REPORT RESUMES

ED 018 250 PS 000 278 RESULTS OF THE SUMMER 1965 PROJECT HEAD START. VOLUMES I AHD II. BY- CORTI H. RUSSELL* JR. AND OTHERS PLANNING RESEARCH CORP., WASHINGTON. D.C. REPORT NUMBER PRC-R-795 PUB DATE 9 MAY 66 REPORT NUMBER 0E0-753 EDRS PRICE MF-42.2S HC.422.52 561F.

DESCRIPTORS- *PROGRAM EVALUATION, *PRESCHOOLEDUCATION, SUMMER PROGRAMS, *CULTURALLY DISADVANTAGED, *PRESCHOOL CHILDREN, *PARTICIPANT CHARACTERISTICS, ADMINISTRATIVEORGANIZATION, TEACHER CHARACTERISTICS, COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT,SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT, FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS, CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTERS, MEDICAL EVALUATION, TEACHER ATTITUDES, HEADSTART, COMMUNITY ACTION PROGRAM, PPVT, PSYCHOLOGICALSCREENING PROCEDURE, PRIMARY MENTAL ABILITIES, DAP, PSI,STANFORD BINET,

AN OVERALL SURVEY AND ANALYSIS OF THE SUMMER 1965 PROJECT HEAD START IS PRESENTED IN THIS REPORT. THE FIRST SECTION DISCUSSES THE INCEPTION, IMPLEMENTATION,AND FORMAL ORGANIZATION OF THE PROJECT. THE SECOND SECTION PRESENTS DETAILED INFORMATION ON THE COMMUNITIES, CHILDREN, PARENTS, STAFF, AND WORKERS INVOLVED IN THE PROJECT.THE THIRD SECTION DISCUSSES AND EVALUATES SPECIFIC HEAD STARTPROGRAMS. THE FOURTH SECTION CONSIDERS THE IMPACT OF THE HEADSTART PROGRAM ON THE PARTICIPATING COMMUNITIES, ON THE HEALTH,MENTAL DEVELOPMENT, AND SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE CHILDREN, ON THE PARENTS, AND ON THE STAFF OF THE CHILD DEVELOPMENTCENTERS. THE FINAL SECTION SUMMARIZES THE RESULTS AND PRESENTS SEVERAL SPECIFIC RECOMMENDATIONS. VOLUME II OF THE REPORTCONTAINS THE APPENDIXES. (DR) C) Lrl co RESULTS OF THE SUMMER1965 cz PROJECT HEAD START LU

PRC R-795

VOLUME I

9 May 1966

Prepared for

OFFICE OFECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY DIRECTOR, PROJECT HEAD STARTRESEARCH AND EVALUATION Under Contract 0E0-753

By

.Russell Cort, William D. Commins, Jr, Kenneth L. Deavers Ruth Ann O'Keefe James F. Ragan, Jr.

PLANNING RESEARCHCORPORATION LOS ANGELES, CALIFORNIA WASHINGTON, D.C. .11111...... U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATHIN & WELFARE

OFFICE OF EDUCATION

THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRODUCED EXACTLY LS RECEIVED FROM THE

PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIGINATING IT.POINTS OF VIEW OR OPINIONS

STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDUCATION

POSITION OR POLICY.

RESULTS OF THE SUMMER 1965 PROJECT HEAD START

PRC R-7 9 5

VOLUME I

9 May 1966

Prepared for

OFFICE OF ECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY Cr/DIRECTOR, PROJECT HEAD START RESEARCH AND EVALUATION

- N.

PLANNING RESEARCH CORPORATION LOS ANGELES, CALIFORNIA WASHINGTON, D.C. ABSTRACT

Data on children, parents, workers, programs,aad communities involved in the Summer 1965 Project Head Start have beencollected and analyzed. Descriptions and evaluations of theparticipants, operations, and results of the program are presented.Implications for future plan- ning and research are noted.

iii TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page .

ABSTRACT iii INTRODUCTION xv I. HISTORY I-1 II. DESCRIPTION OF HEAD START COMMUNITIES AND PARTICIPANTS 11-1

A. Introduction II -1 B. Head Start Communities II -2 C. Head Start Children I1-21 D. Head Start Parents and Families II- 96 E. Head Start CDC Staff and Workers U- 122 F. The National Samples II- 148

III. PROGRAMS 1

A. Introduction 111-1 B. Background Information III- 2 C. CDC Services, Activities, and Resources III- 12

IV. RESULTS IV -1 A. Introduction IV-1 B. Impact on Communities IV -2 C. Impacts on Children IV- 28 D. Impact of Head Start on Parents IV- 128 E. Impacts on CDC Staff Workers IV- 138

V. SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS V -1

A. Summary , V- 2 B. Recommendations V -15

LIST OF REFERENCES R -1 ...... ,...a.

LIST OF EXHIBITS

Page I -1 Organization Chart of 0E0 . 1-4 1-2 Organization chart of CAP . I-5 I-3 Summer 1965 Head Start OrganizationChart I-7 1-4 Head Start Task Evaluation FlowChart 1-8

I-5 Research and Evaluation Flow Chart I- 1 5

U -1 Counties Served by Head Start II- 3 II -2 Regional Distribution of Total andHead Start Populations II-4

II- 3 Family Income by Region II- 5 11-4 Urban-Rural Distribution of Head StartChildren . II- 7

II-5 Special-Target Counties 11-9 II- 6 Concentration of Head Start Children inSpecial- Target Counties II -1 1.. 11-7 Characteristics of Special-Target Counties II -18 11-8 General Characteristics of CulturalDeprivation..a e II-22

11-9 Age . II-27 II- 1 0 Sex II-27

II- 1 1 Race 11-29

11- 1 2 Cultural Background II- 29 II- 1 3 Size of Household . 11- 31 II-: 14 Family Income 11-33

II- 1 5 With Whom Child Lives II- 33 II- 1 6 Mother's Education , II-34 II -17 Does Mother Work? ,. II -34

vii LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued) Page

II -18 Child's Race II -35 II- 19 Family Income II- 37 II -20 Mother Works Outside the Home by Family Income II -39 II-21 Number of People in Household by Education of Mother , II-40

11-22 Income by Child's Race 11-41

11-23 Household Size by Income 11-42

II-24 Household Size by Ethnic Classification 11-43 11-25 Medical/Dental History (Total) 11-47

11-26 Medical/Dental, History and Race II-49 II-27 Medical/Dental History: Head Start Survey Comparisons . . II- 50

11-28 Immunization History (Total) ,. I.t- 51

11-29 Immunization History and Race 'II- 52 II-30 Immunization History: Survey Comparisons 11-54

11-31 Examination Results (Total) II- 57

1I -32 Medical Impressions: Survey Comparisons 11-61

11-33 Diagnoses and Impressions 11-65 11-34 PPVT Pretest Scores by Age 11-68 11-35 PPVT Pretest Scores by Race 11-69 11-36 PPVT Pretest Scores by Sex 11-70 11-37 PPVT Pretest Scores by Family Intactness 11-71

11-38 PPVT Pretest Scores by Household Size. II-72

viii LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued) Page II -39 PPVT Pretest Scores on the Basis of Whether the Mother Works II -73

II -40 PPVT Pretest Scores by Urbanization 11-74 11-41 PPVT Pretest Scores by Income II-75 II-42 PPVT Pretest Scores by Region II- 76 11-43 Psychological Screening Procedure: Symptoms (Nationwide Sample) 11-80 II- 44 Psychological Screening Procedure: Typologies (Nationwide Sample) II- 82

11-45 Psychological Screening Procedure: Referrals . 11-83 II- 46 Psychological Screening Procedure:Symptoms (Berlin) II -84 II -47 Psychological Screening Procedure:Typologies (Berlin) II-85

11-48 Activities, Habits and Environment (Total) . .. II-87

II- 49 Activities, Habits and Enlrironrnet (White).. ... 11-89

II-50 Activities, Habits and Environment (Negro). 11 -9b.

II- 51 Lingual Background 11-95 11-52 Family History: Parents (Total) II- 97 II -53 Family History: Parents (White-Negro) II-100 II- 54 Description of Mothers (Total) II -105 II- 55 Description of Mothers (White) II-106

II- 56 Description of Mothers (Negro) 11-107 II- 57 Description of Fathers (Total) II-108 II-58 Description of Fathers (White) II-109

ix LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued) Page II -59 Description of Fathers (Negro) II -110 11-60 Description of Families (Total) II -112

II- 61 Description of Families 11-114 II- 62 Family Habits (Total) II-117 II- 63 Family Habits (White) II-118 11-64 Family Habits (Negro) II-119 11-65 Staff and Worker Characteristics II-128 II -66 Three-Way Classification of Staff and Workers II-132 11-67 Selected Characteristics of Professional Staff Members by Age 11-133 II -68 Characteristics of Subprofessional Workers II-135

11-69 Training and Experience of Head Start Teachers. 11-136

11-70 Worker Attitudes (Berlin) II- 141 11-71 Comparison of 1-Percent Samples with Census Tabulations III-150 11-72 Analysis of Differential Impact on Children With Low Pre-PPVT Scores II-154 II -73 Comparison of Children Having Matched PPVT Scores With Those Without Them II -156

III -1 Seven Head Start Regions III- 3 III- 2 State and Regional Distribution of Federal Head Start Funds and Child Development Centers 111-4 III- 3 State and Regional Expenditures Per Pupil (Head Start Versus Public School Systems) III-7

111-4 Medical/Dental Planning 111-13

III- 5 Organizations Providing Assistance III- 14 LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued) Page

III -6 Availability of Services-Workers' Evaluations .. III-16 III- 7 Medical/Dental Examinations-Workers'

Opinions . , . III -19

III- 8 Medical Consultant Report 1 OOOOOO 4 III-2 1 III -9 Program Characteristics-.Workers' Evaluations III-24 III-10 Services and Activities-Workers' Opinions III 2:3 III- 1 1 Program Characteristi.cs- Educational Consultants' Report III- 30J III- 12 Availability of Social Services- Workers'

Evaluation. II O III- 34 III-13 Parent Participation- Educational Consultants' Reports.... III 3 5

III -14 Selected NUEA Trainee Characteristics III -37 III-15 Experience of NUEA Trainees III- 38

III-16 Staff and Workers-Consultants' Comments... . ill- 41

IV- 1 Head Start Grantees and Sponsors by Category. IV- 5

IV- 2 Organizations Providing Assistance by Region .. . IV- 6

IV -3 Plans for Full-Year Child Development Center . . IV- 1 1

IV-4 Plans for Follow-Through in Elementary Schools.. .IV- 12

IV -5 Plans for Fall Programs IV- 1 5

IV- 6 Organization of Operation Head Start, Omega County IV -18

IV -7 Health Referrals IV -31

IV- 8 PPVT Mean Raw Scores (Nine Studies) IV- 34

IV -9 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Age IV -36

IV-1.0 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Race IV -37

xi LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued)

IL'aLe_

IV-11 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Sex IV -38 IV-12 Comparison of PPVT Scores by FamilyIntactness. IV -39 1V-13 Comparison of PPVT Scores byHousehold Size. IV -40 IV-14 Comparison of PPVT Scores on theBasis of Whether the Mother Works IV- 41 IV-15 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Urbanization IV-42 IV-16 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Income IV -43 IV-17 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Region ...... IV -44 IV-18 Comparison of PPVT Scores by FamilyIncome (Two Levels) ... IV-19 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Income and Race :. IV-50 IV-20 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Urbanization and Race IV- 51 IV-21 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Urbanization and Family Intactness IV- 52 IV-22 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Urbanization and Income IV- 53 IV-23 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Race and Degree of Mother's Educz.tion IV- 54 IV-24 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Race andWhether Mother Works IV- 55 IV-25 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Fa.mily Intactness and Sex IV- 56 IV-26 Comparison of PPVT Scores by Race and Sex IV-57 IV-27 Comparison of PPVT Scores byUrbanization and Sex IV-.58

111-28 Comparison of PPVT Scores by FamilyIntactneos and Race IV- 59

xii LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued) Page IV-29 PPVT Change in Relation to Initial Score (Eisenberg) IV -66 IV- 30 Distribution of PPVT Raw Scores by Chronological Age (Eisenberg) IV-68 IV-31 Summary of PPVT and DAP Scores (Eisenberg) IV-69 IV-32 Effects of Special Training in Perceptual and Conceptual Skills (Eisenberg) IV -72

IV- 33 Cognitive Study Results (Holmes and Holmes) IV- 74

IV--34 Preschool Inventory Results (Horowitz and R.osenfeld) IV-81

IV-35 PPVT and PSI Test Results (Pierce-Jones) IV -83 IV-36 PMA Test Results (Chesteen) IV-88 IV-37 Summary of Pre-DAP Scores- -Grouped by Family Income Level (Jacobs and Shafer) IV -9Z IV-38 Summary of Pre-DAP Scores--Grouped by Parents' Education (Jacobs and Shafer) IV -9Z IV-39 PSI Test Results (Silberstein) IV-94

IV-40 Metropolitan Readiness Test Results (A) IV -96 IV-41 Metropolitan Readiness Test Results (B) IV-96

IV-42 IQ Comparisons, DAP and PPVT (Eisenberg) IV -99

IV-43 IQ Scores Obtained on Different Tests (Eisenberg). IV-99 IV-44 PPVT and DAP IQ Results (Johnson) IV- 100 IV-45 PPVT and S-B IQ Estimates (Oze) IV-100 -IV-46 Impact on Social Behavior (Harding, Warminster Township) IV-111 IV-47 Impact on Classroom Behavior (Van Egmond) IV-112 aarammumIiNewe- +taw, +et

LIST OF EXHIBITS (Continued)

IV- 48 Summary of Projective Test Results (Holmes and Holmes)

IV- 49 Selected Sample Characteristics (Lamb) IV-117

IV- 50 Changes in Head Start Children (Workers' Opinions)

IV-- 51 Changes in Head Start Children - Parents' Opinion (Total) IV-122

IV-52 Changes in Head Start Children -Pa'rents' Opinion (White) IV -123

IV -53 Changes in Head Start Children - Parents' Opinion (Negro) IV -124

IV- 54 Effects on ParentsTotal (Percent) IV -129

IV- 55 Effects on Parents-White (Percent) IV -130

IV- 56 Effects an Parents -Negro (Percent) IV -131

IV -57 Effects on Parents -Workers' Opinions IV-133

IV -58 Benefits of Workers'Participation - Workers' Ratings IV-139 IV-59 Worker Enthusiasm - Workers' Opinion ..... IV-142

xiv INTRODUCTION

This is a report on results of the1965 Project Head Start, pre- pared by the Planning ResearchCorporation (PRC) under Contract OEO- 753. The task called for in this contract was to prepare areport that would assist OEO in evaluating theaccomplishments of the 1965 Project Head Start. The task wasaccomplished by collecting, analyzing, evaluating, and integrating all available data from avariety of sources. The basic organization of the report wasdeveloped under the direction of the Director of Research and Evaluationfor Project Head Start, Dr. Edmund W. Gordon. There were eight primary sources of data: 1. Tabulations of data on a 1-percent nationwidesample of Head Start children made by the U.S. Bureau of theCensus, and computer data files and reports of essentiallythe same sample prepared by the OEO Information Center. 2. Tabulations and processed data from questionnairesfilled out by staff members and workers in ChildDevelopment Centers (CDC's) from which the 1-percent sample of children was drawn. 3. Results of interviews of a stratified nationalsample of parents of Head Start children conducted by theNational Opinion Research Center (NORC) for OEO. 4. Reports of medical, educational, and childdevelopment consultants to OEO. 5. All available preliminary and final reportsof independent research and evaluation studies performed farOEO. 6. Available reports of local studies, not fundedby OEO, con- ducted during or after the Summer1965 Head Start program. 7. Program proposals submittedto OEO in the spring of 1965, and various OEO brochures, reports, and program documentation. 8. Census statistics on communities anddemographic distributions. xv PRC reviewed a large amountof literature, including newspaper articles, and talked with many people who were connectedwith Head Start either during the summer of1965 or subsequently. However, it is information from the sources listed abovethat is documented and analyzed in this report. We have madeinterpretive comments on the data where it has seemed appropriate,but we have also presented as much data as possible so that the reader canmake his own analyses and interpretations. Throughout the report certain terms have beenused as descriptive shorthand labels without careful or precisedefinition. The primary case in point is the terra. "cultural deprivation," or itsadjectival form "cul- turally deprived." Variations of the tern; are"socially disadvantaged," "low income," "poor," and "culturally disadvantaged."These terms appear throughout thetechnical literature with varying degrees of speci- ficity and consistency. The problem of definition here is that the conceptof cultural depri- vation is a relative, behavioral concept.It has nothing inherently to do with ethnic background, geography,education or occupation of parents, etc.It is possible that, at least on many tests, alow-income "socially handicapped" preschooler may be indistinguishablefrom a middle-income slow learner, for example.In this report, we take the term"culturally deprived" to refer to children who comefrom lbw-income families and who are likely to encounterincreasing difficulties in achievement in school, or purely for convenience, tothe children in the program. Another term that is used with varying degrees of precisionin this report is "impact."It should refer, specifically, to an observablechange unambiguously attributable to a definable treatment.This idealistic concept is virtually unobtainable in largeprbgrams for a variety of reasons, including cost,political constraints on experimental designs, and the state of the art in measurement. Wehave generally taken im- pact to refer to an implied change ormodification. There is no implied duration of change.Of course, indications of change do not necessarily prove thatchange actually occurred, any more than absence ofindication of change proves that none occurred. xvi We have not attempted to examineproblems of construct and pre- dictive validity with various tests.Similarly, we have not attempted to assess or estimate testreliability in depth.Generally we have tried to analyze results as objectively aspossible, s...rith full *awareness of many uncertainties and constraints. The report contains five majorsections. Section I is a history of Project Head Start in 1965.Section II contains descriptive data on the communities and participants inthe summer program.It also contains a discussion ofthe characteristics of thesamples of data used. Section III describes various aspectsof the programs carried out at Child De- velopment Centers.It also contains evaluationsand opinions of workers, parents, and consultantsabout the different :features or programcharac- teristics.Section IV provides data on andevaluations of the impact or effectiveness of the 1965 ProjectHead Start.Impacts on communities, children, parents, and staffmembers are discussed in turn.Section V is a summary of significantfindings and conclusions. A list of recom- mendations for future planningis given.The List of References contains all reports, published andunpublished, used in preparing this report. For the convenience ofresearchers who wish to pursue thecollection and exchange of data in their areasof interest, we have provided theaddresses of the principal investigatorswho conducted independent researchstudies for OEO. Appendix A contains copies ofthe research instruments used in the program, with theexception of the Peabody PictureVocabulary Test (PPVT). For ease of reference,items from these instruments are referred to in the text by theirnumber on the form. Appendix Bprovides a descriptionof the statistical models usedin analyzing PPVT scores and National Opinion ResearchCenter (NORC) data. Appendix Cpresents height and weight centilesby age and sex for children includedin the 1-percent sample. Appendix Dgives a further discussion of theeconomic characteristics of thepopulation served in the HeadStart program. Ap- pendix E provides a comparisonof IQ scores for children whoreceived more than one typeof intelligence test. Appendix F presentsthe results

xvii of an analysis of variance of CDC's, where the measuresof center performance were PPVT D-scores.Appendix G presents the response frequencies of different types of CDC staffmembers in the national 1-percent sample to questions on thePaid and Voluntary Workers' Evaluation Form. Appendix H provides a summaryof several addi- tional Head Start studies that werereceived shortly before our publi- cation deadline. Several reports onlanguage are of particular interest. It was our original intention to coverthis topic in the body of the text. However, pressures of time have madeifnecessary to treat this sub- ject separately in an appendix.Exigencies of time also forced analyses of the nationally collected scores on thePre-School Inventory and the Behavior Inventory to be treated in a separate appendix.Appendix I presents conclusions from analysesof results of these two tests.It also provides results of analysesof intercorrelationsbetween the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, thePre-School Inventory, and the Behavior Inventory. This report is the result of theefforts of a large number of people. We should like to express ourappreciation for the assistance and coopera- tion of personnel in 0E0 andof others associated with Project Head Start. We especially wish to acknowledge thecontributions of Dr. Robert S. Drachman, The Johns HopkinsHospital, Baltimore, Maryland, who de- voted much time and effort inassisting with analysis and interpretation of the medical and dental data.We have incorporated his suggestions and recommendations in the textwherever possible. Finally, much credit must be given to thePRC technical personnel who contributed to the projectin all areas: David R. Alexander Jules P. Aronson Miss Patricia A. Eller Mrs. Naomi H. Henderson Dr. Norman W. 'Kettner Walter Lawrence Miss Lynn W. Paul

xviii Patrick W. Roche James F. Rowe Mrs. Susan Starr The project has beenperformed under the supervision of Dr. Allen R. Ferguson, DeputyManager, Systems Economics Division. Dr. H. Russell Cort, Jr., wasthe Project Manager.

xix I. HISTORY

Head Start began as an idea--an idea that the culturally deprived child is a key element in the poverty cycle, and that to break the cycle, one needs to reach these children early.This idea and Project Head Start have become a primary strategic component of the War on Poverty. Ad President Johnson has stated, a major goal of this war is to save the children of poverty from passing "poverty's curse" onto their offspring "like a family birthmark." The reasoning behind this idea is quite simple. Experience has shown that the child from an economically or socially deprived home lags behind his classmates before he embarks upon his formal education.The child from a more affluent home has been surrounded by a relatively diver- sified and intellectually stimulating environment in his first 4 or 5 years, while the deprived child is apt to have a more restricted range of experi- ences.It is not uncommon for the underprivileged child to enter school with a severely limited vocabulary and little contact with books, story- telling, or even crayons.His experiences with the world around him are equally limited. He may receive little encouragement for exploring his world, noticing different relationships in it, attempting new tasks, and forming abstract concepts. He may experience quite different patterns of reinforcement than the non-socially disadvantaged child.This social and educational poverty is typically produced by the economic poverty of the family. Many of these children are also lacking in medical and dental care; they may be poorly fed and poorly clothed.Thus, the whole child is affected by his poverty-strickenenvironment' Having identified early childhood as one of four crisis points in a person's life, an OEO Stiff Committee, assembled to evaluate the National Poverty Program, recommended that a high priority begiNien to establish- ing some sort of operation to widen the perspectives of deprived children.

1More extensive discussion of these points is given in Sectiori Acting on this recommendation, Mr. Sargent Shriver and Dr. Jerome Bruner" reviewed the idea and decided that such a program was both desirable and feasible. Further staffmeetings provided the program with a name, and Project HeadStart was on its way. At this point, Head Start was expected to serve some100,000 culturally disadvantaged children from 200 communities across the nation. A National Planning Committee for Head Start wasorganized.This committee, headed by Dr. Robert E. Cooke, was asked to suggestthe specific objectives and kinds of programs that might be mosteffective in meeting the challenge of Project Head Start.These programs were to increase the achievements of and opportunities for poor children.3 As conceived by this committee, Project Head Start was tobe an experience in early childhood development, not merely apre-school read- ing readiness program. All aspects of the child were tobe served, with basic goals being improvement of thechild's physical and mental hilalth, emotional and social development, improvement inconceptual and verbal skills, greater self-confidence, better family relations,development of a responsible attitude towardsociety, and, finally, an increased sense of dignity and self-worth. Head Start was publicly announced on Friday,February 19, 1965 at the White House. With Mrs. Johnson acting asChairman, Dr. Julius Richmond

1Professor of Psychology, Harvard University. 2Given Foundation Professor of Pediatrics,School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University. 3It will be recalled that these childrenhave previously been described as culturallydeprived.This descriptive phrase has been the cause of considerable disagreement. Disagreement has occurred overattempts to identify the contributoryelements and observable consequences of such a state, over efforts to determine thepossibility (or impossibility) of constructing an index to measure it, overwhether programs in fact served children who could appropriately be said tosuffer from it; etc. However, for purposes of this report, culturallydeprived and poor are taken as synonomous. 4Dean of Medical Faculty, State University.Upstate Medical Center, Syracuse, New York.

1-2 acting as Project Director, and many prominent citizens lending their support, the campaign to arouse community interest in Head Start began. A letter. was sent by Mr. Shriver to various public officials throughout the country inviting their communities to participate in Head Start. As the responses to this letter came in, the first evidence accumulated indi- cated that the projected program was not nearly large enough to meet the surge of local interest. Some projectionsdone by the Research, Plans, Programs, and Evaluation Division of0E01showed that as many as 600,000 to 1,200,000 children might be reached. Project Head Start was a national program in that broad guidelines, overall administration, and the bulk of the financing were provided bythe Federal Government. But the programs that werechosen to execute these broad guidelines, the administration of the individual programs,and pro- vision of the necessary physical facilities and personnel were allprovided by the local communities. 0E0 set up qualificationsfor determining the eligibility of applicants for grants to operate Head Startsummer programs. Since Head Start was a part of the Community ActionProgram (see Ex- hibit 1-2), the applications were to be prepared andsubmitted in coopera- tion with the local Community Action agency.In the absence of a local Community Action program, other public agencies such as localschool boards or private non-profit organizations were eligible.In the latter case, particular care wastaken to ensure that the private organization would in fact undertake a program that met local needs.Application mate- rials went out to prospective grantees beginning in lateFebruary, with an April 15thdeadline for their return.This deadline was not adhered to, and receipt and processing of theapplications continued until funds were exhausted. The processing of the completed applications was animmense task. The staff of Head Start numbered onlyabout 300 people at its peak, and many of thesepeople were hired only on a temporarybasis. However, they all worked with dedication, sometimesfor as long as 16 hours a

'Exhibit I-1 shows the organization of 0E0.

I-3 il-,,ff,..-allef,=1TMWRIPI,I7r7,...... ^...... -

Director National Advisory Council Economic Opportunity Council Deputy Director Executive Secretary Office of Office of Office of Office of Office of h, Plans. InteOffice ragency of GeneralOffice of Counsel Information Center RelationsCongressional inspection 1 Prays:0 Groups /,=01. Public Affairs EvaluationPrograms and Relation JobAssistant Corps Director CommunityAssistant ActionDirector Programs toVolunteersAssistant America Director in(VISTA) Service ManagementAssistant Director 41=1. StartProject Head Office of Econothic4M chart isOpportunity intended to during be a representationthe Summer of oft965. the organization of the EXHIBIT I-I ORGANIZATION CHART OF 0E0 EconomicDirstiov, OpportunityOffk of CommunityDeputy Action Assistant Program Di Asa latent Director I 1 OfficeDeputyAaaotDevriormantProgram. of late negotiate Policy.Director andDiroctor after(PrimarilyOfficeresignedAssociateProgram the of Associate Supportearly Dirrctor in Di the(Training) program.) "paper' operation Oak.AssociateFight Deputyof Operations DirectorAssociate Director Special I Employment ChildPrograms Management 1 DiviaionProjects DivisionEducation Program Division R h SystemsInformation ManagementGrants DivisionDevelopmentEconomic DivisionWeiler* DivisionDevelopment Division ManagementFinancial andMonitoring Centro' DirectorProject Head Start DirectorProject Upward ..r...... 1Round I SpecialChief Field Projects AssistantChlofApplications Chief Rewires ChiefRural Task Force II1 jIi r_._I Torritoriee ism...... 4 Rredirras e'7.."11 lions Migrants RoglonalOffices I1 L.I ...... jI I ''thisthe chart Community is intended Action to Programke during Ut. Summer of 1%3. rep ***** lotion of the organisation of 1..I - ...... I EXHIBIT I-2 I ORGANIZATION CHART OF CAP day, 6 days a week, to ,get thejob done. .An organizationchart for Head Start Summer 1965 is shownin Exhibit 1-3. The local Head Start programs wereto be organized into Child Development Centers(CDC's). These centers were tobe the focus of all activities relating tothe child.With the classrooms and outdoorplay areas comprisingthe physical plant,other materials and services to be offered by the community wereconcentrated in the CDC.Considerably more detail onthe CDC and its organizationand operation appears in Section III.It should be notedthat this focus of activity was oneof the more novel aspectsof Head Start. The application review process was adetailed, multi-stage opera- tion (see Exhibit 1-4).Qualifications for the Center's staffdirector and professional personnel wereestablished.These included degrees in ed- ucation (with majors inearly childhood, kindergarten,human growth and development, or elementaryeducation) and degrees in psychology,soci- ology, or home economics.Experience as a social worker, nursery school, kindergarten, orelementary school teacher,director of a day care center, orsupervisor of playgrounds alsoqualified a person to assume a professionalrole in a center. The proposed program wasthen evaluated in terms ofthe geographic area which itwould cover. When available,information in the application was examinedthat described the socio-economiccharacteristics of the area in whichthe CDC would be located. The age level of thechildren who would participatehad to be ascer- tained, as well aseconomic conditions of theirfamilies. The analysts were instructedthat 85 percent of thechildren were to be "poor."This economic criterion caused aconsiderable problem, since the 0E0 instruc- tions accompanyingthe application forms saidonly: "family income need not be a specificrequirement for admission, aslong as the program is primarily reaching the poorwithin the neighborhood." An attempt to solve thisproblem was made by inclue' .3 astatement about the economiccomposition of the participant families as acondition of grant. However, withfamily income informationsketchy or unreliable,

1-6 - - .,rwxwityryTwit rimmumel, A Lae' cr 411 0 Deputy Director Direelot EvaluationandR Piddle Affairs LCongressisnal Affairs [.. Grants, Prucessing, and Review ...... , Inwrctlen Legal Affairs Liaison Consultants StaffProf...Iona Member Rwtewbudget TrainingAnalysis Post Analysis Processing Big City Task nuts Ilvglirn I Region U 4116 Training Analyst.!teem,.Analysts III AnalystsRegionAnalysis 1V Analysts'Region V AnalystsRegion VU Analysts111110 VI *MeProject chart IsHoed Intanslad Start dialog to bit atot rsiirsosittallosi Itarnrrotr actaataattot of . EXHIBIT I-3 SUMMER 1965 HEAD START ORGANIZATION CFART a

h and I.It hand Evaluation of Project Evaluation Head Start A:cyan

smasamatimmommoliamonismosmMINI

U. Tralain of Selection of Analysts analysts Brief training On-the-job Assignment to sessions training Regional Desks Development of checklist and teacktaa materials mommummemounsmionseutminommuimmmemmnumummmommumummnamismsommt MININIONNININNIMIMINIMMIMINONNMINNININIrri

III.Use of Selection el el,..,mwom.mw Consultants Consultants Stimulation and assistance in preparation of applications

IIMINNWMIONIONNIINIMINNINKINIIIIIIMINMINIONNNNININMENSMONINNSMOSIMMINIONNONNI

Processing of Stimulation and assistance Applications in preparing applications by Women Volunteers. Development of Head Start -Initial Logging Logging Management Interns. Receipt in rip applications by receipt in Grants. and Llaisor and State Technical Assistance counties. etc. Control Control Agencies. Ifcessing and Review Consultants. and other Federal Agencies .. Big City Task Legal Review Force

Liaison Analysis Budget Review

L-gal Review Inspection

Program rework

4 Budget problems 32 . Post Analysis Processing 0. 0 . Preparation of Packet an Telegram

Senior Review and Approved so Budget Review Sign-off twawwwwes applications al

Announcement Drafting

V. Orientation of Grantee's Professional Selection of Staff Members Proiessioral Staff Members Placement of Staff 1...... Members at Inatitutes Selection by National University Extension Association of colleges and universities to be Orientation Inst.tutes

IDevelopment of orientation program Development of question:me to be used by consultants during visits to local projects

SONNUNIMINNINNOINININ Assignment of Consultant tripe to consultants to 111 advise and evaluate visit local projects local projects

Use er. Consultant Reptris L prepara. Own of seta year's Howl Start program

INNINIMUUNPIONIUMNINIKIWANNINIINNMINIONN NININNNINNNINININNINNININININININUNININNINKINN

INI ging Logging Receipt in[4, Region I Analysis Redevelopment by Liaison and consultants going rol Control to the field Region II Analysis

.0 ty Task Legal Review Region III Analysis 4 Region IV Analysis 0 Reston V Analysis t Review 0 Region VI Analysis Final check out of application before Inspection Region VII Analysis budget review J

Sending of Packet at Amara, Processing to Governor and teparation of Packet a telegram to Grantee elegram Director of OEO approval of announcement al Announcement Public Affairs Announcement Drafting

Notification of Congressional Zffairs Appropriate House Processing and Scnate Members

IIIHNNOTOgiiiitiliiiglEVNINNIMONVONZINNIMIXMIRRINIMMISIMMIPMMIVIMM

otification to GranteeHI Orientation vf training placements by lastititte 4111(1111.1

2,/The announcement of an approved grant was made in the following order: lies Exhibit 1S first by Congressional Affairs to the appropriate House andStnate :rem- liars. second to the public by Public Affairs. and third Post Analysis Pro. esti:Ong seat a telegram to the Grantee and the packet of materials to the Gcrternor and the people in charge of Professional Staff Member training noti:led the Grantee s to where its Staff Memtsre nit:, to go for trainin:.

START TASK EVALUATION FLOW CHART and with the use of "meanstests" discouraged by 0E0, the localcommu-- nities experienced difficultyin achieving the desired ratio of"poor children."1 Another area of concern wasparental involvement. The parents were to be includedin the planning of the locationof the center, its time of operation, and the kind of programenvisioned. There was also to be provision of activities forparents' participation within the program. Provisions for the medicalservices to be provided by the CDC were examined. Since in many instancesHead Start was expected to be the first experience of the childrenwith medical professionals, this aspect of the program was givenconsiderable attention.Planning for follow- up medicalservices was to be of particularimportance. The nutritional program wasalso investigated. Meals and snacks were to be providedat the centers, with surplusfoods to be used when possible.Plans to inform parents aboutfamily nutrition were to be con- sidered, Physical facilities andtransportation arrangements had to be provided; for instance, centersshould be located so that the children did not have to ride a bus for morethan 1/2 hour one way. The budget had to satisfy adetailed analysis which established criteria for personnel costs,consultant and contracts services, travel, space, consumableand expendable supplies, rental orpurchase of non- expendable items, and other items.An analysis then followed of the 10- percent share to be put up bythe grantee to ascertain whetherhis money or in-kindcontribution fulfilled the requirementsimposed. For example, the costs of staff assigned to the programand the use of office space and office equipment were allowable,but time spent by members of the govern- ing body or advisory committeesof the applicant agency was not.The

IM/NIMOMINNIO. 1The definition of'poor" was modified several times as thereview proc- est! continued.It began as a family income of$3,000 or less.Later, an allowance wasmade for differences in the sizeof family.Still later, some recognition wasgiven to the differences in living costsfor urban and rural areas. This processresulted in a precise definition of"poor" being made a part of theinstructions to applicants for full-yearHead Start grants.

