<<

CPXXXX10.1177/2167702613482119Jamieson et al.Conceptualization of in Social Disorder research-article4821192013

Empirical Article

Clinical Psychological Science XX(X) 1­–12 Changing the Conceptualization of Stress © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav in Disorder: Affective and DOI: 10.1177/2167702613482119 Physiological Consequences cpx.sagepub.com

Jeremy P. Jamieson1, Matthew K. Nock2, and Wendy Berry Mendes3 1University of Rochester, Rochester, NY; 2Harvard University, Cambridge, MA; and 3University of California, San Francisco

Abstract Social (SAD) is a pervasive problem associated with debilitating impairments in social settings. This research explored the affective and physiological reactions to social evaluation and examined the efficacy of an arousal reappraisal intervention in altering cardiovascular reactivity and affective responses. Across two studies, socially anxious participants exhibited a disjunction between subjective ratings and physiological responses. Whereas anxious individuals reported more anxiety and negative affect during a stressful public speaking task relative to nonanxious controls, no differences emerged in physiological reactivity as a function of anxiety. In the second experiment socially anxious and nonanxious participants instructed to reframe stress arousal as a positive coping tool exhibited reduced attentional bias and improvements in physiological functioning: decreased vasoconstriction and increased cardiac efficiency. This research suggests that outcomes in SAD may be improved by reappraising stress arousal.

Keywords social anxiety, emotion regulation, reappraisal, psychophysiology, replication

Received 10/31/12; Revision accepted 2/6/13

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is among the most preva- 2010; Jamieson, Mendes, & Nock, 2013; Jamieson, Nock, lent of all mental disorders, with an overall lifetime rate of & Mendes, 2012). 12.2% (Kessler et al., 2005). People with SAD perceive that Although stressful evaluative situations commonly elicit they are being negatively evaluated by others, and this feelings of anxiety and negative affect (e.g., Daly, often leads to debilitating impairments during social situ- Vangelisti, & Lawrence, 1989), these outcomes are exacer- ations where there exists some element of evaluation bated in SAD (e.g., Etkin & Wager, 2007). It is interesting, (e.g., dates, interviews, talking to strangers; Stein & Kean, however, that research on acute physiological responses 2000; Stein, Torgrud, & Walker, 2000). The current research to evaluation has yielded inconsistent findings. Some explored the effects of negative social evaluation in a research suggests that socially anxious and nonanxious sample of socially anxious individuals with two goals in individuals exhibit similar physiological responses to eval- mind. The first goal was to assess the subjective, physio- uative threat (e.g., Mauss, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2004), logical, and attentional consequences stemming from whereas others indicate that SAD is associated with mal- negative evaluation in socially anxious and nonanxious adaptive patterns of responding in such situations (e.g., participants to compare possible differences in affective increased blood pressure; Turner, Beidel, & Larkin, 1986). responding. The second goal was to examine the efficacy of a reappraisal intervention that was effective previously Corresponding Author: in non–socially anxious samples to determine if socially Jeremy P. Jamieson, 437 Meliora Hall, University of Rochester, anxious individuals also could benefit from reappraising Rochester, NY 14627 stress arousal (Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, E-mail: [email protected] 2 Jamieson et al.

