<<

original papers

Adv Clin Exp Med 2013, 22, 6, 855–860 © Copyright by Wroclaw Medical University ISSN 1899–5276

Agnieszka Olchowska-Kotala Individual Differences in Cancer Patients’ Willingness to Use Complementary and Różnice indywidualne w skłonności do stosowania niekonwencjonalnych metod leczenia wśród chorych na nowotwory

Department of Humanistic Sciencies in Medicine, Wroclaw Medical University, Poland

Abstract Background. Cancer patients very often combine conventional methods of treatment with complementary and alternative medicine (CAM). It is not clear why some cancer patients are inclined to use CAM, whereas others are not. Objectives. The aim of the study was to find predictors of cancer patients’ willingness to use CAM. The study investigated whether personality traits, cognitive preferences and paranormal beliefs are determinants of these patients’ willingness to use CAM. Material and Methods. The study participants were 49 cancer patients. The Polish version of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory was used in the study to asses personality traits; cognitive preferences were measured with Nosal’s Types checklist (CTU-96); an Inexplicable Phenomena scale was used to examine paranormal beliefs; and willing- ness to use CAM was assessed with a Decision-Making Choices Questionnaire (DMCQ). Results. Among the studied personality traits, extraversion, and a lower level of openness to experi- ence were significant predictors of cancer patients’ willingness to use CAM, along with emotionality and rationality as cognitive preferences. Conclusions. Further research is required to verify the of different roles of individual predispositions in making decisions about the use of CAM, depending on the subject’s health condition (Adv Clin Exp Med 2013, 22, 6, 855–860). Key words: cancer patients, Complementary and Alternative Medicine, personality traits, cognitive preferences, paranormal beliefs. Streszczenie Wprowadzenie. Chorzy na nowotwory bardzo często obok leczenia konwencjonalnego stosują niekonwencjonalne metody leczenia, najczęściej nazywane w literaturze światowej: Complementary and Alternative Medicine (CAM). Nie jest jasne, dlaczego niektórzy pacjenci stosują CAM, inni zaś nie. Cel pracy. Celem badania była znalezienie predyktorów skłonności do stosowania CAM wśród chorych na nowo- twory. Badanie sprawdzało, czy cechy osobowości, preferencje poznawcze oraz przekonania o istnieniu zjawisk paranormalnych determinują skłonność u chorych na nowotwory do zastosowania CAM. Materiały i metody. W badaniu uczestniczyło 49 chorych na nowotwory. Cechy osobowości badano polską wer- sją NEO Five-Factor Inventory; preferencje poznawcze – skalą Typy Umysłu (CTU-96); przekonania o istnieniu zjawisk paranormalnych – skalą Niewyjaśnione Zjawiska; gotowość do stosowania CAM – Kwestionariuszem Wyborów Decyzyjnych. Wyniki. Spośród badanych cech osobowości istotnymi predyktorami skłonności do stosowania CAM u chorych na nowotwory były: ekstrawertyczność, neurotyczność i mała otwartość na doświadczenie. Gotowość do stoso- wania CAM u chorych na nowotwory była związana także z emocjonalnością i racjonalnością jako preferencjami poznawczymi. Wnioski. Potrzebne są dalsze badania, aby zweryfikować hipotezę o odmiennej roli cech indywidualnych w podej- mowaniu decyzji o sięgnięciu po CAM w zależności od stanu zdrowia (Adv Clin Exp Med 2013, 22, 6, 855–860). Słowa kluczowe: pacjenci chorzy na nowotwory, medycyna komplementarna i alternatywna, cechy osobowości, preferencje poznawcze, przekonania o istnieniu zjawisk paranormalnych. 856 A. Olchowska-Kotala