1-9 I

value of volunteers'services could be allowed,but at a rate not to ex- ceed $1.50 per hour. And acommunity had to guaranteethat the Federal to supplant local moneyalready money for HeadStart would not be used a . 1 being spent. important policy The plan of daily activities wasone of the more -1 considerations in Head Startplanning.It was decided that Head Start should provide a varietyof experiences tothe children and prepare them in this way for thelearning experience ofschool. Attitudes, life exper- iences, and interrelationshipswere to beacquired from individual at- tention and a widevariety of activity. A programthat would provide this content was lookedfor in every application. The proposed program wasthen evaluated in depth in termsof the following components: The medical program The socialservice program Parent activities Daily activities Training arrangements Follow-through Staffing . Special quality factors Analysts tried to incorporate asmany of thefeatures of what was considered an excellent programinto everyapplication. When a defi- cient program wassubmitted, a consultant was sentto help the prospec- tive grantee, or analystsin the Head Startoffice helped the community to write a program.Not all programs werestrong in all areas, and some wereaccepted with recognitionthat the programs weredeficient. The local communities wereresponsible for recruitingthe children to be served bytheir Head Start programs.Various techniques were used by communitiesto perform thistask, including: The usual census of theschool districts, accomplishedby sending letters to homesof children alreadyenrolled in school.

I- 1 0 Advertisements in the local newspapersto explain the pro - gram and inviteparticipation. . Public meetings explainingHead Start. Announcements on local radioand/or television. House-to-house canvassing of theneighborhoods from which culturally deprived children werelikely to come. These techniques succeededin encouraging the large summer1965 turnout. As it happened,however, some of these techniques werenot well suited to obtain participationfrom the economically and culturally disadvantaged population that was toconstitute a majority of Head Start families. Use of the normalpublic or private school systemrecords, or of mass communication, waslikely to leave uninformed a large segment of this group. Moreover,publicity that accentuated the"poverty" aspects of Head Start unduly oftendiscouraged potential participantsbecause of the social stigma attached.This aspect of the recruiting effort offers some lessonsfor further Head Startactivities.Sufficient data for a com- prehensive evaluation of 'recruitingis not available, therefore this topic is not explored further inthis report. A special effort was made to encouragelocal programs in the nation's 300 lowest per capitaincome counties. Cards were made onall of these special-targetcounties, and in those cases where no response to Mr. Shrivel.' s letterhad been received, some countyofficial was con- tacted. When necessary, aconsultant was sent to help structurethe pro- grams, preparethe budgets, and completethe application forms. These consultants were drawnfrom the ranks of the governmentManagement Interns.They went to these communitiesand met with the officials who would be instrumental insetting up the program. The internsinstructed these officials in ways to arousecommunity support and in methods of fulfilling their portion ofthe cost when this was required.In all, the' Management Interns made169 visits to meetings aitended by represent- atives from the 300 special-targetcounties and from 200 other counties adjacent to these areas. Theparticipation of the 66 percent ofthe 300 special-target counties resultedlargely from the concentrated effortsof 0E0. While the application forms werebeing prepared and distributed, OEO was planning the trainingof Head Start teachers.In its original report, the NationalPlanning Committee had commentedthat: Personnel recruitment andtraining represents perhaps the most difficultlogistical need. The numbers of health, socialservices, and educa- tional personnel trained to workwith young children are relatively small.The numbers trained to work with disadvantaged young children are only a fraction of the total.It is apparent, therefore, that heavyreliance must be placed on speciallydesigned train- ing programs which can be developedand implemented by early summer. OEO realized that time did notallow it to negotiate individually with the schools that would prepareteachers and other staff in allregions of the country. Thus, a contract wassigned with the National University Extension Association, with NUEAagreeing to coordinate the national training course for Head Startpersonnel. A conference for representativesfrom all universities planning to participate in the teachers'training took place April 11 to 14,16 days after the contract was signedwith NUEA. This conference was attendedby representatives from 11.9institu- tions. Administrative meetingsdealt with matters related to subcon- tracts, the relationshipsexisting among NUEA, OEO,training institu- tions, and Child DevelopmentCenters, and on- site administrationof training programs. Curriculum aapects were discussed in termsof the sociology of poverty, education,health, nutrition, and parentalinvolve.: ment. At the conference,118 institutions tentativelyagreed to sponsor training sessions. Whenfirm commitments were madeby these insti- tutions, subcontracts wereissued by NUEA. Allocations of Head Startstaff were made on a state and regional basis. The plan proposedthat teachers attend theinstitution closest to their homes. Wherethis was not possible, they were tobe assigned to institutions in the generalregion of their locale.This plan was vastly complicated by the matching ofnumbers of teachers against spacesavail.... able and against the timesof Head Start center openings.Also, the

I-12

Vransimo... "' ---:

continued processing of Head Start grants meant that even well into June more teachers had to be assigned to the training sessions. When 0E0 contracted with NUEA, the number of teachers estimated to participate in the program was 12,000.In the end, 29,933 professional staff members were trained.This was one of the major logistic feats accomplished for the summer Project Head Start. A major decision that had to be made before the classes were formed focused on child integration.It was decided that "freedom of choice," that is, freedom of a child to attend a school of his choosing, would be allowed in Head Start. However, every center was to be open to children regard- less of their creed, color, or race.In order to effect this, the centers in the South which appeared to present a civil rights problem were advised to advertise that the centers were open to all.This injunction was not honored in many instances or the notice used ambivalent language, such as "Head Start" or "the program" is open to all. One means of encouraging integration was to require that staff per- sonnel of both Negro and white races be included in every center. The staffing pattern became the crux of compliance for the summer's program. In order to evaluate compliance, the Office of Inspection made telephone inquiries before the approval of programs to all the applicants in Region Three, which was comprised of Tennessee, Alabama, Mississippi, Georgia, Florida, and South Carolina.In addition, checks were made on applicants from North Carolina, Virginia, the eastern shore of Maryland, Louisiana, Arkansas (with concentration on the eastern part), the south- eastern corner of Oklahoma, the area east of Wichita Falls in Texas, and some locations in Delaware, West Virginia, and Kentucky. By June1, 1,600 phone calls had been made. These applicants were asked how they intended to comply with the civil rights regulations and their answers were reported in writing by the telephone interviewers. When the centers were in operation, numerous visits were made to them to investigate whether or not integration existed.In all, about 550 programs were visited by representatives of the Office of Inspection. Given the limitations imposed by the relatively small. staff of the General Counsel's Office and the Office of Inspection and the self-imposed

1-13 restriction of the "freedom of choice" plan, the civilrights accomplish- ments were noteworthy. According to Office ofInspection estimates, 75 percent of the Head Start centersin the deep South were integrated;that is, there was integration of pupilsand/or staff.In the upper South and border states, 90 percent integration wasachieved. The General Counsel's Office is more conservative in itsappraisal, and maintains that although much progress was made, much moreneeds to be done. Between 5 and 10 percent of the Head Start programs were run,in whole or in part, by religiously affiliatedorganizations. A number of them were Negro groups in the south.In the large cities, there were numerous programs runby religious organizations, many of them Roman Catholic.In the major cities, the Diocesan schoolsusually sponsored a program of relativelylarge size; for instance, in New York City the Diocese of Brooklyn sponsored a programfor 2,130 children, second in size to that run by the New York CityBoard of Education. In Chicago, the largest program was sponsoredby the School Board of the Archdiocese of Chicago. The policy of favoring a non-sectarianorganization as the sponsor of a program was an issue in only afew cases.In general, all the forces that could be marshalled to establish centers werewelcome. No major church-state problems arose in theHead Start operation. From the beginning of Head Start,research and evaluation were to play an important role. A flowchart of the Research and Evaluation process of ProjectHead Start is shown as Exhibit 1-5. In April, Dr. EdmundGordonwas appointed by0E0 to direct and monitor this effort.In May he met with the Research PlanningCommittee to formulate the aims of theresearch program. A major.policy decision was madethat to the maximum extent possible theinstruments used for research programs must have a servicefunction also.Thus, tests administered were supposed to provideinformation that could be used to the benefit of teacher and child.However, there was strong interestin obtaining as much information aspossible on the total Head Start population.

1Professor, Graduate School ofEducation, Yeshiva University, New York

1-14 OfficeEducation of ,/ w Studies.'LocalNational ResearchDirector of Test&CenterReearchOpinion NORC Processing Analysio by Independent DirectoritReport by of Independent Re it Firm to the h ProjectitReport ofHead the Start,Director to theof h and Evaluation. OpportunitytheReport Office of ofthe toEconomic Directorthe United of ProjectEvaluation. Head Start OpportunityEconomicOfficeData of 5/ Sampling0E0Bureau Information andof Census Proteslog Center and h Firm "This chart is Intended to rep 11 Headand Evaluation. Start Project t the process of EconomicDirector of Opportunity the Office of State. Cflet111. ConsuitantsifHead Start , e V Z4heingevaluating InInformationpercentthe Officefirst Summer grade. Projet ofof all Eduton ofchildren Heedt 1965Headatom Start and has StartIn theshortlyChildrenmade as first it a took thereafter.gradesurvey who place are of nowSdur and has AIndependent hers doneiflationalinitiative, through 1LncalOpinionbut c ttttt were munitySimiles Rerarrh not orfunded are Individual Count those by whichOED.H. under were contract Eepertenre.withchildren,largerFamily OF.O. amountand gathered and onie theThe then ofonImpat Office information,inpros only urniatton I of ofa Economicasiiple. on some the OpinIrtonilyChildon all and Head gathered d the relt:trt on Social ibis informs. MemberforpletedCenter.byHon theInventory. was Durrettwere: The thenInformation tests PPVT1sampledofPsychological Census and PreschoolForm. Andforms or thepro, PaidScreening. whtth C.410 eased Inventory. and 01.0 information Voluntarytinder Staffhad Itehay. contract corn. - ftrheWorkerstitudeDentalParent Head Scale.Evaluation, andEvaluation Start Family Conan, of?ventInformation; Project ant Putt, supplied Head apation and Start. somethe Prem.& Worker Medical. vslu At. 177-fit'etrirgin the Consultants EXHIBIT 1-5 RESEARCH AND EVALUATION FLOW °The IndependentStarttoCall7reirrihecklIsnt.rmt"n eTirri prosrem. ne and R .valuate VCTiOUll parts of the Head hers were hired by 0E0 CHART Because of this interest, a large amount oftesting and data collection was required of all centers.It will be noted later in this reportthat the burden of data collection placed on the centers was a sourceof considerable dis- satisfaction at the operationallevels. The data collection and testinstruments used were: 1. For Children a. Medical/Dental and Family History and Social Experience Information b. Psychological Screening Procedure Given pre- and post- as measuresof impact on children were: c. Pre-School Inventory including GoodenoughDraw-A- Person d. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test e. Behavior Inventory 2. For Staff and Workers a. Staff Member Information Sheet b. Paid and Voluntary Worker'sEvaluation Given pre- and post- as a measureof impact on staff and workers was: c. Worker Attitude Scale 3. For Parents a. Parent Particiration Record b. National Opinion Research CenterParent Interview 4. Other Consultant Checklist Copies of these instruments,excluding the Peabody Picture Vocab- ulary Test, appear inAppendix A. The Head Start ResearchOffice contracted with the NationalOpinion Research Center to conduct anextensive interview withthe parents or guardians of a sample of2,000 children.The material in the interview covered the social experienceof the family, as well as theexperience of the parent and child withHead Start.Questions were asked about the employment, the income, thelanguage spoken, and the recreational activities of the family.

1- 1 6 Besides the in-house research planned, over40 research and evaluation contracts were developed by the HeadStart Research Division. These efforts were to be directed to specialproblems and/or special populations. For instance, the College of theVirgin Islands received a grant to do a sociological sturl,,T of HeadStart children from a public housing project 071 Saint Thomas. The LouisianaState University will make a comparative study of school achievementlevels, through second grade, of Head Start children versus a groupof children not culturally deprived. Another study is to focus on the impactof the program on bilingual pupils and families. The assessment of theHead Start impact upon the commu- nity is the purpose of the contractawarded to Antioch College in Yellow Springs, Ohio. Beginning late in June and continuingthrough August, children from all across the nation made thedaily trip to their Head Start classrooms. A program originally planned to serve some100,000 children in fact served more than 560,000 from over1,500 communities. Head Start Child Development Centers had astaff of 41,000 professionals, utilized and paid for the services of46,000 neighborhood residents, and benefited from an impressive volunteercontribution.It is a tribute to the imagina- tion, resourcefulness,skill, and hard work of thousands of people in local communities and in Washingtonthat an effort of this magnitude could be put together in such a shortperiod of time. The remainder of this report deals indetail with the people and communities that were a part of HeadStart, the programs that the local communities undertook to meetthe goals of Head Start, and the impact of these programs on theparticipants--children, parents, staff, workers, and communities. II.DESCRIPTION OF HEAD START COMMUNITIES AND PARTICIPANTS

A. Introduction In this section of the report, the scopeof the 1965 Project Head Start is discussed; the relevantcharacteristics of the communities, children, and parents served byHead Start are presented; and the staff and workers who served theother participants are described. In looking at the scope ofHead Start 1965, the discussion centers on (1) the geographicdistribution of grants, (2) the distribution of grants according to county-populationsize, and (3) the extent of Head Start's concentration in the 300- lowest percapita income or special-target counties. The description of communities(1) emphasizes economic and social characteristics identified byindependent research studies, and(2) corn. pares thosespecial-target counties with Head Start programsto the special-target counties without programs.In the descriptions of the participating children, the focus is on(1) the child's social and medical history, (2) his Head Startmedical and psychological examinations,and (3) comments ..)n his activitiesand environment. Familyinformation highlights parental economicand social history and activity. A list of selectedcharacteristics of staff members andworkers and a discussion of HeadStart teacher-motivationconclude the section. There are a number of reasonsfor assembling data describingthe various elements andparticipants of Head Start. Thedescriptions are necessary inputsfor evaluations of scope andeffectiveness a the pro- gram. In many casesthey are helpful in indicating ordefining program areas inwhich improvement ormodification in planning andprocedures are needed.Finally, the data are of greatpractical and theoretical interest.Indeed, one of the majoraccomplishments of Head Start was the collection of arich store of informationabout an important sector of our nation.

U-1 B. Head...wolmlywoNleallommay..1=0.1 Start Commimities1=1110 1. Scope of Head Start a. Geographic Distribution During the summer of1965, 47 percent of the nation's 3,142counties'were servedby one or more Head Start programs. Ex- hibit II-1 shows the percentageof counties within each region served by one or moreHead Start programs. Over50 percent of the counties in five regions had atleast one Head Start program in1965.In the North- east, with projectsin more than 100 counties,only Maine had centers in fewer than 50 percent ofits counties. Programs existed in theMid- dle Atlantic in 66 percentof the counties; Virginia was theonly state with less than 50 percent countyparticipation.In the Southeast, all states had programs in atleast 50 percent of their counties.In con- trast, only 9 of the29 states in the other regions(Midwest, Southwest, West, and Far West)had projects in over 50 percentof their counties. In general, then, HeadStart had a broader geographicalspread--i. e., 2 more countiesparticipated--in the eastern part of theUnited States. When compared with thepopulation distribution in the country, the 1965 Head Startenrollment by region suggests thatthe program was mostheavily concentrated in the threeregions with the poorest populations: the Middle Atlantic,the Southeast, and the Southwest (see Exhibits II-2 and II-3).These three regions have 37percent of the total population, but theyaccounted for 56 percenz. of theHead Start participants.The region with the highestmedian family income, the

3,072 counties andparishes; 62 countyequivalents (mainly independent cities); 4 Alaskanjudicial districts; andAmerican Samoa, the Virgin Islands, Puerto Rico,and Guam were served.The 47 percent county participation wasdetermined by an analysisof the listing of approved 1965 grants, which wereorganized by county.In a few cases, the grants were formulti-county prOgrams.Where identifiable, eachcounty in such programs isreflected in the 47 percent.It is possible that somemulti- county programs werenot identified and,therefore, that some counties are notrecorded.It is believed, however,that this number is small. 2Amap of statesby region appears inSection III, Exhibit. III-1.

II-2 Northeast 0 100 150 Counties 200 300 Number of Counties 400 500 600 700 800 SoutheastAtlanticMiddle 500 Counties516 Counties SouthwestMidwest 502 Counties 523 Counties WestFar West 27% 197 Counties 750 Counties Note: Shaded portion is the percentage EXHIBIT 11-1 of(PercentageCOUNTIES the Seven Regions)SERVED of Counties BY HEAD START of counties with one or more 1965 Head Start Programs. Served in Each NATIONAL POPULATION,1960 CENSUS: 179.3 MILLION

I MIDDLE/ ATLANTIC I6% SOUTHEAST NORTHEAST 11% 17%

FAR WEST 13% MIDWEST 22%

HEAD START 1965POPULATION: 560,000

MIDDLE ATLANTIC 20%

SOUTHEAST ORTHEAST 20%/ 12% / TERRITORIES 3% / FAR WEST SOUTHWEST 7% 1 6c70 IDWESTWEST 15% 6%

median family Note: Shadedportion representsregions with lowest income and greatestpercentage of familiesearning less than $3,000 per year.

EXHIBIT 11-2REGIONAL DISTRIBUTIONOF TOTAL AND HEAD STARTPOPULATIONS EXHIBIT II- 3 FAMILY INCOME BYREGION

Percentage - Median Family Family Income Income Under$3 000 Northeast $6,372 13.3 Middle Atlantic 5,181 24.5 Southeast 4,078 36.8 Midwest 6,161 16.4 Southwest 4,589 31.9 West 5,226 23.8 Far West 6,517 15.1

Source: Bureau of the Census,County and City Data Book/1962.

11-5 Far West, had the least Head Start enrollmentconcentration, namely, 13 percent of the total population, whichrepresented only 7 percent of the 560,000 Head Start children.The Northeast and the Midwesthave a almost 40 percent of the total populationand enrolled 27 percent of the Head Start children. Although there was a Head Startenrollment concentration in the three regions with the lowest medianfamily income, there was not a similar concentration in the West(Mountains and Plains states), where the economic characteristics areonly slightly better than those in the Middle Atlantic.This region accounts for 9 percent of thetotal popula- tion and enrolled only 6 percentof the students - -the fewest of anyregion. The largest program in the West wasin Missouri, which has one-third of the population of the regionand enrolled almost one-half of theHead Start participants. The summer 1965 Head Start program,then, served a greater percentage of the nation'scounties in the Northeast, theMiddle Atlantic, and the Southeast. Whenrelated to population distribution,however, it appears thatHead Start's concentration wasgreatest in the MiddleAtlantic, the Southeast, and theSouthwest.In each case, the region'sshare of the total Head Start populationexceeded its portion of the nationalpopulation. b. Distribution of Head Start Childrenby County Size Over 40 percent of the 1965 HeadStart children lived in 123 counties with apopulation of over 250,000. Another19 percent lived in over 950 countiesof less than 25,000 inhabitantseach. Most of the latter countieshave large percentages of ruralpopulations. Exhibit 11-4 shows thedistribution of the Head Startchildren by region within 10 county-populationgroupings. Eighty-five percent of the HeadStart children in theNortheast, 69 percent in the FarWest, and 67 percent inthe Midwest live in counties withpopulations of more than250,000 persons.Thirty-two percent of theparticipants in the Southeast,30 percent in the West, 27 percent in theMiddle Atlantic, and 22 percentin the Southwest live in counties withless than 25,000 inhabitants. I 100,000 94,451 80,000 7 i /0. REGIONS I=IIIiII MIDWESTMIDDLENORTHEASTSOUTHWESTSOUTHEAST ATLANTIC X*SEE M- FARWEST WEST 20,000 18,841 3 369 300 3,108 IP00+ EXHIBIT 11-4 URBAN-RURAL DISTRIBUTION OF HEAD START500-1,000 250-500 COUNTY100-250 POPULATIONS (THOUSANDS) 50-100 25-50 10-25 5-10 CHILDREN 2.5-5 0-2.5 IDENTIFIED NOT c. Special-Target Counties_...... As mentioned above, Head Startparticipation was more concentrated in the nation'sthree regions with the lowest median family income.Ninety-five percent of the 300special-targetcounties/ are contained in the MiddleAtlantic, Southeast, and Southwest. An equal percentage of the 182 countieswith a per capita income of less 2 than $750 per year are located inthese regions. Three questions arise with respect toHead Start programs in these counties: To what extent did Head Start programs servethem? Did Head Start have a greaterconcentration in the special- target counties than elsewhere? Did Head Start reach the poorestchildren within these counties? In 1965, Head Start programsappeared in 67 percent of the special-target counties.These 201 counties represent 13 percentof all counties served byHead Start. As indicated in Exhibit11-5, more of the nation's special-targetcounties are located in the Southeastthan in any other region,accounting fog 55 percent of the300. The 1965 Head Start program entered58 percent of these. Both theMiddle At- lantic and Southwest, whichhave proportionately fewer ofthese counties, had a greater percentagewith programs: 79 percent ofthe Middle At- lantic counties and 75 percentof those in the Southwest participated. When compared with thepopulation of the special-targetcounties, Head Start enrollment appears tohave been concentrated moreheavily in these countiesthan in other communities.The 284 special-target counties in theMiddle Atlantic, the Southeast, and theSouthwest, for example, account for 6.5 percentof their total regionalpopulations; the children inthese counties served by HeadStart were 15 percentof the total enrollmentof the regions.The greatest Head Startassault on

lAs indicated in Section I, the 300 countiesin the United States withthe lowest per capitaincome. 2So identified because in Community ActionPrograms the Federal grant mayexceed 90 percent. .11- 8 0 50 Number of Counties 150 AtlanticMiddle ,79070 .74 Counties 100 200 SoutheastSouthwest ///// ///A 44 Counties 166 Counties West-Far West 4 Counties 12 Counties Note: Shaded portion is the percentage of counties EXHIBIT II-5. SPECIAL - TARGET COUNTIES SERVED (Percentage of Counties Served in Each of S Regions)with one or more 1965 Head Start Programs. BY HEAD START special-target counties was in the Middle Atlantic region.The 74 poor counties account for 4 percent of the total population,and Head Start participation in 59 of these counties was 14 percent ofthe regional total. Kentucky led the way: 31 of the 34 counties inthis group had programs for almost 9,000 children, representing36 percent of the state's total Head Start enrollment. The populationof these 34 counties is 17 per- cent of the state total. Ninety-six special-target counties in theSoutheast enrolled 20 percent of the Head Start childrenin that region.The 1966 special- target counties of the region contain12 percent of the population.Con- centration was greatest in Georgia andTennessee.(See Exhibit II-6 for these comparisons.) The most difficult question to answeris whether the 1965 Head Start effort reached the mosteconomically and culturally deprived children within these counties.Although no survey of all or a sample of these counties was directed tothis question, two independent research studies of relatively small programsdiscuss the extent to which Head Start reached the target group in aspecific area.Alfonso Ortiz exam- ined Project Head Start in anIndian Community, San Juan Pueblo in New Mexico. Seven Indianchildren participated in this program,which was sponsoredby the San Juan Public School. OnSeptember 1, 1965, there were 71 Indian childrenbetween the ages of 4 and 6 in San Juan, 52 of whom came from economicallydeprived families.Only five of the seven Indianchildren attending were from theseeconomically deprived families. Ortiz concludes that"...Head Start did not serveSan Juan to any significant degreein 1965" (Reference 80). Knox County, Kentucky, with apopulation of 25,000, has a per 1 capita income of$612 per year.Head Start enrolled 452children from this county in 1965.Psychological Associates, inAn Evaluation of the Knox County` Kentucky, Project HeadStart, indicates that approximately 450 deprived children were eligiblefor this program. The extent ofde- privation is not defined. Although the reportdid not state that the

'1The U.S. per capita income in1963 was $2,449.

II -10 1111111912 a MENNEr REGIONAL POPULATION, REGIONAL HEAD START 1960 CENSUS ENROLLM NT MIDDLE \ ATLANTIC

4

29.1 MILLION 113,000

20.3 MILLION 1 1.4,000 SOUTHWEST

17.9 MILLION 90,000 Note:Only the Middle Atlantic, theSoutheast, and the Southwest are shown here.As indicated in Exhibit11-5, there are few Special-Target Counties in the Westand Far West. Shaded ,. )rtion: Special-TargetCounties

EXHIBIT II-6CONCENTRATION OF HEAD STARTCHILDREN IN SPECIAL-TARGETCOUNTIES 452 enrolled were all deprived children, it is presumed thatthe target group in Knox County didparticipate (Reference 4). As was discussed in Section I, OEO emphasized theinclusion of the 300 special-target counties in Head Start.This effort was rewarded with programs initiated in over two-thirds of the counties for almost 50,000 children.While all of the eligible children did not have the op- portunity to participate, the percentage ofeconomically deprived fam- ilies in the special-target counties is so greatthat it must be presumed that most of the children fulfilled the economiccriteria forparticipation.' 2. Characteristics of Head Start Communities Except for the 300 special-target counties mentionedabove, the emphasis on participation in the summer program wasplaced on economically deprived children and families, rather than onparticularly depressed communities.Therefore, any measure of Head Start success in reaching its target population must bebased upon the characteristics of the participating children andfamilies, rather than upon descriptions of the communities.2 The Head Start program has stressed that the incomelevel of the family is only one factor in determining anindividual child's need for the Head Start experience. Family characteristicswhich might contribute to the need include ethnic background,household size, extent and type of employment for either or both parents, the numberof parents (one or two) in thefamily, and the educational level of the parents.It is evident from its emphasis on the special-targetcounties that OEO also believes that the community environment maysubstantially affect the child's need for this experience.For example, the child of a poverty- level family living in a relatively prosperouscommunity may have more opportunity for a variety of experiences thandoes the child of a "slightly- above" poverty-level family living in adepressed area.This, of course,

Economic criteria are defined in Section 1, page1-9. 2Theextent to which Head Start reached this targetpopulation is dis- cussed in subsection II. C. 1.

11-12 is but one example and interpretation, butit does serve to illustrate the importance of describing the community as apotential factor for influencing the child. Descriptions of the communities served by the1965 program, then, may be veryvaluable in providing a framework for understanding the characteristics of and the program impact on the variousparticipants, and in suggesting types of information oncommunities which may be helpful in evaluating applications and programs. This subsection will focus on two areas:(1) a presentation of descriptive economic and social data fromselected communities served by Head Start,1and (2) a comparison of special-targetcounties served by Head Start with those special-targetcounties without programs. Seven independent research studiesprovide descriptions of com- munities served by Head Start programs.Four of these communities are in theNortheast, with a sample of Head Start communitiesin Mas- sachusetts; Northfield, Vermont;Rochester, New York; and Syracuse, New York. One communityis in the Midwest (GreeneCounty, Ohio); one in the West(Denver, Colorado); and one in theSouthwest (San Juan Pueblo, New Mexico).(See References 25, 99, 16,113, 21, 88, and 80.) As will be seen, theseindependent studies emphasize those demographic characteristics whichwill tend to indicate the economicand social conditions of the Head Startcommunities. In Massachusetts, some30 of 120 communities with1965 Head Start programs were sampledby Dr. Sarah T. Curwood.(See Refer- ence 25.) Fromcertain economic and social characteristics se- lected by Dr. Curwood, it would appearthat the population of the30

If the community is to beconsidered, then the boundaries ofthe com- munity must be clearly defined.It will 1.-e noted later in thissubsection that community will mean county,city, town, ward, orneighborhood, depending on the independent studyreviewed.Ln most cases, the eco- nomic and social characteristicsof a county or city are notsensitive enough to the conditions of theneighborhood in which the childlives. It will also be noted that thecommunity characteristicshighlighted vary from study to study, depending uponthe data presented.

II-13 -7 7-:--777-_,7-_.rr'',...7.._,"""_"""1"- y

communities is somewhat poorer thanthe entire populationof the counties in which they are located.From the informationpresented by Dr. Curwood, it is observedthat in over 70 percent of thecommunities surveyed, there are proportionately morefamilies with incomes ofunder $3,000 than in the counties as awhole:In over 80 percent of these com- munities, there areproportionately more individuals over25 years of age with lessthan 5 years of schooling thanin the counties as a whole. While there is no indicationthat these communities arerepresentative of the 120 served by HeadStart or that they are the poozestin the state, we can saythat the 1965 Head Starteffort did enter a number of com- munities which appear tohave somewhat poorer populationsthan the total county populations. Forty -eight children inNorthfield, Vermont, were servedby Head Start in 1965.(See Reference 99.) Northfield,with a population of 4,500, has a higher percentage offamilies with incomes of under$3,000 than does Washington County, inwhich it is located. Rochester, New York, had apr:)gram for 675 children.As re- ported by the Social ServicesDepartment of the RochesterArea Council of Churches, the cityand its suburbs arerelatively prosperous, with a substantialgrowth rate. However, itis stressed that, as in so many cities, there is a "...'communitywithin the community' ofpeople who have not been assimilatedinto the Rochestereconomy..." (Reference 16). From 1950 to1960, the city of Rochester'spopulation decreased slightly.The white populationspecifically decreased, while thenonwhite population showed a threefoldincrease.Population projections through 111 1980 indicate that thistrend will continue.This nonwhite populationis primarily centered in two citywards, where Head Start wasconcentrated. In one ward, there was ahigh percentage of childrenunder 18 years of age, and whilein the other section the percentage waslow, it was in- creasing rapidly. The levelof average income in both areas waslow. In 1960 the medianfamily income for the totalRochester population was $6,361; for the nonwhitepopulation it was $2,000 less.The unemploy- ment rate in the areasof poverty is 26.1 percent, ascompared with Rochester's rate of 1.5 to2.9 percent. The twowards had a high ratio of renter-occupied units, high vacancy r.ttes, and a high degreeof de- terioration and dilapidation.The nonwhite population alsohad a very high density rate (persons perhousehold) as compared with the white population. The schools in these areas areheavily nonwhite. The authors state that:"Performances of schools located nearestthe areas of poverty suggest that,while teaching staffs maybe of adequate quality, insufficient encouragement isgiven to students to achievemarks suf- qt. ficient for college entrance." It is the conclusion ofthe authors that the1964 riots were based on economicfactors: "the obvious affluence ofthe general com- munity in sharp contrast tothe 'ghettos'."It is also indicated that the voters in the areasof poverty are emerging as a morepotent force. To meet these problemsthe city government hasinitiated programs in education, recreation,employment training, housing,and urban devel- opment. The Rochesterstudy suggests that these programs werein- direct and that the new programsof the poverty agency,Action for a Better Community, are moredirect.The main emphasis of its programs was oneducation and youth.Interestingly enough, the programreceived a mixed responsefrom the community, withthe critics suggesting that the present programshad not reached the truly poor. Dunbar Association, aCommunity Chest neighborhoodcenter in Syracuse, New York,sponsored a Head Start progra-rnfor the neighbor- hood served. Manyfamilies served by the centerhave been relocated as a resultof urban renewal. Theprevious neighborhoodhad high de- linquency rates and lowannual family incomes.Thirty percent of the center members were1-parent families;approximately one-third re- ceived public assistance.The majority ofemployed fathers andmothers were unskilled orsemi -- skilled. (Reference113). In Denver, Colorado,where over 2,100children participated,the census tractscovered by the centersincluded those with:(1) the highest population density; (2) thehighest number of Spanishsurnamed people; (3) the lowest medianincome; (4) the highestconcentration of nonwhite population; (5) the highestconcentration of unemployedmales; and

11-15 (6) the largest number of general welfare and Aid toDependent Children recipients.(See Reference 88.) Greene County, Ohio, with programs for315 children, is relatively prosperous. Thepopulation growth for 1950 to 1960 was 60.7 percent, and by 1975 it is expected to growanother El percent. The county is first in the state in median income ($6,52C) andeducational level of the population (median school years completed: 12.1).However, 30 percent of the males over 25 years of age have completed 8 orless years of ed- ucation.Eleven percent of the county's families earn lessthan $3,000 per year, and1,786 families have incomes under $2,000.In Greene County, then, there arepockets of poverty.1(See Reference 21.) San Juan Pueblo, New Mexico, is aneconomically deprived com- munity.Of all the Pueblos in New Mexico, it has thelowest per capita acreage (see Reference'80). Compared with the other16 Pueblo Indian communities, it has the greatest percentageof off-reservation population.During the winter months, unemployment runs ashigh as 80 percent.One-half of the population is under 18 yearsof age. On October 1, 1965, 10 percent of theschool-age children were not attending school. Although Ortiz states that the bettereducated and ambitious leave San Juan and that the poorlyeducated and less capable remain, he says that formal education is uniformlyaccepted as a desirable goal by the San Juan population.The people are concerned about the area'seduca- tional facilities, and the county'spublic schools are substandard. As indicated in the above discussion of the300 special-target counties, Ortiz observed that the poor of SanJuan were not served to any signif- icant extent by the 1965 Head Start program. The review of these sevenstudies suggests that, in fourof them, the communities described contained asubstantial number of economically deprived families.In the other three studies, thetotal populations ap- peared to be less poor, although there wereeconomically and socially deprived families. i rIMMIw... Characteristics of families servedby the Greene County HeadStart program appear insubsection II. C. 1.

II- 1 6 Because of the OEO effort to locate HeadStart in the 300 special- target counties, information hasbeen organized to permit adescription and comparison of these counties i. e.,those served by Head Start versus those with no1965 programs. This was an attempt to discover whether differences in selected economicand social characteristics provided any clue as to why 33 percent ofthe special-target counties did not affirmatively respond to theOEO effort.The characteristics were selected fromthe County and City Data Book(Bureau of the Census, 1962).Exhibit II-7 presents the comparisons. From the characteristicscompared, it appears that the 67 percent with Head Start programs had aslightly poorer population group than the 33 percent with no program.The weighted average of themedian family income was 5 percent less inparticipating counties.There were pro- portionately more families with incomesunder $3,000, and the unemploy- ment rate was slightly higherin these counties.Finally, the percentage of housing units with completeplumbing facilities was less in the par- ticipating counties.It is to be emphasized,however, that these differ- ences are small. The population of the countydoes not appear to have contributed to its decision to have aHead Start program. Even oneof the smallest counties, Buffalo County inSouth Dakota (population 1,500),had a spon- sor and a programfor 75 children. On the otherhand, some of the largest of the special-targetcounties did not participate. Urbanization did not affect thesubmission of a proposal.The non-participating counties in theMiddle Atlantic, Southwest, and West do have greater rural farmpopulations, which are presumably more difficult to organize for projectssuch as Head Start.In the Southeast, however, where 55 percent of the targetcounties are situated, the par- ticipating counties have largerrural farm populations. The most consistent andsignificant differences betweenthe groups of counties are in the ethniccomposition of the population.Thirty-fiv.e percent of the populationof the participating countiesis nonwhite; 47 percent of the population ofcounties without programsis nonwhite. The nonwhite percentage of all 300counties is 39 percent.