The discrepancy in the empirical data may hinder the passive and require instrumental cognitive responses development of process-focused and pharmacological such as giving a speech, taking a test, or negotiating. In treatment options because it is unclear if interventions situations like these, individuals must marshal biological should target actual physiological arousal or simply per- and cognitive resources to actively cope with the tasks. ceived arousal. Although some CBT methods include information about the adaptive functions of biological responses (e.g., Stress Appraisals and Physiological Barlow, 2007), the clinical literature has yet to test approaches that seek to maintain adaptive levels of SNS Responses activation during acute stress. Cognitive appraisals play an integral role in the genera- Toward this end, the current research examined the tion and regulation of affective states (e.g., Barrett, potential benefits of reappraising stress arousal in SAD. 2006a; Gross, 2003). Commonly held beliefs imply that The arousal reappraisal perspective seeks to change the the heightened physiological arousal that accompanies conceptualization of stress by informing individuals that stressful situations accumulates and results in poor arousal can be thought of as a resource that can enhance (Geronimus, Hicken, Keene, & Bound, 2006; Matthews performance (cf. Barrett, 2006b; Lindquist & Barrett, 2008; et al., 2004; McEwen, 2006). However, not all increases in Lindquist, Wager, Kober, Bliss-Moreau, & Barrett, 2011). physiological arousal are necessarily deleterious for per- That is, it seeks to promote approach-oriented patterns of formance and health. In addition to associations with responding while maintaining stress arousal necessary for negative outcomes, increases in sympathetic nervous sys- optimal performance. In previous research, participants tem (SNS) activation during active tasks have been linked who reframed arousal as an adaptive coping strategy dur- to benign, approach-related responses (Dienstbier, 1989). ing acute stress exhibited improved physiological and Whether SNS activation is benign or not may depend on cognitive outcomes (Jamieson et al., 2010; Jamieson et al., how the individual appraises the demands of the stressful 2012). Reframing stress arousal is hypothesized to increase situation and his or her ability to cope. appraisals of coping resources, which are predicted to The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat attenuate threat responses and promote a more adaptive provides a theory of how appraisals of situational profile of reactivity in socially anxious individuals. demands interact with appraisals of available coping However, before testing the impact of arousal reappraisal, resources to determine stress responses (see Blascovich we first sought to better understand the physiological and & Mendes, 2010, for a review). Both challenge and threat affective reactivity that accompanies social evaluative states are experienced during situations of acute stress but threat in individuals with SAD. differ in antecedent processes and downstream responses. Individuals experience challenge when appraisals of per- Experiment 1 sonal resources exceed situational demands. Alternatively, threat manifests when perceptions of demands exceed Individuals with SAD exhibit heightened self-reported resources. Although both states are accompanied by negative affect (Etkin & Wager, 2007; Goldin, Manber, increases in SNS activation, challenge is characterized by Hakimi, Canli, & Gross, 2009; Watson, Clark, & Carey, improved cardiac efficiency and dilation of the peripheral 1988) and stronger attentional capture for threat cues in vasculature, whereas threat decreases cardiac efficiency the face of negative evaluation compared to less anxious and constricts the vasculature in anticipation of damage individuals (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans- or defeat. Thus, modifying appraisals may promote adap- Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007; MacLeod, Rutherford, tive patterns of responding. Campbell, Ebsworthy, & Holker, 2002). However, the link between anxiety and physiological responses to evaluative situations is not as straightforward. Whereas many studies Arousal Reappraisal have observed differences in physiological reactivity Changing cognitive appraisals, or reappraising, is a between anxious and nonanxious individuals (e.g., centerpiece of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). Condren, O’Neill, Ryan, Barrett, & Thakore, 2002; Davidson, Reappraisal, as characterized in the clinical literature, Marshall, Tomarken, & Henriques, 2000; McTeague et al., typically either decreases arousal (e.g., mindfulness med- 2009; Roelofs et al., 2009; Stein, Asmundson, & Chartier, itation; Cincotta, Gehrman, Gooneratne, & Baime, 2011) 1994; Turner et al., 1986), others have found no effects or teaches individuals to accept high arousal (e.g., intero- (e.g., Edelmann & Baker, 2002; Grossman, Wilhelm, ceptive exposure; Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004), Kawachi, & Sparrow, 2001; Krämer et al., 2012; Schmitz, but the context in which the arousal is experienced is Krämer, Tuschen-Caffier, Heinrichs, & Blechert, 2011). critical in determining whether arousal is benign or Methodological differences across studies may con- malignant. Social evaluative situations typically are not tribute to these inconsistencies. For instance, a standard Conceptualization of Stress in 3 cross-sectional design exposes a group of anxious and afternoon/evening with start times between 12 p.m. and nonanxious individuals to a stressful task—often the Trier 6 p.m. Participants were compensated with $25. Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, The socially anxious group (n = 33) consisted of indi- 1993)—and examines the group differences in reactivity. viduals meeting the diagnostic criteria for SAD as deter- This approach may overstate the effect of anxiety on mined by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric physiological reactions if controls are selected only from Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998). Nonanxious con- the “nonanxious tail” of the anxiety spectrum. Consistent trols (n = 34) also completed the MINI and did not meet with this notion, in studies that have examined the rela- the criteria for any of the anxiety or mood disorders tionship between social anxiety and physiological assessed. There were no differences between groups on responses to evaluation in subclinical samples, most have age, sex, or race/ethnicity. observed no differences as a function of anxiety (Baggett, Saab, & Carver, 1996; Eckman & Shean, 1997; Mauss, Procedure. Upon arrival, participants provided consent Wilhelm, & Gross, 2003, 2004) versus significant relation- for the physiological portion of the experiment and com- ships (Shimizu, Seery, Weisbuch, & Lupien, 2011). On the pleted an initial set of questionnaires (see the Question- flip side, studying group differences using only evalua- naires section). The experimenter then affixed sensors, tive tasks (i.e., without no-evaluation control conditions) and participants relaxed for a 5-min baseline recording. misses information gleaned from a group’s baseline. After baseline, participants heard a description of the Experiment 1 sought to address some of these factors by TSST, provided a verbal consent, and were assigned to crossing a stress induction (the TSST with and without one of two evaluation conditions: social evaluation or no evaluation) with anxiety level (clinically anxious vs. sub- evaluation. Those subject to social evaluation delivered a clinical controls), with the nonanxious participants falling 5-min videotaped speech about their strengths and weak- anywhere below the threshold for clinical anxiety. Then nesses to two interviewers who provided negative non- we examined physiological reactivity, as well as raw verbal feedback throughout. No-evaluation controls baseline scores, during stress anticipation, throughout delivered the same speech but did so alone and believed the TSST, and during a recovery period. nobody could see or hear them. Prior to speaking, par- Participants assigned to receive negative evaluation ticipants were given 3 min to prepare. An impromptu during the TSST were expected to display a physiological 5-min mental arithmetic task (counting backward in steps threat response and exhibit vigilance for threat cues rela- of 7 from 996) followed the speech. In the evaluation tive to those assigned to the no evaluation condition. We condition, the interviewers again provided negative feed- were agnostic about the effects of social anxiety on phys- back. Controls completed the task alone. iological responses given the inconsistencies in the litera- Participants then completed post-TSST questionnaires ture, though we did expect the socially anxious and performed an emotional Stroop task (Williams, participants to exhibit more attentional bias than less Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996) to measure attentional bias. anxious individuals. Finally, we tested the interaction Participants were asked to name the font color (red, between social anxiety and evaluation to examine green, or blue) of words as quickly and accurately as whether potential differences between anxious and non- possible. Words were printed in two lists of 100 (one list anxious participants were greater in the negative evalua- comprised threat words and the other list comprised neu- tion versus no evaluation condition. tral words) and were presented in four columns of 25 words each. Participants were instructed to read down Method columns from left to right. Order was counterbalanced, and an experimenter unaware of condition assignment Participants. Participants were 72 adults (42 women, recorded errors and how long it took participants to read 30 men; M age = 26.63) recruited from the community via each list. Interference scores were computed by subtract- flyers, Harvard University’s study pool, and postings on ing the time it took participants to read the neutral list Craigslist.org (Boston, MA location). Participants were from their time on the negative list. excluded for , the presence of a pacemaker, cardiac medications, body mass index (BMI) scores Physiological measures. The following measures greater than 33, and pregnancy. Participants were also were collected at baseline, throughout the TSST (prepa- asked if they were taking any prescription medications at ration period, speech, and mental arithmetic), and during the time of the study. Of participants, 43% reported taking a 3-min recovery period immediately following the TSST: at least one medication (18 anxious, 13 nonanxious). In electrocardiography (ECG), impedance cardiography all, 5 participants (3 socially anxious, 2 nonanxious) asked (ICG), and blood pressure. Signals were integrated with to stop participation prior to study completion and were Biopac MP100 hardware. ECG and ICG signals were excluded from analyses. Sessions were completed in the scored offline by trained personnel. Signals were visually 4 Jamieson et al.