Cancer patients very often combine conven- use various evaluation criteria to sort out the in- tional methods of treatment with complementa- formation they receive. A cognitive preference is ry and alternative medicine (CAM) [1]. In surveys an inclination to use a certain criterion frequently. conducted in 13 countries, the prevalence of CAM Cognitive preferences are not only decisive factors use by cancer patients ranges from 7% to 64% [2]. in how particular information will be processed, Although the prevalence of CAM use is difficult to but also determine the final outcome, i.e. the con- establish due to varying definitions of CAM and clusions reached by an individual. For instance, the the manner in which it is assessed [3], a number of fact that somebody feels better after having used studies indicate that the majority of cancer patients may be sufficient evidence of the ef- assert that they use some form of CAM. ficacy of this method for one person and insuffi- It seems that one of the key factors increasing cient for another. There are no systematic studies patients’ willingness to use CAM is their individual­ on the relationship between cognitive preferences outlook. Jeswani and Furnham [4] found that peo- and CAM usage. This study is an attempt to estab- ple with paranormal beliefs (defined as beliefs that lish whether any of the cognitive preferences dis- stand in obvious contradiction to the principles of tinguished by Nosal [11] (Table 1) and a person’s contemporary [5]) are more likely to be- willingness to use CAM are linked. lieve in the efficacy of CAM and that positive at- The decision to use CAM is rarely just a one- titudes to science were associated with increased -off event. People tend to display certain patterns toward CAM. In a cross-sectional study of health-related behavior. It seems that for some on 712 randomly selected adults in a Belgian sam- individuals CAM usage belongs to their repertoire ple, Van den Bulck and Custers obtained simi- of health-related behavior, and they decide to use lar results [6], which confirmed a positive corre- CAM more frequently and willingly. Individu­ lation between paranormal beliefs and openness al predisposition may influence a person’s will- to CAM. Whether or not paranormal phenomena ingness to use CAM. The aim of this study was to ­exist, many people strongly believe in them. find predictors of cancer patients’ willingness to Only few studies have addressed the relation- use CAM. The study examined whether persona­ ships between dispositional personality factors lity traits, cognitive preferences and paranormal and attitudes toward CAM. The most widely ac- beliefs determine these patients’ willingness to use cepted and widely used model of the personality CAM. is the Big Five: openness to experience (intellec- tual curiosity, imaginativeness, willingness to ex- periment), extraversion (sociability, assertiveness, Material and Methods activity), conscientiousness (persistence, goal-di- Willingness to Use CAM rectedness, self-discipline), agreeableness (trust in others, sincerity, non-confrontational attitude) Willingness to use CAM was assessed with and neuroticism (strong negative emotions, stress a Decision-Making Choices Questionnaire (DMCQ) sensitivity) [7]. Sirois and Purc-Stephenson, who constructed by the author of this article. The ques- examined the use of CAM currently and in the past tionnaire uses so-called choice dilemmas [12]: among CAM clients found a correlation between Respondents are presented with some hypothet- agreeableness and the breadth and frequency of ical situations in which they might find them- CAM consultations [8]. Furthermore, a relation- selves and are requested to state what they would ship between openness to experience and the use do if they were in those particular situations. The of CAM has been substantiated by some research- DMCQ consists of 13 decision-making situations. ers [8–9]. However, in Furnham’s study personali- First, the respondents are asked to picture them- ty traits were not related to CAM usage and beliefs selves in the given situation. After reading a de- about CAM [10]. It is worth noting that these stud- scription of the situation and the definition of the ies were based on heterogeneous samples in terms form of CAM in question, the participants decide of sick and healthy people, and researchers used whether they would use that form of CAM or not. different methods to measure the participants’ at- All of the situations outlined in the DMCQ refer titudes toward CAM. to various health dysfunctions and suggest vari- It is likely that an individual’s attitude toward ous forms of CAM. Particular health dysfunctions CAM is linked to the domain of cognitive prefe­ have been matched with particular forms of CAM, rences. A cognitive preference is an information based on an analysis of the popularity of various processing strategy that characterizes a person’s forms of CAM in Poland. The respondents indi- typical style of perceiving, remembering, thinking cated their decisions on a five-point Likert-type and problem solving. Information that is received scale with response options ranging from 1 (“I will must be assessed and systematized, and individuals certainly not decide on this therapy”) to 5 (“I will Use of Complementary and Alternative Medicine by Cancer Patients 857 certainly decide on this therapy”). The decisions with 30 statements and asked to rate their agree- that respondents make in all 13 situations form ment with each statement on a five-point scale the overall outcome. The DMCQ had been previ- from 1 to 5. The scale was prepared in accordance ously validated on a Polish population. The scale with the standards of scale construction [14] and has adequate psychometric properties [13]. The validated on a Polish population [13]. The Confir- questionnaire has good concurrent validity; pre- matory Factor Analysis showed that the 30 items vious studies based on the DMCQ indicated that were loaded on a single factor: paranormal beliefs. the results obtained through this questionnaire re- According to Cronbach’s alpha the internal con- flected the respondents’ earlier experiences with sistency was 0.91. CAM. According to Cronbach’s Alpha, the inter- nal consistency of the questionnaire in the current study was 0.87. The following is a sample situation Cognitive Preferences from the DMCQ: “Imagine that you’ve had a mo- torbike accident and have lost sensation in one leg. Cognitive preferences were measured using A friend of yours told you that his friend used bio- the self-descriptive Mind Types checklist (CTU- energotherapy when he had the same problem and -96) developed by Nosal [11, 15]. This is a 120-item it helped. This method is based on transmitting instrument consisting of adjectives describing cog- bioenergy from a healer to a patient. What would nitive preferences. Respondents assess the extent you do in a similar situation? Would you decide to to which a given adjective relates to their own use bioenergotherapy?” . The degree of agreement with each state- ment is scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale. The higher the score for a given preference, the more Paranormal Beliefs frequently a person uses the criteria represented by The most frequently used scale to survey para- this particular preference. Table 1 provides a de- normal beliefs is the Paranormal Beliefs Scale scription of the 5 cognitive preferences with their (PBS) developed by Tobacyk and Milford [5]. Its Cronbach’s reliability in the current study. items form seven subscales: , spiritua­ lism, extraordinary life forms, psi, , pre- and traditional religious beliefs. Due to Personality Traits cultural differences it was impossible to apply the Personality traits were assessed with the Polish original PBS in the current study, so the author of version of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO- the current article developed the Inexplicable Phe- FFI), which measures the five fundamental dimen- nomena scale (IP), which reflects paranormal be- sions of personality [7]. In the current sample alpha liefs that are popular among Poles. The scale con- reliabilities were 0.83 for neuroticism, 0.78 for con- tains items that represent the same factors defined scientiousness, 0.74 for extraversion, 0.69 for open- by the authors of the PBS, with the exception of ness to experience and 0.68 for agreeableness. traditional religious beliefs, which were not incor- porated in the IP scale because in Poland these be- liefs are very common. Participants were presented

