolescents with nrild mental retardation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8 01171-1183. ,

Rieber, F. M (1992). Training wheel assembly for bicycle. Patent No. 5,133,569.

Roberts, T. D. M. (1995). Understanding Balance. London, UK: Chapman & Hail.

Salomi, A. W., Wchmidt, R. A., & Walter, C. B. (1984). Knowledge o f results and motor skill learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 355- 386.

Saunders, S. R. (1989). Compensating training wheel assembly for a bicycle. Patent No. 4,810,000.

Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning Psychological Review, 82,225-260.

Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, Illinois: Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

Schmidt, R. C., Beek, P. J., Trefiher, P. J., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Dynamical Substructure of coordinated rhythmic movements. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 635-651.

Schmidt, R. A., Carello, C , & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination o f rhythmic movements between people. Journal c f Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 227-247.

Schmidt, R. A., Trefiher, P. J., Shaw, B. K. & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Dynamical aspects of learning an interlimb rhythmic movement pattern. Journal o f Motor Behavior, 2 4 ,67-83.

Schoner, G. S. (1995). Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications. Ecological Psychology, 7, 291-314.

Schoner, G. S., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1988). Dynamic pattern generation m behavioral and neural systems. Science, 239, 1513-1520.

Shea, C. H. & Kohl, R. M. (1990). Specificity and variability o f practice: R esea rch Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 6 1 ,169-177. 155

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Shea, C. FL, Shebilske, W. L., & WorcheL, S. (1993). M otor learning and control. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Shea, C. H., & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effect on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5,179-187.

Shumway-Cook, A. & Woollacott, M. H. (1985). Dynamics o f postural control in the child with Down syndrome. Physical Therapy, 6 5 , 1315-1322.

Singer, R. N. (1978). Motor skills and learning strategies, hr H. F. O'Neil (Ed.), L ea rn in g S tra te g ie s (pp. 79-106). New York: Academic Press.

Singer, R. N. (1986). Sports performance: A five-step mortal approach. J o u rn a l o f Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, April, 82-84.

Singer, R. N., Lidor, R., & Cauraugh, J JL (1993). To be aware or not aware? What to think about while (earning and performing a motor skilL The Sport Psychologist, 7 ,1 9 -3 0 .

Singer, R. N., Lidor R. & Cauraugh, J. H. (1994). Focus o f attention during motor skill performance. Journal o f Sports Sciences, 345-340.

Special Olympics International. (1988). Special Olympics Cycling Sport Skills Program. Washington, DC: Joseph P. Kennedy, Jr. Foundation.

Stucky-Ropp, R. C. & DiLorenze, T. M. (1993). Determinants o f exercise in children. Preventive Medicine, 22, 880-889.

Sugden, D. A., & Keogh, J. F. (1990). Problems in movement skills development Columbia: University of South Carolina Press.

Surburg, P. (1985). Two conditions o f uncertainty and their effects on reaction tune of mentally retarded adolescents. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2136-143. ,

Surburg, P. R., Porretta, D. L , & Sutlive, V. (1995). Use o f imagery practice for improving a motor skill. Adapted Physical A ctivity Quarterly, 1217-227. ,

Thelen, E. (1985). Developmental origins o f motor coordination: Leg movements in human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 1 8 , 1-22.

Thelen, E. (1986). Treadmill-elicited stepping in seven-month-old infants. C h ild Development, 5 7 , 1498-1506.

IS6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Thelen, E. (1989). Dynamical approaches to the development ofbehavior. In J. A. S. Kelso, A. J. Mandell, & M. E. Schelsinger (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in complex S y s. Singapore: World Scientific

Thelen, E. (1994). Three-monthrold infants can learn task-specific patterns of interiimb coordination. Psychological Science, 5 , 280-285.

Thelen, E. & Cooke, D. W. (1987). Relationship between newborn stepping and later w alking: A new interpretation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 2 9 ,380-393.

Thelen, E. & Fischer, D. M. (1982). Newborn stepping: An explanation for a Disappearing reflex. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 760-775.

Thelen, E., Fisher, D., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1984). The relationship between physical growth and a newborn reflex. Infant Behavior and Development. 7 , 479-493.

Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development o f cognition and action. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

Thelen, E. & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs o f the Society fo r Research in Child Development. 56, (I, Serial No. 223).

Turvey, M. T. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with reference to vision. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving acting and knowing: Towards an ecological psychology (pp.211-265). Hillsdale, NJ: Eribaum.