examined, and the ensembled averages were analyzed Results using Mindware software. Reactivity scores were com- puted by subtracting scores taken during the final minute Questionnaires of baseline from those collected during the first minute of Social anxiety. As expected, analysis of the BAI indi- the preparation, speech, and math periods. Analyses cated that socially anxious participants reported higher focused on pre-ejection period (PEP)—a measure of levels of anxiety (M = 1.03, SD = 0.67) than nonanxious sympathetic activation—and two measures that allow controls (M = 0.53, SD = 0.58), F(1, 63) = 10.10, p = .002, distinction between challenge and threat: cardiac output d = 0.80. Likewise, socially anxious participants reported (CO) and total peripheral resistance (TPR). more anxiety (M = 3.97, SD = 0.51) than controls (M = PEP indexes the contractile force of the heart by mea- 2.30, SD = 0.53) on the IAS, F(1, 62) = 161.84, p < .001, suring the time from the initiation of left ventricle con- d = 3.23. traction to aortic valve opening. Greater sympathetic activation is indicated by shorter PEP intervals. CO is the Stress appraisals. Anxious participants indicated they amount of blood ejected from the heart during 1 min and possessed fewer coping resources (M = 3.88, SD = 0.92) is calculated by estimating volume (the amount of compared to nonanxious participants (M = 4.70, SD = blood ejected per beat) and multiplying by heart rate. 0.75), F(1, 63) = 16.90, p < .001, d = 1.03. Analysis of Increases in CO indicate improved cardiac efficiency and resources also produced a marginal effect for evaluation, typically are observed in challenge (Mendes, Blascovich, F(1, 63) = 3.31, p = .073, d = 0.46. Participants subject Lickel, & Hunter, 2002). TPR is a measure of overall vaso- to social evaluation reported marginally fewer resources constriction/vasodilation. When threatened, the vascula- (M = 4.12, SD = 1.03) compared to those not evaluated ture constricts so as to limit blood flow to the periphery, (M = 4.47, SD = 0.79). producing high TPR scores. TPR was calculated with the Analysis of demand appraisals produced main effects following formula: (mean arterial pressure / CO) × 80. for anxiety, F(1, 63) = 41.06, p < .001, d = 1.61, and evalu- ation, F(1, 63) = 10.60, p = .002, d = 0.82, as well as an Questionnaires Anxiety × Evaluation interaction, F(1, 63) = 4.10, p = .047, Social anxiety. The Interaction Anxiousness Scale (IAS; d = 0.51. Contrasts indicate that anxious participants Leary & Kowalski, 1993) and the Beck Anxiety Inven- experienced the task as more demanding (M = 3.87, tory (BAI; Steer & Beck, 1997) were administered prior SD = 0.76) than nonanxious participants in the no evalua- to baseline. tion condition (M = 2.93, SD = 1.08), F(1, 63) = 9.40, p = .003, d = 0.77, and this difference was stronger in the eval- Stress appraisals. Participants completed a resource/ uation condition (Manxious = 5.00, SD = 0.89; Mnonanxious = demand appraisal questionnaire (Mendes, Gray, Mendoza- 3.87, SD = 0.73), F(1, 63) = 34.85, p < .001, d = 1.49. Denton, Major, & Epel, 2007) pre- and post-TSST. Com- posites of demands (Cronbach’s α = .79) and resources Physiological measures. We first analyzed raw physi- (Cronbach’s α = .74) were computed across time point. ological signals (PEP, CO, and TPR) at baseline with BMI included as a covariate to examine whether anxious and Data analytic plan. Unless otherwise noted, data nonanxious participants exhibited baseline differences were analyzed in 2 (Anxiety: socially anxious vs. nonanx- that might mask reactivity effects. Analysis of PEP, CO, ious) × 2 (Evaluation: social evaluation vs. no evaluation) and TPR yielded only one effect. Socially anxious partici- between-subjects ANOVAs. A total of 14 socially anxious pants exhibited less SNS activation (higher PEP scores) at participants (42.42% of the anxious group) reported a baseline (M = 115.51 ms, SD = 11.61 ms) relative to less history of . Research suggests that comorbid anxious participants (M = 111.17 ms, SD = 10.72 ms), F(1, depression moderates the association between social 61) = 4.15, p = .046, d = 0.52. However, this effect runs anxiety and physiology (Yoon & Joormann, 2012). We counter to the expected direction. included self-reported depression history (scored as a Physiological reactivity data were then analyzed in 2 bivariate variable), medication status (scored as a bivari- (Anxiety) × 2 (Evaluation) × 4 (Time: preparation period ate variable), and the date since females’ previous men- vs. speech vs. mental arithmetic vs. recovery) mixed strual cycle as covariates. However, none of these ANCOVAs with time as a within-subjects factor and BMI covariates affected the pattern of results, and they are included as a covariate. See Table 1 for means and stan- thus not reported. dard deviations. One participant did not complete all IAS items and Analysis of PEP reactivity produced main effects for was not included in that analysis. Due to equipment time, F(1, 61) = 7.33, p = .009, d = 0.69, and evaluation, problems, the physiological data from two anxious par- F(1, 61) = 26.32, p < .001, d = 1.31. Sympathetic reactivity ticipants assigned to the no evaluation condition could was higher during the more metabolically demanding not be analyzed. tasks (speech and mental math) versus the less active Conceptualization of Stress in Social Anxiety Disorder 5

Table 1. Physiological Reactivity as a Function of Evaluation Condition and Anxiety Level

Preparation period Speech delivery Mental arithmetic Recovery

Condition M SD M SD M SD M SD Pre-ejection period (ms) No evaluation, nonanxious –1.76 3.93 –3.74 7.04 0.29 5.72 0.88 5.37 No evaluation, socially anxious –2.00 4.93 –3.33 6.73 –1.33 8.55 –1.00 7.12 Negative evaluation, nonanxious –5.12 7.91 –13.54 12.20 –12.65 11.20 –3.82 8.20 Negative evaluation, socially anxious –10.01 8.15 –15.00 8.94 –12.57 9.13 –6.56 5.69 Cardiac output (liters/minute) No evaluation, nonanxious 0.15 0.87 0.48 1.09 0.13 1.08 –0.22 0.78 No evaluation, socially anxious 0.05 0.46 0.45 0.83 0.16 0.69 0.05 0.75 Negative evaluation, nonanxious –0.04 0.72 0.39 0.90 0.44 1.25 –0.36 1.17 Negative evaluation, socially anxious 0.12 1.01 0.45 0.90 0.46 1.75 0.01 0.51 Total peripheral resistance (dyne-sec × cm–5) No evaluation, nonanxious 18.66 85.68 51.33 155.27 58.32 105.78 40.28 106.55 No evaluation, socially anxious 8.19 95.64 14.43 67.58 30.29 105.16 34.70 114.80 Negative evaluation, nonanxious 100.33 120.29 216.84 152.46 216.84 177.93 178.76 202.48 Negative evaluation, socially anxious 122.63 123.89 189.37 160.65 189.37 160.80 159.84 172.96