Table 1. Description of the Scale for Cognitive Preferences (CTU-96). Dimensions and Cronbach’s Reliabilities Tabela 1. Opis skali badającej preferencje poznawcze. Wymiary i współczynniki trafności Cronbacha

Description (Opis) Cronbach Emotionality Preference for judgment based on emotions, moods and personal feelings, and for sub- 0.86 (Emocjonalność) jective comprehension of facts and events Rationality Preference for analytical, logical, insightful, practical, explorative processing of incom- 0.88 (Racjonalność) ing information Creativity Preference for innovative, open and original evaluation of information and problems 0.81 (Twórczość) Intuition Preference for global and comprehensive evaluation; detection and judgment based on 0.70 (Intuicja) relating facts from various and previously unconnected sources Typicality Preference for simplistic, stereotypical and superficial evaluation of problems and issues 0.73 (Typowość) 858 A. Olchowska-Kotala

Table 2. Predictors of Cancer Patients’ Willingness to Use CAM: Regression Analysis Tabela 2. Predyktory gotowości do stosowania CAM wśród pacjentów chorych na nowotwory: analiza regresji

r Persona/eta β t value p value Constant (Stała) –2.46 0.02* Gender (Płeć) 0.09 0.27 1.75 0.09 Age (Wiek) 0.03 –0.29 –1.73 0.09 Education level (Wykształcenie) 0.27 0.01 0.06 0.95 Intuition (Intuicja) 0.17 0.04 0.25 0.80 Typicality (Typowość) 0.14 –0.22 –1.30 0.20 Emotionality (Emocjonalność) 0.31 0.48 2.81 0.01* Creativity (Twórczość) 0.12 –0.05 –0.22 0.83 Rationality (Racjonalność) 0.25 0.45 2.41 0.02* Conscientiousness (Sumienność) 0.14 0.12 0.53 0.60 Extraversion (Ekstrawertyczność) 0.13 0.46 2.84 0.01* Neuroticism (Neurotyczność) 0.23 0.47 2.79 0.01* Openness to experience (Otwartość na doświadczenie) –0.15 –0.53 –2.90 0.01* Agreeableness (Ugodowość) 0.11 0.30 1.58 0.12 Paranormal beliefs (Przekonania o istnieniu zjawisk paranormalnych) 0.25 0.26 1.82 0.08