Turvey, M. T., Rosenblmn, L. D., Kugler, P. N., & Schmidt, R. C. (1986). Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movements. J o u rn a l o f Experimental psychology: Human perception and performance. 12,564-583.

Ulrich, B. D., Ulrich, D. A., & Angulo-Kinzler, R. M. (1998). The impact of context manipulations on movement patterns during a transition period. H um an Movement Science, 17, 327-346.

Ulrich, B. D., Ulrich, D. A. & Collier, D. H. (1992). Alternating stepping patterns: Hidden abilities o f 11-month-old infants with down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 34, 233-239.

United States Department ofHeahh and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity and health: A report o f the surgeon general. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department ofHealth and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.

157

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Van Hmmerik, R_ E. A. (1992). Kinematic adaptations to perturbations as a function of practice in rhythmic drawing movements. Journal o f Motor Behavior, 24, ! 17- 131.

VanEmmerik, R. E. A., Brinker, B. P. L. M., Vereijken, B., Whiting, H. T. A. (1989). Preferred tempo in the learning o f a gross cyclical action. Quarterly Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 41a, 251-262.

Vereijken, B. & Thelen, E. (1997). Training infant treadmill stepping: The role of individual pattern stability. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 89-102.

Vereijken, B., Van Emmerik, R. E. A., Whiting, H. T. A., & Newell, K. M. (1992). Free(z)ing degrees of freedom in motor learning. Journal o f Motor Behavior, 2 4 ,133-142.

Wade, M. G., Newell, K. M. & Wallace, S. A. (1978). Decision time and movement time as a function of response complexity in retarded persons. American Journal o f Mental Deficiency, 8 3 ,135-144.

Weiss, M. R., Ebbick, V., & Rose, D. J. (1992). “Show and tell” in the gymnasium revisited: Developmental differences in modeling and verbal rehearsal effects on motor skill performance. Research Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 63,292-301.

WhitalL, J. & Getchell, N. (1995). From walking to running: Applying dynamical systems approach to the development of locomotor skills. Child Development, 6 6 , 1541- 1553.

Wickerstom, R. L. (1958). Comparative study of methodologies for teaching gymnastics and tumbling stunts. Research Quarterly, 2 9109-115. ,

Winstein, C. J. & Schmidt, R. A. (1990). Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning Journal o f Experimental Psychology; Learning Memory, and Cognition, 16, 677-691.

Winther, K. T. & Thomas, J. R. (1981). Developmental differences in children’s labeling of movement J o u rn a l o f M otor B ehavior, 1 3 , 77-90.

Wright, J., & Cowden, J. E. (1986). Changes in selfconcept and cardiovascular Endurance of mentally retarded youths in a Special Olympics swim training Program . Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 3, 177-183.

Wrisberg, C. A. & Ragsdale, M. R. (1979). Further tests o f Schmidt’s schema theory. Development of a schema rule for a coincident timing task. J o u rn a l o f M otor Behavior, 11, 159-166.

158

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Wul£ G., McNevin, N. EL, Fuchs, T., Ritter, F. & Toole, T. (2000). Attentional focus in complex skill learning. Research Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 7 /, 229-239.

Yang, J. J. & Porretta, D. L. (1999). Sport/leisure skill learning by adolescents with mild mental retardation: A four-step strategy. Adapted Physcial Activity Quarterly, 16, 300-515.

Zanzone, P. G. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1991). Experimental studies of behavioral attractors and their evolution with learning. In J. Requm& G. E. Stelmach (Eds.), T u to ria ls in motor neuroscience (pp. L21-133X Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Zigler, E. & Hodapp, R. M (1986). Understanding mental retardation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX A

BICYCLE PHOTOGRAPHS

i

160

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. B ikeB j

161

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. BikeC

BikeD

162

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. B i k e E

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX B

ADAPTED BICYCLE DESCRIPTION

The independent variable for this study is the use o f a series of four adapted bicycles (see

Appendix A) created by Dr. Richard Klein, professor emeritus of mechanical engineering

from the University of Illinois. Dr. Klein provided the following description of the

adapted bicycles. Three of the four adapted bicycles feature 12 inch long by 3.25 inch

(mid-point) diameter rollers in lieu of conventional pneumatic tires. The rollers are made

of solid polyurethane rubber molded onto a two inch OJD. (outside diameter) steel tubular

core. The thickness of the molded polyurethane covering material is 518 inch, and thus

the rollers start out as cylindrical at 3.25 inches in diameter (2" +- 5/8" + 5/8") prior to

being crowned by machining on a modem lathe. The inner steel cores (for each of the

respective rollers) are hollow, with a 1.25-inch internal diameter. The rollers are first

internally bored on each end to accept a 1 3/8-inch x l/2-inch standard flange bearing

Then, using the internal bearing bores as centering mounts, the outer polyurethane rollers

are turned on a numerically controlled (NC) lathe to exacting proportions. In short, each

roller is machined to have a precisely controlled crown, where the description of the

respective crowns are termed in "arc-inches" using tire industry terminology. Thus, each

roller has a crown in its respective center, which gradually tapers down to the ends.