recovery period, F(1, 61) = 7.98, p = .006, d = 0.72, and participants subject to evaluation exhibited more sympa- thetic arousal compared to those who were not evaluated. We then examined the impact of anxiety and evalua- tion on CO. This analysis produced only a main effect for time, F(1, 61) = 5.33, p = .024, d = 0.59. CO reactivity was greater during the speech and math tasks versus recov- ery, F(1, 61) = 4.75, p = .033, d = 0.56. Finally, analysis of TPR produced the predicted main effect for evaluation, F(1, 61) = 26.31, p < .001, d = 1.31. Replicating previous work (e.g., Kassam, Koslov, & Mendes, 2009), partici- pants subject to social evaluation exhibited greater vaso- constriction relative to nonevaluated participants. The data indicate that, in general, social evaluation (vs. no evaluation) produced a threat pattern of respond- ing: sympathetic activation, little or no change in CO, and increased vascular resistance. These effects not only were observed when the evaluators were present, but also emerged during anticipation of speaking and lasted into recovery. It is interesting that no differences in reactivity emerged as a function of anxiety. Consistent with this Fig. 1. Attentional bias as a function of anxiety level and evaluation notion, neither IAS nor BAI reports were correlated with condition. Error bars represent +/– standard error of the mean. any of the physiological reactivity measures, ps > .21. d = 0.53, and socially anxious participants were margin- Attentional bias. The emotional Stoop assessed par- ally more vigilant than non–socially anxious participants, ticipants’ vigilance for threat cues, and consistent with F(1, 63) = 3.69, p = .059, d = 0.48. predictions, we observed the two main effects (see These attention effects cannot be attributed to a speed- Fig. 1). Participants subject to social evaluation were accuracy trade-off as neither anxiety nor evaluation more vigilant for threat cues compared to those in affected the number of errors participants made on the the no-evaluation condition, F(1, 63) = 4.37, p = .040, neutral (overall M = 0.76, SD = 1.32) or emotionally 6 Jamieson et al. negative word lists (overall M = 1.04, SD = 1.15), ps > .20. individuals, but the anxious and nonanxious groups’ Further examination of the relationship between anxiety physiological reactivity was not significantly different. It and attentional bias revealed a correlation between IAS is important to note that any conclusion regarding the reports and attention. Participants reporting more social lack of physiological reactivity effects is preliminary anxiety exhibited greater threat-related attentional bias, because it is based on interpreting null results on a rela- β = .276, p = .035. tively small sample. However, these data are informative because they suggest a disjunction between socially anx- Discussion ious individuals’ biological responses and their self- reported perceptions of affective states. We capitalized Consistent with published research, social evaluation on this pattern in Experiment 2 by exploring if changing produced a threat pattern of physiological responding the way individuals appraised a stressful socially evalua- (e.g., Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Salomon, 1999). tive experience would result in shifts in physiological However, we observed no differences in autonomic reac- reactivity and threat vigilance. tivity between the SAD group and less anxious controls. Moreover, individual differences in anxiety reports were Experiment 2 not associated with physiological responses to negative evaluation. Socially anxious individuals’ cardiovascular In Experiment 1 both socially anxious and nonanxious responses to social evaluation were similar to those of participants subjected to social evaluation exhibited a nonanxious individuals. The lack of statistically signifi- maladaptive pattern of physiological responding and cant differences as a function of anxiety, however, does greater attentional bias compared to the nonevaluation not suggest that socially anxious participants responded controls. These types of physiological responses impair adaptively to evaluative threat. Rather, all participants dis- decision making and are associated with negative health played a threat pattern of reactivity. outcomes (Jefferson et al., 2010; Kassam, Koslov, & In contrast, anxiety and evaluation conditions signifi- Mendes, 2009; Matthews, Gump, Block, & Allen, 1997), cantly affected attentional bias and self-report measures. and attentional bias plays an integral role in the manifes- Socially anxious participants exhibited more attentional tation and maintenance of anxiety (Mathews & MacLeod, bias than did nonanxious participants. Likewise, partici- 2002). In Experiment 2, we tested a strategy aimed at pants assigned to receive social evaluation were more improving these outcomes by shifting appraisals of stress vigilant for emotionally negative information compared arousal. to those not evaluated. This pattern is consistent with Experiment 2 used a design similar to that of Experiment meta-analytic data demonstrating that anxiety is associ- 1 but replaced the no evaluation condition with a reap- ated with biased attention for threat cues (Bar-Haim praisal condition. We hypothesized that reappraising et al., 2007). On the other hand, some previous work arousal would yield three benefits. First, instructing partici- suggests that social anxiety is associated with less atten- pants that stress arousal is functional was expected to tion to threat cues (Amir et al., 1996). However, in that increase appraisals of coping resources. Second, improve- research, anxious participants completed the attention ments in resource/demand appraisals were expected to measure prior to a stressful public speaking task during translate into more adaptive physiological responses. an anticipatory period, whereas here it was completed Specifically, relative to no-reappraisal controls, arousal following the stress task. Preparation takes effort that reappraisal instructions were hypothesized to maintain may suppress attentional bias given that increased cogni- SNS activation (i.e., no change in PEP) and promote tive effort can tax resources (Beilock, Rydell, & McConnell, approach-oriented physiological responses (i.e., decreased 2007). However, in the current research participants had TPR and increased CO). Finally, we hypothesized that no need to use cognitive resources to plan the speech reappraisal would reduce attentional bias, which has because the stress task was over. implications for future experiences of anxiety. Evaluation and anxiety also altered appraisals. In addition to testing the effectiveness of the arousal Evaluated participants reported an increase in situational reappraisal manipulation, the design of Experiment 2 demands and a decrease in their available resources to allowed for an exact replication of Experiment 1. That is, cope with the stressor, with socially anxious participants in the evaluative stress, no instruction condition, the exhibiting an exacerbated pattern (lower resource physiological reactivity of socially anxious and nonanx- appraisals, higher demand appraisals) compared to non- ious participants could again be compared. If we again anxious individuals. observe no differences as a function of anxiety, we can Experiment 1 indicates that socially anxious individu- be more confident in the conclusions from Experiment 1 als self-reported more anxiety and behaviorally exhibited that the magnitude of autonomic responses to social greater vigilance for threat cues compared to less anxious evaluation may not differ across the anxiety spectrum. Conceptualization of Stress in Social Anxiety Disorder 7