The Participants the correlation ratio eta was calculated. Descrip- tive statistics were generated, including frequen- Forty-nine cancer patients (36 female and cy data for the sample characteristics and means 13 male) at the Lower Silesia Oncology Center took with standard deviations for continuous variables. part in the study. The inpatients were awaiting med- Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were calculated to ical procedures or further diagnostic examinations. examine the internal consistency of the DMCQ, The group was heterogenous in terms of the type IP, CTU-96 and NEO-FFI. P values < 0.05 were of cancer, duration and the severity of the disease. considered statistically significant. The eligibility criteria were as follows: (a) a diagno- sis of cancer, (b) awareness of the diagnosis, (c) age over 18 years, (d) literacy and (e) no history of se- Results rious mental disorder. The mean age of the partici- pants was 48 years (SD = 11.83). Their educational The following variables were entered in the re- levels were: primary 6.1%; vocational 28.6%, upper gression model: age, gender, education, personali- secondary 51% and university 14.3%. This popula- ty traits, cognitive preferences and paranormal be- tion has been described elsewhere [13]. liefs. The model turned out to be well adjusted to the variables (F (14.34) = 3.033; p = 0.004). Overall, the variables accounted for 37% of the variation in Statistical Analysis the cancer patients’ willingness to use CAM. The statistical analysis was performed using As Table 2 shows, the influence of demo- SPSS statistical software, version 18 (SPSS Inc., graphic variables in the sample was not significant. PASW Statistics for Windows, Chicago, USA). Among the studied personality traits, extraversion, Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to ana­ neuroticism and a lower level of openness to expe- lyze the association between willingness to use rience were significant predictors of willingness to CAM and individual traits, with the exception use CAM. Willingness to use CAM also increas- of dichotomies and ordinal variables, for which es in those cancer patients who were more likely Use of Complementary and Alternative Medicine by Cancer Patients 859 to use cognitive preferences, such as emotionali- attitude towards CAM. Perhaps healthy individuals ty and rationality. Paranormal beliefs made a mar- are inclined to use CAM due to cognitive curiosi­ ginally significant contribution to the variation in ty and willingness to , whereas cancer the cancer patients’ willingness to use CAM. patients are driven by more pragmatic, down-to- - reasons rooted in reality. This is an aspect which requires verification in a further study. Discussion In studies carried out on the general popula- tion, beliefs in favor of CAM were associated with The study was intended to find factors de- paranormal beliefs [5, 18]. In the current study, termining cancer patients’ willingness to use the relationship between paranormal beliefs and CAM. The results indicate that such personality willingness to use CAM was only marginally sig- traits as neuroticism, extraversion and a low degree nificant. On the basis of the results, it can be hy- of openness to experience, as well as emotiona­lity pothesized that different factors determine posi- and rationality as cognitive preferences, are signif- tive attitudes towards CAM in healthy people in icant predictors in this sample. and cancer patients. Perhaps healthy individuals’ In the study, the cancer patients scoring high willingness to use CAM is more associated with in neuroticism displayed a stronger tendency their outlook, whereas in seriously ill patients oth- to use CAM. These findings are consistent with er variables could come into play when deciding a study by Maskarinec et al. involving 1.168 cancer to resort to CAM. This hypothesis requires further patients, among whom those with lower emotion- verification. al functioning were more likely to use CAM [16]. In cancer patients, a preference for judgments Individuals scoring high in neuroticism are more based on emotions, moods and personal feelings likely than the average to experience such feelings contributes to willingness to use CAM. The likeli- as anxiety and/or depressed moods; they have an hood of using CAM also increases in those patients ongoing tendency to experience negative emotion- who use analytical, logical and insightful processing al states, respond more poorly to environmental of incoming information. The patients who base stress and are more likely to interpret ordinary sit- their processing of information on the criteria of uations as threatening. The incidence of such char- rationality probably conclude that using CAM in- acteristics among cancer patients is a one of the creases their chances of recovery. Even though some predictors of a willingness to use CAM. CAM are not scientifically proved, a willingness to This study shows that extraversion as a perso­ employ all possible methods of therapy when suffer- nality trait increases cancer patients’ willingness to ing from a serious illness may imply the pragmatism use CAM. Extraversion is a personality trait chara­ typical of rationality as a cognitive preference. cterizing the quality and quantity of social interac- The current study had several limitations. tions, as well as the degree of activity and . Firstly, it looked at the participants’ willingness to Therefore, those cancer patients who do not tend use CAM and not their actual use of CAM. It is well to hide emotions or to prefer solitude (as introverts known that actual behavior may differ from asser- do), but are rather active and like to share prob- tions of how one would behave in given situ­ations. lems with others, display a stronger willingness to Secondly, the results are based on a small conve- use CAM. An investigation among patients with nience sample. In the future, it would be advisable self-reported chronic back pain in a survey carried to confirm the findings on a larger, more repre- out in Canada also found that being more active sentative sample of cancer patients. Finally, in this and more involved in social life is correlated with study the stage of the patients’ disease was not tak- using CAM [17]. en into account. Future studies should be designed Surprisingly, an inverse relationship was found to enable assessment of the extent to which the between openness to experience and in cancer pa- course of cancer may affect the obtained results. tients’ willingness to use CAM. Previous studies The study identified a profile of a cancer patient found no association of openness to experience who is inclined to use CAM: He/She is a down-to- with CAM use [10] or showed a positive relation- -earth person, susceptible to negative emotions, ship [8–9]. However, those studies were conducted who actively deals with problems, is sociable, and on different populations. For instance, Honda and processes information analytically, pragmatically Jacobson’s survey [9] was carried out on a large and according to the emotional charge. It would be (3.032) representative sample of the US popula- worthwhile to carry out research on various sam- tion. Most of the respondents in such samples en- ple groups in order to verify the hypothesis of dif- joy good physical health. It is therefore possible ferent roles of individual predispositions in mak- that there are other reasons why both physical- ing decisions about the use of CAM, depending on ly healthy and seriously ill people share a positive the health condition. 860 A. Olchowska-Kotala