II-17 EXHIBIT II -? CHARACTERISTICS OF SPECIAL- TARGET COUNTIES") Average County Percent Medianincome PercentFamilies PercentHouses National Head Start Number of Counties 201 3.208,098Population Total Population (mean)13,96315,961 Population1.547-89,102 Range Percent Urban 13.513.2 FarmRural35.234.1 NonwhitePercent 46.734.5 (weightedaverage)2,1132,215 Under $3,000with income 62.264.8 Unumployed Percent 6.25.6 Under AgePercentage 5 12.412.2 Plumbingwith all 32.130.4 MiddleNon-Head Atlantic Start Head Start 155999 1,282,4321,131,177 106,776 19,172 7,118 4.520-89,1021.513-81.493 6.37.3 41.931.5 42.316.2 2,2902,193 60.862.6 5.57.5 12.511.4 33.629.8 Non-HeadNon-HeadSoutheast Sta-rt Start Head Start 9670 1,525,972 949,432 13,56315,896 3,247-54,4643,221-20.2783,576-41,989 13.614.8 34.038.3 49.446.8 2,1802,050 66.662.9 5.45.2 12.4 30.530.4 Non-HeadSouthwest Start Head Start 1133 444,518215,378 19,23113,470 6,319-81,4934,393-37,60.5 21.822.8 30.825.4 43.128.0 2,2572,088 61.264.8 ' 6.57.0 13.012.2 35.932.2 West Head Start 39 10.84663,768 3,6157,065 1,547-31,3501,513- 4,749 8.6 - 64.141.4 26.1 7.4 2,4742,290 58.661.2 8.15.9 13.212.3 45.331.0 Non-HeadFar Went Start Head Start 4. 42,663 - 10,665 - 3,128-30,438 - - 13.7 - 81.6 - 2,562 - 56.5 - 15.4 - 141.8 - 24.1 - 1 Note: (1) All statistics are based on 1960 census data. Percentages may have changed.

."1 The Middle Atlantic area, principally Virginia, shows the great- est difference between the two groups ofcounties being compared. In Virginia the nonwhite population accounts for over 40 percentof the counties and county-equivalents without programs, and only20 percent of the participating counties. Although between special-target counties with Head Start programs and those without programs there were apparentdifferences in the ethnic composition of the population, it should be emphasized that it cannotbe concluded from this comparison that certain ethnic groupswithin these counties were served to the exclusion ofothers.'

5. Summary The summer 1965 Project Head Start entered 50 states, the District of Columbia, and 4 territorial possessions.Forty-seven per- cent of the counties in the nation were served.Over one-half of the counties in four of the seven regions (Northeast, MiddleAtlantic, South- east, and Southwest) had at least one Head Start grant. When compared with the population distribution of the UnitedStates, Head Start appears to have been most concentrated in the threeregions (Middle Atlantic, Southeast, and Southwest) with the most special-targetcounties.While the majority of children came from predominantlyurban counties, a significant number came from small counties withlargely rural populations. With OEO's special encouragement and assistance tothe country's 300 lowest per capita income counties in initiatingHead Start programs, it is noteworthy that two-thirds of these countieshad one or more pro- grams. When related to thepopulation size of these counties, their programs appear to have served agreater proportion of the children than did programs in counties elsewhere in thenation. Independent research studies on selected communitiesserved by Head Start reveal chat the researched areas with programstended to have poorer populations (in terms of familyincome) than state or

Available sources suggest that in every casethe Head Start application included a signed civil rights compliancestatement.It is not known whether other counties would have applied,had there been no compliancerequirement. II -19 national averages. When the special-target counties were examined to determine whether there were any differences in the population characteristics between counties with and those without programs, it was found that participating counties tended to have slightly poorer populations, but the most important difference was race. The available information, then, indicates that Head Start pro- grams appeared morefrequently and with greater concentration in com- munities with somewhat poorer families than state or national averages. The extent to which Head Start reached these poorer families is dis- cussed in succeeding subsections.

Or

11-20 C. Head Start Children

1. General The kinds of children HeadStart was intended to serve have been observed and describedby many educators and psychologists. Their findings have contributed to arapidly growing body of knowledge about the so-called culturally deprived orsocially disadvantaged child. There has been increasing studyof various characteristics of children who, by virtue of poverty,prejudice, lingual background, orother con- straining conditions appear toshow a progressive deficit in socio- psychological development. HeadStart was designed to find and help such children.In this subsection, we willtherefore present descriptive data to aid in evaluating programeffectiveness. In order, however, to establish aframework for (1) the descriptive or characteristicdata and (2) the data obtained onthe impact of the pro- gram on thechildren (see Section IV), we willfirst present a brief summary of someof the observations of social andpsychological characteristics of culturallydisadvantaged children which have been reported in the professionalliterature. The overview presentedin Exhibit 11-8 focuses onfour areas of particular relevance to HeadStartmotivation, cognitivefunctioning, social-emotional behavior, andlanguage characteristics of young, culturally disadvantagedchildren. The exhibit is intendedsimply to highlight certain relevantobservations made by each authorcited, and is in no way intended tobe exhaustive or to includeall the relevant ob- servations made by any of theinvestigators. Exhibit 11-8 shows that there is ageneral consistency of findings among observers.Of course, there are a great manyindividual varia- tions from child to child,with some childrenexhibiting more or fewer (or different) characteristicsthan other children. In summary, then, the culturallydisadvantaged child has often been found to becharacterized as fdllows: EXHIBIT 11-8 GENERAL CHARACTERISTICSSource Motivation Cognitive Functionin OF CULTURAL DEPRIVATION Social Emotional Development Language Behavior Hunt (Reference 60) InhibitionInappropriateFewer interests of questioningmotivation intrinsic(p.. 89) Perceptual deficiencies (p. 87) a. b.Recognize Fewer interests; fewer objects differentinterestsmiddle-classand situations children than most LackLocomotor of appropriatesocial ttndment approval(P.impaired manipulative 89)must behavi.)ral almostfor(p. appropriate89) develop- modelsinevitably be sharply Linguistic liabilities (p. 87) c.b.a. SyntacticalLimitedPoor articulation vocabularies deficiencies- -sentences.reliance onwith unusually faulty grammer short InhibitionLack of ofopportunity questioningwide(p. 89)variety to (p.encounter 89)of objects, ac. pictures Objects recognized differmiddle-classfrom those recognized children by behavior (p. 89) Guidelines forTesting Minority Less motivatedscholastic toward and academic LessCoded knowledgeableexposed information tolatingthatally intellectually indeprivedstored about materials middle-class inthe childrencultur- world stimu-in the children homediffers (p. (p. from 132) 90) MoreLess self-confident fearful"irritable" of strangers (p. (p. 132) 132) (p. 132) MoreLanguageLess apt verbal to heform (p. bilingual 132)may be (p. highly 132) con. (ReferenceGroup Children 45) Less competitive(p.intellectualachievement in 132) the realm (p. 132) (May view testinganding;inghave(p.outside 132)as asrandomshows primaryunpleasant, rapidly his immediatemore responses objective as guessing,possible, neighborhood (p. of 133))complet. skippingescap- CanLess influence varied in others recreationaldesirable(p. 132) to work or undesirahteoutlets toward goals (p. 136) (p.crate, 142) not abstract or symbolic Bruner MayLess reasonlearn opportunity rapidly, soundly,ilyteacherto those toconclusionsbut butdevelop notlessons (p.not necessar- 136)come adequate expectedassigned by(p. the 136) Principal deficiencythe broadestis linguistic sense in (References 11 and 12) Diminished hzterogeneitywithmodels(Referencenipulationducedp. 207)environment and set ofstrategiesand and12, opportunities discrimination p.re-(Reference 199) for dealing for12, ma- c.b.a. Lack ofof opportunityopportunity to toto share inter-parathoughtnalizein phrasedialogue speech (Reference as a vehicle 11, p. 72) to EXHIBIT III. 8 (Continued) (ReferenceAusubel and 3) Ausubel Source Likely to have ambivalentorityattitudes figures, toward to auth-cope Motivation Lower class ofmediate;children spondobjects totrainedparticularized (p. concrete, 114); to re-this tangible, properties(lack el im- Cognitive Functioning Social/Emotional Development Laneuage Behavior withfiguresactingdistance,appropriatemaintainingimplicitated them show with incompliance,by andofterms exagger-authorityeociaiformally overt, inter- of solvingmulation,expression,relationalemphasis (p. propertiesonlearning, 114) memory, abstract, and ] conceptaffectscategorical, problem verbal for- (ReferenceDeutsch 27) Less expectationward(p.completionattributesformalized of 17Z)for re- successful (p. ofrole 114) tank SocialPoor form conditions discriminationexperiencespatial reduce organization range and visual of (p. 170) ScarcityLess apt toof have toys,(p. a utensils,good167)167, 170)male etc. model SparesPoor auditory syntactical discrimination organization (p. (p. 171) 175) ' (References,26,Improving 116 115; and 98, 36)Eng liehSkills of Cultural?. (trent outs NegativePreference attitude for immediftandplanning,thanate (Smiley,toward pleasure controldelayed rationality, p. of ratherpleasure 44) their en- Attends substandard(Smiley,ing physical schools p, 36) resources (includ- and teachers) Aggressive"Hostility" behavior, (Moore,ance"learnsbut"Proc.:err"He not (Smiley, notgetsaction-oriented,p. inliscriminate 195) to his orrerciprOZITy... fear thrashingsp. them"45) (Smiley, (Davis, regularly of p. p.3 43) assist- 13)and Three.degrc'es"Tendency"Poor in use toward of ofp.than verbs" language43) physical verbal (Smiley, communication"handicap rather p.b.a. TrueFull (Smiley,but verbal not standarddestitution language 39) (ReferenceReissman, 94) Antagonism to schoolteachervironment and (p. (Smiley, 4-5) p.45) LackDo ofbetter education on (p.performance tradition4-5) in tests home of LikePoor tohealth, draw improper(p.(p. 67) 4-5) diet, noisy home PoorInsufficient with verbs, language(p. better 4-5) and with reading descrip- skillsc. Undeveloped, unconceptualized(Frazier,development p. 70-72) CreativityMoveUseThink fingers lips in whenoften spatialwhenacademicporalintelligence shownreading counting rather terms in (p. waysthannon- (p. (p.67) 67) tem-67)(p. 59) Slow,AreLessUseLike physical physicalcareful, role-playingintrospectivesports and (p. auralpatient,forms visual67) (p. (p.(p. of 67)rather more73)73)discipline than than (p.quick, 67) Understand more(p.tive than'76) adjectives can express (p. 76) Problem-centered,OrientedInductive,Content-centered, in not spatial,termscentered deductivecentered notnot (p.not abstract.(p. iovrn73) temporal(p. 73) 73) structuredExpressEnjoy fantasy self satiation bestmatters (p.clever, 76)in (p.spontaneous, (p. facile,76) 73) flexible un- in important EXHIBIT II-8 Source (Continued)Motivation Cognitive Functioning Social /Emotional Development La nctuagn Behavior (aGordon flowance 40) LessUse by affected parents by postponedbolicdiateof sym-reward imme- punishmentrewards (p. gratification 378) and and DifficultyLackLacking of systematicin in books, tion(p.andtransition 377) self-instructional(p. artwork, visual 378) from stimula- core, concrete equipment LessOverprotectionCrowdedEnvironment family homes activity is disciplineovercrowded, ofnoisy,(p. 377) (p. disorganized, 379) of boys austere (p. 37e) (Ir. 377) girls, inadequate UseWeaknessesHome of environmentlanguage in deficientfeelingstivethe as utilization aabstractless cognitive and1.p. verbal experience 380)symbols of tool(p. nonna- 378) is to (p. represent 379) LearnDrive more not complemente.tyquicklymaterialmentwith(p. academic384)383)(p. with 383)incentive achieve- InflexibleInferiorPoor in in visualin abstract intellectual(p.andto imagery abstract 379) categorizationconceptualization functioning (p.mode 379) of ofthought visual (p. stimuli 379) DistortedMoreImpairedLow behaviorial self-esteem patternsinterpereonnel(p.organization disturbance disorders383)of (p. personal-social 383) relationships (p. (p. 38).)383) 383) SkillsWeakConcepts inbetter conceptual arein thanabilityphysical content-centeredthan andvisual, (p.auralform-centered perceptualbehavior 381) and(p. 382) bettor rather (p. in Self-depreciation380) visual (p. 383) Fearful and passive (p. 363) HinesGrotberg(Reference 56)44) ParenteLess able do to not develop providestimulibooks,evaluating strategies learning information magazines, for (p. 418) etc. Vocabulave differences;wordsdisadvantaged than non-deprivedculturally use less "standard" (p. 418) Edwards(Reference 32) NeitherPoorInefficiency (short)language inattentionlem quantitativereasoningnor solving concepts span (p.and (p.thinking 6)of prob- 7) are familiar Passive/indifferentSpatiallySmallRestricted geographical confined physicalinquiry(p. and8) attitudeenvironmentarea deficient(p. (p. 10) toward3) concepts (p. 2) PoorDeficientOralRestricted languageauditoryvisual in vocabulary discriminationlistening discriminationoften(p. in comprehension (p.a dialect 4) (p. (p. 9) (p. 4) 4) 7) Was rudimentary(P.tative,(p. concepts8)8,9) spatial, in quanti-temporal aspects NoNotLowPoor clear, much levelsense notions supervisedof oi aspirationpersonaltions(p.a "nobody") of 12) valuesof social broaderworth (sees and(p. acVvity self(p.10) expecta-society 10) as (p. 12) Poor directional(p. orientation 9) for reading a. Motivation Responsiveness to immediate and concrete reward and punishment Less responsiveness to symbolic and delayed grati- fication Less expectancy of any reward for successful task completion Less positive responsiveness to teacher and school environment Less likelihood of being a recipient of social approval b. Cognitive Functioning Little access to books or instructional. materials More receptiveness to a physical mode of learning, less receptiveness to a visual mode, with aural mode being least likely to lead to the desired learning Poor form discrimination, visual-spatial organization, and visual imagery Inflexible intellectual functioning, with tendency to respond more to concrete than abstract concepts Primarily spatially oriented; poor temporal orientation c. Social-Emotional Behavior Lovr level of self-esteem and self-confidence Fewer and less varied recreational outlets a Fewer appropriate behavior models Unclear notions concerning values and expectations of broader society d. Language Characteristics Little and poor opportunity at home for development of language and speech Poor articulation; limited and non-standard vocabulary; short sentences; few verbs; and faulty syntax and grammar

11-25 Poorly developed auditory discrimination ability Language that is concrete rather than abstract or symbolic, and that is at variance with even regional standard English 2. Background Selected social, cultural, and economic characteristics of Head Start children are presented in this subsection in order to determine the extent to which the specific target group (children from economically and socially deprived families) was reached. The characteristics selected are age, sex, race, cultural background, size of household, family in- come, family intactness,mother's education, and mother's employment status.1The descriptions are from the 1-percent nationwide sample2 of Head Start children and from the children examined in a number of independent research studies.3 Fifty-five percent of the participating children were 4 and 5 years old (as shown in Exhibit 11-9). An estimated average age for the sample is 5 years, 10 months.Inspection of the special studies indicates an age range and distribution comparable to that shown in the 1-percent sample. One important factor affecting age for any given center was whether or not the community had a public kindergarten. Head Start communities wWaout public kindergartens tended to enroll older children than_did those with public kindergartens.In addition, Head Start children tended to be older than middle-class children enrolled in private preschool facilities. For example, in a study by Horowitz and Rosenfeld (Reference 59) com- paring Head Start children and University of Kansas Nursery School children, the difference in mean age between the Head Start children and the University nursery school children was 1 full year (5 years and 2 months for the Head Start children, and 4 years and 2 months for the

1The selection of characteristics was suggested by the Director of Re- search and Evaluation, Project Head Start. 2See subsection II. F for a discussion of the sample.Tables are adapted from those in Reference 89, or from a report prepared by the OEO Infor- mation Center.

3Someof the studies, called "special studicts,were funded by OEO, while others, called "local studies,were undertaken with other funding by uni- versities, institutes, and local agencies. 11-26 EXHIBIT II-9AGE(1)

Percent 3 years, 11 months and under 0.7 4 years to 4 years, 11 months 13.1 5 years to 5 years, 11 months 41.5 6 years and over 38.5 Not available 62 Total 100.0

EXHIBIT II-10SEX(1)

Percent

Male 49.5 Female 46.4 Nct reported 4.1 Total 100.0

Note:(1) N = 5036. From Reference 89. University nursery school children).In a study involving 178 Head Start children in Oklahoma (Mildred 0. Jacobs andJames K. Shafer, University of Oklahoma, Reference62), about 60 percent of the children were 6 years or older.In Massachusetts, where 8,444 childrentook pare in Head'Start, about 90 percent were 5 years orolder. From available data sources, it appears thatthe bulk of Head Start children were 5 and6 years of age. According to the information obtainedfrom a 1- percent sample of Start children (see Exhibit11-10) there was a 8148 ritlr ' J'i "'-1,-----TA alt rer 4.---- per - centage of boys than girlsin the programs. There is little information in the independent studies whichspeaks directly to the sex distribution of the children examined. The Jacobsand Shafer study of Cleveland County's (Oklahoma) 178 Head Start children is oneexample of a program where a slightly larger percentof boys than girls (52 percent boys) attended (Reference 62).It should be noted that severalinstitutions andstates/ are in the processof establishing data bases, so it will bepossible at a later time to compare sections of the countrywith national samples. The 1-percent sample datashown in Exhibit II-11 suggest that there was a fairlyequal number of white and Negrochildren in Head Start.Since descriptive data on race for nearly16 percent of the sample were not re- ported, it is not possible to makefirm or conclusive statements about the distribution of the Summer 1965 Head Startersby race.While special studies indicate that often there was not an evendistribution of Negro and Caucasian children within lasses, it isdifficult to obtain complete data, since information is often givenonly for children specially selectedfor inclusion in a study. Information available from the 1-percentsample on distribution of Head Start children by culturalbackground is shown in Exhibit 11-12. About 16 percent of the children camefrom the two cultural subgroups specified (Puerto Rican andMexican-American) with equal percentages (about 8 percent) from each.

111=6,1==011../MINMIIIMONION1111.11111111111 'WestVirginia, Iowa, and Urban Child Center(University of Chicago, Illinois).

II-28 1) EXHIBIT II-11RACE(

.104111..111,,..Percent

White 42.8 Negro 40.6 Asian 0.3 Eskimo American Indian 0.5 Unknown 1.4 Not reported 14.4 Total 100.0

EXHIBIT II-12CULTURALBACKGROUND(1)

Per cent Puerto Rican 8.0 Mexican-.American 7.8 Other 20.4 Unknown 63.8 Total 100.0

Note: (1) N = 5036.,From Reference89.

11-29 Exhibit 11-13 indicates that at: least 58 percentof the Head Start children came from households of six or morepeople.The Bureau of the Census estimates that, in1963, only 14 percent of the nation's families had six or more persons(Reference 79).Cohnstaedt's study of 424 Head Start children(representing 334 families) from Greene County, Ohio, reported thatthe average number of children in a Greene County Head Start family was3.2 (Reference 21). A study of the San Diego program indicated that themajority of Head Start families in- cluded 1 to 5 children, whilefully 30 percent included 6 to 10 children in the home (Reference10).When compared with Census data, then, it appears that Head Startchildren tend to come from larger households than national averages. Data from the 1-nercent sample shownin Exhibit H-14, indicates that at least 36 percent of the Head Startfamilies had annual incomes above $3,000 and that at least38 percent had annual incomes below $3,000.The large percentage (25.7percent) of unknown (unreported) incomes makes it impossible toobtain a true picture of the income level and range of Head Start parents.Even if that limitation were eliminated, however, data on reportedfamily income are often suspect.Typically, it is difficult to obtainreliable family income information without some independent assessment or checkingprocedure. Several of the independent studiesprovide data on the level of family income.Cohnstaedt's study of Greene County, Ohio,reported a median income range of$3,000 to $3,999, and/or a mean income of $3,700.Seventy percent of the Head Startfamilies had incomes below $3,000, as compared with 38 percent in thenational sample.In San Diego, 50 percent of the Head Startfamilies had annual incomes of less than $3,000 and 90 percent hadincomes below $5,000.In Northfield, Vermont (described by theinvestigator as a "typical Vermonttown"), where incomes of all 43 Head Startfamilies (representing 48 children) were obtainedfrom tax records, only 7 percent of theHead Start families had annual incomes below$3,000; 32 percent had incomes in the$3,000 to $4,000 range; about 20 percenthad incomes in the $4,000 to$5,000 range; and 42 percenthad annual incomes above$5,000 (Reference 99).

. II-.30 i 1) EXHIBIT11-13 SIZE OFHOUSEHOLD(

Percent

2 .8

3 4.8

4 12.5

5 17.3

6 16.3

7 . 13.5

8 9.6

9 6.6 10 or more 12.2 Unknown 6.4 Total 100.0

Note:(1) N = 5036. From Reference 89. In Massachusetts, Curwood pointed out that it was the consensus at the Head Start teachers' reunion in September 1965 that too few of the "hardcore poorn benefited. Most of the families were those with several children and were "slightly up" the economic scale (Reference 25). As has been noted in Section I, one of the original goal's of Head Start was to draw 85 percent of its children from families with incomes of f:;3,000 or less.The available information suggests that this goal was not meteFurther analysis of the problem is given in Appendix D. Additional information on statewide bases will become available when the several Head Start data banks (such as Iowa and West Virginia) have processed their information. Exhibit II-15 indicates that a high percentage (68.5 percent) of Head Start children came from 2-parent homes. There was little supplemen- tary information from available sources concerning "presence of parents, " but Cohnstaedt's special study on Greene County, Ohio, involving 424 children reported that 78 percent of Head Start households were headed by fathers (Reference 21)3 Exhibit 11-16 in that at least 60 percent of the mothers of Head Start children did not complete high school, while about 20 percent were high school graduates. Two independent studies also report sta- tistics concerning the level of Head Start mothers' education.Of the 334 Head Start families studied by Cohnstaedt in Greene County, Ohio, 24 percent of the mothers were high school graduates. The San Diego sta- tistics indicate that in 14 of the 18 centers (serving 544 children), more than 50 percent of the mothers had not completed high school.(In only 9 centers had more than 50 percent of fathers not completed high school.) Exhibit 11-17 shows that at least the majority (54.8 percent) of Head Start mothers were not employed outside the home. This finding is consistent with the high incidence of 2-parent families, where the father might be likely to be the sole breadwinner, especially since the mothers would be needed to care for the several children in the home. Certain background characteristics of the children, the majority of which were introduced above, are ielated to each other in 15 2-factor descriptions (Exhibits II-18 through II-24).Two-factor descriptions

II-32 EXHIBIT II-14FAMILYINCOME(1)

--- Percent Less than $1,000 9.0 $1,000 to $1,999 11.9 $2,000 to $2,999 17.2 $3,000 to $3,999 13.0 $4,000 to $4,999 9.2 $5,000.to $5,999 6.8 $6,000 to $7,999 4.9 $8,000 to $9,999 1.5 $10,000 or more 0.8 Don't know 15.3 Not reported 10.4 Total 100.0

(1) EXHIBIT 11-15WITH WHOM CHILD LIVES

Percent Mother and father 68.5 Mother 16.6 Father 0.8 Other 5.3 Unknown 8.8 Total 100.0 Note:(1) N = 5036. From Reference 89.

11-33 EXHIBIT II-16 MOTHER'SEDUCATION

Hi 122st Grade Completed Percent(1)

0 1.7

1-6 12.3 7- 8 16.8 9-11 29.7 High school graduate 20.5 College 5.4 Unknown 13.6 Total 100.0

EXHIBIT II-17DOES MOTHER WORK? 'Percent(1)

Yes 28.3 No 54.8 Not sure 1.8 Unknown 15.1 Total 100.0

Note: (1) N = 5036. From Reference89. EXHIBIT II-18 CHILD'S RACE(') White Other Unknown ByUnder 3 yrs., 7 mos. Child's Age 6 mos. 1.3 .4 Negro 1.4 ,4 .1.0 5.3 .5.2 B. 3Over4 yrs.,Unknown5 yrs., yrs., 67 7 yrs., mos.7 mos. mos. 6 to mos.to to4 5 yrs.,6 yrs., yrs., Sex 6 6mos. mos. 22.910.1 5.4 .7 23.613.0 2.8 .8 .8.6.0.1 3.75.0 .9 (Acp.) MaleFemaleBy Child's 21.518.7 20.7 .9.7 . 6.33.75.6 C. UnknownMotherBy works Mother's Employment Status 8.1 .6 18.Z16.8 .6 .0.9 7 8.33.2 ByUnknownMotherNone does not work Level of Mother's Education 26.8 6.0 .7 7.0 .3 .1.4.1 4.1 .4 High1UnknownAny97 to to 6 sch,00l118 collegeyears years years graduate 11.6 6.08.97.85.5 .4 c. 17.0 3.58.96.45.0 .8 .0.3.6.2.3 3.82.71.61.95.0 .2 1111111111111111111111MMILVOlt EXHIBIT II-18 (Continued) White Negro Other Unknown E. 3 inBytoI Household to 5Number 2 ofUncre:776" Children 18.613.0 5.7 . 17.810.3 9.2 .3.6.4 2.66.43.4 Note: UnknownOver6 to 8(1) N = 6309. Figures in each column of the tables are percentages of total sample. 2.51.0 1.82.9 .2.1 2.4 .8 EXHIBIT II-19 FAMILY INCOME(1) Mai A. By Child's....Ase $3,000Under $3,000 to$6,000 $6,000Over Unknown Under43Over5 yrs., yrs., 3 6 7yrs., 7 yrs.,mos. mos. 7 6 mos.to mos.to 45 yrs., 6 yrs., 6 mos. 6 6mos. mos. 22.7 4.27.51.4 .8.3 10.414.2 2.01.2 .9.4 3.22.7 .1.0.2.4 12.3 2.?8.41.42,4 .2 B. MaleUnknownFemaleBy Child's Sex 17.618.0 1.3 12.914.7 1.4 3.13.3 .3 12.213.3 1.9 UnknownMotherBy Mother worksdoes s not work Employment Status 20.612.5 21.118.5 8.3 3.92.3 .5 11.111.2 5.1 D. UnknownNoneBy1 toLevel 6 years of Mother's Education 3.97.56.7 .8 4.22.6 .2 .2.0.5 4.22.8 .4 UnknownAnyHigh97 to 118college school years years graduate 13.7 2.35.7 .2 11.1 2.28.0 .6 2.13.0 .4.5 10.7 3.1.6.1 .2 1 0 EXHIBIT 114 9 (Continued) Under $3,000 to Over 0 E. wzgiraraTEZETT6'By1 toNumber 2 of Children in $3,000 9.8 $6,000 8.4 $6,000 2.7 Unknown 6.2 Note: (1) N = 6309. UnknownOver3 6to to 85 8 Figures in each column of the table are percentages of the total sample. 16.9 8.51.6 .3 14.3 1.24.9 .2 3.1 .7.0.1 3.67.59.2 .8 EXHIBIT I/-20 MOTHER WORKS OUTSIDE THE HOME BY FAMILY INCOME LessFamily Than Income $1,000 (Annual) Total 11.9 9.0 Mother Works OutsideYes14.011.7 the Home 12.0No 8.3 Not Sure 7.66.5 (%)(1) 7.86.8 $4,000$3,000$2,000$1,000 to $4,999$3,999$2,999$1,999 13.017.2 9.26.8 18.118.9 7.47.6 11.215.018.1 7.5 15.2 6.56.53.3 10.9 2.95.56.2 $5,000Don't$10,000$8,000$6,000 knowto to $5,999or $9,999$7,999 more 15.3 4.90.81.5 14.0 5.21.02.4 15.7 5.80.81.3 34.8 3.30.0 13.7 0.30.81.8 N/ANote: (1) N = 5036. From Reference 89. Total 100.0 10.4 Details are percentages of caption total.100.0 4.7 100.0 4.2 100.0 16.3 100.0 43.3 EXHIBIT 11-21 NUMBER OF PEOPLE IN HOUSEHOLD BY EDUCATION OF MOTHER( 1) 0 Educationof Mother Total 2 3 4 Number of Peo le in Household (%) 5 7 8 9 10+ '----NTg. 1 Don't know 100.0 1:4 8.5 9.5 16.9 12.2 11.3 12.2 5.2 10.8 13.1 HighSome school college 100.0 1.5 6.9 18.2 24.5 15.0 12.8 . 10.2 4.4 5.8 0.7 Grades 9-117-81-6graduate : 100.0100.0 0.20.70.91.1 4.04.85.93.1 10.513.018.6 6.6 26.110.014.517.9 15.819.316.718.3 14.213.915.412.7 12.611.710.4 6.9 11.0 8.58.12.5 23.517.510.8 6.3 0.81.12.40.6 N/ANo school Total 100.0100.0 0.80.61.2 4.82.7 12,5. 8.76.0 17.311.9 8.5 16.310.7 6.4 13.510.7 2.7 9.64.67.1 .242.11.9 0.6 34.512.2 5.4 50.5 6.31.2 Note :. (1) N = 5036. From Reference 89. Details are percentages of stub total. EXHIBIT II-22 INCOME BY CHILD'S RACE

Income(%)(1) Ethnic ...... Classification Under $3,000$3,000-$6,000Over $6,000UnknownTotal White 12.6 12.9 3.5 11.8 40.8 Negro 19.7 10.6 2.0 9.5 41.8 Other 0.5 0.3 0.1 0.6 1.5 Unknown 4.1 5.1 1.0 5.5 .15.7 Total 36.9 28.9 6.6 27,4 998

Note:(1) N = 6309.,Details are percentages of total sample.

11- 41 EXHIBIT 11-23 HOUSEHOLD SIZE BY INCOME Number of People in Household (To)( 1) Less than $1,000Family Income (Annual) Total 9.0 22.525.0 2 -11.3 14.6 3 11.3.5.6 4 11.8 6.2 9.78.96 14.0 7.07 12.610.7 8 15.313.2 9 13.717.010+ N/A 2.22.5 $2,000$1,000$3,000 to to $2,999$1,999 $3,999 11.913.017.2 9.2 17.5 5.07.5 14.220.8 5.4 10.514.818.6 15.615.8 9.5 19.712.414.0 16.412.a 9.7 13.419.0 9.9 20.411.4 9.9 12.117.3 7.5 2.24.70.9 $6,000$5,000$4,000 to $7,999$4,999$5,999 4.96.81.5 2.50.0 4.25.8 2.46.57.8 3.06.89.5 8.11.55.2 6.51.27.9 4.71.26.8 2.11.85.1 0.33.43.6 0.0n:00.6 $10,000$8,000 to or $9,999 more 0.8 0.0 0.4 1.6 1.7 0.5 1.0 0.8 0.0 0.2 0.0 it Don'tN/A know 15.310.4 12.5 7.5 15.8 7.1 14.9 6.0 15.9 4.2 14.8 5.2 14.8 8.7 15.1 5.8 15.9 4.8 19.1 5.9 78.1 8.8 il Note: (1) N = 5036. Total From Reference 89. 100.0 100.0 100.0 Details are percentages 100.0 100.0 100.0 of caption tcitals. 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 iiI', EXHIBIT 11-24 HOUSEHOLD SIZE BY ETHNIC CLASSIFICATION Ethnic 3 Number of People in rousehold 5 6 7 8 (%)(1) 9 10 + N /A MexicanPuerto Rican American Classification Total100.0 0.20.32 3.35.7 11.9 5.34 18.011.6 16.717.3 12.315.4 14.2 6.4 10.4 3.7 15.919.5 6.5 4.98.8 ttPI41-4 WhiteNegro 100.0100.0 0.70.01.0 4.70.05.3 19.1 7.19.0 15.321.614.3 28.616.517.2 13.113.814.3 11.5 8.47.1 0.03.59.5 21.414.6 3.94.87.1 AmericanEskimoAsianUnknowa Indian 100.0100.0 0.0 0.04.2 0.06.32.9 16.711.5 0.0 12.5 0.08.8 20.811.8 0.0 20.612.5 0.0 0.08.35.9 0.00.08.8 19.1 0.04.2 10.312.5 0.0 N/ANote: (1) N = 5036. Total Reference 89. 100.0100.0 0.81.1 Details4.83.5 are percentages of 12.510.6 17.317.2 16.311.5 stub13.512.6 total. 9.65.7 4.66.6 12.211.3 21.9 6.3 S.*- 41,18Narr

by race and income for age, sex,mother's emplOyment status, level of mother's education, andnumber of children under 16 in thehousehold are summarizedin Exhibits 11-18 and 11-19.Comparisons of family income and mother'semploy-xi:lent status, mother's education andsize of household, familyincome and race, and family income and sizeof household are presented in2-factor descriptions in Exhibits 11-20 through 11-23. ExhibitII-22 shows a breakdownof income distributions by race.Of the total I-percent sample,nearly 20 percent came from Negro families whose incomes wereunder $3,000 per year, while only about 13 percent came fromwhite families whose annual, in- comes were under$3,000.The large percentage of unreported fam- ily income prevents onefrom obtaining a complete and accurate

. picture. As reported in the 1-percentnational sample, at least 38 percent of the Head Start familieshad incomes under$2,000.Sixty-four percent of these had six or more personsin the household.While most of the 64 percent had from 6 to 8 in thefamily, many had 10 or more persons per household(see Exhibit 11-23). Fifty-six percent of thefamilies with incomes over$3, 000 had six or more persons.Fewer families were extremelylarge. From this data it appears that HeadStart children of all incomelevels tended to come from largefamilies. Inasmuch as there is arelationship between economic well-being andfamily size for any given level of income, an evaluation of the extent to whichHead Start reached impoverishedfamilies must, as has been suggested,take into account the size of theHead Start family. Finally* Exhibit 11-24 shows thenumber of people per household for different ethnic groups. 3. Medical and Dental An important objective forProject Head Start, Summer1965, was to providemedical and dental evaluationsfor all the participating children and to initiate referralsfor the care of existing health problems

11-44 "42.-Hrtn!"--tr"'""*115.11-6.,--1111111111,110111110t

detected among the children. Some of the data collectedin these evalu- ations will be summarized in this subsection. Inasmuch as the majority of Head Startchildren were to come from disadvantaged families, it was hypothesized that thesechildren might have more medical problems and many more unmetmedical needs than children in the population as a whole. The datacollected in the 1965 program was notdesigned to test this hypothesis.It was designed as a service program to provide medical anddental care for children in low income families, and not to undertake anextensive research effort to test these hypotheses. The data andtheir interpretation are primarily useful from the point of view of further programplanning, if not of basic research. That is, there are valuable implicationsin the results for planning of services, personnel, facilities,and data collection or recording forms. Head Start required the completionof medical and dental history and evaluation forms (See AppendixA). The forms were intended to suggest what might constitute acomplete examination and to provide a starting point for referrals forneeded care. As a research tool, they had severe limitations. For example,there was no independent check used to estimate the reliability or validityof some of the data entered on the form. No special training wasprovided for the nurses, doctors, and others who completed the forms.In addition, there was little provision for controlling the consistency in the useof the instrument. Criteria for diagnoses or classification of conditions wereprovided with the medical/ dental form. However, there is everyreason to believe that these were not followed uniforn-dy.It is known, too, that in many cases,there were not adequate facilities or personnelfor proper examination or diagnosis. Nor were there standardized procedures. These factors are reflected in the dataaccumulated and reported from these forms.EV!! example, it is known that medicaland dental examinations were not provided fcr allchildren, even though approved programs had planned toprovide such services.It is difficult to determine from the data, however, how manychildren actually received examina- tions rather than simply interviews.In the National Opinion Research

II-45 --,--...... ,..4.,

Center (NORC) interview survey,84 percent of the respondents said that their child had received amedical examination, and only 69 percent said that their child hadreceived a dental examination.In Chicago's program, only68 percent of the Head Start children wereexamined (13, 720 out of a total of19,980 in the program), although it should be noted that Drs. IrvingAbrams and Ralph Spaeth specificallydecided to examine only thosechildren whose parents would bring them toequipped medical centers, so thatexaminations could be thorough. Despite the substantial qualificationsmentioned above, information onmedical/dental history and evaluations obtainedfrom the 1- percent sample and selected independentreports are presented here because they are valuable for several reasons.First, a goal of Head Start was to provide adequatemedical and dental services to the children.To do this, it is necessary tc, provide anadequate detection or case-finding system, which meansadequate personnel, facilities, equipment, standards, and procedures. Tothe extent that the data point to specific inadequacies, it is possible forplanners to determine where and how to improve programs. Second,although the nationally collected data, as provided in the I-percent sample,cannot justifiably be regarded as re- liable research data, they do in anumber of instances provide some rough estimates and inferencesabout medical/dental deficiencies.Thus, they provide some basisfor future program planning. Athird reason for presenting these data is that there were some programswhich provided thorough examinations for theirparticipants.While all of these local I programs will notbe described here,the data obtained are of interest, especially as contrasted with the1 - percent sample findings. The medical/dental historyof the Head Start children issummarized in Exhibit II-25.This exhibit contains frequencydistributions for the history items from theMedical/Dental Form for the 1- percent sample. The items include informationabout hospitalizatic a, general health, activities, illness, and visits todoctors.Interpretation of the data is compromised by the large numberof wunknowe responses.