Method a recovery period, post-TSST questionnaires, and the emotional Stroop task, in that order. Participants. Participants were 73 adults (45 women, 28 men; M age = 25.82 years) recruited using the same Physiological measures and questionnaires. The procedures as described in Study 1. Participants were physiological signals collected and questionnaires were also asked if they were taking any prescription medica- identical to those in Experiment 1. tions at the time of the study. Of participants, 48% reported taking medication (21 anxious, 14 nonanxious). Data analytic plan. Unless otherwise noted, data In all, 4 participants (3 socially anxious, 1 nonanxious, all were analyzed in 2 (Anxiety: socially anxious vs. nonanx- assigned to the no instruction condition) asked to stop ious) × 2 (Intervention: reappraisal vs. no instructions) participation prior to the conclusion of the experiment. between-subjects ANOVAs. A total of 11 socially anxious Participants were compensated with $25. participants (7 reappraisal, 4 no instructions; 33.33% of The socially anxious group (n = 34) comprised indi- the final anxiety sample) reported past depression, and viduals meeting diagnostic criteria for SAD according to again this was included as a covariate but did not affect the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1998). Nonanxious controls the results. As in Experiment 1, we also included self- (n = 35) did not meet the criteria for SAD or any other reported medication status (scored as a bivariate vari- anxiety or . There were no differences able) and the date since females’ previous menstrual between anxiety and intervention groups on age, sex, or cycle as covariates. However, these covariates did not ethnicity. affect the pattern of results and are not reported. One participant did not fully complete the BAI, IAS, or Procedure. The procedure was identical to that in resource/demand questionnaire, and this participant’s Experiment 1 except that the no evaluation condition data were excluded from those analyses. Because of a was replaced with a reappraisal manipulation. All partici- problem with the BP monitor, TPR was not computed for pants were evaluated during the TSST, and socially anx- one participant. Three color-blind participants could not ious and nonanxious participants were assigned to complete the Stroop task. receive either reappraisal instructions or no instructions prior to the evaluative task. The reappraisal instructions informed participants (19 anxious, 18 nonanxious) about Results the functionality of stress responses. Specifically, partici- pants were told, Questionnaires Social anxiety. As expected, socially anxious individuals In stressful situations, like public speaking, our exhibited higher BAI scores (M = 1.08, SD = 0.63) than bodies react in very specific ways. The increase in their less anxious counterparts (M = 0.34, SD = 0.45), arousal you may feel during stress is not harmful. F(1, 63) = 32.07, p < .001, d = 1.43. The anxious partici- Instead, these responses evolved to help our pants also reported more anxiety on the IAS (M = 4.03, ancestors survive by delivering oxygen to where it SD = 0.52) compared to controls (M = 2.37, SD = 0.52), is needed in the body. We encourage you to F(1, 63) = 173.20, p < .001, d = 3.32. reinterpret your bodily signals during the upcoming public speaking task as beneficial. Stress appraisals. Socially anxious participants reported possessing fewer resources to cope with evaluation (M = After oral instructions, participants read three summa- 4.12, SD = 0.87) than nonanxious participants (M = 4.84, ries of scientific journal articles (that we created) outlin- SD = 0.69), F(1, 63) = 14.75, p < .001, d = 0.97. Supporting ing the positive outcomes associated with stress arousal predictions, reappraisal instructions increased appraisals and the adaptive benefits of stress (also see Jamieson et of coping resources (M = 4.68, SD = 0.87) relative to no al., 2012). Each summary was followed by two questions instruction controls (M = 4.26, SD = 0.80), F(1, 63) = 5.38, that required participants to endorse the information pre- p = .024, d = 0.58. There was no interaction, so the net sented before moving on. increase in resource appraisals following reappraisal was Participants in the no-reappraisal condition (18 anx- similar across anxiety level. ious, 18 nonanxious) were given no additional instruc- Analysis of situational demands produced only a main tions and instead completed a nondemanding task (a effect for anxiety, F(1, 63) = 35.39, p < .001, d = 1.56. The driving game; Mather, Gorlick, & Lighthall, 2009) to con- socially anxious individuals indicated that TSST was more trol for time. Instructions and the game took 10 to 15 min demanding (M = 4.58, SD = 1.09) compared to nonanx- to complete, after which participants completed the TSST, ious participants (M = 3.14, SD = 0.91). In sum, the 8 Jamieson et al. reappraisal manipulation boosted perceptions of coping The overall pattern of data indicates that reinterpreting resources, but did not affect appraisals of task demands. stress arousal helped attenuate threat responses in antici- pation of, during, and in recovery from the TSST. The Physiological measures. We first analyzed raw physi- PEP findings indicate that reappraisal improved respond- ological signals (PEP, CO, and TPR) at baseline with BMI ing without decreasing sympathetic activation. Moreover, included as a covariate to examine whether baseline dif- none of the physiological reactivity measures produced a ferences between groups might mask reactivity effects. main effect for anxiety, nor was the Anxiety × Reappraisal We observed no significant effects of anxiety or interven- interaction significant; the physiological benefits of reap- tion conditions on baseline PEP (overall M = 111.69 ms, praisal were observed in both anxious and nonanxious SD = 11.35), CO (overall M = 7.51 l/min, SD = 1.67), and individuals. TPR (overall M = 913.29 dyne-sec/cm–5, SD = 236.48) We also explored whether improvements in appraisals scores, Fs < 1. predicted the change in physiology. To do so, we first Physiological reactivity was analyzed in 2 (Anxiety) × 2 computed a physiological index by averaging standard- (Intervention) × 4 (Time: preparation period vs. speech vs. ized CO and standardized, reverse-scored TPR scores mental arithmetic vs. recovery) mixed ANCOVAs with time (PEP is nondiagnostic of stress type). Increases corre- as a within-subjects factor and BMI included as a covariate. spond to adaptive physiological reactions. Higher resource See Table 2 for means and standard deviations. appraisals measured prior to the speech were associated Socially anxious participants exhibited less sympa- with improvements in physiological reactivity during pub- thetic reactivity overall versus their nonanxious counter- lic speaking for both anxious and nonanxious participants parts, F(1, 64) = 6.47, p = .013, d = 0.65. Analysis of PEP assigned to the reappraisal condition, β = .326, p = .011 also yielded a main effect for time, F(1, 64) = 8.62, p = (see the supplemental online material for additional anal- .005, d = 0.75, with the more metabolically demanding yses and scatterplots). aspects of the TSST eliciting greater SNS activation. We then examined the data to determine if we repli- Reappraisal had no impact on SNS activation. cated the null findings observed in Experiment 1. Pairwise Consistent with predictions, participants assigned to contrasts compared PEP, CO, and TPR reactivity of anx- reappraise arousal exhibited an increase in CO versus ious and nonanxious participants in the no instruction those given no instructions, F(1, 64) = 4.77, p = .032, d = condition at every time point. The only effects approach- 0.55. Reframing arousal as functional was associated with ing significance were PEP reactivity during the speech, improved cardiac efficiency. Also in accord with predic- F(1, 64) = 2.90, p = .093, d = 0.42, and CO reactivity at tions, reappraisal participants exhibited a decrease in recovery, F(1, 64) = 2.66, p = .108, d = 0.41, but these vascular resistance relative to their no instruction coun- trends suggested that socially anxious participants had terparts, F(1, 64) = 9.26, p = .003, d = 0.76. less, not more, sympathetic activation and better, not