Moreover, the crown can be varied (in the NC lathe operation) to give the respective

rollers differing arc-inch crowns. The rollers to be used in this study have crowns ranging

164

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. from 60.15 to 180.05 arc-inches. The rollers have been mounted onto a 16 inch (the

diameter), single-speed bicycle frame. The frame o f the bicycle is adapted in its structure

(by frame modification) to preserve the original ground points of contact Hence, the

frame accommodates the rollers by extending downward as the rollers have a

significantly smaller diameter as compared to the original 16-inch bicycle tire

configuration. Moreover, to provide a means of propulsion, a drive train system exists

using a standard and rear sprocket, which in turn drives a standard 5/16-

inch automotive style V-belt that in turn drives the rear roller. The rollers are designed

such that all rear rollers have been machined at the respective centers) to have an

accommodating V-groove to accept the drive V-belt The gearing is altered sufficiently to

slow the training bicycle's forward speed down relative to pedaling cadence. The

bicycle's gearing is about one-third that of a conventional similarly sized bicycle. The

first two bicycles in the series have modified drive trains. This adjustment converts the

design to that of a fixed gear as opposed to a free wheel or "coaster brake" system." The

fourth adapted bicycle in the series features a wide, 16x8x5 inch inflatable garden tractor

style tire as opposed to rollers. To summarize, there are four adapted bicycles in this

series featuring a combination of rollers, wide tires, lower gearing ratios, and modified

drive trains, designed to accommodate the needs of children in the acquisition of

conventional cycling skills.

165

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX C

ILLUSTRATED PATENTS

Note; The numbers associated with the following illustrations refer to specific parts of the patented equipmenL Refer to the list of references for information on patents.

166

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 3 0 1 4 ^ / 4 4 4 0

Egley, 1994

Cudmore,l972

167

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. GeLIer, 1991

Harrison, 1995

168

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Henrichs, 1996

169

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. BCiauss, 1993

Nanassi, 1995

170

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. t)

Pearson, 1993

171

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Saunders, 1989

172

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX D

PILOT DATA

173

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acquisitions • Maintenance^ A Generalizations O Probes &

A I *

• « €—»,

L-,

E

T C«I E

R /V

S D“

TRIALS Participant #t-P

174

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acquisitions • Maintenance^ A Generalizations O Probes©

D A 1 I I S

T A’ 2 A N C

E 8“' a a a—« I /

M B»f| ® * ° I E

T " 1 E i f ] i R

S * ■ 9

O i n „ 9 n & i

TRIALS

Participant #2-P

175

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. i 1-2 § 1 3 0 3 c ■ -2 2 3 B « S l s i

• I

tn_4 < t-* Participant #3'PParticipant

I

v T i. » r - -X.

Q_e«t-

176

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. cI » i> .2 s

H i * f l s l Participant #4*P Participant

177

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. o . < I •I It o <= o S s II J® to o « I itt« s |o 5 s P

Q_tot-

I7S

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX E

BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL SCIENCES HUMAN SUBJECTS INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB)

j

179

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. BEHAVIORAL. AND SOCIAL SCIENCES HUMAN SUBJECTS INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) The Ohio State University. Columbus. OH 43210

Research Involving Human Subjects ACTION OF THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD

FtiU Committee Review X Original Review x Expedited Review Continuing Review Amendment

With regard to the employment o! human subjects in the proposed research protocol:

01 BOW THE USE OF ADAPTED BICYCLES IN THE ACQUISITION. MAINTENANCE AND GENERALIZATION OF CONVENTIONAL CYCLING SKILLS BY CHILDREN WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION, David L. Porretta. Tammy L. Burt; Physical Activity & E d u catio n al S e rv ic e s

THE BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL SCIENCES HUMAN SUBJECTS IRB HAS TAKEN THE FOLLOWING ACTION:

APPROVED DISAPPROVED

X APPROVED WfTH CONDITIONS WAIVER OF WRITTEN CONSENT GRANTED ' Conditions stated by the IRB have been met by the Investigator and; therefore, the protocol is APPROVED.