"Pewlesommar.IM11 1Reports of several local programs arediscussed in Section III.

II-46 EXHIBIT 11-25 MEDICAL/DENTAL HISTORY (TOTAL) 11B.IIA. Medical/Dental Item Number HospitalizedHowyear during past often?(3) CrOnceYes 69.4Ma No 2-3 Times Don't Know 3.3 4 or More 1.9 UnknownPercent(1) 11.3 Unknown 15.2 12.13. HospitalChildren born in problem(3) 1747-HospitalIllness 71.13 --127TSurgery 5 -MTHometo 7 8 777toOther 10--14.6Accident Don't KnowOver 10 4.3Tonsillectomy 13.9 Unknown Unknown 9.4 OtherTu7r 13.6 14.17.15. AnythingWeightGeneralbirth(nearest at pound) unusualhealthbirth at --"Kir-UnderYesVery 5 Good 77:9No 55.5 Don't-173-- GoodKnow51.2 Fair19.6-KT - Unknown Poor0:4-13.4 --lc).Unknown 1 20.4 19.20. . ActivitiesLast visit of to children doctor -ET--53.-r=UnablePast Year 50.6 Limited 6.6 1 -2 Years 15.7 Not Limited 79.6 Over 2 Years 12.2 Unknown-712.4 -67T-Never Unknown 21.22. accidentSeriousLast visit illnessIn to past dentist or year 'CZ'PastYes Year 20.1 7 ."r6 No Don't1-2 Years Know 2.84.3 Over 2 YearsUnknown 18.5 .--- Never57.5 Unknown"7673--=KT- Note: (1) N = 6309 (2)(3) Some N = 527questions (total "Yes" responses Whereis the applicable,remainder of unless otherwise noted. included a space for a these are reflected.the N which may not to 11A). "Don't Know" or "Can'tForhave each answered question orthe been Remember""Unknown" response.percentageprocessed. _jt. 4.1141144.411111.10

It is assumed that the 'responses tothe questions in Exhibit II-25 were obtained through aninterview, probably with a parent.Although this was not always the case, andalthough the parent may not always have been able. to providereliable information, nonetheless, some interesting observations arepossible. For example, the datasuggests that the populationexamined did indeed represent a groupthat had received poorer carethan other socio- economic groups. Anindication of this is the number ofchildren who were not bornin hospitals and had notvisited a doctor or dentist in the recent past. As shownin Exhibit 11-26, there arehigher percentages of Negro children bornoutside hospitals and of Negrochildren who have never visited adoctor or dentist.Other differences by race appear to be in the number ofhospitalizations, where the Negroeshave a lower percentage. However,the Negro children appear tohave had more serious illnesses.This data is analogous to thatreported by the National Health Survey. Children in lowincome families were hospitalizedless frequently but they tended to havelonger hospitalizations, suggesting moresevere illnesses. Exhibit 11-27 compares selected1-percent samplemedical/dental history information with twospecific programs. A description of theimmunization history of the HeadStart children from the I-percent sampleis given in Exhibits IT-28and 11-29. Again, the data in these exhibits aredifficult to analyze due to thelarge per- centage of"unknowns" for each item.In most cases this percentage ex- ceeds 30 percent.While many immunizationhistory forms may not have been completed for reasonscited in the discussionof the medical/dental history, it is also likelythat many parentsinterviewed did not know the immunization status of theirchildren.In Chicago, a reviewof 2,460 Head Start recordsrevealed that many parents wereunable to recall the number of immunizationstheir children hadreceived. The "yes" and "no" responses tothese questions may also be suspect.Parents may not havebeen informed, forexample, as to what constitutes a com- pleted DPT or Oral Polioseries.Thus, the best estimatethat can be

.11-48 EXHIBIT 11-26 MEDICAL/DENTAL HISTORY AND RACE Medical/Dental11A.11B. Item Number HowpastHospitalized year during often?(3) SM.= '67.78Once10* -0*1"' Yes 2-3 Times No Don't Know4 or More White-Ne77E ro(1)UnknownUnknown AMININIONNIND 12.13. HospitalChildren pr born in oblem(3) -3T.:31rIllnessHospital 80-70 sue/ '7r :TT--Sur ery At home 8-Trair°Accident to 10 71MOther Over 10 Don't KnowTonsillectomy Unknown Unknown /MTOther =6:ToUnknown 14.15. AnythingWeight(nearest at pound) unusualbirth Under67:3-Yes 5 . No J85 to 7 trDon't Know 737:Tir Unknown 17.19. GeneralatActivities birth health of children UnableVery Good 37-3Z -- Limited 5-84ci-1 Goodto 2 Years FairNot Limited OverPoor 2 Years Unknown Unknown 7 9 Never Unknown 20.21. Last visit to doctordentist . --3-67:Ttr7Past Year 1 to 2 Years Over 2 Years 2-2 57-64-778Never Unknown-1=7-- Note:22. (1) White N = 2575, Negro (3)(2) White*White;accidentSerious N **Negro.= illness in252, past Negro or year N = 199 (totalN = 2648,"Yes" unlessresponsesse:74.Yes otherwise r7m. No Don't Know noted. Unknownto 11A). 11111114111111111111.1.111.11111111111116111111101iikiridslikiiiiirarak...:: _ EXHIBIT 11-27 MEDICAL/DENTAL HISTORY: HEAD STARTPercent SURVEY COMPARISONS o N1 = -Percent 6309Sample 11011110.1101111NO, N = 345 (341)Alexandria Warminster, NPerna. = 30 Last visit to doctor OverPast1-2 yearyears2 years 12.215.750.6 6.1 80.013.0 2.05.0 16.720.053.3 HI4 $ NeverUnknown 15.4 - c$kil SeriousLast visit illness to dentist or accident UnknownNever'Past1 year year or more . 20.1(1)16.357.5 5.6 40.049.011.0 - 10.076.7 6.76.7 past year NY Yes 4.2 - 13.3 NO Hospitalization past year NoUnknownDon't know 18.574.6 2.8 - ON Yes 8.3 - 13.3 WO Note: (1) By way of comparison, the NoUnknownDon't know the 4- to 5-year-old children of 809 families 195611.377.0 California3.3 Health Survey reports that 49 percent of had visited the dentist at least once.- ON ..0Appplowingpmpippummilimmumnimplamommuini CM It= I EXHIBIT II-28 IMMUNIZATION It= HISTORY=3 (TOTAL) I 111111111001a1111.1100111111111011111011111243. Medical/Dental Item Number DPT series complete 51.5Yes 14.418.7No Don't KnowPercent 8.55.8 (1) Unknown 28.320.9 27A.26B.25. MeaslesSalkOral vaccine polio vaccine series received received complete 48.351.911.8 6.2 61.413.154.5 4.47.28.4 32.134.218.4 U111-4 28.29A. TBVaccination vaccine (BCG) scar presentreceived 54.6 22.8 5.3 17.3 0.1 ' 293. Howsmallpox many vaccination years since last Within-439.2 3 Years Yes More Than 3 Years 14.9No Don't Know-TrrNever . Don'tUnknown Know 17.4 Unknown Note:31.30A. (1) NtuberculosisChildrensummerTB = 6309. test received1965 exposed before to active 1-674 2.8 627271.5 11.0 7.5 14.7 EXHIBIT 11-29 IMMUNIZATION HISTORY AND RACE 0 0 'WM Medical/Dental Item Number White (%)(1) ...... -- IMININIMB Negro Don't (%)(1) 25.24B. DPT series complete ----15-67riYes6156 t No 2113 Know 78 Unknown 1619 Yes 4854 ONONo1817 Know 1913 Unknown 16.15 27A.26B. SalkMeaslesOral vaccine polio vaccine receivedseries received complete 1356 6713 367 .28 14 4712 8 626114 1114 7 322113 29A.28.30A. VaccinationTBTB vaccine test received (BCG) scar present beforereceived 61 3 226471 75 2512 9 6013 2169 11 8 95 Note:31. (1) White N = tuberculosisChildrensummer i965expz)ved to active 2575; Negro N = 2648. 14 3 75 14 9 3 76 13 9 made from this data is that a large numberof Head Start children had received some of these inoculations.This uncertainty is a significant problem in assisting disadvantagedchildren; it suggests that a great deal more attention needs to be given to waysof obtaining adequate and valid data from which the kindand amount of services needed can be determined. The immunization historyinformation from the 1-percent sample indicates that at least 2.8 percent of thechildren had been exposed to active tuberculosis, which is probably anunderestimate. Assume for a moment thatthis indeed represents an exposure rate in the Head Start population of 2,800 per100,000.Then consider that the national incidence of new cases of tuberculosis per yearis around 30 per 100,000. If the data have any significance, they suggest anespecially high proba- bility of occurrence of the disease in thepopulations served by Head Start.Thus, it is of particular interest to note that the majority of the children had not received a tuberculin test.The implication is clear enough; special efforts must be made toreach these children with medi- cal service. One of the most readily identifiable itemsin an immunization his- tory is the presence of a vaccination scar.Slightly over half the children were reported to havehad a scar.The percentage of children who were never vaccinated ishigher than data provided in studies of the general population.It is possible, of course, that the low percentage of vaccina- tions for these children may stem from the fact thatimmunizations are generally brought up to date at the insistence of school authoritiesprior to entrance into school. Aside from the data provided in the 1-percent sample, there is some data in threeindependent studies. The San Diego study reported that, for 547 children, approximately 77 percent neededboth a measles vaccine and a tuberculin test.In Chicago, a review of 2,460 Head Start records revealed that many parents were unable to recall thenumber of immunizations their children had received. Exhibit 11-30 primarily compares data in the 1-percent samplewith certain local Head Start studies, in addition to a1956 survey of child health in California. I1- 53 EXHIBIT II-30 IMMUNIZATION HISTORY: SURVEY COMPARISONS Head% StartSampleN = 6309 N =Greene County375 Head Start (%) San DiegoN = 547 Warminster, NPenna. = 30 1956 SurvmCaliforniaGeneral (%)N = 809% '1 DPT inoculations Yes 51.5 -(3) IND 53.5 ,.0 18.0(2) No 14.5 21.7,. IMP 46.7 Live attenuated Don'tUnknown know 28.3 5.8 polio vaccine NoDon'tUnknownYes know 48:334.213.1 4.4 200.;(4) . 23.33.6.740.0 ON Measles vaccine 4 II11 r . Yes 11.8 ON ece=ea--- OP 79.0 NM presentVaccination scar NoDon'tUnknown know 18.461.454.6 8.4 Yes IMP UrNo1."):.).11't known know 22.817.3 5.3 34.1. SNP EXHIBItr 11-30 (Co tinud) A Head% StartSampleN = 6309 N = 375CountyGreene Head Start (%) San DiegoN = 547 Warminster, NPenna. = 30 1956 SurvmGeneralCalifornia (%)N = 8091" I . . Number of years since ilM el. last vaccination 0 0 MS Under 3 years 39.2 0 00 a On NeverOver 3 years 14.915.513.1 0HO a0 Oft SO 1.41-4LTI TB test received Don'tUnknown know before 1965 NoYesDon'tUnknown know 62.216.413.5 7.5 a0.. 77.0 .0 ..0OW .0000 Notes: -(4)(1) (2)Percentage(3) Percentage Percentage who 1956 survey of 809 whodeemed have adequately had no have had no Salk or families of 4 to DPT shots. protected. 5-year-old Sabinchildren. vaccine. The results of the physical examinations for the1-percent sample are shown in Exhibit 11-31.The tests to be given to each child included visual screening, tuberculin testing,urine tests, blood tests, and hearing tests, as well as a dentalexamination. The reports of these examinations suggest that there was'often a need for additional personnel and facilities to conduct adequate examinations.There is evidence that such resources were not sufficientlyavailable in 1965. There is no indication onthe forin of whether or not the child was given a physical examinatiOn.The data front the dental examination indi- cate that 44 percent of theChildren had some type of dental defect.The usual finding is a much higherincidence of children with dental defects in this age group.In Head Start it is unknown what typesof examinations were given or whetherthey were given by a dentist, doctor, or nurse. If X-rays had been available, forexample, the incidence Might have been higher. The problem of adequatedental examinations is heightened by the fact that several counties had toofew dentists to be able to participate in Head Start. The results of the tuberculin testing arenegated by the large number of unknown responses. These suggest that a test mayhave been given but that it WF:t. S not read at the appropriate48-hour interval:, .thus probably indicating poor follow-up.Urinalyses for protein and sugar also showed 50 percent unknown responses. Quiteprobably, "unknown" means that the test was not carried out, indicating nofacilities and personnel were avail- able for this procedure. In GreeneCounty, ,Ohio, for example, urinalyses were performed onlyin one of eight communities with Head Start programs. Almost half of the children in the sample did nothave a hemoglobin determination or were designated as unknown fur this category.This is particularly unfortunate since the test is relatively simple andthe results generally reliable.The personnel and eqUipment may not have been available for this test. The available data for the sample testedindicate that at least 34 percent had hemoglobin levels suggestinganemia (see Exhibit 11-31, Item 39). A somewhat smallerper6'entage was found in Chicago (see Exhibit II-32).The determination of possible anemia with

ID. 56 EXHIBIT II-31EXAMINATION RESULTS(TOTAL)(1) C r Item(2) Percent L 32. Visual screening tests A. Acuity Abnormal 3.6 Test normal 45.1 L Not done 5.7 Unknown 45.3 C. Hyperopia Abnormal .3 Test normal 8.5 Not done 17.4 Unknown 73.8 D. Cover test Abnormal .5 Test normal 14.1 Not done 14.3 Unknown 71.0 E. Worth dot test Pass 4.5. Fail .3 Unknown 95.2 F. Other abnormality noted .3 33. Dental examination A. Performed by Dentist 47.0 Physician 19.9 Nurse 6.5 Other 2.5 Unknown 24.1

II -57 EXHIBIT II-31(Continued)(1) .

Item(2) Percent

B. Findings Carious teeth: 1 -3 21.3 More than 3 17.8 Unknown - Infection 1.9 Malocclusion 3.9 Periodontal disease .5 No disease 32.3 Other 1.6 Unknown 30.5

34. Weight (in pounds) Under 31 1.2 31 to 36 7.3 37 to 42 26.7 43 to 50 38.3 Over 50 10.8 Unknown 15.7

35. Height (in inches) Under 36 1.1 37 to. 40 3.1 41 to 44 . 28.1 45 to 48 46.2 Over 48 4.7 Unknown 16.8

36. TB test given at centers Mantoux 17.0 Tine 22.4 Patch 4.6 Unknown 56.0

II- 58 c

EXHIBIT II-31(Continued)(1)

Item(2) Percent

C 37. Results of TB test Positive .8 C Negative 52.0 Unknown 46.7 C Uncertain .4

38. Urine Test C A. Albumin Positive 1.5 Negative 46.5 Unknown 52.0 n B. Sugar Positive .3 Negative 53.0 Unknown 46.7 II 39. Blood determination Microhematocrit (percent) C Below 36 3.4 36 or above 6.3 E Hemoglobin (gram-0/0) Below 11 34.1 11 or above 13.6 II Unknown 42.5

40. Hearing fl A. Screening test . Not done 9.0 Li Audiometry 38.0 Voice or other 20.8 Unknown 32.2

II- 59 EXHIBIT II-31 (Continued)(1)

Item(2) Percent B. Results Unsatisfactory test 2.3 Normal 59.2 Abnormal 1.2 Unknown 37.3

Notes:(1)N = 6309. (2)All item numbers refer to medical/dental form. "71 --"-C-7;L471 E.:17 "T":.7 W.sys rr ...... 4.4...111TIMPIWIPPirtw7/4WWWw.rwwwwillIppigpm.ispplimpuipppuppow, EXHIBIT II-%32 MEDICAL IMPRESSIONS: SURVEY COMPARISON Greene I iI Head Start Surveys General Survey NastiornvaelyMalth Medical Problems Head Start/. Sample ...Chicago County, 0.11.4135) Cambridge, T. T. San Diego(N = 547) Alexandria,(NVirginia :: 330) Warminster, Penna. ozalLe211 MO 5.9 Year, al..fain_ iI%1 N % 1.4assachusetts T. I la,. 291 I I I lc otherAsthma, allergies hay fever, 4.1 - . 1i I - ...... 5.7 8.0 3,2. bronchitisillness,FrequentotherSkin infections disease including respiratory and 2.93.8 - 5.6 -. 12.3 3.7 - - -- ..- 4.5 -- 6.7 - 2.7 .6 3.7 .6 4.5. AnemiaTonsil/adenoid diseaseor10.9 less grams hemoglobin 34.1 8.75.3(4 13,720 2,3192,319 . 31.6 8.5 - -- - -.- -- 4.8(2) .. 0.0 .. .4(2) - .3(4 "- ry 7.6. HeartHernia disease or9.9 less grams hemoglobin 2.1 .8 - 13,720 .. 2.22.4 5.91.3 -- -. - 4.22.4 . -- .3.9 .4.5 10. 8.9. ParasiticEnuresisOrthopedic infestation problems . 4.22.82.3 - - 6.7 ..- -... - -- .3 -.. 23.0 . -. - 14.13.12.11. ConvulsiveHearingSpeechUrinary abnormality infections(3)impairments disorder 2.01.2(5)2.3 .5 17,11513,720 - 2.25.5(9) .- 14.7(4) 3.5 330341366 .. 13.0(5)10.0 7.0 ..- -... 5.2 -.. 13.310.013.3 - -.. .4.8 -.. Notes:15.16. DentalVision defects (2)(1) HemoglobinBased on household count not interviews identified. (July 19595-9: 18.692,000). . June Percentages aro based on average annual number of 44.1 7.1(6) 14,5932,376 36.810.4 1961). 58.7(7)Percentages in this table are. based on 1960 Census 398 chronic conditions. 33.0 21.0 23.0 data for these two age groups (0.4: 46.7 . 20,321,000; - (7)(6)(5)(4)(3) UrinalysesAccumulativeNPercentageIncludes s 342. urinary performedwho percentages fallentract onlyinfections, or had ofon defectsabnormal150 kidney children; noted results disease, inpercentage several from and test. teats; is see albumin in urine. of 150. Exhibit II-31. this relatively objective test in such a large percentage of children suggests that special efforts should be made to investigate thisproblem further. The results of the hearing test indicate that only a small percent- age of the childrenhave hearing abnormalities. However, it must be stressed that Exhibit 11 -3I indicates that as many as 60 percerit of the children might not have received an adequate hearing test.1 The large number of "unknowns" may mean that the test was not given, that there was loss of information at some stage of data handling, or that some children were notable to cooperate with this type of test- ing.2It is possible that children from this socio-economic group are not familiar with testing techniques and will, therefore, require more of the tester's time.This last point is another factor to be considered in planning the scope and cost of a program. The independent studies provide little additional information con- cerning the vision and hearing tests administered as a part of Head Start, Summer 1965 (Exhibit 11-32).In Chicago, 70 percent of the Head Start children received vision tests in the schools, and 9 percent of the tested children showed vision defects.Eighty-three percent of the children received hearing tests in the school (that is, the vision and hearing tests were not part of the medical examinations, which were conducted at regular clinic facilities), and 5 percent of those tested showed defects.In the visual acuity tests conducted by the Cambridge, Massachusetts, Health Department for the Head Start program, 9 percent of the children showed some kind of visual defect, 7 percent failed the hearing test, and 70 percent required dental treatment. The problem of non-testability was not discussed in these studies, but its implications have been noted above.In general, the need for adequate assessment of vision and hearing takes on added importance with socially disadvan- taged children in particular, since these modalities are so important

'Question40A: 9 percent not done; 20.8 percent received voice or other test (generally considered unacceptable); and 32.2 percentunknown. 2For example, a study of visual testing of low incomepreschool children by Dr. Roberta A. Savitz, et al. , found that raore than1/3 of the children were unable to understand the requirements of the test and to cooperate. (Children's Bureau Publication No. 414, 1964.) II-62 in the learning process and sincethese children are apt to be subject to reduced visual andauditory discrimination ability from nonphysio- logical stresses as well. Height and weightdeterminations are reasonably objective measures ofgrowth and provide one of the few relativelyuniform measures forcomparisons in these examinations. AppendixC includes percentiles of heights andweights for different age levels by sex. In Greene County the averageweight and height of the Head Start children were comparedwith normal values from data collected bythe Department of Maternal andChild Health, Harvard School of Public Health. Results suggestedthat in some age ranges Head Start children are lighter than theHarvard weights for the same age and sex.Over- weight is also manifest. Both of theseconditions can be indicative of poor nutritional status. The information concerning thediagnostic categories for the 1-percent sample, selected Head Startmedical reports, and the National Health Survey of the totalpopulation is given in Exhibit 11-32. As stated earlier, it had been anticipatedthat the Head Start children, comingfrom low income families, wouldhave a higher incidence of medical problems than other child populations. Thedata in the 1-percent sample at least suggest a much lower incidencethan expected. The prevalence levels found in the independent medicalstudies tend to be higher than the 1- percent sample finding.However, it is not known whether therelatively low incidence shown in thesample is (1) representative of the general health of the children, or (2) suggestivethat the examinations were in- aacquate. As noted before, someevidence indicates that in many programs adequate personnel and facilitieswere lacking. Also, in order to diagnose completely many of the medical problemslisted in Exhibit 11-32, trained personnel and specialized facilities areneeded. The extent and implica 1.ons of theseuncertainties are worth further consideration. The diagnosis of allergies,for example, requires special- ized personnel and facilities, as well assufficient time to explore with a parent significant areas of pasthealth.The factor of time and experience in interviewing is important in elicitingreliable information about enuresis

11-63 1 and many other problems.Skin problems frequently require more than one examination.The diagnosis andevaluation of anemia needs equipment. The detectionof heart disease requires that 6ildren be sent to specializedclinics for proper diagnosis.Enuresir, was noted for 2.8 percent of childrenin the sample; other studies haveindicated that the rate for childrenaged 4 through 6 years is about10 to 25 per- cent. Enuresis isdifficult to determine except byintensive probes to elicit this inform& .fori.The observed result in the Head Startsample may be due to alack of time arid trained personnel. Diagnoses were made of somemedical problems not listed in Exhibit 11-32.These are chronic otitismedia, frequent gastrointegtinal upset, mental retardation,cerebral palsy, and nutritionaldisorders. In these cases the percentageswith such. problems were either sodi- vergent from generalstudies or the classifications were sogeneral that anymeaningful interpretation was notconsidered possible; hence, the data were not included here. There appear to be someracial differences, as shown in Exhi- . bit 11-33.The total number of disorderschecked for the white children was1,162, compared with only 833 forthe Negro children. For mostof the disorders, the whitechildren had greater frequencyof symptoms checked, except for a fewincluding asthma, skin infection,and nutri- tional disorders for whichthe Negroes showed greaterfrequency. How- ever, thesedifferences may not be real; it is notIca lown whether children of different races receivedthe same level of service. In looking at the prevalenceof medical problems among variousHead Start groups, there maybe some value in examiningdistributions r.) f data on disorde s among childrenaccording to other variables such asthe mother's employment status and/oreducational level.These and other possible classifications were not made forthis report, although the datacould be obtained from thd 0E0 data filesdeveloped from the 1965 datacollection. Similarly, it is possible thatsomething could be learnedabout the health of theI Head Start children bydetermining the number of children with multiple medical problems.It was indicated in the Chicagostudy, for

v II-64 EXHIBIT 11-33 DIAGNOSES AND IMPRESSIONS Negro(1) Medical/ Dental Item (Percent)Present 4 White" (223221.tSuspect 1 (Percent)Present (percent)Suspect SkinAsthma,includingFrequent infection hay bronchitis respiratory fever, or other other disease illness, :allergies 41 51 <1 1 24 1 AnemiaTonsil /adenoid disease <1 85 <1 32 315 <1 2 HeartHerniaOrthopedic disease problems 21 <1 1 1 11. EnuresisFrequentUrinary infections gastrointestinal upset 31 <1 1 <1 1 1 ParasiticNote:ConvulsiveSpeech abnormality infestation disorder (1) White N = 2575; Negro N = 2648 <1 62 <1 1 <1 1 <1<1 1 example, that there were an average of 1.29 defects per child withde- fects. However, such counts were not made at this time, althoughthe data in storage in the 1-percent sample tape files could beused to generate such tables in future analyses. In summary, the available information on the medical anddental evaluations suggests that the health of the Head Start children was better than had been anticipated.While there is no sound methodological or statistical basisfor such a conclusion, we do feel that much was accomplished in the first stage of providing preventive and corrective medical and dental services, namely detection and identification of'dis- orders. We also feel that the information available from these evalua- tions is particularly helpful in suggesting steps that can be taken in planning how the goals of the medical program can better be met. 4. Results of the PPVT Pretest- In this subsection, the results of the PPVT pretest scores will be discussed for the national sample of eaildrenfor whom pretest scores could be identified.The total sample size for this analysis was 634. The mean PPVT pretest raw score for this sample was47.6. Th.-3 average age of thechildren in the sample was approximately 5 years and 8 months. The averagePPVT raw score for this age group as re- ported in the PPVT test manual isapproximately 53.Thus, this sam- ple of children averaged somewhat lower than thechildren who were used to establish the norms. However,there are indications that the average pretest scoresof Head Start children were even lower than those suggested by the results of theI-percent sample. Lower scores are reportedin each independent study which has beenreviewed. The reader is cautioned, then, about theexternal validity of the :results. More detailed discussion of the possible sourcesand direction of bias is given in Section II. F. The mean PPVT pretest scores were obtainedfor groups within a number of factors or categories: age, race, sex,family intactness, household size, mother working or not working,urbanity, income, and region.The differences between the groupswithin each factor were tested for significance.The results of this analysis are presented in Exhibits 11-34 through 11-42.Each exhibit contains the name of the groups within thecategories, the number of children in each group,the PPVT pretest mean for each group,the difference between means for each pair of groups, and the differencebetween the mean score for each group and the totalunweighted mean. Thus, in Exhibit 11-34, results are shownfor the age factor clas- sification.The first column on the left gives the groupnames (less than 5, etc.).The number (N) of children in each groupis given in the next column. Pretest means for each group aregiven in the next column, with the total unweighted mean forthe factor or category at the bottom. The difference between each pairof means is given in the matrix inthe next column. The letters atthe top (A, 13, C) designate the groups named in the rows. The lastcolumn, labeled "Total," gives the

11-67 EXHIBIT 11-34 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY AGE O _ Mear.c A B Difference C Total 1C.A.B. 5LessMore years than than 5 5years years 354187 93 48.1737.3851.42 X -10.79*(1) X --14.04* 3.25* X -8.28* 2.51*5.76* Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < .05. 634 45.62 .0 E.1 C7".= En C.7.3 F.771 7 r--1 EXHIBIT 11-35 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY RACE rn r3 7-1 =3 B.A. Negro Race White 313270 Pretest Mean 44.950.9 XA 6.C44( 1B X Difference -5.9 C .1 Total-3.9 2.1 Note:C. TotalOther (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p r .05. 591 8 48.850.8 X 1.9 EXHIBIT 11-36G PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY SEX 0 Sex . N Mean A DifferenceB Total B.A. FemaleMale 621320301 48.94'.6.3. X 6.0*(1) X -1.3* 1 .3* ' Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < Total 47.6 .05. crIn t=ti c=1 EXHIBIT II-37 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY FAMILY ThTTACTNESS B.A. Mother andonly father Family Intactness 425121 Mean45.448.7 X 3.3 X -5.7-2.4 C Difference 0.33.6 D -2.2Total 1.1 D.C. Other Father only Total 598 3319 45.147.651.1 X 6.0 X -2.5 3.5 ....._amlsuu...u.mwmmairmptraus EXHIBIT 11-313 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY HOUSEHOLD SIZE Household Size N Pretest Mean A Difference B Total B.Ae 'More than 8 8Total or less 634528106 46.848.145.6 X -2.5 X -1.2 1.3 all111..-""Ntemratam r.:11 CZ31 EXHIBIT II-39 PPVT PRETEST SCORES ON THE BASIS OF WHETHER THE MOTHER WORKS Cal

Mother Wol-ks N Mean A . Difference B Total B.A. NoYes Total 563348215 47.747.647.8 X .2X .1 ...... mmaismartawomarrimmillaillitill11111 I4 EXHIBIT II-40 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY URBANIZATION 0 A. Urbanization Urban 383 N PretestMeans46.1 XA -7.3*(1) B Difference -4.3* C 2.8 D Total -2.2 D.C.B. Rural,FarmOther Nonfarm 159 4722 43.350.453.4 X 3.0 X 10.1 7.1 X -5.0 2.1*5.1* Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) 611indicates p < .05. 48.3 c7n C:=1 E=1 Crn =I t=23 0 EXHIBIT 11-41 PPVT PRETESTN SCORES BY INCOME Means A B C D DifferenceE F G H I Total A.B. Income $1-1,999$1,000Less than 4897 46.543.5 X -3.0 X -1.5-4.5 -1.5-4.5 -1.5-4.5 -5.5-8.5 -9.6-6.6 -5.9-2.9 11.8 8.8 --3.5 .5 E.D.C. $4-4,999$3-3,999$2-2,999 150 6083 48.048.0 X 0.0 X 0.0 X -4.0-4.0 -5.1-5.1 -1.4 13.313.3 1.01.0 H.G.F. $8-9,999$6-7,999$5-5,999 3142 5 49.452.053.1 j X -1.1 X 3.72.6 X 14.718.417.3 2.46.15.0 w. I. $10,000orTotal more 519 3 47.0034.7 X -12.3 EXHIBIT 11-42 PPVT PRETEST SCORES BY REGION 0 A. Region Northeast ' 56N Means 46.7 XA -6.2 B -2.0 C . 3.6 DifferenceD -8.6 E 1.81 F -2.5 G Total -1.94 D.C.B. MidwestSoutheastMiddle Atlantic . 151210 65 43.148.752.9 X 4.2 X 9.8*(1)5.6 X 12.2-6.-2.4 -1.8 3.88.0* -6.1- .5 3.7 -5.61- .01 4.22 .F.G.E. FarSouthwestWest West 101 3912 49.244.955.3 X 10.4 X -4.3 6.1 X -3.75 6.62 Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates 634 p < .05. 48.7 .48 difference between the subgroup mean and the total unweighted mean. All differences that were calculated to be significant beyond the 5-percent level are indicated by an asterisk. For the three age levels, there were significant differences between all pairs of means, and between each mean and the total mean. Exhibit I1-35 presents the data for race.In the sample there were 270 whites, 313 Negroes, and 8 from other races.There was a sit ifi- cant difference between the means for the white and the Negro race; however, no group differed significantly from the total. Exhibit 11-36 presents the data for sex.This shows that the male children scored significantly higher than the female children. For the family intactness question, no significant differences were found, as shown in Exhibit 11-37.Similarly, household size and the question whether or not the mother works showed no significant differences. This is shown in Exhibits 11-38 and 11-39.Exhibit 11-40 indicates that there are significant differences between urban and farm children, and urban and rural nonfarm children.Exhibit II -41 presents the data by income. Again, no significant differences were found, although there is an indication that children from families with higher income tend to do better.Finally, Exhibit II-42 shows data for the seven Head Start regions. A few words of comment seem appropriate here.First, the age results do not seem surprising, since raw scores are used as the mea- sure of performance.It would be expected that there would be an in- crease with age.The finding that boys scored significantly higher than girls is definitely surprising, however.This result was contrary to common expectations ofdevelopmental rates.There was one independent study in which a similar finding was reported (Reference 59). Head Start boys and girls were compared with a middle-class nurseryschool group on discrimination learningproblems. Performance of boys in Head Start was superior to performance of Head Start girls in the sim- ultaneous discrimination learning problems. The investigators were surprised at the relationship and suggested tentatively that "the gen- erally accepted proposition that girls show faster developmentthan boys might hold true for the middle income ... group but not for the low