Table 2. Physiological Reactivity as a Function of Reappraisal Condition and Anxiety Level

Preparation period Speech delivery Mental arithmetic Recovery

Condition M SD M SD M SD M SD Pre-ejection period (ms) No instruction, nonanxious –10.10 7.71 –17.06 9.36 –15.59 10.14 –5.29 8.38 No instruction, socially anxious –7.81 7.06 –12.81 7.73 –13.43 11.21 –5.94 7.79 Reappraisal, nonanxious –15.29 9.16 –20.29 9.43 –18.24 8.09 –7.94 6.39 Reappraisal, socially anxious –9.44 8.02 –11.67 8.04 –10.56 9.06 –5.00 8.91

Cardiac output (liters/minute) No instruction, nonanxious 0.38 1.30 0.53 1.51 0.88 1.60 –0.37 0.81 No instruction, socially anxious 0.25 0.91 0.32 1.24 0.74 1.59 0.08 0.84 Reappraisal, nonanxious 0.89 0.93 1.13 1.23 1.07 1.20 0.02 0.63 Reappraisal, socially anxious 0.81 1.18 1.44 1.57 1.41 1.65 0.24 1.19 Total peripheral resistance (dyne-sec × cm–5) No instruction, nonanxious 83.42 111.80 208.01 180.41 151.51 152.22 134.00 151.16 No instruction, socially anxious 106.11 132.09 240.10 195.23 137.21 202.30 97.59 138.56 Reappraisal, nonanxious 6.18 103.05 105.73 140.48 114.97 183.93 81.13 135.99 Reappraisal, socially anxious 28.21 158.48 49.41 166.40 37.56 171.53 7.73 93.22 Conceptualization of Stress in Social Anxiety Disorder 9

worse, cardiac efficiency versus less anxious participants, pattern of data observed provided tentative evidence of a respectively (all other ps > .23). To further examine pos- disjunction between physiological responses and subjec- sible differences, we analyzed raw baseline signals. tive experiences in socially anxious individuals. Again, no effects emerged as a function of anxiety for It is important that Experiment 2 tested the potential PEP, CO, or TPR (ps > .22). benefits of a reappraisal intervention that targeted per- ceptions of stress arousal in SAD. Supporting predictions, Attentional bias. Analysis of attentional bias produced both socially anxious and nonanxious participants main effects for anxiety, F(1, 61) = 4.22, p = .044, d = 0.53, assigned to reappraise arousal as functional exhibited and reappraisal, F(1, 61) = 7.32, p = .009, d = 0.69 (see improvements in resource appraisals, physiological Fig. 2). Socially anxious individuals exhibited more atten- responses, and attention. That is, reappraisal participants tional bias for emotionally negative information com- appraised more resources to cope with evaluation than pared to less anxious participants, and participants those provided no instruction. As predicted, arousal reap- assigned to reappraise arousal were less vigilant for threat praisal did affect perceptions of situational demands. cues compared to those not given instructions. Reframing That is, reappraisal participants still perceived the public the meaning of stress arousal decreased attentional bias speaking situation as demanding but believed they pos- similarly in both anxious and nonanxious individuals. sessed greater coping resources compared to those These findings cannot be attributed to a speed-accuracy receiving no instructions. Consistent with the stress trade-off, as neither anxiety nor intervention condition appraisal findings, reappraisal instructions also helped to influenced errors made on the threat (overall M = 0.84, improve physiological responses to evaluative stress, SD = 1.13) or neutral lists (overall M = 0.74, SD = 1.10), while maintaining adaptive levels of sympathetic activa- ps > .24. tion. Finally, reappraisal decreased vigilance for emotion- ally negative information, which has the potential to Discussion reduce the likelihood that individuals will experience anxiety in the future. The no-reappraisal condition in Experiment 2 served as an exact replication for the social evaluation condition in Experiment 1. Again, analyses revealed no significant dif- General Discussion ferences in physiological reactivity as a function of anxi- This research examined the impact of negative evalua- ety. Just as before, though, socially anxious participants tion on physiology and attention in SAD and tested the diverged from less anxious individuals in their reports of potential benefits of reappraising stress arousal. There anxiety, self-report appraisals, and attentional bias. The are two noteworthy findings from this research. First, socially anxious and nonanxious participants assigned to reappraise stress arousal exhibited increased resource appraisals, a more adaptive profile of physiological reac- tivity, and reduced vigilance for threat cues compared to no instruction controls. Second, no differences in physi- ological reactivity were observed as a function of social anxiety in either study. All participants, regardless of anx- iety level, assigned to receive negative evaluation during a public speaking task exhibited a threat profile of physi- ological reactivity. It may seem surprising that altering appraisals of stress arousal is sufficient to change responses to negative eval- uation in SAD, but the overarching concept is not new. Theorists have speculated for years that humans can cog- nitively control stress responses (e.g., Schachter & Singer, 1962). Arousal reappraisal shares the same underlying theme—changing cognitions produces downstream benefits—as emotion regulation and CBT (e.g., Gross, 2003; Hofmann & Smits, 2008), but also differs in impor- tant ways from previous reappraisal-based approaches. The aim of arousal reappraisal is not to encourage Fig. 2. Attentional bias as a function of anxiety level and intervention socially anxious individuals to distance themselves from condition. Error bars represent +/– standard error of the mean. threats or to reframe the meaning of anxiety-provoking 10 Jamieson et al. situations, as is common in the emotion regulation litera- is, we were unable to quantify depressive symptoms par- ture (e.g., Kross & Ayduk, 2011; Ochsner & Gross, 2008). ticipants may have exhibited during the study, which is Nor does arousal reappraisal encourage socially anxious potentially important as socially anxious individuals’ individuals to “relax” during evaluative situations (e.g., physiological reactions to evaluation may depend on Beck, Stanley, Baldwin, Deagle, & Averill, 1994; Goldin & depression comorbidity (Yoon & Joormann, 2012). Future Gross, 2010; Rubia, 2009). Rather, this perspective focuses research might consider examining the potential benefits on altering perceptions of stress arousal so as to improve of arousal reappraisal in socially anxious individuals with responses by maintaining sympathetic activation. This and without comorbid depression. notion is consistent with the idea of physiological tough- In addition, this study cannot speak to the long-term ness, which suggests that SNS activation facilitates effec- effects of arousal reappraisal on outcomes. Rather, this tive coping and improves performance during acute research provides empirical evidence for the potential stress (Dienstbier, 1989). In more recent research, adap- benefits of arousal reappraisal in a single-session labo- tive patterns of SNS responding during a TSST occurred ratory experiment. Related to this limitation, the current after an intranasal dose of oxytocin, which was associ- research did not examine the efficacy of reappraisal ated with improved social functioning (Kubzansky, outside the laboratory. It is our hope that follow-up Mendes, Appleton, Block, & Adler, 2012). research will extend this research to clinical settings and We must note, however, that arousal reappraisal is not seek to study possible long-term benefits of arousal a “silver bullet” for treating SAD, but is just one of the reappraisal in a longitudinal (or at least a multisession) many tools clinicians have at their disposal. For instance, design. attentional bias and affective responses can also be modi- On a more meta level, these data suggest an approach fied by changing how attention is allocated, as demon- that may help clinicians maximize the benefits of treat- strated in the attention retraining literature. One such ments. The first step in any prevention-focused treatment retraining method uses a dot-probe paradigm that approach is to identify the mechanisms (biological, psy- instructs participants to attend to a cue that predicts the chological, and situational) that underlie psychopathol- location of a nonthreatening target (e.g., Amir, Weber, ogy. Too often intervention research is concerned only Beard, Bomyea, & Taylor, 2008). By repeatedly orienting with improving outcomes rather than understanding the attention to neutral targets, the threat-related target loses process that produces those improvements. The experi- its saliency. The subsequent reduction in attentional bias mental procedures such as those used here are important then predicts improved functioning in clinically anxious tools that may help clinical researchers identify and test individuals (Amir et al., 2008). the mechanisms of change in preventative interventions Some important limitations should be considered (Kazdin, 2011; Kazdin & Nock, 2003). when interpreting the current research. For one, the extant literature is not clear on the relationship between Declaration of Conflicting Interests social anxiety and physiological responses to evaluative The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest threat. In addition to methodological issues related to with respect to their authorship or the publication of this control groups, stress inductions, and the use of reactivity article. scores that the current research sought to clarify, there remain factors that warrant consideration. For one, the References specific physiological measures used in a study should Amir, N., McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., Burns, J., Lorenz, M., be taken into account. Although some SAD research has & Mullen, J. T. (1996). Suppression of the emotional Stroop examined endocrine responses (), others, like the effect by increased anxiety in patients with social phobia. studies presented here, have relied on autonomic mea- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(11), 945–948. sures (e.g., vascular resistance). Though each measure Amir, N., Weber, G., Beard, C., Bomyea, J., & Taylor, C. T. has been linked to psychological threat states, there is (2008). The effect of a single-session attention modifica- not a “one-to-one” relationship. Although the current tion program on response to a public-speaking chal- data provide evidence that autonomic responses to nega- lenge in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal tive evaluation do not differ as a function of social anxi- , 117(4), 860–868. ety, this research cannot resolve all inconsistencies in the Baggett, H. L., Saab, P. G., & Carver, C. S. (1996). Appraisal, coping, task performance, and cardiovascular responses literature. Additional work is needed to examine poten- during the evaluated speaking task. Personality and Social tial moderators. Psychology Bulletin, 22(5), 483–494. One potentially noteworthy moderator is comorbid Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, depression. Although efforts were made to control for M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related atten- depression in this research, participants did not complete tional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta- a measure of depression during either experiment. That analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24. Conceptualization of Stress in Social Anxiety Disorder 11