References [1] Richardson MA, Sanders T, Palmer JL: Complementary/alternative medicine use in comprehensive cancer center and the implications for oncology. J Clin Oncol 2000, 18, 2505–2514. [2] Ernst E, Cassileth BR: The prevalence of complementary/alternative medicine in cancer: A systematic review. Cancer 1998, 83, 777–782. [3] Kristoffersen AE, Fønnebø V, Northeim AJ: Use of complementary and alternative medicine among patients: classification criteria determine level of use. J Altern Complem Med 2008, 14, 911–919. [4] Jeswani M, Furnham A: Are modern worries, environmental concerns, or paranormal beliefs associated with per- ceptions of the effectiveness of complementary and alternative medicine? Brit J Health Psych 2010, 15, 599–609. [5] Tobacyk J, Milford G: Beliefs in paranormal phenomena: Assessment instrument development and implication for personality functioning. J Pers Soc Psychol 1983, 44, 1029–1037. [6] Van den Bulck J, Custers K: in complementary and alternative medicine is related to age and paranormal beliefs. Eur J Public Health 2009, 20, 227–230. [7] Costa PTJr, Mc Crae RR: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO- -FFI) professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa 1992. [8] Sirois FM, Purc-Stephenson RJ: Personality and consultations with complementary and alternative medicine practitioners: A five-factor model investigation of degree of use and motives. J Altern Complem Med 2008, 14, 1151–1158. [9] Honda K, Jacobson J: Use of complementary and alternative medicine among United States adults: The influences of personality, coping strategies and social support. Prev Med 2005, 40, 46–53. [10] Furnham A: Are modern health worries, personality and attitudes to science associated with the use of comple- mentary and alternative medicine? Brit J Health Psych 2007, 12, 229–243. [11] Nosal C: Diagnosing mind types: The development and application of Jung’s theory. PWN, Warszawa 1992. [12] Kogan N, Wallach M: Risk taking: A study in cognition and personality. Holt, Reinhart and Winston, New York 1964. [13] Olchowska-Kotala A: Who uses unconventional methods of treatment and why. Adam Marszałek, Toruń 2009. [14] APA. The Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing [Online]; http://www.apa.org/science/programs/ testing/standards.aspx [15.05.2012]. [15] Babiak J, Nosal C: Leaders’ mind types and their relations to preferred leadership styles. Paper presented at Science Technology Higher Education Conceptual Age Conference, July 5–7, 2011, Krakow, Poland (in press). [16] Maskarinec G, Shumay DM, Kakai H: Ethnic differences in complementary and alternative medicine use among cancer patients. J Altern Complem Med 2000, 6, 531–538. [17] Foltz V, St Pierre Y, Rozenberg S: Use of complementary and alternative therapies by patients with self-reported chronic back pain: a nationwide survey in Canada. Joint Bone Spine 2005, 72, 571–577. [18] Lindeman M: Biases in intuitive reasoning and belief in complementary and alternative medicine. Psychol Health 2011, 26, 371–382.

Address for corresspondence: Agnieszka Olchowska-Kotala Department of Humanistic in Medicine Wroclaw Medical University Mikulicza-Radeckiego 7 50-367 Wrocław, Poland Tel.: +48 53 52 18 02 E-mail: [email protected]

Conflict of interest: None declared

Received: 30.10.2012 Revised: 28.01.2013 Accepted: 26.11.2013