11-77 income Head Start group.In fact, the reverse might be true" (op. cit., page 21). 1 The relative performance of whites, Negroes, and others on the pretest is interesting.Unfortunately, there is but little direct check on the consistency of this ordering in some of the more controlled studies, since the composition of groups as reported was generally not specified in terms of these characteristics.In a progress report, Chesteen (Reference 18) reported pretest PPVT IQ scores for lower income and middle income white and Negro groups. At each income level, the white group was higher than the Negro group. No difference was detected in pretest- s':anding for most of the socio-economic variables considered. Rank order correlation coeffi- cients were calculated for the means ordered by income level (see Exhibit II-41). When the calculation included the highest level group (over $10,000), rho was not significant. When that very small and ex- treme group was dropped, a significant positive relationship (rs .90, p < .01) was obtained. Another surprising result was the higher performance of rural farm children and rural nonfarm children, as compared with urban children.It is very difficult to interpret this result; there are many alternatives and too little information.Similarly, it is difficult tdin- terpret or even speculate on the meaning of the obtained differences by region.There were only two, both involving the Middle Atlantic region, which was significantly higher than the two lowest scoring regions, Midwest and Southwest. 5. Emotional and Social Development The primary source of descriptive information on the emo- tional and social characteristics of the Summer 1965 Head Startchildren was thePsyea-..-ilogical Screening Procedure (PSP).The PS? was a rat >age& ing form d..ve.i.upect especially for Head Start to identify childrenwith fairly severe psychological/emotional/behavioral problems.(See Ap pendix A for a copy of the PSP.) Instructions were that the PSP be used by the teachers about 4 weeks after the program was underway, so they would be more familiar with the children.Thus, the PSP was not a prepostinstrument intended to measure changein children.(Con- siderable descriptive informationis included in impact information concerning psychological,emotional, and social areas. Seesubsection IV. C. 3.) Exhibit 11-43summarizes the data reported for 30 specified symptoms for (1) the1-percent nationwide sample; (2)white children in the 1-percent sample;and (3) Negro children in the1-percent sam- ple.Exhibit 11-44 summarizes thedata, using the same categories of children as Exhibit 11-43,for reported incidence of nine specifictypologies of emotional disturbance.Exhibit 11-45 summarizes information onthe percentages of childrenwho were referred for furtherdiagnosis and treatment of emotionalproblems. Exhibits 11-43 and 11-44 reflect aremarkably high incidence of emotional health, as indicatedby the extremely small percentagesof reported emotional orbehavioral problems. Only three symptoms were reported in more than 5 percentof the sample--selfish or greedy hoarding, anxiety in new situations,and inability to remain seated very long--and it would appear thatthese three symptoms reflect some- what less disturbancethan most of the other symptomsdefined. One independent study(Reference 6) also reported results onthe PSP for 393 children.The findings of Dr. 1. N. Berlin(University of Washington) are summarized inExhibits 11-46 and 11-47.(Exhibit 11-46 lists symptoms in rank orderof frequency of reportedoccurrence.) It can be seen that for allitems in symptoms andtypologies, Berlin's teachers reported severaltimes as many occurrences as were reported by teachers in thenationwide sample. And yet, Berlinindi- cated that he believed hisfindings reflected a substantiallyartificial, low incidence of occurrenceof the symptoms andtypologies. He attri- buted this to a generallyhostile attitude on the partof the teachers (who made the ratings) towardthis research task.The teachers, for exam- ple, had indicated that theyfelt protective toward thechildren and did not want them to appearinferior. The scarcity of availableand reliable research data onthe emo- tional and psychologicalcharacteristics of the Head Startchildren makes it impossible to present acomplete or accurate pictureof the children; however, there are somesubjective comments that shedlight on the EXHIBIT II -43 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE: SYMPTOMS (NATIONWIDE SAMPLE) Number Item 2,1. SelfishRefuses or togreedy eat or hoaiding drink Description 5.9 .9 Total (15.5)(2.5)(1) Percent4.4 .5 White (18.5)(4) 8.11.4 (11)(2) 4.6.5. HoldsAbusesTemperTemper breath selftantrum--throws tantrum--attacks until loses typical self others about, color etc. .4.9 (20.5)(15.5)-- (28.5) (28.5) .5.7 (18.5)(14.5)-- (27.5)(27.5) 1.2 (14) .5 (21)-- (28)(28) 10. 7.9.8. StuttersFaintsPlacesBites others foreignorexcessively passes in objects anger out in body openings 1.6 .2.3 - (28.5)(10)(24)(23) 1.7 (9) .5 (18.5)- (27.5) 1.2 .5.2 - (28)(1.4)(21)(24.5 ) 13.12.14.11. CriesInterestedComplainsFrequently excessively/anxious in of wandersfew pains activities in or head runs when or away stomach reprimanded 2.74.71.4 .9 (15.5)(6)(4)(11.5) 2.54.71.01.2 (13)(6.5)(11.5)(3) 3.35.21.2 (14)(6)(4) 18.17.16.15. AlmostAsksExcessWill notto constant clingingbe feed called self thumbsucking to by some another object name 1.4 .4 - (20.5)(11.5)(28.5)(20.5) .5 - (27.5)(18.5) 2.6 .5 - (2.8)(21)(7) ! EXHIBIT II-43 (Continued) Number Item 19. Very anxicius in new Description situations 5.9 (2.5) Total 6.9Percent (1) White 8.3 (1) Negro 21.20.23.22. NoOftenConstantlyCannot interest cries communicate criticizesinor playinglaughs withselfinappropriately languagewith other children 2.13.81.1 (13)(5)(7) .4 (20.5) 2.53.9 (5)(6,5).2.7 (23) (14.5) 4.01.91.7 (5) (8.5) (10).5 (21) 26.25.24.27. SadSitsAudibleFear orrocking frightened of clamping urinating back or or andfor forthmost of the day movinggrinding bowels of teeth 1.7 (8.5).5.1 (18).1 (25.5) (25.5) 1.7 (9) .2 (23)- (27.5) 1.9 (8.5).9.2 (17.5) (24.5)- (28) 29.28.30. InabilityCriesUnablePercent when to with remaininteract mother no symptomsseated leaveswith strangers for long indicated 32.7 6.11.7 (1) (8.5) .9 (15.5) 35.5 6.41.7 (2)1.2 (9) (11.5) 32.9 7.41.2 (3) (14) .9 (17.5) Note: (1) Numbers in tcorrelation = 5.29, coefficient rs df = 28, p <.011 parentheses indicate rank (Referencebetween 125). White and order in column. The Negro is significant: Spearman rank rej = .89, EXHIBIT II-44 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE: TYPOLOGIES (NATIONWIDE SAMPLE) ' . Number Item 1. The disruptive child Description 6.7 (1)(1) Total Percent5.6 (1)White 8.8 (1)Negro 4.3.2. The provocativeisolated child child 2.33.73.4 (8)(4)(5) 4.02.72.2 (3)(7) (8) 2.64.04.5 (6)(8)(3.5) NcoP 1 6.5. The childsilentfearful whochild or tearfuldoesn't childlearn 3.14.1 (6)(2.5) 3.24.2 (6)(2) 3.64.3 (7)(5) 7.9.8. TheThe unhappychild hyperactive with child separation child problems 4.12.91.2 (7)(2.5) (9) 3.71.5 (4.5) (9) 4.55.01.4 (3.5) (9)(2) Note: (1) Numbers in parentheses indicate rank order in column. pcorrelation coefficient<.05. (rs) between White and Negro is significant: rs = .68, The Spearman rank EXHIBIT 11-45PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE: REFERRALS

Referral/Treatment Percentages

Total . White Negro

1. Child guidance clinic .2(4)(1) - .5(2.5)

2. Mental health center - MP MP 3. Public health nurse or physician 1.4(1) 1.5(1) 1.4(1) 4. Hospital/medical clinic .5(2.5) .5(3) .2(4) 5. State school for mentally retarded - - MP

6a Hospital for emotion- ally disturbed - - - 7. Foster home - - - 8. Home for dependent children - - - 9. Other .5 (2.5) .7 (2) .5 (2.5)

Note:(1) Numbers in parentheses indicaterank order in column. EXHIBIT 11-46PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE: SYMPTOMS(BERLIN)'1)

Symptoms(2) Item Percent of Number Description Incidence (N=393)

19 Very anxious in new situations 1?

1 Selfish or greedy hoarding (of own and other children's playthings or classroom materials) 12

12 Interested in only one or two objects or activities 12

22 Shows no interest in playing with or being accepted by children 10

29 Unable to remain seatee -for more than 5 minutes 10

13 Cries excessively or becomes very anxious or withdrawn when mildly reprimanded 6 28 Completely unable to interact with strangers 6

14, Frequently wanders and runs away from nurses y 4

Note:(1) See Reference 6. (2) All other 22 symptoms were reported in3 percent o= less of the children.

i 5..5,-*70e-'7!...... !-.1",trA.,;-";

EXHIBIT II-47PSYCHOLOGICALSCREENING PROCEDURE: TYPOLOGIES (BERLIN)

Item Percent of Number Typologies for BehaviorSyndromes Incidence (N=393) 13 1 The disruptive child

3 The isolated child 9 8 5 The silent child 8 8 The unhappy child 0 2 The provocative child

9 The hyperactive child

4 The fearful or tearful child 5 3 6 The child who does not learn 2 7 The child withseparation problems

:S. 85 matter.Dr. E. E. Van Egmond, Lesley College, stated in his report (Reference 105), which involved nine Head Start teachers: ..it is clear that culturally and economically deprived children presented unique learning and socialization needs. This is evident since all teachers commented about the children's characteristics with such remarks as: "The children do not know how to talk or what to talk about. They cannot really play together.These children cannot listen to a story when it is read to them, only when I tell it to them. Sometimes the only thing you can do is stand close to them." 6. Socio -Cultural Characteristics Most of the information available relating to a description of the Head Start children's social and socio-cultural background comet. from the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) parent interviews. Some 1,742 parents of Head Start children were asked questions per- taining to their children's experiences and possessions. Exhibit 11 -48 shows (1) the results of the survey by indicating the percentage of par- ents responding in each answer-category and (2) items for which sig- nificant differences between white and Negro children (in Exhibits 11-49 and 11-50) were found. Exhibit 11-48 indicates that, with the notable exception of Sunday school, most of the Head Start children had not participated in any organ- ized program for preschoolchildren, prior to Head Start. The large percentage of respondents indicating that their child was caredfor during the day in the home by the mother is consistent with the finding ( Exhibit 11-17) that the majority of mothers were not employed . outside the home. A large percentage (60 percent) ,of children were reported to share their bedrooms with two or fewerchildren, and 80 percentwere re- ported to share their bedrooms with no, adult. . Nearly 75 percent of the childreti.were reported to attend "reli- gious services" at least once a month... There is some discrepancy be- tween reported attendance at rel:gious. services (75percent) and atten- dance at Sunday school (70 percent); it is possible that some of the attended churches have children's programs which are not labeled

11-86 EXHIBIT II-48 'ACTIVITIES, HABITS AND ENVIRONMENT (TOTAL) Significant NORCItem 11.11110111, (1) bramsomm Difference('(White ,v§. Number9. A.Has Daychild care attended center one of the following: les 8 87No Don'tPercent Know < 1 Negro)t2i B. <1 1 94 < 1 INS 12. A.E.D.C. SundaySettlementSummerOther school camp house program One 7010 Two286393 Three < Four 6 > Four' 3 None B. Howchild many share.his otheradults bedroom children does child with?does share 35 25 6 12 3 NO 8017 17. WheretheWho day? usually is child takes usually care takenof child care during of? his bedroom with? FatherHome 8512 3 Nursery_Mother<1 66 Relative 19 Other10 4 Don't Know 24C.23C. Doesmore childa attend moviesreligious once services or WeekWeek56 5 MonthMonth17 3 Months 8 6 Months 4 -TTYearYear 2 Never 3810 34A. Howonce manyor more toys a or games does child Many.31 S 27ome Few 35 None Don't 5Know INS 34C.34B. Howhave?doeschild many childhave? bookshave?crayons, or magazinespaints, and does paper Many25Many23 SomeSome2831 Few 3227 NoneNone1616. Don't Don't Know Know ORPIIAMPPIPTIMPPIPP EXHIBITItemNORC II-48 (Continued) Significant(WhiteDifferencesNegro)%2) ys. 36.35.Numbers following:.HowHas child many ever times had has any child pets? gone to the NeverYes OnceNo34 Don'tFewPercent(1) Know tilaii3. Don't Know A.D.C.B. SupermarketSmallLibrary grocery 4469 63 12 12 23272011 756715 5 <1 2-1 03P.4 H.G.E.F. AirportPlaygroundPostZoo office or railroad or park station 50424318 333128 7 11192129 45 36 <1 1 M.L.I.K. Ride A restaurant in a car Beach,Parade,DepartmentFire station lake, circus, store or pool or fair 462518 29 <1 1015 74 4414363138 8327204613 <1<1 2 Notes: (1) Total responses reflect all races in (2) Asterisk indicates significant differencesnotseparately the (seeactual onlytext). number for white of parents and Negro. cited sample. Specific responsesTotalin the N =textby 2,036, race (see whichareAppendix tabulated is the B). weighted sample, f=1 M'73 (=Tai C=3 E-71 r"-1 ti 1-1 EXHIBITNumbersItemNORC II-49 ACTIVITIES, HABITS AND ENVIRONMENT (WHITE) Percent 9. A.HasC.B. SettlementSummerDaychild careattended camp centerhouse one program of the following: <1 61Yes 13 No959694 Don't Know 1 ./... E.D. SundayOther school One13 Two6638 . None Don't Know 12. A.B. How How many many other adults children does child child share his bedroom with? does share 37 8 24 6 Three 11 Four 6 Four 2 8319 OS CO 16. theWho day? usually takes care of his bedroom with? child during 1Father 5 75Mother Relative 10 Other 10 Don't Know 23C.17. DoesWhere child is child attend usually movies taken once care or of? HomeWeek90 6 NurseryMonth15 <1 Relative3 Months 73 6 OtherMonths 20 3 Year 23 Never 30 34A.24C. haVe?HowmoreonceDoes manya orchild more toys attend a or games religious does services child 39Week SomeMonth3112 3 Months Few28 6 6 MonthsNone 36 Year 3 Never 17 346.34B. Howdoeschild many childhave? bookscrayons,have? or magazinespaints, and does paper Many2931 36SomeSome31 Few2026 NoneNone 1415 NumbersItemNORCEXHIBIT 11-49 0 (Continued) Percent Yes No Don't Know OM 36.35. HowHasfollowing: child many ever times had has any child pets? gone to the Never78 Once21 7 Few 10 Many 5 Don't Know 1 A.C.B.D. SmallSupermarketLibrary grocery 44 45 512 253021 741868 <1 00 :a0P H.G.F.E. AirportFirePlaygroundPostZoo officestation or railroad or park station 56404711 8 273032 53 3811192235 48 743 . M.I.L.K.J. Ride A restaurant in a car Beach,Parade,Department lake, circus orstore orpool fair 291517 1 <118 78 42491138 40872015 c=1 L7.7.3 trn EXHIBITItemNORC II-50 ACTIVITIES, HABITS AND ENVIRONMENT (NEGRO) Numbers No Don't Know Percent IN* 9. A.HasE.C.B. DayD. Summerchild care attended center camp one of the following: OtherSundaySettlement school house program 79 8 137-*62209293 <1 12. A.B. How How many many other adults children does child does share hischild bedroom share with?his bearoom with? One1535 26Two 5 Three 13 Four 61 Five 5 None 7815 . 17.16. WheretheWho day? usually is child takes usually care taken of child care during of? HomeFather84 2 Nursery60Mother 1 Relative 27 9 Other 11 5 Don't Know 24C.23C. moreDoesDoes childa child attend attend movies religious once sc orrvices 57WeekWeek Month2218 3 3Months Months 9 6 Months 10 3 YearYear14 Never 43 5 34B.34A. Howhave?once manymar: or more books toys a or or games magazines does childdoes Many2027 Some27 Few 4036 None18 7 Don'tDon't Know Know v2411. 34C. Howdoeschild many childhave? crayons, have? paints, and paper Maim20 Some29 Few 33 None18 Don't Know MO rammisnavams- ItemEXHIBITNORCNumber II-50 0 (Continued) Percent 0 36.35. HowHas child many ever t.mes ha-...3 has any child pets? gone to the 56Yes 44No Don't Know IN. .,. A.following?C.B. SmallSupermarketLibrary grocery NeverUr Tb---26 Once17 12 2025Few251- 2 Many 771267 5 Don't Know <1 2- E.D. PlaygroundPost office or park , 4423 30 7 2126 43 8 <1 .r,tvPI H.G.F. AirportFireZoo station or railroad station ' 47434010 3731 5 ' 401019 45 53. <<1 1 2 I.M.K.J.L. ARide restaurant in a car Beach,Parade,Department lake, circus, stare or poolor fair 59341 9 2 <11412 7 41231635 822518 8 <1< 1 31 "Sunday school," thus accounting for t e discrepancy, or that some churches have children's plogrl,m, so that the child attends services but not Sunday school or any children's religious program. Although a large percentage of children (65 percent) had pets, a large percentage ofchildren were reported to possess few or no (a) toys and games (40 percent),(b) books or magazines (48 percent), or (c) crayons, paints, and paper (43 percent), indicating some extent of "cultural disadvantage" or "experiential deficit" among children parti- cipating in Head Start. In response to questions about places children have gone, the categories of riding in a car, to a small grocery store, to thesuper-: market, to a departinent store, and to a park or playground were listed by the largest percentages of parents (83, 75,67, 46, and 45 percent, respectively). However, there were at least some Head Start children who were reported never to have been to the places listed (library, supermarket, small grocery, post office, playground-park, zoo, airport- railroad station, fiie station, department store, parade-circus-fair, restaurant, beach-lake-pool, ride in car). Although none of the independent studies described the children in terms similar to those used in the NORC survey, there was aninterest- ing related observation in Dr. Allen Soule's report on Northfield, 'Ver- mont (Reference 99).Not only had many of the 48 Northfield Head Start children never been swimming, but also "some of the parents had never even seen the pool andplayground, which was a real eye-opener since most of Northfield's children spent the biggest part oftheir summer days there." In addition to the overall tabulations shown in Exhibit 11-48,the results for the same items were tabulated for white and Negrochildren, to permit comparisons between the two races on itemsrelating to chil- dren's environment and activities.These results are summarized in Exhibits 11-49 and 11-50, but the indication of significance between white and Negro is noted by an asterisk (*) in Exhibit 11-48. There are a number of items for which significant differences ap- pear to exist.Negro children attend a day-care center and a Sunday

II: 9 3 school more often than white children. Morewhite children are usually taken care of by their mothers at home than Negrochildren. More Negro children have never seen a movie. Negro children havefewer toys and games, books and magazines, crayons and paints,and pets. More white children have never been to a library, zoo, or fire station, while more Negro children have never been to a rc_Istaurant.More white children have been on car rides, but fewer white childrenhave been to a circus or parade. Some information concerning the language environment(bilinguality) of the Head Start children was obtained fromthe 1,742 respondents to the NORC survey.Exhibit 11-51 summarizes the available information. The exhibit indicates that about 12 percent of the children inthe NORC sample of 1,742 families came from homes where somelanguage other than English was spoken. Spanish was the predominant"other language" (6.65 percent), and French ranked second (4.53percent). EXHIBIT II-51LINGUAL BACKGROUND

Number of Language Spoken Homes Percent Spanish 116 6.65 French 79 4.53 Italian 12 .68 German 8 .46 Swedish 3 ..17 Portuguese 3 .17 Japanese 2 .11 Chinese 2 .11

Greek 1 .06 Other 10 .57 Total 236 12.40

.

II-95 D. Head Start Parents and Families Some basic descriptive information about parents and families ob- tained from the Head Start medical/dental form was presented in Exhi- bits 11-14, 11-16, and 11-17, regarding "Family Income," "Mother's Education;" and "Does Mother Work?", respectively. Exhibits 11-52 and 11-53 summarize additional descriptive information relating specifically to Head Start mothers and fathers (total sample: Exhibit II-52), and to Negro and white mothers and fathers, taken separately by race (Exhibit 11-53).Information for Exhibits 11-52 and'II-53 was obtained from the 0E0 Information Center's tabulations of the 1-percent sample of medical/ dental forms (see Appendix A for a copy of this form). Inspection of Exhibit.II-52 shows that, for all seven of the appli- cable items for fathers,1at least 25 percent of the information was re- ported as unknown. The large amount of "unknown" information for fathers may be related to the fact that 70 percent of the parent respon- dents to the medical/dental form were mothers.This high percentage of unobtained information makes it difficult to get an accurate picture of family history as it related to Head Start fathers.Reported informa- tion does indicate that about 40 percent of the fathers had at least 9 years of education. At least 58 percent usually work (34 percent un- known), with most (60 percent) working full-time.The category of work most frequently reported was laborer (32 percent).Slightly more than 50 percent reported that they had not been unemployed within the past year. All 10 items were applicable to the mothers; for 2 items, at least 50 percent of the information was not reported, and for 2 items at least 90 percent was re-ported "unknown" (items related to work, unemploy- ment, and marital status, if not married). Reported information indicates that about 50 percent of the mothers had at least 9 years of school (20 percent were high school graduates). About 50 percent had four or more living children.About 10 percent worked full-time, and another 10 percent worked part-time.

1Full-term pregnancies, number of living children, and marital status (if not married) were considered not applicable.

11-96 (1) EXHIBIT II -5ZFAMILY HISTORY: PARENTS (TOTAL)

Percent Medical/Dental Item Number Father Mdther 77, 86 Parents deceased Yes 3.2 1.3 No 72.1 79.6 Unknown 24.6 19.1 79,88 Level of education None 1.7 1.5 1 to 6 yea:r., 14.9 12.3 7 to 8 years 16.6 16.4 9 to 11 years 22.6 33.0 High school graduate 16.0 19.8 Any college 1.7 1.4 Unknown 26.5 15.6 80 Full term pregnancies 1 to 3 31.9 4 to 6 33.2 7 to 9 13.8 Over 9 6.2 Unknown 14.8 Number of children living 1 to 3 32.4 4 to 6 34.9 7 to 9 13.3 Over 9 4.5 Unknown 15.0 81A, 89A Parent usually works Yes 57.9 28.3 No 7.6 54.2 Unknown 34.5 17.5 11-97 EXHIBIT 11-52(Continued)

Parent Medical /Dental Item Number Father Mother 81B, 89B If yes, works Full-time 60.4 9.4 Part-time 5.3 10.0 Occasionally .9 3.6 Unknown 33.3 67.0 81D, 89C If yes, type of work Pr ofe s sional/ technical / managerial 6.3 3.0 Clerical/sales 3,2 3.5 Craftsman/ foreman/ operative 14.2 2.6 Service/private household 4.6 13.3 Farmer 3.2 .9 Laborer 32.3 7.2 Student .2 .1 Unknown 36.0 69.4 82, 90 Parent without job in past year Yes 13.4 10.7 No 51.5 46.7 Unknown 35.1 42.6 83,91 Unemployed for 2 months or less 4.6 2.3 3 to 6 months 5.2 3.6 7 to 9 months 1.3 1.7 10 to 12 months 1.2 2.1 Unknown 87.7 90:4

11-98

1...... o...... w..-...-,...... EXHIBIT II-52(Continued)

Parent Medical/Dental Item Number Father Mother 78, 87 Parent is Widowed .2 Divorced .9 Separated 1.6 Other 1.3 Unknown 96.0

Note:(1) N = 6309. EXHIBIT 11-53FAMILY HISTORY: PARENT (WHITE-NEGRO)

Percent White Negro Medical/Dental Item Number Father Mother Father Mother 77, 86 Parents deceased Yes 3 1 4 2 No 78 83 69 78 Unknown 19 16 27 20 79, 88 Level of education -None .2 2 1 <1 1 to 6 years 16 14 14 8 7 to 8 years 20 19 15 15 9 to 11 years 25 28 23 41 High school graduate 17 22 18 21 Any college 2 1 2 2 Unknown 19 14 28 12 80 Full term pre gnancie s. 1 to 3 38 27 4 to 6 34 35 7 to 9 10 19 Over 9 5 7 Unknown 13 12 Number of children living 1 to 3 39 27 4 to 6 35 37 7 to 9 10 17

Over 9 4. 5 Unknown 12 14 81A, 89A Parent usually works

Yes 62 . 20 55 40 No 10 66 6 43 Unknown 28 14 38 17 II-100 EXHIBIT II- 53(Continue 1)

Percent White Negro Medical/Dental Item Number Father Mother Father Mother 81B, 89B If yes, works Full- time 66 14 57 26 Part- time 7 7 4 15 Occasionally 1 3 1 5 Unknown 27 76 38 54 81D, 89CIf yes, type of work Professional/ technical/ managerial 8 3 5 3 Clerical./ sales 4 5 2 3 Craftsman/ foreman/ operative 19 4 11 2 Service/ private household 2 4 7 24 Farmer 4 <1 3 2 Laborer 32 7 33 8 Student <1 <1 <1 Unknown 30 78 40 58 82, 90 Parent without job in past year Yes 16 6 12 16 No 54 46 52 51 Unknown 30 48 36 33 83, 91 Unemployed for 2 months or less 5 1 5 3 3 to 6 months 7 2 4 6

7 to 9 months . 2 1 1 3 10 to 12 months 1 1 1 3 Unknown 86 95 89 85

II-101 - -

ID EXHIBIT 11-53(Continued)

Percent Medical/Dental White Negro Item Number Father Mother FatherMother 78, 87 Parent is Widowed - <1 - <1 Divorced - I - 1 Separated - I - 2 Other - 1 - 2 Unknown - 97 - 95 One local study, that of Dunbar Center in Syracuse, NewYork (Reference 113), has described 45 of 46 families participating in that center's Head S'art program. The report is particularlyinteresting, because many QI the factors listed previously in this section are inter- related to prey -,,.,t a more composite picture of the familycharacteristics of the Head Start children. The researcher, Roslyn Gerard, suggests that there are four dis- tinct groups represented among these families: the 1-parentfamily, the 2-parent family whose father is disabled and is receivingtotal wel- fare assistance, the large 2-parent familywhose head is employed but whose pay is insufficient to support the family, and the large self- sufficient family with both parents working but needing some community assistance. Forty -one percent (19) of the participating families were 1-parent families, all headed by the mother. Gerard states that"the level of functioning is low and these families appear to have unmet needsin all areas of life." Only twoof these families received their entire income fromemployment.1These mothers were usually younger than parents of 2-parent families.In these families, the mothers' educational level was lower than that of themothers of 2-parent families.The educa- tional level was particularly low for those motherssupported entirely from welfare assistance. Housing tended to be poorerfor these families than for 2-parent families. There were two 2-parent families (9 percent of total)with the breadwinner incapacitated and supported entirelyby welfare. Gerard observed that they were similar in many ways tothe 1-parent families, in that the fathers appeared to have abandonedthe dominant male role of head of the family.Older sons had been involved with the law. Baia families had school drop-outs. The third family type was the large family in which the headof the family could entirely support his family.In most cases, the mother did not work outside the home, but inthe two families where she did, her skill level and income were extremely low.It was indicated, that the older

'Thiscompares with13 of the 26 2-parent families.

Ct.:1 II -103 children in these families were lower inachievement than their peers. Most of the families had at least one child whohad not been promoted every year. Unlike thefirst two groups, the children had had noseri- ous problems with thepolice. The last family group wasthe homeowners, with both the mother and father employed. Thesefamilies were large, and Gerard otates that the child's principal need was"... a pre-school experience because working mothers have difficultiesin spending sufficient time necessary to stimulate thechild's interest in the world about him." The discussion above is but a summaryDf a rather inteLsive study of these 45 families, inwhich their characte:71stics are presented in the context of the communityin which they live.It serves to highlight dif- ferent types of familieo. withvarying priorities of need for Head Start. Further information, primarily descriptiveof the parents from a socio -cultural point of view, wasobtained through the NORC interview survey of1,742 Head Start parents (see descriptionof sample in Section II. F).Exhibits 11-54 and 11-57 summarizein percentages the overall NORC data relevant to Head Startparents. In addition to the overall tabulations made for the NORCparent-description items, comparisons- for Negro and white mothersand Negro and white fathers were made. (See Exhibits 11-55 and11-56 for white and Negro mothers, and 11-58 and 11-59 for white and Negrofathers.) Tests of significance of differ- ence inproportions were made between racesfor the parents. Items that differed at the 5-percentlevel are indicated by asterisks(*) on Exhibits 11-54 and 11-57 (see AppendixB for statistical analysii pro- cedures for all NORC white versusNegro comparisons in thisreport). The habits and activities of Negro andwhite mothers can be com- pared in Exhibit 11-55 and11-56.The results are that for all butthree of the significant items, thewhite mothers engage in the activity more frequently than the Negro mothers.Thus, for example, whitemothers are morelikely to go to movies, visit, entertain,take trips, go to res- taurants, picnic, play cards, andhave a hobby, while Negromothers, in their spare time, are morelikely to belong to clubs, engagein some kind cf musical activity, or just sitand relax.