Barlow, D. H. (2007). Clinical handbook of psychological dis- Goldin, P. R., Manber, T., Hakimi, S., Canli, T., & Gross, J. J. orders: A step-by-step treatment manual. New York, NY: (2009). Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: Emotional Guilford. reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physi- Barrett, L. F. (2006a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives cal threat. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(2), 170– on Psychological Science, 1(1), 28–58. 180. Barrett, L. F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291. and Review, 10(1), 20–46. Grossman, P., Wilhelm, F. H., Kawachi, I., & Sparrow, D. (2001). Beck, J. G., Stanley, M. A., Baldwin, L. E., Deagle, E. A., & Gender differences in psychophysiological responses to Averill, P. M. (1994). Comparison of cognitive therapy and speech stress among older social phobics: Congruence and relaxation training for panic disorder. Journal of Consulting incongruence between self-evaluative and cardiovascular and , 62(4), 818–826. reactions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(5), 765–777. Beilock, S. L., Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2007). Stereotype Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral threat and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and therapy for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of spillover. Journal of : General, randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical 136(2), 256–276. Psychiatry, 69(4), 621–632. Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2010). Social psychophysiol- Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, ogy and embodiment. In S. T. Fiske & D. T. Gilbert (Eds.), T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: The handbook of social psychology (pp. 194–227). New Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. York, NY: John Wiley. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 208– Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Salomon, K. 212. (1999). Social “facilitation” as challenge and threat. Journal Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., & Nock, M. K. (2013). Improving of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(1), 68–77. acute stress responses: The power of reappraisal. Current Cincotta, A. L., Gehrman, P., Gooneratne, N. S., & Baime, M. J. Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 51–56. (2011). The effects of a mindfulness-based stress reduction Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Mind programme on pre-sleep cognitive arousal and insomnia over matter: Reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular symptoms: A pilot study. Stress and Health, 27(3), e299– and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental e305. Psychology: General, 141(3), 417–422. Condren, R. M., O’Neill, A., Ryan, M. C. M., Barrett, P., & Thakore, Jefferson, A. L., Himali, J. J., Beiser, A. S., Au, R., Massaro, J. H. (2002). HPA axis response to a psychological stressor J. M., Seshadri, S., . . . Manning, W. J. (2010). Cardiac index in generalised social phobia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, is associated with brain aging: The Framingham Heart 27(6), 693–703. Study. Circulation, 122(7), 690–697. Daly, J. A., Vangelisti, A. L., & Lawrence, S. G. (1989). Self- Kassam, K. S., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2009). Decisions focused attention and public speaking anxiety. Personality under distress stress profiles influence anchoring and and Individual Differences, 10(8), 903–913. adjustment. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1394–1399. Davidson, R. J., Marshall, J. R., Tomarken, A. J., & Henriques, Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Single-case research designs: Methods J. B. (2000). While a phobic waits: Regional brain electrical for clinical and applied settings. Oxford, England: Oxford and autonomic activity in social phobics during anticipa- University Press. tion of public speaking. Biological Psychiatry, 47(2), 85–95. Kazdin, A. E., & Nock, M. K. (2003). Delineating mechanisms Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: of change in child and adolescent therapy: Methodological Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological issues and research recommendations. Journal of Child Review, 96(1), 84–100. Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(8), 1116–1129. Eckman, P. S., & Shean, G. D. (1997). Habituation of cognitive Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. and physiological arousal and social anxiety. Behaviour R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age- Research and Therapy, 35(12), 1113–1121. of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Edelmann, R. J., & Baker, S. R. (2002). Self-reported and actual Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General physiological responses in social phobia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 62(6), 593–602. Clinical Psychology, 41(1), 1–14. Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). Etkin, A., & Wager, T. D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of The “Trier Social Stress Test”—A tool for investigating anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 76–81. Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10), 1476–1488. Krämer, M., Seefeldt, W. L., Heinrichs, N., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Geronimus, A. T., Hicken, M., Keene, D., & Bound, J. (2006). Schmitz, J., Wolf, O. T., & Blechert, J. (2012). Subjective, “Weathering” and age patterns of allostatic load scores autonomic, and endocrine reactivity during social stress among blacks and whites in the United States. American in children with social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Child Journal of Public Health, 96(5), 826–833. Psychology, 40(1), 95–104. Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of nega- stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social tive experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10(1), 83–91. Psychological Science, 20(3), 187–191. 12 Jamieson et al.