• It is the responsibility of the principal investigator to retain a copy of each signed consent form for at least three (3) years beyond the termination of the subject's participation in the proposed activity. Should the principal investigator leave the University, signed consent forms are to be transferred to the Human Subjects IRB for the required retention period.

• This application has been approved for the period of one year.

• You are reminded that you m ust promptly report any problems to the IRB and that no procedural changes may be made without prior review and approval.

• You are also reminded that the identity of the research participants must be kept j confidentiaL

Date: August 24.2001 Signed:

HS-Q25B (Rev. 2/94)

180

with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX F

INFORMED CONSENT

/

181

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. T * K - E School of Physical Activity Phone 614-Z9Z-5679 OHIO and Educational Services FAX 614-292-7229 SPOE UNIVERSITY

CONSENT FOR PARTICIPATION IN SOCIAL AND BEHAVIORAL RESEARCH

Protocol title: The me Of adapn-d hif-m-ly* ; t» -x-qmaition- nafmrnmrtt^tirf^nm lrntinniif’ennmtrinnal cycling stalls hv r-hitrfreg with m 'M i w m l retardation

Protocol number 01B0147

Principal Investigator Dr. David Porrata

I consent to my participation in (arm y child's participation in) research being conducted by Tammy Burt of The Ohio State University and his/her assistants and associates.

The investigators) has explained the purpose of the study, the procedures that will be followed, and the amount of time it will take. I understand the possible benefits, if any. of my participation (and/or my child's participation).

I know that I can (and/or my child can) choose not to participate without penalty to m e (and/or my child). If I agree to participate. I can (and/or my chftd can) withdraw from the study at any time, and there will be no penalty.

• I consent to the use of audiotapes and/or videotapes, t understand how the tapes wilt be used for this study. • t consent to the use of photographs. I understand how the photographs win be used for this study.

I have had a chance to ask questions and to obtain answers to my questions. I can contact the investigators at (614)292-0849 or (6141830-0659. If I have questions about my rights as a research participant. I can call the Office of Research Risks Protection at (614) 686-4792.

I have read this form or I have had it read to me. I sign it freely and voluntarily. A copy has been given to m e.

Print the name of the participant;

D ate;______Signed: /PWflcfMnO

S ig n e d : ______S igned: I (Panop«ttocadg^nr ar tieamr tum aumi npnsantaOn) (Fnoon auftariatf toeonswir ArpvttapwiC t n q u n d )

W itness: |W fw > n q u n 4

HS4BT(Rmr. 05iav

IS 2

with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX G

FLOOR DIAGRAM

i

183

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Cone placements « Gymnasium dimensions 18.7 m x t L2 m Generalization only

Video-camecL

9 m. r A

12 m

<

V S o n line t_5 m

Ready Line investigator

184

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX H

DATA COLLECTION SHEETS

185

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Coding Sheet A Phase ______Session ______Date______Participant Total # of Trials Trial I Trial 11 Trial 21 T rial3 l Distance; Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 2 Trial 12 Trial 22 Trial 32 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 3 Trial 13 Trial 23 Trial 33 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike: Trial 4 Trial 14 Trial 24 Trial 34 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trials Trial 15 Trial 25 Trial 35 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 6 Trial 16 Trial 26 Trial 36 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 7 Trial 17 Trial 27 Trial 37 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

TrialS Trial 18 Trial 28 Trial 38 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 9 Trial 19 Trial 29 Trial 39 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

Trial 10 Trial 20 Trial 30 Trial 40 Distance: Distance: Distance: Distance: Bike: Bike: Bike: Bike:

indicate bike as A. B. C D or E Bicycle Probe=BP Generalization Probe=GP

186

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Coding Sheet 3

Participant's Name. ______D ate______Phase.

T rial 1 eDownsroket 3 sec/m w Trial U *Do'»«tstrokes B ike_____ Elapsed Time B ik e____ „ Elapsed Tune sec

T ria l! 3 Trial IZ *Downstrokes 3 see/mm B ite _____ Elapsed Time ____ B ike_____ BaoacdTtine

T hat I 3 sec/m ut T nai 13 « Howtwrm Ir« fpr/im n B ik e_____ Elapsed Time Bike _ ESaosedTune _ J « c .

T h a i* * Downstrokes 3 sec/rnta T nai U * Dowmtrokes 3 sec/m in Bike Elapsed Time sec. B ik e____ Q am ert Ttm e fme*

T h alS 9 sec/m ui T nai 15 * O o w m trete 3 sec/m ut B ik e_____ Elapsed Time Sec. Bike Elapsed Tune sec.