n-104 OF MOTHERS (TOTAL) EXHIBIT II-54 DESCRIPTION NORC Item Numkm___ Percent ...(WhiteSignificant vs. Negro)(1)Difference ;;; I a. DoesA. she belong to any clubs? If yes, does she attend once weekyes36 monthno962 occas.12 44A.23A. Does she attend moviesreligious once or more a or more a ? services week314 month12 3 mos.5 6 mos.13 year21 never43 DKrt16. 25A.26. Does she read a newspaper once onceorDoes moreor more she a do a one of the following weekday5945 2month days821 week3 mos.1310 weeks6 mos.157 yearDK5 never10 0 onceC.B.A.D. or more a GoGoReadTake to toa apart sportssee book infriends eventorsports magazine or event relatives ? week446836 AA2 wk.L4--26 _La,LL_-I--month6 _Z___....9____3 mos.519 never 4876213 1-I1-4: G.E.H.F. Go to a concert, play, or museum at GoEatEntertainhome onin a restaurantpicnic, friends ridlior or relatives swimming <1 "3T1167 141 ...... 1-4-- 48 TT44..._6 74-713 4 p(TIre 28. A.November?WasI. she registered to vote last MeetIfstreet yes, and did talk she with vote? friends on the Tz---yes7960 $Tr"no4639 TSDK1.---5 4 =- 6 30. Has slit taken a trip outside the C.A.cityE.D.B. in a AirplaneTrainBusCarBoat .5.8..._.yes2--124183 15_no78825212 31A. How many trips of over 50 miles has NoneNo armaIMINOtwO four >four 21 32. A.sheWhich madedo ofin theduring4theher following spare time last things year? does Sit and relax yes517930 1.oneno462818 12 three rorrorr Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates significant differences E.D.C.B.F. PlaySewGrow°the:: acards flowers musical special or orother instrumentthings vegetables gatms or sing 'fir4r-3) 18 (sea /text). .0.5079Z7 :. E?C.HIBIT II- 5 5 DESCRIPTION OF MOTHERS (WHITE ) 19. A.Does she belong to any clubs? orIf yes,more does a she NORCattend onceItem NuMber ? weekyes729 nomonth5 occas.16 Percent 21A, Does she attend movies once or :nor.: a 2 week3 11111,010 month11 3 mos.5 6 mos.17 year27 never33 DK 25A,.24A. DoesDoes sheshe attendread a religiousnewspaper once onceor more or more a a ? ? services weekday4461 2month days712 week83 15mos. weeks6 mos.1Z10 year*6DK ..... never1? DK 26. onceDoes or she more do onea of the following ? week6 2 wks.5 month41 , 3 20mos. never'8&-64 D.Tr.:..C.B.A. ReadTakeGoCo to apart seea book sports friendsin orsports eventmagazine or event relatives 53733 210 713 .1.-...... 4 16 ,-.. H.G.F. CoGo toon a a concert, picnic, ride,orplay, or swimming imuseurn<1 43at EatEntertainhome in a restaurant friends or relatives 7112 111..-.111012 ..4Z___18 .A5...... 5.AQ___ I 11IQ..24_,.LI.-4--. Ga% Zs. November?WasLA. she registerted to vote last IfstreetMeet yes, and did talk she with vote? friends on tho 311TMtes 1no.61--40 A..L4DK .3. -Li. 30.. RasB.A.city she Car in takena a trip outside the Bus yes55__91 4--`no5 T- ' 31A. How many trips of over 50 miles has E.C.sheD. made during the last year? AirplaneTrainBoat .1_6None2138 11one16748059 two10 throe10 four8 >four 33 . B.A.sheWhich Grow do ofin 6=herflowers following spare or time, veotables things does Sit and relax 3r--20yes76 no4428.22 C.F.E.D. Sew Play cardsa musical or other instrument games or sing 13Other special things ..42,_17 li4184 . EXHIBIT II-56 NORC Item Number DESCRIPTION OF MOTHERS (NEGRO) 1 1:t. A.Does she belong to any clubs? orIf yes,more does a she attend once ? 1942weekyes 4712monthno 11occas. Percent 24A.23A. Does she attend moviesrefigiosts once services or oncemore ora more a ? ? 1...... ,week ( month 1.5-.....month 6...... 123 mos. 5LIX.-...6 mos.6 mos. 4LZyear MOM 4MI--nevernever Llm...mMDK 26.25A. Does she read a newspaper once orDoes more she a do one of the following ? ? 4559day 9282 days 11week 17weeks :-.--DK . - A.onceB. Co or to more a sports a event Take part in sports event 5 week ?...... 6 2 wks. r. 41.--8 month S213 mot. ID 11760never E..C.F.D. atGo EatEntertainReadhome to insee a abook friendsrestaurant friends or magazine or or relatives relatives 6E4,--5.65 615U.16 6.-...._9LQ.--11-- 8L.3...- Z.fia...._55123 . G.I.H. MeetGo onto and a concert,picnic, talk with rido,or play, friend, orswimming or the museuz.nfour A.E.B.F.D.C. SitOtherPlaySewGrow and a cards relax specialflowersmusical or thingsotheror instrument vegetables games or sing ZZ - 411--.40ZfL-....50 415844..-...tk_,_2L EXHIBIT II-57 811, NORC Item Number DESCRIPTION OF FATHERS .1111011410 (TOTAL) Percent ,MMI/VIIM111/! Significant DifferencmiWhite vs. No/roll's 23B.26. DoesA. he belongattend moviesto any clubs? once orIf yes,more does a he attend once ? weekyes 28 7 monthno 47 6 occas. 13 DK 24B. onceorDoes more or hemore a attend a religious services 7 ? week 2 month 11 8 3 mos. 47 6 mos. 79 year 17 4 never 1633 DK<1 1 27.25B. OrDoes more he a doread one a newspaperof the following once ? weekday 4528 2 wks.days 5 weekmonth 8 3 mos. wks12 neverDK 3 A.onesE.D.C.B. Gn or to more a ports a event EntertainGoReadTake to apart see book friendsin ora sportsmagazine or relatives event 4<4833 g 87 8763 16 58 .s.12,_ 27438027 ** COC:ibg i G.I.H.F. Co to a concert, play or museum MeetGoEatat home onin and a restaurantpirnic, talk with ride friends or swimming on the 6 <12715 II 1 283 1512 2 __L9__A....2.9 53 * 62 4 29. A.WasNovember? he registered In votc. last Ifstreet yes, did he vote? 146yes 51 9no 22 3 DK 1 * 3113.33. How many trips, of over 50 miles has heWhich made of during the following the last tIningsyear? clotss he yes one42 one io 12 two 9 three 7 four 5 > 22 'Our * A.doC.,B. inD. his spare time: SitPlaythingsWorkFish and orcards relaxwith hunt orcars other or tinker games with 3"r 5247 2127 * Note: (1) Asterisk (C) indicates significant diaerences,H.G.F.E. Grow flowers or vegetables withOtherPlayDo a choiracarpentry specialmusical thing. instrumentjust for himself or sing 2341 12 r 4r--zirrr3-0 60 r * EXHIBIT20. U-58 DESCRIPTION A.Does he belong to any clubs? If yes, does NOW::he attend Item once Number OF FATHERS (WHITE)weekyes 32411 monthno 49 ..minn11,11 Percent 11111,. 11111111084M 24B.23B. Does he attend religiousmovi's once or more a or more a services weekweek 63 monthmonth 4 occas.3 mos.22 5 6 6mos. mos.13 yearyear23 never 30 DK<1 258. Dose he read a newspaper once onceor more or more a a day 5329 2 days 5 week 11 1010.1wks DK 61 23 27. A.onceB.Does or he more do one a of the following GoTake to a partsports in eventa sports event ? week 68 ....E...2 wk.. 5.7 month 63 3 mos.18 4 never 815833 D.C. GoRead to see a book frtznds or magazine or relatives .5.L.. 43 __Si__ 7.m. 1018.....0.._...7._ io __J.__A_ AL. 1..4 G.E.TF. i GoEatatEntertain home toin aa concert,restaurant friends play or relatives or museum ..,57,...31L.<1._21,_,_ ..L.1;.....&.....f___,<, MM.*MIMES .1..I.- 1 At_IILED_-7__. Ii_ vpI-41.1I 29. WasI. he registered to vote last MeetGostreet on and a picnic, talk with ride friends or swimming on the IA_ ...... ,....no i...... -al. DK . 1011114211 lit27 0 318. How many trips of over 50 miles A.November? If yes, did he vote? has noneyes 4856 one 24 wo 2 three four 7 > four 35 33. he madeWhich during of the thefollowing last year? things does he . yes30 no 10 10 8 A.doC.B. inD. his spare time: PlaySitthingsWorkFish and orcards with relaxhunt orcars other or tinker games with 59_P§..- 48536? 332326 G.E.H.F. Grow flowers or vegetables withPlayDoOther acarpentry choira musical special just thingsinstrument for himself or sing . '3 3414 22 4766 EXHIBIT20. 71-59 Does he belong to any clubs? NORC /tem Number DESCRIPTION OF FATHERS (NEGRO) yes no Percent 0 23B. Doer. he attend movies once A. orIf yes,more does a he attend once ? ? week24 9 month46 8 occas. 7 6 mow. DK 24B. Does he attend religious services onceor more or morea a ? week 2 month 8 . 3 mos. 5 le6 mos. 7 yearyear12 never 35 <1DK 1 26 14 7 6 3 12 2.1.25B. Does he read a newspaper once onceA.or?fines more or more he a do a one of the following ? ? weekday1038 2 wksdays 85 weekmonth 77 14 3 mos.15 wks neverDK60 4 E.D.C.B. EntertainCoGoReadTake to asee a partsports book friends in oraevent sports magazine or relatives event -6Tr--47143 Ti"10 3 "Fr--2.-- 8 s-.5 33TrTI-69 I4 t H.G.F. GoEatat home ontoin aa concert,picnic,restaurant ride play or or swimming museum _IL_20 1 924 74 1312 9 37-..89 0t-s- 29. A.November?WasI. he registered to vote last MeetIfstreet yes, and did talk he with vote? friends on the yes44*857 no20 42 DK I2 3 5135 33.31B. How many trips of over 50 miles has hedoWhich made in his ofduring sparethe following the time: last year?things does he yesnone53 noone2814 two<1 9 th.tee 5 four 3 -1. fbur X4 . A.D.C.B. SitthingsWorkFish and or with relaxhunt cars or tinker with .4048 20 <1 1 H.G.E.F. Grow flowers or vegetables withPlayOtherDoPlay acarpentry choircardsmusical special or thingsinstrumentjustother for games himself or sing 'Tr*"."ZS-Tr--58-11 73-16"--lrMr-57 <1 31 There were no significant differences for regular church attend- ance (about 45 percent for both groups), meeting and talking with friends (about 80 percent for both groups), sewing (70 percent for both groups), and reading daily newspapers (about 60 percent for both groups). The habits and activities of Negro and white fathers can be com- pared in Exhibits 11-58 and 11-59.Items in which the differences in proportions were significant are again indicated by asterisks (*) in Ex- hibit 11-57.The results are that in each of the significant items, the white fathers engage in the activity indicated by the item-more frequently than the Negro fathers.Thus, for example, they read a newspaper more, eat out more, andhave more picnics.They are more likely than Negro fathers to vote:, take trips, hunt and fish, play a musical instru- ment or sing with a choir, play cards, or just sit and relax. Thereaie no significant differences betweenthe subgroups in belonging to clubi (the majority of those answering the item do not), in going to the movies (the majority go two or three times a year or less), and attendance at church. . Examination of results of comparisons between white and Negro fathers and results of comparisons between white and Negro Mothers on essentially the same items reveals some interesting differences and similarities. For example, Negro mothers are more likely to belong to a club or organization than white mothers and less likely to go to the movies than white mothers, while the fathers do not differ significantly in these activities.There are no significant differences in voting rate between mothers, while there are such differences between fathers. Exhibits 11-60 and 11-61 summarize some additional descriptive in- formation for Head Start families that was obtained from the 1-percent sample (Medical/Dental Form). The information was obtained primarly from mothers (70 percent). According to Exhibit 11-60, nearly. 62 percent of the children were cared for during the day by a paint; this figure corroborates figures concerning incidence of working mothers (Exhibit 11-17).The finding that about 65 percent of the families were not receiving public assistance EXHIBIT II-60 DESCRIPTION Or FAMILIES (TOTAL) Medical/DentalItem Number 92 Public assistance/ADC payments Yes in past year Percent 16.1 94 Care of Head Start child UnknownNoParent during day 64.861.819.1 2.2 NurseryOtherSiblingBabysitterOther. adult school relative 12.6 2.25.12.1 95 Number of people in Unknown485 or to less710 household 21.046.014.0 8.1 96" Number of children under OverUnknown1, 10to 2 16 27.017.9 6.9 Over63 toUnknown 58 8 43517.8 8.03.7 ,p- En rni EXHIBIT II -60 r=3 (Continued) r::zi rim tr-71 r-71 r-.1 r-1 r--, r'i ri 0 r.--1 MedicalItem /Dental Number - .. Percent 98 Respondents Mother and father , 70.0 7.13.9 FatherUnknownOtherGuardianFoster parent 8.48.21.9 .6 . , , e ) EXHIBIT II-61 DESCRIPTION OF FAMILIES Medical/DentalItem Number 92 ' Public assistance/ADC payments in past year Percent) White 12 . (Percent) Negro 94 Care of Head Start child during day YesUnknownNo . 7018 642016 NurseryParentBabysitter school 72 25I A. 51 643 SiblingOther adult relative , 81 19 95 Number of people in household UnknownOther4 or less 11 6 14 73 UnknownOver85 to 710 10 224917 5 264415 8 r....r..*4 t:TI r."1 E"---3 1 EXHIBIT II- 61 (Continued) 0 .Medical/DentalItem Number 96 Number of children under 16 (Percent) White (Percent Negro Over6 3to 1to 8to 5 2 463214 3 224224 4 ,-.I-.1--i)-4 98 Respondents MotherUnknown and father 86 77 ul FatherMother S. 71 5 70 23 .GuardianUnknownOther.Foster parent <1 72' <110 8 or Aid to Dependent Children(ADC) would appear to support the possi- bility that children whose mothers were on ADCdid not participate to a large extent in Head Start. From the INTORC survey,certain information descriptive of the socio-cultural environment hasbeen summarized in Exhibit II-62. In addition to the overalltabulations for "family-environment" items, comparisons were madefor Negro and white respondents. Re- sponse percentagesfor these items for the two races are shown inEx- hibits II-63 and II-64. Of these items, two werefound to be significant as a result of the tests of significance betweendifferences in proportions: (1) more Negro than white families rent theirhomes, and (2) more Negroes have lived longer at their present homes thanwhites. A final independent study (Refe:.:ence16) pertinent here compared social activities and attitudes of 40Head Start and 40 non-Head Start families in Negro neighborhoods of Rochester,New York, served by Head Start.The purpose of the study was to z.ssessthe cultural depri- vation'of these groups of families, perhapsdiscovering if there were any patterns toparticipation or nonparticipation in the program.The authors believed that Head Startdid not reach the most culturally de- prived families in the areas served.Data were collected by interview. Some of the factors investigated were:(1) attitudes of the family to- ward the police, political parties,church, and the anti-poverty programs such asHead Mart; (2) how the families found outabout Head Start; (3) socialization; (4) education;(5) health; (6) financial status; . and (7) family status'. NI

'Definedby the researchers as "...lackingopportunities, or being blocked from opportunities, to have social experiencesthat are common to the people of the dominant society.It will also mean having certain attitudes toward these social experiences andcertain expectations regarding the self, others, and social institutions, which mayperpetrate the social conditions which deny to the person experiencesotherwise available to members of the dominant group."

II-116 EXHIBIT II-62 FAMILY HABITS (TOTAL) (1) Item Number NORC Percent (White vs SignificantDifference . Negroj 10A10 IfOwn rent, or partrent? of public housing? YesOwn 39 RentNo5451 Other T 1311 HowUpHow to:many long rooms?in present home? <6 Ivics. 8One <110 Two1Yr. 19 2Three Yrs. 12 9 --11---Four3 Yrs. 24 ~26Five5 Yrs. 18 >Five * t-4 1-..t 13A If less than 2 years, Twofra WW.11.110 -.3.." 14 sameWasnumber the last of movemoves? from ? borhoodNeigh- One40 18 CityNo 3910 StateThree 11 OtherFour <1 5 > Four >Ei ht 2121B21A DoisIf yes,youit on? havehow manya radio? hours a day Respondent HusbandEi ht 7 Never ItemEXHIBIT Number II-63 NORC FAMILY HABITS (WHITE)(1) Percent 10 Own or rent? "46-OwnYes r- RentNo45 Other 7 1310A11 HowUpIf to:manylong rent, in rooms?part present of public home? housing? 6 Mos. One 13 762--1 TwoYr. 11 Three2 Yrs. 14 4 3 Yrs.Four 22 10 5 Yrs.Five 1728 >Five>5 Yrs. 38 13A14 WasIfnumber lessthe last than of move moves?2 years, from the borhoodNeigh- One 3417 City."TrTwo40 StateThree 19 4 OtherFour >Four >Eight <1 21A21 isIfDo yes,it you on? how have many a radio? hours a day Eight Never 3 21B22A22 IfDoWho yes, you usually how have manyhours alistens? TV set? a day Respondent Yes 91 2 HusbandOne NoChild 47 2- 3 Other 444-7 >Eight Never Note: 22B (1) N = 832 Whois it on? usually listens? Respondent Husband-10 Yrs. >Five 13A numberIf less than of moves? two years, One 19 Two 7 Three_ 2 Four >Four<1 <1 00 2114 DoWassame you the have last movea radio? from the borhood Neigh- Yes 4587 CityNo40 State Other ° 21A If yes, how many hours a day E,iyht Never. 2221B isDoWho it on?you usually have alistens? TV set? Respondent Yes 888 3 HusbandNo Child11 9 55 Other 23' 14 Z Note: 22A22B (1) .N-= 1,129 (weighted). WhoisIf yes,it on? usually now many listens? hours a day respondent Eight Never In general, it would appear that the Head Startfamilies had some- what more positive attitudes toward the community's various institu- tions.While more Head Start families considered the police tobe help- ful or friendly, however, almost everychild in the program expressed negative attitudes.There were insignificant differences between the two groups with regard topolitical parties (both were indifferent) and church, 'Jut Head Start families were morepositive about social pro- grams such as HeadStart. Almost all of both groups saw their future as "better," but they werealso pessimistic about leaving the "ghetto." The researchers conclude that inasmuch as there were nodifferences between the groups concerning expectations forthe future, Head Start "...had no real effect in raising hopes for a betterlife...." Looking to the ways in which families found out aboutHead Start, it is interesting that one-third of the nonparticipating grouphad not heard about the program--this despite an intensiverecruiting campaign. The researchers suggest that inasmuch as everyfamily could have heard of Head Start, this might be an additional piece ofevidence to suggest that cultural deprivation "...is characterized bynon-involvement in the institutions of the society, including thecommunications institutions and processes." It was observed that in terms of socialization,Head Start families participated more in the various community programssuch as the church, lodge, school, and settlement houses than didnon-Head Start families. In addition, Head Start families tended toparticipate more frequently in MI personally initiated social activities. Onlyslightly more than half the non-Head Start group had any reported social life. The investigators found that both Head Staztmothers and fathers had a higher level of education than fnenonparticipatars. No marked differences were shown in the family health ofthe two groups. Inter- estingly, more Head Start families received sometype of welfare as- sistance. However, proportionately moreHead Start fathers worked and brought money home than did non-HeadStart fathers. Looking at family status, 82 percent of theHead Start families were "whole"(mother and father together and married), as compared with 58 percent of the non-Head Start group.Fifteen percent of thenon- Head Start group were separated or divorced, while 11 percent of the Head Start families showed this condition.Nine percent of the nonpartici- pating families had a deceased parent; only 4 percent of the participators had a deceased parent.Finally, while 18 percent of the non-Head Start parents had common law marriages, only 4 percent of the Head Start group were so married. The investigators conclude that the Head Start program in Rochester did not reach some of the more severely culturally deprived families prob- ably because their greater isolation and/or insulation from the community, financial vulnerability, and pessimism made them less responsive to such programs. E. Head Start CDC Staff and Workers In the Summer 1965 programthere were approximately184,000 CDC staffmembers,'47 percent paid and 53 percentvolunteer.The four types of staff membersand their approximate numbers were: Paid professionals,41,000 Paid neighborhood residents,46,000 Volunteer neighborhoodresidents, 56,000 Other volunteers, 41,000 Personnel working in thesevarious categories included teachers, teachers acting as aides,psychologists, social workers, volunteer women withvarying degrees of training andexperience, nurses, students from the Neighborhood YouthCorps, school principals, physiciansand dentists, parents of participatingchildren, and numerous otherspecialists and nonspecialists. In this subsection the totalstaff member population, then more specifically the teachers and thesubprofessionals, will be described in terms of selectedcharacteristics, their relevantexperience, and their Head Start interest.This will be followed by adiscussion of teacher styles and attitudesand information on theclassroom duties of the subprofessionals. There are several reasonsfor describing variouscharacteristics of staff and workers whomanned the CDC's. They were,in the first place, a major class ofparticipants in the program.They performed the many diversifiedfunctions associated with theoperation of a com- prehensive program. Second, it was aHead Start objective to enlist the interest of manydifferent types of people, withdiversified skills, backgrounds, ages, and capabilities,in recognizing anddealing with the complex and widespreadproblem of the growthand development of culturally disadvantagedchildren and with the manyfactors that affect their progress.Third, there is the factthat the various workers, whether they are teachers,physicians, artists,housewives, teenagers,

d..=.1...... 'Thesenumbers were taken from alisting of proposalsfunded for the Summer 1965 project.Other estimates ofthe total number ofHead Start workers showsomewhat different totals.

11 -122 bus drivers, secretaries, or whatever, are in one way or another par- ticipants in the system and the environment in which the children are growing up, Head Start or no Head Start. As participants, their be- havior or actions, as stimuli, may potentially be inappropriate, ir- relevant, or even harmful to the development of the children, despite the best of intentions.Thus, in one sense, while the workers can be regarded as teachers --fixed resources with services and activities to provide to the children--in another sense they can also be regarded as learners who must gain much knowledge about the children in order to be effective. This is a roundabout way of stating that not only could the workers have an impact on the children, but also that the children, indeed the entire program, could have an impact on the workers. The same point applies, of course, to the parents and to the agencies and organizations that make up the community. The point seems self-evident, but it is nonetheless important and worth a few more words of elaboration. The basic point is that the child does come to school with an ex- periential and cultural background, and is "deprived" or "disadvantaged" only in relation to some other standard or to the later demandsof school and a technologicalsociety.1The child himself has a set of abilities and perceptual discriminations, but these may not be the abilities and discriminations that lead to success in school. He has a vocabulary and concepts, but these may not be appropriate to school. He hasgoals, expectations, evaluations, desires, fears, beliefs, and attitudes, but these may be at variance with the goals held for him by theselool. While he is attracted or responsive to some people, places,objects, or situations, he is unreceptive to orfrightened by or insensitive to others. However, his abilities, characteristics, and "culturaldiffer- ences" undoubtedly have survival value, at least in hispreschool years, and in some respects, beyond.

1See Section II. C for a detailed dEcussion ofpsychological character- istics of culturally deprived children.

II-..123 Thus, he does have somemodes of communication; he does have sets and emotional reactions; he does filter, combine, compare, weigh, distinguish, reject, and otherwise operate upon inputs. In the school or CDC, the teacher produces or affects manyof the inputs, either directly, through his overt behavior, or indirectly, by arranging, organizing, or constructingthe environment and situations in which the child is placed.The teacher does not ever have complete control over the child's mental processes; however,it is a reasonable hypothesis that the teacher's behavior and operations canbe such that they are more or less compatible with the presentcapabilities and re- sponse tendencies ofthe child. Some criteria of compatibility are that the inputs available can be processed andutilized by the child to produce a desired responseand that the child will be more or less likely to do so, given the inputs.It is a further hypothesis that the development and learning will be more efficient and successfulif the teacher is able to recognize a state of incompatibilityfrom the child's responses and modify the inputs in the direction of compatibility. This suggests that the teacher must have or developstandards, protocols, discriminations, schemata, or whateverthat permit or lea:d to an appropriate recognition orcategorization of the child's status. Such standards may be of a dual nature:(1) a definition or set of cri- teria for what constitutes a child's response(whether positive or nega- tive) or absence of response to an input, and (2) a setof criteria for responses (e. g., skills, behaviorpatterns) associated with the develop- inent in the child of different (and eventuallydesirable) levelsand quali- ties of capability, motivation,discrimination, problem-solving, con- ceptualizing, and other psychological processes.1A third possible con- sideration is that the teacher also needs criteriafor the relevance of his own behavior to the psychological statusof the child, on the one hand, and to the ability or response goals desired forthe child, on the other.

1Itis, of course, possible to view the latter setof criteria as applying to the categorization responses per sewithout considering their implied relationship to psychological constructs. However,that issue will not . be explored here.

Ii- -124 Each of these sets of criteria or standards relates tothe teacher's operations of recognizing and identifying the present statusof the child, recognizing and identifying a change in status, and behavingappropriately to the present status so as to encourage achange toward a desired status. Development of these criteria and operations mayrequire as much learn- ing and modification of customary behavior on the partof the teacher as is expected of the child.These elements assume particular importance when part of the solution to the overallproblem of the socially disadvan- taged child "is to proceed by a carefullydeveloped and sequential pro- gram to bringhim up to a level where he can learn in school as well as other children and eventually under the sameconditions as other children" (Bloom et al, page 23, Reference 7). A few examples of at least implicit orpotential incompatibility of the sort described above have been culledfrom Head Start data: One consultant expressed concern because ateacher, whom she described as inflexible, categorizedchildren upon first meeting them as"dull, average, or alert." "Most of the teachers engaged were trained and experi- enced in teaching older children°Those with the most years of experience as gradeschool or junior high teachers found it more difficult to adjust to the Head Start situation than the younger teachers lesshabituated to methods unsuitable for the youngerchildren." (Berlin, Reference 6). In one case, the procedures of the public health nurses in administering shots left a scene ofbedlam at the center in question and "requiredall the skill" of the staff for an hour after the departure of the nursesto quiet the hysterical 'children. II...a visit from an ambulance[was] arranged to familiarize the children with what might be afrightening part of the city's services.(Unfortunately, the driver, well-meaning as could be, knew nothingabout small children and his entire tail( was either overtheir heads

11-125 or expressed in the mostnegative and threatenin words! 'Too bad that people are afraid of ambulances... hurt, injured, sick people to the hospital... shock... due to fear... very dangerous... can't breathe so we use this oxygen... etc.' )"(Reference 6). the doctor seemed stern and not compatible with the children.This woman doctor made remarks about some children having pierced earlobes.... "(Quoted from a teacher by Montez, Reference 74). These very gross examples do not necessarily identify specific or quantitative variables, nordo they imply a particular long-term outcome or effect in the children.They do, however, suggest areas in which it is reasonable.to suppose that changes in theadults--one form of impait--would have desirable consequences. A problem for research is to seek out ways of defining and measuring the appropriate variables so that evaluation can bemade.' There is no intention here to espouse a theory of psychological processes or social interaction.The preceding remarks are intended to provide, a framework for examining therelationship of staff members to children. The problems of research on and evaluation of the relationship . between inputs to the child (whether these come from thepeople, ac- tivities, or facilities of a CDC or from parents andfamily), on the one hand, and change in performance of the child, on the other, are very complex both theoretically and methodologically (cf. Gage,pissina, Reference 38). 'There are.problems of definition, measurement,and categorization of teachers, teacher's aides, parents, programstructure, and activities.Similarly, there are problems of classificationand/or

'Thepoint has been made with specificreference to assessing the effec- tiveness of teaching methods by Wallenand Travers (Reference 106).In criticizing many comparative studies they. say,For the most part, studies which supposedly comparethe effectiveness of two teaching methods are generallystudies which compare two largelyunknown conditions" (page 466).

11-126 measurement of children onrelevant characteristics andperformance variables.Finally, there are problems ofmeasuring and interpreting relationships between the two realmsof variables.Throughout, there is the need for theidentification of practical and significantvariables and relationships, especiallythose that may point to remedialsteps to improve effectiveness indealing with the complicatedproblems of devel- opment and education. There is one final reason forconsidering characteristics andfunc- tions of staff membersand workers:they are importantsources of in- formation about the programs.It is thus of more than passinginterest to define, to the extentpossible, who and what theworkers are.

1. Worker Characteristics Demographic data on the totalstaff member population, both professional and subprofessional,is based on Staff MemberInformation forms collected from the1-percent sample ChildDevelopment Centers (on a nationwide basis).These are presented in ExhibitII-65.The figures shown are percentagesof a total sample of 5,200forms. The number of centers representedin the figures shown isabout 432.All but three states (Idaho,Montana, and Wyoming) arerepresented. There are approximately900 staff members in this samplewho served several centers rather than beingaffiliated with a single center. Like the other figures, thepercentages of "No Response"refer to the sample processed, not tothe sample expected inthe survey. For example, in Item 3, there were213 cases (or abouc 4 percentof the 5,200) for which responsesconcerning the sex of the staffmember were not reported orprocessed. The sample reported hereobviously includes overfour times as many workers asexpected in the CensusBureau's 1-percent sample. It includes, however,only about 33 percent of theforms received in the Census 10-percentsample (based on 1,114centers). No determi- nation of the contaminationin the sample shown herehas been made. However, the percentagesfor most factors andlevels shown are very close to the percentagesof the sample re-portedby the Census Bureau (see Table 7, Reference 89).It is true that 75 percentof the workers

II- 127 EXHIBIT 11-65 STAFF AND WORKER CHARACTERISTICS 1. Age Item(1) IIMIIININNIMMINIIMINIIIMMIYINIMIIIMIMMIIIMIIIMIIIIIMONI Under 16 16-21' 21-30 72T:1- Percent31-4529.9 46-60-787cr ---"4Over Z 60 No Response 1.5 2. culthralRace and/or background b.a. ----"2:17----'Kr'NegroAmer. WhiteZ3.4 Indian36.8 OrientalMexican ./= 6.2 No'Rican ResponsePuerto---T.T. French 7:T"''"'"".." --717.--Creole Eskimo .06 Other No Response 67.9 4.3. a.EducationSex Graduated completed elementary MaleYes Female12.9 No Response No83.0 4.1 -rrr b.d.c. Graduated HaveGraduated M. A. high college schoolschool 36.548.551.313.4 16.511.215.7 4.4 5. developmentPositione. cave inPh. child D.center Professional hood Resident.8 34.5 14.0 ------4-078---Paid Neighbor- VolunteerResidentNeighborhood 5.9 VT'OtherVolunteer No Response 15.9 EXHIBIT 11-65 (Continued) Item(') OMMIN1111.111MIMMPII, Percent 6. Usualincome family per year Under$8,000-$1,000 6.4 $1,000- $10,000Over1,999 6.2 -17=$2,000- 2,999No Response $3,000--37'3°3,999N... $4,000--TO=4,999 $5,000-, 5,999 1 2. 6 $6,000. 7,999 15:1 7. withPreviousconditions children experience offrom poverty None44.4 9,999 1-3 Years 16.3 15.4 3-5 Years 6.2 Over 5 Years 30.5 No Response 2.6 8. withPrevious preschoolers experience Ow0.110None44.4 1-3 Years No Response 3-5 Years 1).7 3.0 10. 9. HeadwithfluentlyotherCanUsed speakchildren thanStart this languageEnglishlanguageprogram in 22.388.4Yes (%No of number (N = 76.7 1.0 1,160) answering Yes to Item .9) . 11. Attendedtraining NUEA session 37-77Yes -637No No Response Note: (1) copyItem ofnumbers this refer to the form. Staff Member Information Sheet. See Appendix, A for a in the processed sample were paid(professional and neighborhood resident),1while only 47 percent of the totalworkers are listed as paid. This disparity is probably theresult of a large number of factors, including the matter of definition ofwho were workers for sampling purposes. The best use of the data is toprovide an indication of the general order of proportions ofcharacteristics. No attempt is made toestimate the reliability of the rateof occurrence of these characteristicsin the Head Start worker universe.2 Viewed in the light describedabove, there are points of interest in Exhibit 11-65.The modal age of workers wasaround 30 to 45 years, but the second most frequent age was16 to 21 years.This would appear re- lated to the bimodal distributionof responses in Items 7 and 8.Staff members had had either little or noexperience with preschoolers or children from conditions of poverty orhad had more than 5 years of such experience. Another pointof interest in connection with Items7 and 8 is the extent to which the twodistributions agree in percentages at each experiencelevel. A sizable portion of workersprobably had had little or no experience eitherwith preschoolers or with childrenfrom con- ditions of poverty.This is important because, as willbe seen in a later section, the majority of workersin the sample felt they had gained agreat deal from both of these aspectsof their Head Start experience,The same point is stressed in anumber of local studies. Another point worth noting is thehigh number of workers whoused their ability to speak alanguage other than English(Item 10).item 10 consists of responses from1,025 workers, or about 20 percentof the whole sample (that is, about1-1/2 times the number of people inthe sample represented by the percentagesfrom the cultural backgrounds identified in Item 2b).

1See Exhibit 11-65, Item 5.

ZAninteresting comparison of characteristicsis provided by Exhibits111-14 and III- 15 for CDC staffmembers and workers who attendedNUEA training sessions.

11-130 The percentages of respondents who checkedwhether or not they had graduated from elementary school, highschool, etc. (Item 4), are not mutually exclusive.That is, they are percentages of Yes or No responses to the second part ofQuestion 4 of the Information Sheet (see Appendix A). An attempt was made to assessthe characteristics denoted in Exhibit 11-65 by sorting forms on threevariables: ethnic and cultural background, position in the center(professional and other), and age (under 30 years, over 30 years).Five levels of ethnic and cultural background we=e used.There was a loss of about 65 percent in the sample by making this classification,the resulting sample having N = 1814.The resulting frequencies of occurrencewithin classifications are shownin Exhibit II- 66. The data are ofparticular interest from one point of view.They suggest something of thebias in the incompleteness of the larger samples.It would appear that the professionals,and es- pecially the older professionals, were morethorough in completing the data forms used in the program. Exhibit 11-67 shows percentages of theprofessional workers having selected characteristics. The figures arebased on all professional workers regardless of ethnicity. A crosscomparison with figures in Exhibit 11-65 shows no greatdifference in terms of proportion of sexes, regardless of age. However, there are somestriking differences in proportions having various amountsof experience with cllturallydeprived children and with preschoolers.Similarly, a much larger number of pro- fessionals, young and old, attended anNUEA training session than might be suspected from the overallsample.Within the sample, intra-age differences appear about as would beexpected. While there is no usableinformation available in the1-percent sam- ple on the characteristicsof Head StartsubProfessionals, there is such data obtained by an independentstudy. Martin B. Millerand Barrie Cassileth (Reference 73)conducted a survey of 86subprofessionai workers in 30 Head Startcenters.'Ten of the centershad a predominatelywhite

r.... =1/... The number of centersvisited in each region were:5 in the Northeast, 3 in the MiddleAtlantic, 4 in the Southeast,3 in the Midwest, 4in the Southwest, 6 in the West,3 in the Far West, and2 in the Virgin Islands. EXHIBIT 11-66 THREE-WAYCLASSIFICATION OF STAFF AND WORKERS

Racial and/or Cultural Professional Other Sub - Background Under 30 Over 30Under 30Over 30 Totals (1) 1,167(64) _White 444(25) 609(34) 79(4) 35(2)

Negro 179(10) 364(20) 13 18 574(32)

White and Puerto Rican 9 15 1 0 25(1)

Negro and Puerto Rican 2 1 0 0 3

Mexican 21(1) 19(1) 4 1. 45(3)

Subtotals 655(36) 1,008(56) 97(5) 54(3)

Total 1,663(92) 151(8) 1,814

Note: (1) Approximatepercentages of the samplethat are greater than 1 percent are given inparentheses. EXHIBIT- II-67 SELECTED CHARACTERISTICS OF PROFESSIONAL STAFF MEMBERS BY AGE 3. b.a.Sex Item Age OverUnder 30 30 ...... R. Male17.113.4 FemalePercent(l)82.986.6 1,008 655N 7. witha,conditionsPrevious children experience of from poverty T'ader 30 38.6None 1-3 Years 37.310.1 3-5 Years 12.5 .....,.Over 5 Years N 11.6 655 44(A1. 8. withb.a.Previous preschoolers experience UnderOver 30 30 None46.814.3 1-3 Years 38.6 3-5 Years 8.87.7 Over 5 Years N 66.8 6.9 648968 11. Attendedb.b.a.training NUEA session OverUnder 30 30 65.932.952.9Yes 20.1 34.147.1No8.2 38.8 980643993N Note: (1) Percentages are for the total number in each age level in each item. I enrollment, 11 Negro, 2 mixed,2 Puerto Rican, 3Mexican, and Indian. At each center aconsultant interviewed andobserved one teacher and twosubprofessionals. While thesample is small, there appears to havebeen an attempt toinclude workers from manyethnic backgrounds.Eight social and culturalbackgrounds are represented by the 86 workers:white, Negro, Mexican,West Indian, Puerto Rican, American Indian,Japanese, and Egyptian. Exhibit 11-68 presentsselected characteristicsof the sample of 86 subprofessionals.It may be noted that41 percent were in their teen years.The mean age isabout 27.While the frequenciesof marital status, education,and workers with childrenin the program are given, they are not correlatedwith age.Thus, while 56 percentof the total sample had completedhigh school, it is notknown what percentage of the 20 years and older grouphad done so.Six percent of thesample had completed 4 yearsof college.