Kubzansky, L. D., Mendes, W. B., Appleton, A. A., Block, J., & for your health: Physiological thriving during stressful inter- Adler, G. K. (2012). A heartfelt response: Oxytocin effects group encounters. Psychological Science, 18(11), 991–998. on response to social stress in men and women. Biological Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regula- Psychology, 90(1), 1–9. tion insights from social cognitive and affective neurosci- Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1993). The Interaction ence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), Anxiousness Scale: Construct and criterion-related validity. 153–158. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61(1), 136–146. Roelofs, K., van Peer, J., Berretty, E., Jong, P. D., Spinhoven, P., Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). & Elzinga, B. M. (2009). ? Pituitary? Adrenal The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion axis hyperresponsiveness is associated with increased social on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon avoidance behavior in social phobia. Biological Psychiatry, dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior 65(4), 336–343. Therapy, 35(4), 747–766. Rubia, K. (2009). The neurobiology of meditation and its Lindquist, K. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Constructing emotion: clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biological The experience of as a conceptual act. Psychological Psychology, 82(1), 1–11. Science, 19(9), 898–903. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physi- Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & ological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Barrett, L. F. (2011). The brain basis of emotion: A meta- Review, 69(5), 379–399. analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 121– Schmitz, J., Krämer, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Heinrichs, N., & 143. Blechert, J. (2011). Restricted autonomic flexibility in chil- MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & dren with social phobia. Journal of Child Psychology and Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vul- Psychiatry, 52(11), 1203–1211. nerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Janavs, J., Weiller, E., . . . Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini- Journal of , 111(1), 107–123. International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The Mather, M., Gorlick, M. A., & Lighthall, N. R. (2009). To brake or development and validation of a structured diagnostic accelerate when the light turns yellow? Stress reduces older psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of adults' risk taking in a driving game. Psychological Science, Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33. 20(2), 174–176. Shimizu, M., Seery, M. D., Weisbuch, M., & Lupien, S. P. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases (2011). Trait social anxiety and physiological activation: have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, Cardiovascular threat during social interaction. Personality 16(3), 331–354. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(1), 94–106. Matthews, K. A., Gump, B. B., Block, D. R., & Allen, M. T. (1997). Steer, R. A., & Beck, A. T. (1997). Beck Anxiety Inventory. In Does background stress heighten or dampen children’s C. P. Zalaquett & J. R. Wood (Eds.), Evaluating stress: A cardiovascular responses to acute stress? Psychosomatic book of resources (pp. 23–40). Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Medicine, 59(5), 488–496. Education. Matthews, K. A., Katholi, C. R., McCreath, H., Whooley, M. A., Stein, M. B., Asmundson, G. J., & Chartier, M. (1994). Autonomic Williams, D. R., Zhu, S., & Markovitz, J. H. (2004). Blood responsivity in generalized social phobia. Journal of pressure reactivity to predicts hyper- Affective Disorders, 31(3), 211–221. tension in the CARDIA study. Circulation, 110(1), 74–78. Stein, M. B., & Kean, Y. M. (2000). Disability and quality of life Mauss, I. B., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Autonomic in social phobia: Epidemiologic findings. American Journal recovery and habituation in social anxiety. Psychophysiology, of Psychiatry, 157(10), 1606–1613. 40(4), 648–653. Stein, M. B., Torgrud, L. J., & Walker, J. R. (2000). Social phobia Mauss, I., Wilhelm, F., & Gross, J. (2004). Is there less to social symptoms, subtypes, and severity: Findings from a com- anxiety than meets the eye? Emotion experience, expres- munity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(11), sion, and bodily responding. Cognition and Emotion, 1046–1052. 18(5), 631–642. Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Larkin, K. T. (1986). Situational McEwen, B. S. (2006). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis determinants of social anxiety in clinic and nonclinic sam- and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of ples: Physiological and cognitive correlates. Journal of Sciences, 840(1), 33–44. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(4), 523–527. McTeague, L. M., Lang, P. J., Laplante, M. C., Cuthbert, B. N., Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and nega- Strauss, C. C., & Bradley, M. M. (2009). Fearful imagery in tive affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive social phobia: Generalization, comorbidity, and physiologi- disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(3), 346– cal reactivity. Biological Psychiatry, 65(5), 374–382. 353. Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Lickel, B., & Hunter, S. (2002). Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The Challenge and threat during social interactions with White emotional Stroop task and . Psychological and Black men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24. 28(7), 939–952. Yoon, K. L., & Joormann, J. (2012). Stress reactivity in social Mendes, W. B., Gray, H. M., Mendoza-Denton, R., Major, B., anxiety disorder with and without comorbid depression. & Epel, E. S. (2007). Why egalitarianism might be good Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 250–255.