T hai 6 eDownstroke* 3 w t/m in Trial 16 • Dowftftmkei sec/nuik B ite FlanSfdTime sec Bike ElansedTIme sec.

T eal 7 • Downstrekes 3 sec/nuik T nai 17 *DnwiH(rekn 3 sec/mm Phptnrt Tine see. Bike Elapsed Tim e sec.

T h alS *Dov«ntrakes 3 sec/mu* T nai IS * Dowftstmfces B ite ______s c c . Bike Elapsed T iinc _ sec.

Trial 9 * Dowmtrokes 9 teC/reua T nai 19 * Oowasirotces 3 sec/m ia B ite _____ sec. B ik e_____ B am ed Tim e sec.

T nai (0 * OnwmcrokM 9 sec/m ut T nai 20 * Oowftsmxte* Bike Elapsed Time ______see B ik e____ Elapsed T une sec.

187

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. C oding S heet C

Participant's Name. S ession ______D ate_ . P hase.

Trial_ B ite . Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals » Intervals w f HP up je 100 = Head Position Up= (>■) Head Position Down= (-) Total Intervals ___ I X X ZE 1 ------1 10 12 14 16 IS

Trial Bike Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals » Interval? wf HP,ug, I 100 = Head Position lTc*=x f+) Head Position Down= (-) Total Intervals I I V I T X X X s 10 12 1 4 16 IS

Trial______B ite______Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals » Intervals w f HP up j t 100 = _ Head Position Up= f+) Head Position Down= (-) Total Intervals l \ V > t x r~—i s 10 12 1 4 16 IS

T rial. B ite . Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals * Intervals w f HP u p jc 100=_ Head Position Up= (+) Head Position Down= (-) Total Intervals X X to 12 14 16 18

T rial. .B ite . Partial Interval Recottfing: 2 second intervals » Intervals w f HP un jt 100 =. Head Position Uq =(-i-> Head Position Dowa= (-) Total Intervals X X T: ----- I 0 2 4 S 10 12 1 4 16 13 Trial______B ite Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals 0 Intervals w f HP tro j t 100 = Head Pttsitioa Ups (+) Head Position Down= (-) Total Intervals r I X 10 12 1 4 16 13

Trial B ite ______Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals * Intervals wf HP up J t ioo = _ Head Position Ut*= M Head Position Down= f-) Total Intervals ~ '" i > i ~t t r~ I 0 2 4 3 10 12 1 4 1 6 18 Trial______B ite Partial Interval Recording: 2 second intervals § Intcrrab HP up. j t 100 = Head Position Up=(-v) Head Position Down= (-) T otal Intervals LZ X a s IQ 12 1 4 16 18

188

with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX I

Procedural Reliability Checklist

Yes No

L Did the participant wear a helmet while riding the b i c y c le ? ______

2. Did the investigator say "Ready? Here we go!" at the start

o f each trial? ______

3. Did the investigator release the bicycle at the start of each

tnai? ______

Did the investigator provide positive feedback following

baseline and probe conditions? ______

Did the investigator provide positive/corrective or

positive/specific feedback at the completion o f each tnai during

intervention? ______

6 . Did the participant dismount the bicycle and walk back to the

starting line at the completion of each trial? ______

7. Did the investigator announce "stop” at the completion of

each trial?

189

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX J

Social Validity Questionnaire

Circle the number that best corresponds to the following statements.

l=Strongly Agree 2=Agree 3=Disagree 4=Strongly Disagree

SA A D SD

1. A gradual introduction to bicycle-riding is less threatening than the traditional approach o f trial and e rro r. 2 4

2 . My child looked forward to attending cycling sessions. ? 4

3. The use of adapted bicycles made it possible for my child to ride a conventional bicycle.

4. My child will ride his/her bicycle with friends and/or family.

5. The ability to ride a conventional bicycle will increase my child’s level of physical activity.

6. The ability to ride a conventional bicycle will increase my child’s feeling of independence. 4

Learning to ride a conventional bicycle has boosted my child’s selfesteem .

Bicycle riding is a popular recreational activity in my neighborhood. 4

9. Bicycle riding is a lifelong sport/leisure activity. 4

[90

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDIX K

Interobserver Agreement Observation Schedule

Participant 1 Session I

Participant 2 Session 2

Participants Sessions

Participant 4 Session 4

Participants Sessions

Participant 6 Session 6

Participant? Session 7

Participant 8 Session 8

Participant 9 Session I

Participant 10 Session 2

/

I9t

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.