Z. Worker Experience With respect to theexperience of the staffmembers, it was stated above that the1-percent sample indicatesthat 44 percent had had ex- no previousexperience with preschoolers;25 percent had had some perience (1-3 years);and ever 20 percent hadworked with preschoolers for more than 5 years.Also, 44 percent reportedthat they had had no previous experience withchildren from conditionsof poverty; 16 percent had little experience(1-3 years); and 30 percenthad more than 5 years of work in this area. If we look morespecifically at the teachers,the educational con- sultants' reports showthat in 64 percent ofthe centers visited, a ma- jority of teachers hadhad one or more years'experience in kindergarten in only and elementaryeducation. However, asshown in Exhibit-II-69, 10 percent of the cases' werethe majority ofteachers experiencedin visited, the nursery schoolteaching.In about 35 percentof the centers' majority of teachershad received professionaltraining in child orhuman growth and development orearly childhoodeducation.This suggested concentration of Head Startteacher experience inelementary education projects. is supported byindependent studies ofspecific Head Start

11-134 IM1.1 411101111 C.T.M oo.m1Ni en EXHIBIT II-68 cri =3 CHARACTERISTICS OF SUBPROFESSIONAL WORKERS r-71 CM c cm r--2 r---3 rn rn r r-1 0 r..i 1. Age Item 12-1940r."7- 20-29 18.6 30.39 18.6 40-49 18.6 Percent 50-59-13-.1)- (1) 60-64 ilimi.111111110, No Data INNS 3.2. SexMarital status MaleMarried4.3 36.0 Female Single 58;0 Divorced-----4:6----- or Separated -27- )-I1-1 4. Ethnic background White Negro40.7 Mexican West Indian -53= 6-1---4-.1Puerto Rican American Indian cp(A) . 5. Community of SameJapanese Community as Center 1.2 --Mr Egyptian 1.2 Other 6.7. EducationWithresidence children in Yes0-8 Years 72:17- No 81.0 9-12 Years 61.7 College + 256Mr 7 Note: (1) program N = 86 2O 7in EXHIBIT II-69TRAINING AND EXPERIENCE OFHEAD START TEACHERS(1)

"Yes" Answers as "No" Answers as Consultants' Checklist a Percentage of a Percentageof Question Number Total "Yes" Answers Total "No" Answers A majority ofteachers had professional training in: 9.A.1 Child or humangrowth and development 16.7 10.1 9.6 2 Early childhood education 18.7 1.4 3 Elementary education 35.2 11.7 4 Child psychology 14.2 19.4 5 Pediatric nursing 2.3 19.1 6 Social work 4.5 14.0 7 Other 5.3 14.3 8 Mixed 2.7

A majority of teachershad oneor more years'experience in: 14.3 9.B.1 Nursery school 10.5 10.2 2 Kindergarten 20.2 1.4 3 Elementary school 43.9 17.1 4 Nursing 4.3 16.3 5 Social work 5.4 15.1 6 Recreation 5.7 13.2 7 Mixed 3.0 12.0 8 Other 6.6

Note: (1) Percentages arebased on column totals, not onitem totals. In Greene County, Ohio, for example, 11 of asample of 24 Head Start teachers currently teach in grades 1-4,6 teach in grades 5-8, 2 in high school or above, and only 5 in preschool.The majority of teachers in the Greater Anchorage Area SchoolDistrict had one or more years' ex- perience in kindergarten or elementaryschools.Jn King County, Washington, very few experiencedteachers of 3- and 4-year-olds could participate in Head Start, sinceonly certified teachers could be accepted. In the report on the Newark,New Jersey, Head Start program it was stated that only regularly assignedNewark teachers of kindergarten and the primary grades wereappointed.It can be hypothesized, of course, that this reliance on elementaryschool teachers resulted from the great number of programs that weresponsored by public school systems.It must be emphasized, however,that it is not known just what thevarious selection procedures were in many cases. The available national information onthe years of teaching experi- ence of the HeadStart instructors attending NUEAsessions is shown in Exhibit III-15. Of the 24 GreeneCounty Head Start teachers,18 had had 6 or more years of experience,while 11 had 11 years or more of ex- perience (Reference 21).These 24 teachers had spent approximately one-third of their careers at oneschool.Cohnstaedt compared these teachers with 25 non-Head Startteachers and found that 14 had had6 or more yearsof experience.The non-Head Start teachershad spent approximately two-thirds of their careersat one school. Montez, in his Impact of Programs onBilingual Pupils and Families (Reference 74), remarks that in a SouthernCalifornia program for Mexican- Americans, 75 percent of theteachers were nbt familiar withthe culture of the children.Three of the teachers were ofMexican-American ex- traction; two had majoredin Spanish in college.Only 20 percent spoke Spanish. Montez infers that agreater percentage of teachersshould have been familiar with theculture of the children.The extent of this problem in programs with otherethnic groups is not known atthis time. The experience of thesubprofessionals varied tremendously,for generally there were noeducational or vocational requirements.It was stated earlier, for example,that only 56 percent of a sampleof 86 workers had completed high school.Many of the subprofessionalsin Head Start

II-137 rig-WK,-,.W.#4.10*05"Y MsiNItstlelleeSsYStriffIllarilt , 11,W 17,VtIr2.5M-VII-Irtz,17""'.

were parents and other residents of the community.Miller and Cassileth report that 43 percent of the workers were parents, and half of themhad children in the program. Eighty-one percent of these workers were resi- dents of the community in which the center was located,For examples in Charlestown, Massachusetts, the mothers were recruited as neighborhood aides (with work primarily outside the classroom).All but two had chil- dren in the program. In Newark, New Jersey, the priority for positions as teacher aides wasgiven to parents of Head Start children and to resi- den4s of the poverty areas.The only requirement was that the applicant read and write. The data from the subprofessional sample does not includeinforma- tion on income, so the extent to which the disadvantaged were employed is not known. 3. Worker Head Start Interest There is no available information on a national level concern- ing why some 184,000 staff workers sought employmentin the summer program. There is anindication, however, that many workers had a high expectation that the program would be of value.S. T. Friedman, J. Pierce-Jones, and W. E. Barron(Reference 86), of the University of Texas, addressed this point empirically as part oftheir series of special studies funded by 0E0. They studied the attitudesand expecta- tions of 1,250 teachers who participated in the Universityof Texas train- ing program. Answers to questions were analyzed with respectto sev- eral teacher variables: ethnic group membership,socio-economic back- ground, experience, and willingness to volunteer.The latter variable refers to whether the teachers volunteered "on their own" or werestimu- lated in some other way. The findings with respect to eight items pertaining tothe teacher's initial evaluations and expectations about the programand their predic- tions a:lout its likely effectiveness were that theteachers did enter the program with enthusiasmand confidence in its, value. Negro teachers and experienced teachers expressed greaterconfidence in their ability to be effective and in the program as awhole.There were no basic differences in initial attitudes about the program as afunction of socio-

II-138 'at

economic background or volunteer status,although volunteers did have significantly greater interest infulltime participation.The authors note, however, that "of greaterinterest is the fact that no matterhow the teacher group came intothe program, they all thoughtit would be effective and useful." teachers (out of a total of ... In Greene County,Ohio, 24 Head Start 29) were comparedwith 25 teachers offirst grade children who had not taught in Head Start.Of the 24 Head Startinstructors, 6 stated that the reason they applied wasprimarily financial.Nine of the 24 stressed the challenge of workingwith poor children; 5of these had previously taught low-income children.Several teachers wanted togain experience in. teaching preschoolchildren.Five instructorsindicated that they needd a summerjob, although a few ofthese listed this as asecondary motiye. For many, it wasthe challenge of workingwith smaller classes. Only 4 of the 25 non'-Head Start teacher, hadconsidered applying One did apply but was notaccepted. Of the remaining21, 13 had wanted a free summer,3 had other obligations,and 2 stated that advancig age was the reason.Only 2 of the 25 had noknowledge of the progiam. In the subprofessionalcategory, Miller andCassileth did ask the 86 workers how they wereemployed and why theyhad applied.The re- sponsesand their percentagedistribution follow:

Offered Neighbor- Personal hood YouthOther How Own by HS Agency Other Employed?InitiativePersonnel Connections Corps . 30.2 27.9 19.8 7.0 5.8 9.3

Wanted to anted Had Child Why Like the Needed erience in Program Other Applied? Work Money Help Ex 39.5 14.0 9.3 7.0 4.6 29.1

One of themeasures of impact onboth the subprofes sionals and the teachers is whether theywould consider Head Start employmentagain. This will be consideredin Sectioi. IV.

II-139 Concerning training, it will be noted in Section III thatapproxi- mately 30,000 professionals (86 percent were teachers) attendedHead Start training sessions sponsored by the NUEA. However, available sources suggest that there was lessprovision for training subprofes- sionals prior to work in HeadStart.'Inasmuch as their roles were not uniformly defined, this does not appearsurprising.Miller and Cassileth reported that 76 percent of their samplehad no training, and of the 21 workers who did receive some orientation,only 7 had had more than two sessions of instruction.The Chicago Committee on Urban Opportunity, reporting on the volunteer effort, statedthat while training for all 1963 volunteers was a program objective, thatgoal was not met.In Newark, New Jersey, the Office of Homemaker Serviceof Essex County trained the subprofessionals.Therefore, while it is likely that the majority' of subprofessionals were not trained for their Head Startwork, there were projects which made training an important partof the program.

4. Teacher Styles and Attitudes The Worker's Attitude Scale (see AppendixA) was designed to measurechanges in workers' attitudes about the poor and poor children as a result oftheir Head Start experience.Results from data obtained in the 1-percent sample are not available atthis time.Berlin (Reference 6) reported results obtained from samples of28 and 24 teachers and teacher's aides, respectively.The pretest results are shown in Exhibit 11-70.The means shown in theexhibit are for the two main sections of the scale. Berlin considered items in Part 1 of thescale to be related to a "general attitude toward poverty." The items inPart 2 were scored and interpreted to relate to "attitude towardHead Start children as compared with'most children'. " As can be seen, attitudesin all cases were generallystrongly favorable. Although posttest meansare not shown here, there were no significant differences or changes ineither subgroup on either part of the scale after a 5-week interval.Berlin notes that "several teachersand aides expressed annoyance[about Part 2 of this scale' because theyfeared that it was an attempt to portraythe children from homes of the poor as 'less good' behaviorally."

. 'Only10 of the 151 nonprofessionals represented inExhibit II -66 said they had attended an NUEAtraining session.

II -140 (1) EXHIBIT II-70 WORKERATTITUDES (BERLIN)

Attitude /Group Possible ScorePretest Group Mean N

1. General attitude toward poverty -41 to 78 28 a.Teachers 48.57 b.Teacher's 46.17 24 aides

2. Attitude toward HS children as compared with "most children" 0 to 90 28 a.Teachers 42.14 24 b.Teacher's 43.92 aides

Note: (1) Reference 6.

11-141 Five studies (References 21, 68, 33, 86, and 88) havelooked spe- cifically at teacher styles and attitudes.The Cohnstaedt study is pri- marily directed towards community impact, butthere is some informa- tion on Head Start teacher styles.The Lamb and Eisenberg studies, because they relate teacher styles andattitudes to the development of the child in Head Start, arediscussed in Section IV. The study on Teacher-Belief Systemsand Preschool Atmospheres (Reference 88) focused on 168 teachers participating in theHead Start training program conducted by the Universityof Colorado Extension Division.The authors hypothesized that the belief orpersonality systems of teachers would influence their teachingstyles.To test this hypothesis the teachers were classified in terms offour conceptual systems; a sample from each system was then observed and rated on 26 dimensions "assumed to reflect educationally desirable and undesirablebehavior toward their preschool students." The classification was made on thebasis of performance by the teachers on the "This I Believe"(TIB) test.1 Two of the classification systems stress concrete modes" of dimensionalizing and construing the world," and two stress abstractmodes. Systems 1 and 2 (concrete) differ in that System 1 would include personswho have "highly positive attitudes toward institutionalreferents,"2while System 2 would include concreteness with negative attitudes.System 4 is the highest level of abstractness, while System 3 represents thesecond highest level of abstraction.3 Of the 168 teachers tested, none wasclassified in System 2, and only 10 were classified in System 4.The remainder were in either System 1 or 3, although the authorsdid not provide these frequencies.

1The test was developed specifically as a measureof conceptual or belief systems; it requests the respondent toindicate his beliefs about a number of socially and personallysignificant concept referents. i 2 For example, religion, friendship,the American way of life. 3For a complete discussion ofthe TIB and the classification, seethe independent study.

II-142 In order to test whetherthe conceptual systemsinfluenced the teacher's classroom styleand attitude, tenrepresentatives of Systems1, this includes 3, and 4 were selectedfor observation.It will be noted that Systems 1 and all the representativesof System 4, butonly a sample from experience. System 1teachers 3.All of the teachershad prior teaching teachers' (most concrete)had taught on an averageof 10.1 years. System 3 had taught 5.8 years;System 4 teachers, 5.4 years.Only one System 1, previously taught at the two System3, and three System4 teachers had preschool level.All teachers hadparticipated in a 1-weekHead Start training program. As stated earlier,these 30 teachers wereobserved and rated on 6-point scale of above 26 teaching dimensions,each one measured on a and belowaverage.1 It was hypothesizedthat the moreabstract teachers would score higher than the moreconcrete teachers ondimensions 1-19, and lower than the more concreteteachers on dimensions20-26. The hypothesis wassupported. The predicteddifferences between Systems 1 and 4emerged on all 26dimensions. In addition,System 3 teachers scored betweenSystems 1 and 4 on23 of the 26 items.The authors also statedthat the Systems 1and 4 teachers differedsignifi- of cantly on 15 dimensions.The study alsoincluded a factor analysis the ratings on alldimensions. 13 of An importantobservation of thisindependent study is that these 15 significantdimensions werecontained within eitherthe factor

toward the children, 1Thedimensions were: (1)expression of warmth (2) perceptiveness ofthe children's wishesand needs, (3) flexibility in meeting the needsand interest of thechildren, (4) ability tomain- tain relaxed relationshipswith children, (5)attention to the individual child, (6) taskinvolvement, (7) enjoymentof teaching, (8)enlistment of child participation,(9) encouragementof individualresponsibility, (10) encouragement offree expression offeelings, (11) encouragement of creativity, (12)teaching new concepts,(13) ingenuity in improving teacblaz and playmaterials, (14) utilizationof physical resources, (1.; task effectiveness,(16) diversity of activitiessimultaneously per- of classroomoperation, (18) consistencyof mitted, ,(17) smoothness explanation of rules,(201 use rule enforcement,(19) use of functional rules, of nonfunctionalexplanation of rules,(21) use of unexplained (22) rule orientation,(23) determination ofclassroom and playground structure inteaching activities andrelation- procedure, (24) need for by the ships with children,(25) punitiveness,and (26) anxiety induced observers' presence. 11-143 of dictatorialness or task orientation.The authors state that"System 1 teachers were significantly moredictatorial than representativesof either System 3 or System 4and at the same time significantlyless task- criented than teachers from System3 or System 4." The study concludes,therefore, that "... the more abstractteachers in this study were clearlysuperior to the more concreteteachers in the extent to which theyproduced educationally desirableatmospheres in their classrooms." Theauthors do state further that they can"only con- jecture at this point onthe differential effect of ...teacher differences ... uponthe learning and behaviorof the children." If one accepts theconclusion from the data presented,then it would seem importantthat these teachers with differentconceptual systems and teaching styles be studiedin terms of their effect onHead Start children.' As mentioned earlier,the Lamb and Eisenberg works,which also classified teachers according toconceptual style, were the onlyindependent Head Start studies whichattempted to do this.Because of the program impact implications of these studies,they are discussed in SectionIV.If the results of these and otherstudies show that different stylesdo have a significant effect upon thelearning and behavior of HeadStart children, this will, of course,have important implicationsfor the selection and training of teachers.

Thereis a definite need to pursuesuch studies.In a review of the literature of research onauthoritarian versusnonauthoritarian methods of teaching as related toacademic achievement, Wallenand Travers (Reference 106) concludedthatresults are not readilygeneralizable to the "environmentallydeprived sector of ourcountry." Other findings relevant to the area beingdiscussed here are that therehas been found in at least one study asignificant relationship betweenperformance of pupils and the behavior ofteachers as perceived bythe pupils (Reference 106).Stern (Reference 101) makesreference to two studies in theresearch literature which have indicated ageneral disparity betweenavowed beliefs about educational philosophyand actual classroombehavior. He also con- cludes that "direct evidence[of the effect of teachers'attitudes on pupil performance]... is surprisingly meagre."However, one study found "clear evidence that theteacher's personality has amarked and meas- urable effect on the progressof her pupils academicallyand socially. There also appeared to be aninteraction between the type ofteacher and her children'semotional adjustment. as shown onthe children's feeling test" (Washburne andHeil, quoted in Getzels andJackson, page 532-3,Reference 39).

II -144 Pierce-Jones conducted a factor analysis ofobserved -interactions between Head Start children and teachers,taking a sample of 70 Head Start centers in 40 Texas communities.These teachers were rated on 47 different items (compared with26 in the Colorado study), and a factor analysis grouped these 47 in 8interpretable factors: stimulating cognitive perceptual development, warmthand supportiveness, respect for child, motor skills and psychologicalsupport, dependency needs,positive versus negative reinforcement,perceptual and emotional control, and middle class orientation. In Greene County, Ohio,the 24 Head Start teachers and 25 non- Head Start teachers werecompared in terms of preschool goalswhich they considered most andleast important.It should be mentioned that these teachers were interviewed afterHead Start had concluded; there- fore, the extent to which Head Startitself influenced the teachers is not known. Also, of the 24 Head Startteachers, only 13 currently teach preschool, first, or second grades.All of the non-Head Start teachers are in this group,and it is not known if the experienceof teaching older grades influences the selectionof the most important program goals. Therefore, the results, as Cohnstaedtindicates, are suggestive only. He found that three out of fourHead Start teachers were oriented to "innovative-expressive" goals, while only twoout of five non-Head Start teachers were so oriented. Asubstantial minority of the latter group were oriented tothe "traditional-restrictive" goals. From the availableresearch'information it can be concluded that there was a diversity of teacherstyles and attitudes in the 1965 Head Start program. From the Coloradostudy it appears that a minorityof the teachers were classified inthe abstract systems, althoughthe authors did not indicate the nut-11.0er inSystems 1 and 3.Only 10 of the 168, how- ever, were inthe most dt.: stract system.This is the only indication of order of, magnitude in teacherstyles or attitudes of 1965 HeadStart.

Someof the research studies have beendiscussed in more detail than others. The reader should notinfer an implicit value judgmentbecause of this.

II-145 Observations were not made nationally,but only in selected areas such as Colorado, Texas, and Ohio.In addition, the instruments used in the three cited studies were notsimilar. Two studies, the ones in Colorado and Ohio, suggest that the moreabstract or innovative teachers are better for HeadStart.These are not conclusive, however, for they were not related to theperformance of the children. 5: Sub professional Duties Subprofessionals varied in their duties and responsibilities. The Miller and Ca,ssileth study of86 subprofessionals found that physical-active and verbal-responsiveactivities were the two most frequently observed kindsof behavior.This is generally supported by the subprofessionalsand teachers interviewed.Over 70 percent of each group said that thesubprofessionals regularly satand/or ate with the children and wereinvolved with groups of childrenin outdoor and indoor play.Over 60 percent of therespondents indicated that these workers regularly wereinvolved in setting up, cleaning,and rearrang- ing the room and in field tripswith other adults.One-half the subpro- fessionals said that they wereregularly involved in indoorplay with the individual child; slightly lessthan half indicated regularinvolvement in outdoor play with the individualchild.Sixty percent of the teachers stated that the subprofessionalsregularly participated in this way. Subpro:essionals almost never weresupervised by another sub- professional, nor did they go onfield trips with the childalone, make home visits, act as atranslator, or transport children. Selected independentstudies stress the above-mentionedduties and others.In its study of the useof volunteers, the Chicago Committee in Urban Opportunitystated that in the Board ofEducation program, the volunteers were notused in the classroom nor as anaid to the profes- sional.They were office workers,pupil recruiters, medicalassistants, and chaperones for trips.A number of teen-agegirls were used in Warminster, Pennsylvania;they babysat during parentmeetings, went on trips,walked children to andfrom classes, and participatedin the Graduate students were used by the Archdiocese of Chicago School Board to recruit children, encourage parents to participate, visit homes, identify and refer problems, and work with the parents.Mothers, as neighborhood aides in Charlestown, Massachusetts, were involved in similar activities. In this subsection we have attempted to describe various aspects of some of the 184,000 CDC staff members: their characteristics, ex- perience, and Head Start interest. We have also discussed teacher styles and subprofessional duties.In Section IV we shall discuss the impact of the staff workers on the program a:ad the children and the impact of Head Start on the workers.

11-147 F. The National Samples In this subsection we shall discuss the sources and reliability of the data that is called elsewhere in the report national 1-percent sample data.Ideally, a description of the source of sample data should also be, at least implicitly, an argument that the data reflect the target population up to the limits of sampling error. We shall describe the sampling schemes first; we shall then examine the worth of the data. We shall describe first a Census 1-percent sample that is not the 1-percent sample of this report. Census chose a 10-percent sample of Child Development Centers (CDC's). From the children in these (roughly) 1,100 centers, Census chose a 1-percent sample of Head Start children as follows: 62 centers were singled out as interestingbecause of ethnicity or size and were included with certainty; the remaining centers of the 1,100 were chosen with probability (number of classes in the center)/10; finally, each child in the chosen centers was' included with probability 1 /(number of classes in the center).This usually resulted in children being drawn from more than one class within the center. For each child so chosen, Census attempted to collect:(1) the Medical/Dental and Family History; (2) the Psychological Screening Procedure; and (3) the Preschool Inventory. The collection was pursued vigorously, with a great deal of follow- up effort.and, as of November1965, the nonresponse rate for (1) and (2) above was low (less than 8 percent).The nonresponse rate for (3), however, was over 40 percent. For each staff member in the centers chosen for the Census 1-percent sample, Census attempted to collect the Paid and Voluntary Worker's Evaluation and the Staff Member Information Sheet. The non- response rate on them. as of November was 25 to30 percent. We turn now to describing what we have called (by necessity and not by choice) the 1-percent sample in this report.1It is the Census 1-percent sample forms as of November plus others, some of which were included by mistake. These others areprobably: (1) forms that Census intended to include but which arrived in November or later; 'Obtainedfrom 0E0 Information Center data files.Census 1-percent sample data are referenced as such.

11-.148 (2) forms from children that Census did notselect in centers it did choose for its 1-percent sample; and(3) forms from children and workers in the 10-percent sample of centers. Census tabulated the answers to about ahundred questions from their 1-percent sample, so it ispossible to compare the 1-percentsample of this report with the Census1-percent sample and thus get an idea of the amount and kind of extraneityinsinuated into the 1-percent sample of this report.Exhibit 11-71 contains answer frequencies to fourof the Fam- ily History Questions tabulated by Census, alongwith the partly extraneous additional answers in the 1-percent sample.Also included are some num- bers which facilitate the comparison.It is clear from Exhibit II-71 that the incremental children added to the Censussample contain more Negroes and slightly more children from familieswhose incomes fall between $3,000 and $5,999 than would a random selection of childrenfrom a pop- ulation from which the Census figures werealso a random selection. How- ever, even ideally theincremental answers are not a random selection but rather the re.:;ults from some late reportingclusters. Moreover, the effect of the differences from expectations ofthe incremental answers on the total is not large in the sense of percentage error. We have two more facts to add to theevaluation of the possible bias from the extraneous forms. Census processed5,036 Medical/Dental Forms in November, and the 1-percent samplecontains 6,309. About 14 percent of the centers named by the 1-percentstaff forms (but nothing as great as 14 percent of theforms) were not chosen by Census for its 1-percent sample.It is most unlikely, then, that the extraneousforms, of themselves, could have changed thedescriptions and conclusions drawn from the 1-percent sample. In addition to the sample data collectedby Census, a sample of parent interviews was obtained by theNational Opinion Research Council (NORC). NORC drew its sample of parentsby drawing a 2-strata sample of children from the Census 10-percentsample of Child Development Centers. The two strata were the ruralMidwest, with a sample size n2 of 89, and the restof the country, with an n1 of1,653.If N2 and

II -149 EXHIBIT 11-71 COMPARISON OF 1-PERCENT SAMPLES WITH CENSUS TABULATIONS AnswersQuestions/ Answer Frequenciesfrom the Census Tabulation Answer Frequencies in the 1-Percent AdditionalSample Values Used in the Corresponding AverageCensus of Chi Squared Additional1-percent Calculation Chi Sex. remaleMale' 2,4912,336 612562, . 2,3312,496 607567 2.88 Race OtherNegroWhite 2,0472,155 41 420601 61 2,0522,110 81 538523 21 136.5*(1) FamilyMother Income works $3,000Less$6,000 than to or $5,999$3,000 more 1,4571,916 367 418368 55 1,4901,906 345 428335 7? 11495* Note: (1) AsteriskYes (*) indicates significance at a 2,7591,425 level660359 lower than .005. 2,7491,435 670349 .55 N1 are the correspondingpopulation sizes, NORC calculated that:

n,117 1tf- = 4.3 n 2N1

In order to avoid furtherstratification (or sample more heavily than 2/3 from the classes),NORC chose 230 children from ruralsouth- ern centers notin the Census 10-percentsample. In addition to the samplejust described, NORC madeeight special samples of about 30 eachfrom five ethnic groups and three poor groups. Before discuss3.ng the problemof extrneous forms, wementioned nonresponse. Wenow return to nonresponse and themuddling caused by it, which for the staffforms and especially the PPVT scoresis far more severethan that from the unwantedforms. We shall discuss first the staff forms, becausetheir response rate is so muchhigher (70 to 75 percent) than that for matchedPPVT scores (10 percent) thatthe dis- cussion is almost qualitativelydifferent.Before proceeding further, how- ever, we mustmake two general remarks forthe benefit of the reader who likes to skip tenuous arguments.Fir.7t, it would have beenunthink- able not to have looked atand given our readers theopportunity to look at the data that wasobtained; it is truly sui generis.On the other band, an, attempt todelineate the possible biasesdue to nonresponse is in the final analysis a discussionof what. those who did not answerwould have answered. Th two staff forms of interest arethe Staff Member Information Sheet and the Paid and VoluntaryWorker's Evaluation (PAVWE). The nonresponse rateof 25 to 30 percent is onlymarginally greater than the 20 percent that the CensusBureau is willing to accept.The gross find- ing of overwhelmingenthusiasm could not bereversed by those who did not respond; no matterhow disgruntled. For, as anexample, consider the 71.5 percent of responses onthe PAVWE that indicategeneral morale was"Very Good ". Now, suppose that25 percent did not respond.(The exact figure cannotbe obtained because .of lackof knowledgeAout the

11-151 extraneous forms.) Then, if pis the percentage of nonrespondents who would have answered"Very Good", a p as low as 45,5 percent would reduce the overall percentageof "Very Good" to only 65 percent; a p of 25.5 percentwould reduce it to 60 percent; and no p could re- duce the percentage of"Very Good" below 53.6 percent. The final point in favor of the staff forms'reliability is that a follow-up effort was made and no glaringly debilitating consistenciesin the nonrespondents were reported. Insofar as possible, the Peabody PictureVocabulary Test (PPVT) was to be given twice toeach child in the Summer 1965 Head Start pro- gram. The intent was togive the PPVT the first time as early as pos- sible and within the-first 3 weeksof class. The test was to be given the second time to the same child at least 4 weekslater and preferably in the final week of class. The raw scores wererecorded on the same forms on which were recorded the item responsesof the Pre-School Inventory (PSI), which was also apre/posttest. We considered only raw scores from the1-percent sample. The PSI forms of the 1-percent sample children were scanned for the PPVT scores.The ID numbers of about 3,500 different children were found onthe PSI forms; of these, 1,686 had at least one PPVT score. The number of the weekof center operation was to be marked on the PSIform at the time of PSI and PPVT administration.If a child had two PPVT scores recorded on PSIforms on which were alsorecorded the week of center operation, he was said to have both a pretest and a posttest scoreif the difference in week a numbers was at least three; there were634 such children.Otherwise, a single score wasattributed to a child, if possible, and called a pre- score. Out of a possible 6,309 1-percent samplematched PPVT scores, then, 634 were selected by a largely unknownmechanism. Selection usually singles out the better--e. g., thebrighter child is easier to give a test to and more likely to be therewhen posttest time comes. A glance at the special study resultsreported in Section IV suggests that the 1-percent in PPVT scores areindeed high, even when adjusted for the fact that the 1-percent m PPVTchildren are older, by and large,

11-152 than the special study children.[Interestingly enough, the rest of the1- percent sample children. areolder stillsee Exhibit 11-73, page11-156. Since there is no reason tobelieve that the small amount ofselection in- volved in getting age data onthe whole 1-percent sample exertedmuch bias on the age estimates,there is a strong suggestion that thespecial studies selected youngchildren--and if young, why not low PPVT scorers? ] Our interest, however,is not so much in where the children are on the PPVTscale as in where they are'relative to their starting points or to each other.More precisely, we are interestedin pre/post differ- ences and factorlevel differences of both prescores(for description) and difference or regresseddifference; translations on the PPVTscale that preserve thesedifferences are of little interest.Our hope, then, is that even if-thereis a bias towards bright children,the differences of inter- est are preserved. The special studies suggestthat the raw score differences are,if anything, accentuated in the1-percent m PPVT scores.[Whether or not higher prescoresresult in smaller pre/postdifferences when the selection of the high scores is on abasis other than highness receives considerable discussion elsewherein this report (subsection IV.C).There is no evidence of it for thisselection.] A way of generating someevidence about whether selectionof high PPVT scores destroys differentialeffects is to restrict one's analysis PPVT scores. This was done tothe extent sum- to children with low. ire - marized in Exhibit II-72.The result (that nothing matters but ageand that impact on the young is lessthan on the older) parallels pretty accur- ately the differential effectsdiscovered below by an analysisof covariance of all the scores.This is a weak indication that areal elimination of the putative bias towards high scoresmight leave the differential effects unchanged. We have another exhibit, but noargument for it to support.Instead we resort to ananecdotal challenge to thereader. The Literary Digest poll that predicted Landon' svictory in the 1936 presidentialelection col- lected, along with its selectedstraw votes, somesecondary information

11-153 EXHIBIT II-72 '"NALYSIS OF DIFFERENTIAL FrequenciesBig of Gainers Frequencies of Small IMPACT ON CHILDREN Corresponding Average ValuesUsed in the Calculation of WITH LOW PRE-PPVT SCORES Sex Male Factor Level WhoseWas Prescore 39 or LessAmoi.,g Those 31 Gainers Among Those Whose WasPrescore 39 or Less 27 Big Gainers 29 Chi Squared Small Gainers 29 Chi Sguared Race FemaleNegroWhite 472443 484919 454922 464721 .46.54 Age 6Younger5 years years or than older 5 years 401718 3433 8 25.536.513 25.536.513 10.18 *(1) Urbanization FarmRuralUrban nonfarm 5016 4 5411 4 5214 4 5213 4 .76 FamilyIncome Intactness $3,000Less than or $3,000more 453837 453050 4541.533.5 41.533.550 1.32 Note: (1) Asterisk (*)FatherMother indicates onlyonlyand father 17 < .01. 19 1 17 1 19 1 0 about the selected straw voters--e.g., how they had voted in 1932 and whether they were Republican or Democrat. After the fact analyses have been made, using the secondary information to adjust the straw vote totals, which indicate that Roosevelt should have been predicted winner. We have a lot of secondary information about the children who provided 1-percent matched PPVT scores vis-a-vis the 1-percentsam- ple children who did not. Some of it is summarized in Exhibit II-73, which makes it pretty clear that the children who provided 1-percent matched PPVT scores were younger, poorer, and had more working mothers and smaller households than those who did not. Two final points are worth noting.First, the nonmatching PPVT scores called "pre" throw nolight on what the missing scores might be like, because they have no known mates and becausethere is no infor- mation about the times during the program when they were obtained. Second, there were many mistakes in scoring the PPVT. Ofthe first 16 sheets that we looked at, 9 had incorrect raw scores. The reported raw score averaged 1.5 points toohigh over the 16 sheets. We also found a center where 17 out of 18 postscores were identicalwith the cor- responding pre arnrs1- In summary, a reasonable position on the worth of the 1-percent m PPVT scores might be thatpre/post differences were positive and that subtler distinctions are worth looking for but must beindependently validated. . COMPARISON OF CHILDREN HAVING MATCHED PPVT SCORES EXHIBIT 11-73. 1 WITH THOSE Number of 1-PercentWITHOUT THEM with NumbersSample of or 1-Percent Census Children Without (C) inAverage the Calculation Value Chi Squared 1-Percent Rest of Used of Sex Male Factor Level MatchedSample PPVT Children Scores 301320 Matched PPVT Scores 2,8022,578 PPVT 321300 Sample 2,5982,782 Chi Squared 2.88 Race FemaleNegroWhite 313270 2,3352,305 294286. 11 2,2892,354 ' 91 g0t.ns-, Age OtherYounger than 5 years 354 93 8 (C)1,739 600 94 281 93 1,812 600 3.30 Income 65 LessYearsyears$3,000 than or - older$5,999 $3,000 187295185 2,0391,7531,640 260264207 1,6802,0701,618 45.58*(1) Mother works NoYea$6,000 or more 215348 39 3,0711,569 383 370193 48 3,0491,591 374 4.28*8.65* I LL. ..1 f t,,.' ...... --%i I ... rn21 E=20 EXHIBIT -II-73 g=1 k=i1(Continued) 1=31 i2=3 . CI r""3 r7771 r""1 O t"vin Factor Level MatchedSampleNumber PPVT Children of 1-PercentScores with MatchedNumberSample PPVT of or 1-Percent Census Scores (C)Children Without inAverp.ge the CalculationPPVT Value Used of Chi Squared 1 -Percent SampleRest of Chi Squared Note:Household (1) Asterisk size (*) Moreindicates8 or thanfewer Elp < .05. 528106 3,243 840 (C) 507127 3,264 819 5.01* III. PROGRAMS

A. Introduction In this section, the total and regionaldistribution of Federal ex- penditures for Project Head Start isdiscussed, and a comparison is made between the per-pupil costs of theHead Start programs and the per-pupil costs of regular elementary andsecondary school programs. A more detailed discussion of the intentand s:ructure of the Child Development Center (CDC) is presented.Finally, the programs of the CDC are examined in relation to five factors:(1) medical/dental ser- vices, (2) daily programs, (3) social services,(4) parent participation, and (5) staff and workers. B. Background Information Project Head Start served the seven regions ofthe United States shown on the map in Exhibit III-1, as well asGuam, Samoa, the Virgin Islands, and Puerto Rico. The total costof the project was $94.6 mil- lion, of which the FederalGovernment paid $82.7 million and the local communities paid $11.9 million.The state and regiOnal breakdowns of these totals are shown inExhibit III-2, as well as the total number of centers operated in each area. The cost per child for the summer programaveraged $168 across all programs. There was,however, considerable difference among states, as is shown inExhibit 111 -3.Connecticut showed the lowest average cost per child of$121, and Mississippi the highest at$214.1These differences, as well as thoseobserved within states, are accounted forpartly by the differences in prices of various classesof service between rural and urban areas, and partly by differences in thecomposition of the programs- - differences in the kinds and amounts of administrativesupport, in the facilities and services of the CDC's, in thelength of the daily programs provided, etc. An interesting comparison is provided, inExhibit 111-3, between Project Head Start costs and scaled estimatesof the various state costs per pupil for regularelementary and secondary school programs. This exhibit shows that sinceHead Start was a more extensive effort than is made by the public school systems on acontinuing basis, it was a relatively more expensive program toconduct.The percentage of scaled public school costs to Head Start costs ranged from alow of 14.5 percent to a high of 56.2 percent. The national average was34.0 percent. The CDC was the operating unit throughwhich Head'Start was to achieve the local goals previouslyoutline.d (see section As a concept- the CDC was to be the focus of allavailable resources contributing to the total development of the child -- resourcesof the family, community, and professionals. In scope andmagnitude, it was new and unique in its

'Thisis exclusive of Alaska, where the averagecost per child was $410. oc .11111. maw I t, 41... f I t ...\ a .0' A ...... A...... -...,#°. .-,,..... l4 r... .. \....% . .1 )% 40....r ...... 1 ...... , /.%s. .: c,' ,...... "r \ i f West (VI) 1 ! Midwest IV 1 ( Northeast I Far best (VII 61r 6 ....- 1 / Middle Atlantic (II) 14... L..'!tg ' . ... ./?) I ...I* 1,` , Southwest (V) c-** Southeast. (III) N 1 EXHIBIT III-1 SEVEN HEAD START REGIONS r;, ... EXHIBIT 111-2 STATEAND REGIONALDISTRIBUTION OF FEDERAL HEAD START FUNDSAND CHILDDEVELOPMENT CENTERS

Funds (thousands Region I - Northeast of dollars) Centers 209 Connecticut 464.6 38 Maine 175.5 405 Massachusetts 1,363.4 68 New Hampshire 288.4 412 New Jersey 1,871.2 830 New York -6,560.0 50 Rhode Island 198.7 98 Vermont 310.7 2,110 Total 11,232.5 Region II - MiddleAtlantic 103. Delaware 237.6 District of Columbia 763.0 69 578 Kentucky 3,884.7 202 Maryland 1,077.5. 804 North Carolina 3,914.8 381 Pennsylvania 2,231.2 316 Virginia 2,126.6 556 West Virginia 2,873.3 3,009 Total 17,108.7

111-4 EXHIBIT III-2(Continued)

Funds (thousands Essisniin:121Itheast of dollars) Centers Alabama 2,068.4 288 Florida 2,349.4 346 Georgia 2,898.5 379 Mississippi 4,152.2 284 South Carolina 1,933.9 262 Tennessee 4,121.0 761 Total 17,523.4 2,320 Region IV - Midwest Illinois 4,431.6 324 Indiana 755.3 115 Michigan 2,356.0 472 Minnesota 476.3 90 Ohio 2,902.6 505 Wisconsin 371.6 46 Total 11,293.4 1,552 Region V - Southwest Arkansas 1,852.7 363 Louisiana 3,311.0 315 New Mexico 921.1 287 Oklahoma 1,252.1 365 Texas 5,377.6 906 Total 12,714.5 2,236

III-5

=.11.01.10NanweamMommowa EXHIBIT III-2(Continued)

. Funds (thousands Region VI - West of dollars) Centers Colorado 940.6 133 Idaho 65.0 15 Iowa 657.2 143 Kansas 316.5 64 Missouri 2,141.0 451 Montana 147.2 Nebraska 98.7 25 North Dakota 216.7 42 South Dakota 123.2 32 Utah 126.4 20 Wyoming 48.0 10 Total 4,880.5 1,055 Region VII - Far West Alaska 839.9 77 Arizona 1,187.5 139 California 2,943.2 527 Hawaii 337.2 90 Nevi.da 104.7 25 Oregon 219.3 34 Washington 5.05.3 86 Total 6,137.1 978 EXHIBIT 111-3STATE AND REGIONAL EXPENDITURES PER PUPIL (HEAD START VERSUS PUBLIC SCHOOL SYSTEMS)

Head Start Expenditures Scaled Public School(2) Per Pupil Percent System Expenditure Region I - Northeast (in dollars) PS/HS(1)Per Pupil (in dollars) Connecticut $121 56.2 $68 Maine 170 27.0 46 Massachusetts 191 34.0 65 New Hampshire 190 28.4 54 New Jersey 188 38.8 73 New York 201 42.8 86 Rhode Island 210 30.5 64 Vermont 190 31.6 60(3)

Region II- Middle Atlantic Delaware 161 422 68 District of Columbia 171 36.8 63 Kentucky 176 23.9 42 Maryland 157 38.2 60 North Carolina 170 24.7 42 Pennsylvania 183 34.4 63 Virginia 162 27.2 44 West Virginia 178 23.0 41

Region III - Southeast Alabama 168 21.4 36(3). Florida 149 32.9 49 Georgia 177 22.0 39 Mississippi 214 14.5 31 South Carolina 155 21.3 33 Tennessee 164 22.0 36

111-7 fo EXHIBIT 111-3(Continued)

Head Start (2) Expenditures Scaled Public School Per Pupil Percent System Expenditure Region. IV - Midwest (in dollars) PS/HS(1)Per Pupil (in dollars) Illinois $147 51.0 $75(3) Indiana 195 32.8 64(3) Michigan 158 42.4 67(3) Minnesota 184 36.4 67 Ohio 157 35.7 56 Wisconsin 184 35.9 66

Legion V - Southwest Arkansas 176 20.4 36 Louisiana 193 28.0 54 New Mexico 164 35.4 58 Oklahoma 192 24.5 47 Texas 141 49.6 70

Lszion VI - West Colorado 166 36.1 60 Idaho 130 36.9 48(3) Iowa 177 34.4 61(3) (3) Kansas 145 42.1 61 Missouri 148 39.2 58(3) Montana 177 35.6 63- Nebraska 144 36.1 52 North Dakota 194 29.4 57 ,South Dakota . 178 29.8 53 30.6 49 Utah* . 160 Wyoming 177 39.5 70

111-8 EXHIBIT III-3(Continued)

Head Start Expenditures Scaled Public School() Per Pupil Percent System Expenditure Region VIIFar West (in dollars PS/HS(1) Per Pupil jin dollars) Alaska $410 21.2 $87 Arizona 159 39.6 63 California 157 42.0 66 Hawaii 152 3Z.9 50 Nevada 1,83 33.5 63 Oregon 185 37.8 70 Washington 168 41.1 69(3) National Average 34.0%

Notes: (1) Percent public school system versusHead Start. (1) These figures have been calculated by taking theOffice of Education expenditure estimates from "Statistics ofState School Systems 1961-62" and multiplying them by afactor (.148) to adjust for the differences in program length between the"typical" school year and the Head Start summer program. (3) Figures are based on average dailyattendance rather than average daily membership.

111-9 attempt to utilize and direct these diverseelements of the child's environ- ment toward the particular needs ofeach child, and in its orientation to- ward the culturally deprived child. The c rc was to be family-centered, aswell as child-centered, and the parents of the Head Startchildren were to play an important role all aspects of the CDC.They were to help formulate its programs and policies,. assist in the centers invario-43 capacities as both paid and volunteer workers, and participateactively in the programs of the cen- ters once they were in operation. Since many of the children andfamilies to be served by Head Start were not awareof the range of community services available tothem, and at the same time the communityagencies administering these ser- vices were not aware of the existenceand needs of some of these families, community social services were to be a keyelement of the CDC activities. Effective use of these services was to beassured through follow-on, referral, and other means. Finally, the specializedservices of various professionals were to besolicited.Experts in such diverse fields as nutrition,health, education, psychology, social work, and recreation wereexpected to play an important partin assuring the success of this"whole-child" approach. The CDC as a physical unit consistedof the building and its associ- ated outdoor play areas.It often had only one classroom, but againthis depended on the location and size of the area tobe served by the center. It is not possible to describe a typicalCDC in terms of either its facilities or its activities and services.Since each Head Start program was undertaken atcommunity initiative, planned primarilyby community members, and administered by a community agency,it reflected the unique combination of needs, skills, and interpretationof overall guidelines of the particular community. It is possible to make some generalizationsand more limited specific comments about the presence, absence,and adequacy of various program elements by drawing on materialprovided by the medical andeducational consultants who visited more than 1,000operating Head Start programs during the summer. In addition, afew research studies have commented on the programmaterial observed, and certain relatedmaterial has been

III-10 provided by data collected on the Staff Member Information Sheet and the Paid and Voluntary Workers' Evaluation Form. In general, it is particularly interesting to compare the reports and opinions ofcon- sultant:. with those of staff members. While such comparisons are not 7.,..:inerally discussed in this report, exhibits and text are organized to make it easy for the reader to crosscheck various points.The material has been organized into components relating to the children, parents, and staff and workers. A discussion of this material follows. C. CDC Services, Activities_and Resources 1. Medical/Dental Services The provision of complete medical/dental evaluations Ior the children was to be an important part of the CDC programs. The program guides distributed to recipients of grants suggested that the basic service include for each child: Medical history, developmental assessment, and physical examination Screening tests of vision, hearing, speech, and tuberculosis testing Laboratory tests of urine and blood Dental assessment Completion of immunizations Psychological evaluation Discussion with pare} its Teacher observations Follow-up services

Summaries of educational consultants' reports on the scheduling. of some of these tasks and on the organizations providing specialized service largely in the medical/dental field are shown in Exhibits III-4 and III-5. As can be seen, the participation of professional groups, in- cluding the medical and dental societies, was less than might have been expected. Indeed, it was much less than hoped for by many Head Start applicants. These exhibits also indicate that, while the scheduling of some kind of medical/dental examinations for the programs v5sited by the consultants was relatively complete, follow-up was a much more difficult task.In only about 60 and 55 percent of the cases of existing medical and dental needs had arrangements been made for services.Consultants also re- ported only a few instances in which the teacher had been assigned any specific responsibility for seeing that the child received medical care. The National Opinion Research Center (NORC) parent interview data indicates that 84 percent of the responding parents stated that

III -12 EXHIBIT III-4MEDICAL/DENTAL PLANNING

Percent of Responses(1) Number of Consultant's ChecklistQuestion Number Yes No Responses

7A. Medical examinationsscheduled for all children 87.9 4.8 928

7B.Dental examinationsscheduled o*6 for all children 85.0 6.7 908 7C.Immunizations arrangedfor 85.I 6.0 7D.Needed medical treatment arranged for 60.5 18.7 875 7E.Needed dental treatment arranged for 55.4 23,9 86] 7F.Glasses provided asneeded 62.5 23.1 809 7G. Responsibility assigned to see children get neededmedical or dental treatment 72. 4 13.5 858

Note: (1) Fromreports of CEOeducational consultants,the remaining percentage was reported as"partial."

III-13 EXHIBIT 111-5ORGANIZATIONS PROVIDINGASSISTANCE

Percent of RespResponses Consultant's Checklist Number of Question Number Yes No Responses

6A.Schools 94.7 5.3 888 6B.Public health 88.0 12.0 892 882 6C. Public welfare 82.8 17.2 810 6D. Hospitals or clinics 59.2 40.8 790 6E.Medical society 43.4 56.6 6F.Dental society 43.5 56.5 787 728 6G.Nur2ing society 27.4 72.6 744 6H. Optometrists 32.9 67.1 6L Dieticians or home economists 53.5 46.5 780 their children received a Head Start medical examination,and that 70 percent felt that the examination was extremelyworthwhile. Sixty- nine percent of the parents said that theirchildren had received a dental examination, and 59 percent felt that it was extremelyworthwhile. Opinions of CDC staff members were also collected regarding the medical and dental programs. A tabulation of responses on thePaid and Voluntary Workers' Evaluation Form for a total of6,320 workers associated with 432 centers in 47 states is shown in Exhibit111-6.This shows the percentage distribution of opinions about the availability of different services, including the medical and dental services It can be seen that for this sample of workers the overallopinion was that the variousservices and facilities listed were satisfactorily available. Of the list, opinions are most positive about the availability of medical and dental services.The high rate of "Cannot Evaluate". responses with respect tothe other services suggests a rather wide- spread lack of familiarity with such services, andthus no standard by which to judge their availability. It should be noted that the sample of staff members andworkers included here is essentially the same as that used todescribe the general characteristics of workers in subsection Reference to Exhibit 11-65 in that section will provide a fairly goodindication of the composition of this sample with respect to the relative percentagesof different attributes or characteristics represented inthe sample. The Workers' Evaluation Forms were also sortedaccording to the position of the worker in the CDC. Frequencydistributions were ob- tained in each item for four categories ofrespondents: paid and volunteer professional, paid neighborhood resident,neighborhood volunteer, and

1For each item, the total number of responses ineach opinion scale level was converted to a percentage of the totalin the available sample. The column entitled "No Response" contains thepercentage of workers in the available sample for whom no response wastabulated, for what- ever reason.Reasons include failure by a worker to fill in the parti- cular item or failure of the data processingoperations to tabulate the response.

ITI-1 5 EXHIBIT 111-6 AVAILABILITY OF SERVICES - WORKERS' EVALUATIONS 13. and/orAvailability dental of services medical 44.4GoodVery Good 33.5 Fair 9.4 Percent Poor2.6 EvaluateCannot6.7 Response 3.4 No ..... 14.15. Availabilityeducation facilities of special- 12.518.2 21.929.8 12.914.0 11.76.9 25.8 34.7 6.35.3 b-GCr%A atriclogicalAvailability services and/or of psychi-psycho- - 16. Ayailabilityservice agencies of Social . 21.5 30.2 13.3 5.9 23.3 5.8 other paid and volunteerworkers.'A chi-square was calculated for each item to test the hypothesisthat the four distributions are not significantly different. All items inExhibit III-6 so tested hadX2's that were signi:- ficant (p < .05, df =12).Indeed, this was the case for all items on the form except two, as willbe seen later. For the questions underconsideration here, the professionals gen- erally gave disproportionatelyhigh numbers of "Poor" responses, and, even morestrongly, took a "Can't Evaluate" position with respectto these aspects of the program.It was true, however, that the majority ,answered "Good" or "Very Good." A sample of proposals submitted byapplicants for Head Start grants was reviewed.They showed an even wider divergence ofmedical/dental programs thanthat .reported by consultants as the result of visits tothe centers. In some cases,preliminary contacts and arrangements had been made for specialists' services andthe support of various medical associations. More frequently,applicants had assumed (often incorrectly) that such services could be arrangedfree, or at a nominal fee. There appear to have been some inconsistencies onthe part of 0E0 in funding for medical programs.Consultants have cited several projects for which Head Start paid the entirecost of both complete medical checkups and follow-up treatments.In other cases, costs in- cluded for medical services were cutfrom programs during the budget review process. Many reports offriction because of these inconsistencies have been noted by the consultants. In the extremely rural areas servedby some Head Start centers, the problem of unavailability ofprofessionals for health programs was particularly acute. The following story istypical: Arranging medical examinations has beendifficult since there are only three doctors inthe entire county. Two doctors are participating - -one hasbeen cooperative

1This sorting resulted in a loss of someforms. The resulting sample sizes, which varied from. question toquestion for all items on the form, were on theorder of 3,500 to 4,400 per item.The consistent proportions of worker categories in thesamples were about as follows:professional; 41 percent; paid neighborhoodresident, 35 percent; neighborhoodvolun- teer, 14 percent; volunteerworkers, 9 percent. III-17 .

but is too busy toparticipate (he hagagreed to assist if no other doctors can befound).There are no health room facilities in the schools, noclinic in the county, and one doctor has refused toexamine children in hisoffice, so blood analysis was notincluded in the examination.A school nurse and the countypublic health nurse are as- sisting in the medicalexaminations. There are only twodentists in the entire county, and one of them will notexamine patients youngerthan fourth graders, so it wasimpossible to scheduledental examinations by dentists.The doctors areincluding a mouth check as part ofthe medical examination,and children with obviousdifficulties are referred tothe dentist for treatment.It is hoped that adental clinic will be set up before theclose of the Head StartPro- gram to treatthe children on welfare.If there is a great demand for treatment,it may be possible to schedule a visit by the Statemobile dental clinic.This would not be possibleduring the summer,however. The medical, dental,and psychological treat- ment needed bychildren in... could beprovided either in.9 or ,but both these places are morethan 100 miles away and transportationcannot be arranged.The state will not paymileage for the publichealth nurse to transport thechildren, and thus far novolunteers are available todo this.There is also the problemof transporting preschoolchildren, some of whomhave never been outof the county, such a longdistance for treatment. Workers also ratedthe value of the medicaland dental examina- tions.The distribution ofopinions of the 6,320 staffmembers about these examinations isshown in Exhibit III-7.Although there is a high examinations, these opinions degree of approval ofmedical and dental Ile can onlylend themselves toambiguous interpretation.They do not necessarily refer to what wasactually done.The question on theWorker's Evaluation Form reads:"in respect to the physicaland psychological health of the childand his educationaldevelopment, my attitudestowards the following are:(1) Medical examination...(2). Dental examination..." Thus, for example, thoseworkers who checked "NotApplicable" or"Waste of Time" may havebeen saying in effectthat there were no such examinations intheir centers, or that they werenot useful for any of several possible reasons.-- . . More interestingand meaningfulare selected observationsof med- ical consultantswho examined thehealth component of 344Head Start III-18

t 11111111111111111111111111/1MEM11.91 tygmet "111k11111111 -leas11111111111111111.1.1.0.1k, . 4=3 =II 1=3 f=1 C-M c rrn rn O reins frrn Workers'EXHIBIT EvaluationIII-7 MEDICAL/DENTAL EXAMINATIONS-WORKERS' OPINIONS Very Much Occasionally Percent Waste of Not No 18(2).Form18(1). ItemDental Medical Number Examination Examination Worthwhile 66.574.0 Worthwhile 21.017.7 Worthwhile 2.82.2 Time .5.3 Applicable 4.410'1 0 Response 4.8 3.8 programs. These areshown in Exhibit Two items should be par- ticularly noted, namely (1) in almost70 percent of these programs,30 minutes or less was spent on thecombined task of the completionof. 98-question medical/dental and familyhistory records and examiningthe child; and (2) these consultants reportfair or inadequate planningfor the medical/dental programin over 50 percent of the cases. The observations of theseprofessionals led to several suggestions for improving themedical/dental portion of Project HeadStart.Two. observations are particularlynoteworthy. First, since successful medical programs nearlyalways had a medical director designated to plan the services,mobilize the medical resources, andprovide a suit- able professionalenvironment, most of the planningproblems which occurred could have beenmitigated by the use of a medicaldirector early in the programdevelopment stage. However, in rural areas particularly, this is not alwayspossible. Second, a good medicalpio- gram required astrong educational component.Good health practices are a familyactivity and are not acquired as aresult of a single physical examination. As professionalmedical consultants suggested,such examinations can become ofsignificant medical service only whenthey are coupledwith a continued follow-up withthe child and family.This can only beassured by establishing organizationalprocedures and re- sponsibilities to carry out this activity.

Z. Daily Pro One goal of CEO was to providethe culturally deprived child with a preschool program todevelop him intellectually,emotionally, socially, and physically. Becausechildren, whether "well .off" or "disadvantaged," exhibit greatdiversity, it was difficult forCEO to de- fine clearly a good program.However, certain generalizations were made about programs, and someof them were directlyimplemented. In the latter category wasthe requirement for smallclasses (with 15 orfewer) that OEO attempted to ensureby the processing of the grants. Where larger class sizeshad been called out in theapplication, OEO modified the staffing andbudget so that this guideline was morelikely to be observed. EXHIBIT 111-8MEDICAL CONSULTANT REPORT 10'

Head Start Programs That: Percent 2. Had medical program planned by Local health officer 32.2 Pediatrician in private practice 10.1 Public health nurse 12.7 Other physician in private practice 15.6 Other 22.3

6. Had a physician available for A. Discussions with center personnel Available 40.4 Sometimes 22.3 Not available 30.8' Other 2.9 B. Frequent or regular visits to center Regular visiting schedule 11.9 Visits when requested 29.9 Not available 46.5 Other 6.6 9. Spent time taking the history and examination of each child About 15 minttes 43,3 30 minutes 25.2 1 hour 7.2 1-1/2 hours 2.9 2 hours 1.1 Longer 1.4 Other 17.1

12. Had reasonably complete examination Records complete 49.4 Partially complete 33.4 Minimal recording 9.3 Had no medical/dental information 1.1 Had no family data .2

III-21

, EXHIBIT III -8(Continued) Percent 13. Had parents present during examinations Present 20.3 Not present 24.7 Most parents present 29.0 A few parents 17.7 Other 1.7

14. Had health education ornutrition sessions scheduled for parents Once a week 9.8 Every two weeks 19.4 Rarely 30.8 Not at all 322

18. Had nurses spend per week at center Less than 2 hours 43.0 2 to 4 hours 10.1 5 to 10 hours 8.7 Over 10 hours 36.6 21. Had initial dentist visit include Examination 79.9 Prophylaxis 15.1 Instruction 36.6 X rays 4.9 Other 10.1 25. Had vision screening test conducted by Volunteer 16.2 Nurse 55.2 Teacher 10.? Other 22.9 26. Had hearing screening test performed at Hearing and speech center 4.3 Clinic 3.7 Child development center 63.6 Other 142 33. Had adequate planning formedical/dental program Excellent planning 36.6 Fair planning 37.5 Inadequate planning 15.6

III-22 Although from available information it is not possible to describe in detail the schedule and activities of the individual Head Start programs, some elements of these programs canbe highlighted. Generally, programs were of two types: all-day or half-day. Most all-day programs providedbreakfast, lunch, and two snacks, while half-day programs provided a snack. Somehalf-day programs also gave breakfast to those who needed it;others provided lunch ::iefore the children returned home. All-day programs oftenincluded nap, rest, or quiet times andperiods for extended outdoor play. Sometimes dancing, singing, games, and gymnastics wereperformed. The daily work/play activities were usuallyorganized so that peri- ods of activity were followed by periods ofrelative quiet, with appropri- ate outdoor periods interspersed.Periods of activity often emphasized free-play time with teachers and teachers'aides attempting to take ad- vantage of the spontaneous learning opportunitiesthat such play activity provided.Teacher-directed activities included finger-painting, science projects, pet care, etc.The quiet periods included time for snacks, as well as songs, stories, and word games. Manyteachers used this time to stress the improvement of verbalfacility and listening skills.Finally, periods spent outdoors used playground equipment,if available, and organized games, walks, and visitsc.Field trips, often including parents, were part of the outdooractivity. It was difficult to assure program content consistentwith modern concepts of child development. Many of theapplications received by OEO included only the most general description of thedaily program to be undertaken--in some cases simply, a fewparagraphs paraphrasing the informational material supplied by OEOwith the application forms. Even when the application showed more planningand a better grasp of such concepts, it was a problem to assure thatthe program as under- taken would be reflective of the proposal. Paid and volunteer workers rated the programsin which they parti- cipated with respect to a variety of variables orcharacteristics. Exhi- bit 111-9 presents a tabulation of the percentageof responses in each

111-23 EXHIBIT III -9 PROGRAM CHARACTERISTICS WORKERS' EVALUATIONS FormWorkers' Item Evaluation Number GoodVery Good Fair Percent Poor EvaluateCannot Response, No 1.2. teacher,GeneralWasaides daily and moraleteacher's volunteersschedule of 69.937.9 24.649.8 1.96.0 .2.9 1.12.3 3.1 2.3 ts,tv: 3. workersWererealistic?program?out goals there to of enoughcarry the 42.552.0 36.033.8 12.6 3.7 3.8 .4 2.45.1 2.75.0 4.5. lumRelevanceparentsCooperation to the ageof fromcuiricu- child 30.748.9. 43.133.4 16.011.1 2.73.0 . 4.61.0 Z.92.6 6.7. bathrooms,FacilitiesplayAmountspace,school materials areawater, ofof etc.)available the(lighting, storage, pre- ' 38.8 37.4 14.5 3.2 3.5 2,6 EXHIBIT III -9 (Continued) Very Percent Cannot No 9.8.FormWorkers' Item Evaluation Number QualityAdequacyplay materials of available of physical Good 36.139.3 Good41.141.9 Fair12.111.2 Poor3.31.9 Evaluate 3.72.9 Response 3.72.8 . i;.1 $ maintenanceschool area of pre- I-INt.71 10.11. HowQualitysupervision helpful of teaching to your 17.958.1 36.630.3 11.3 2.6 3.5 .9 23.0 4.7 7.73.4 12. mentsportationAdeqizacyyoupsychologicalteachingsures completed? forand were arrange- children ofratings trans- mea-the 46.3 30.4 8.3 2.9 8.1 4.0 i 17. parent-teacherAdequacyandconferencesof timeparents set of asideamount for 21.3 36.0 15.0 6.1 16.8 4.8 opinion scale category or level for each question or item.The percen- tages are relevant to a total sample size of6,320 respondents. There are a number of points of interest inExhibit 111-9.The trends of opinions along the scaleshow that Items 11 and 17 appear to be unique when the "CannotEvaluate" category is considered. These items, on the research instrumentsand on the time set aside for parent- teacher conferences, had the lowestpercentage of "Very Good" ratings of any of the items. They alsohad the highest percentages of "Cannot Evaluate" responses.It is true that many people in the sample un- doubtedly had no basis for evaluationbecause of their position and duties in the center, their training, etc.However, information reported in some of the independent research studies(e.g. Berlin, Pierce-Jones, etc.) indicates that there was substantialobjection to some of the instruments employed. Turning to some of the other items,there appears to have been general satisfaction with the relevance ofthe curriculum. (Item 4); morale was high(Item 1); and the quality of the teachingsupervision was con- sidered to be generally very good(Item 10). For all items in Exhibit 111-9,significant differences (beyond the .05 level) were found between thecategories of workers. On Item 4, for example, professional workersshowed a significantly larger pro- portion of "Very Good" responses(61 percent of 1,738 respondents) than nonprofessionals, while neighborhood paidworkers showed larger than expected responses in "Fair,""Poor," and "Can't Evaluate" categories. Since the evaluation item deals with ajudgment about the relevance of the curriculum, more credenceshould presumably be given to the eval- uation of the professional workers.On other items, the differences are more difficult to interpret.Thus on Item 10, fewer professionalsthan expected rated the quality ofteaching supervision as "Very Good," while paid neighborhood workers providedthis response more often than ex- pected. None of the differencesobserved alter the general picture of positive opinion held by theworkers. Review and tabulation of writtencomments on the Paid andVoluntary Workers' Evaluation Form providedfurther information bearing on th 3

111-26 , evaluations in Exhibit III-9. A total of 6,433 forms were examined, with comments classified according to topic.There were 2,857 comments on different topics, most of which were favorable. The modal comment (25.6 percent) was that the children benefited. However, ina number of cases, criticisms or suggestions were made. The following percent- ages of categories of comments are noteworthy here: Parents benefited or participation was good (2.9 percent ), but there was a need for greater participation of parents and/or communication with teachers (2.9 percent), anda need for more social workers and a greater contact with homes (0.7 percent). More or better training and/or selection of teachers' aides was needed (2.8 percent), and more staff and/or smaller groups of children were needed (2.3 percent). Facilities, supplies, equipment, or money were inadequate (4.0 percent). Planned curriculum, special work, discipline, and field trips were needed (2.0 percent). There was too much testing and paperwork (10.6 percent) and there was general criticism of research instruments (1.4=percent). Problems existed in organizing, planning, administration, etc. (4.1 percent), and transportation was inadequate (0.? percent). Workers were asked to express their opinions about the value of various programmatic features or activities.The results for the sample are shown in Exhibit III-10. As noted above (see subsection III. C.1), the responses do not necessarily indicate an evaluation of what was actually done in the various daily programs. In considering all the worker opinion results, the reader should remember that the opinions reported here are for the total sample. There is no way in these data of distinguishing the center affiliations of reporting workers within an item or question.It would be of interest to know, for example, whether responses, regardless of the position of the worker, are more highly correlated for some CDC's than for others.

III-27 . EXHIBIT III-10 SERVICES AND ACTIVITIES - OPINIONS . . Percent Waste of Not FormWorkers!18. (1)Item Evaluation Number ex:aminationMedical WorthwhileVery Much Worthwhile 66.574.0 21.017.7 OccasionallyWorthwhile 2.22.8 Time .3.5 Applicable ' 4.4 2.0 Response 3.94.8 (2)(3) DentalattendOpportunity examination school to at an ` 75.1 18.0 2.5 0 1.0 3.4 ooEcv 1 (4)(5) withtoysIncreasedearlyIncreased a and varietyage games experience experience of 74.466.8 26.019.0 2.62.0 .1.1 1.11.1 3.43.4 (6) withmusicbooks,munityTrips a variety stories, into theof and corn- 63.6 24.2 4.2 .2.1 3.5 3.24.3 (7)(8) OpportunitybygivenIndividualotheractivitiesticipate teacher to children each in attentionwith group to par- and aides child 73.376.6 19.517.2 2.51.5 .1 1.31.4 3.3 A summary of the cor sultants' comments on the programactivities illustrates some of their concern(see Exhibit III-11).This shows that in only about 55 percent ofthe observed cases did the consultantsbelieve that the program met the needs ofthe individual child, and in onlyabout 64 percent did they feel it met the needsof the child with special problems. While teachers and workers appear tohave used facilities and materials well in the opinion of theconsultants, they were less able to help thechild see himself andhis interests as worthy. Even fieldtrips appeared to the consultants to lack real significancein more than half of the programs. In a critique of the summer program,the consultants commented on the tendency toadhere to a rigid daily schedule,particularly in pro- grams sponsoredby school systems. In many of these programs,it seemed that the goal of Head Start wasrather narrowly construed to be one of schoolreadiness. At least a part of thisconfusion could have resulted from various statementsissued by 0E0 that accentuated the purely educational aspects ofthe program. Several independent research studiesfocus on the schedules and activities of selected centers.Observations of two such studies, one in Massachusetts and theother in New York, are presentedhere. Dr. Elmer E. Van Egmond (Reference105) interviewed 9 Head Start teachers (out of 17 in 12 centers)in Cambridge, Massachusetts.As a programdesigned to prepare culturally deprivedchildren for entrance into kindergarten or first grade,Dr. Van Egmond noted thatnothing could differentiate these programs fromother preschool classes with similar ends. "Typical" days variedbetween programs with a completelack of planning to more structuredefforts which might include periods for show-and-tell, free play, washroom breaks,milk and crackers, out- side play, and stories and music.No teacher mentioned parentalin- volvement as part of a "typical" day.Neighborhood trips were taken; of the teachers interviewed,the median number of trips forthe pro- gram was four(the maximum was seven and theminimum was one). Van Egmond concluded thatcareful planning was not a program characteristic. Most of theteachers, he stated, were committed to a

111-29 EXHIBIT III-11 PROGRAMCHARACTERISTICS -EDUCATIONAL CONSULTANTS' REPORT

Consultant's Number of Checklist Characterization of the Percent Question No.Overall Activity of the Staff Yes No Responses

8A Program tailored toindividual child's needs 54. 9 7. 8 890

8B(1) Rapport established with children as a group 90. 3 0. 8 918

8B(2) Rapport establishedwith children with specialproblems64. 6 6. 7 916

8C Implements a relatively unstructured program 58.910.4 891

8D Implements a highly- structured program 15.856.4 813

8E Believes all childrenshould achieve specific levelof goals 10. 465. 3 892

8F Believes goals shouldbe adapted to the individualchild 65. 5 5. 6 908

8G Emphasizes self-discipline and self-control 57.813.0 887

8H Encourages free playand expres sion 59.96.7 907

8J Makes effective useof materials and equipment 100. 0 - 704

8K Provides a wide varietyof outdoor play activitie.s 64.3 5. 1 888

8L Provides for significant field. trips 45.219.6 900

8M Provides group activitiesof interest and shortduration 77. 9 6. 1 896

8R Implements programempha- sizing languagedevelopment 59. 011. 0 902

III-30 EXHIBIT III-11(Continued) Consultant's (1) Checklist Characterization of the PercentNumber of Question No. Overall Activity of the Staff Yes NoResponses.

8S Aware of learning potential of children's play activities 69.54.6 894

8T Encourages child's curiosity, spontaneity, and expression 61.1 7.2 914

8U Accepts behavior ofchildren from different social status 61.1 1.7 897

8V Helps children feelaccepted and good about themselves 86.5 1.7 897

8W Exploits 'things and ideas to assist communicationskills 80.5 1.8 903

8X Encourages children to observe carefully thingsof interest to them 64.3 3.6 893

8Y Capitalizes on different backgrounds and cultures of children 59.55.2 892

8Z Helps child see himself and his interests asworthy 46.821.7 792

8AA Helps children appreciate community symbols of service - -policemen, -firemen, etc. 76.12.1 897

.

Note: (1) Remainder of responsesreported as "partial "

III-31 theory of learning through activity.Their programs included a vari- ety of activities without specific direction orgoal orientation. Dr. John Harding (Reference47) described and analyzed the pro- grams of three centersin Tompkins County, New York. Hestated that the programs in these communities had manysimilarities. Each was planned and directed by public school personnel;the staffs were all trained at one institution; andeach program was intermediate (between a typical nurseryand a typical kindergarten). All programs had in- door free play, snacks, structured groupactivities, and outdoor free play. Two of the centers operated forhalf-days; the two classes in the other center were for 7 hours per day. The chief feature of one program was achange in emphasis as the program progressed.Early emphasis was placed on ficmiliarization of the child with (1) materials, routines, andrules, (2) the child's name, and (3) conversation. Later, more formal groupsituations (including stories, games, MU31C2 and discussions) werestressed. Another program featured a weekly field trip. Mothers werein- vited to accompany the children; there was usually oneadult for every two to three children on these excursions.After the trips, follow- through activities were directed by the teachers. In the third center, one teacher emphasizedunstructured activities. For the most part, she worked aroundinterests of the children and stressed the improvement of the child's self-image, e.g.,placing snap- shots of the children and their artwork on the wall.Frequent meetings with parents were also held. The above program descriptions are notsuggested as being rep- resentative of all centers, but only as illustrativeof the variety of pro- grams found among centers. 3. Social Services Consultants were quite critical of the areaof social services. Many programs included little or no use ofsocial workers or psychologists; however, the absence of these elements,while typically the outcome of trimming during the budget process, wascaused primarily by a Zack of

III-32 ft

understanding of the services to beperformed in this area. Applicants . for grants had less understandingof the functions of the social worker and psychologist than of thoseof any other disciplines involved inHead Start.If this situation is to beimproved so that funding of such services can be initiated, asubstantial educational effort mustbe undertaken. Ex- hibitIII-1 2 shows the workers' opinionsabout the availability of social services. Data are percentagesof responses to those items on the Worker's Evaluation Form. 4. Parent Participation The importance of parentalinvolvement was clearly indicated in the Head Start literatureand was also cited by consultants.Many factors of success depended on theinterest and cooperation of parents. Attendance of 4-, 5-, and6-year-olds hinged on this factor, as didneeded follow-up medical and dental treatment.Further, if there was to be suc- cessful interaction within the classroombetween child and teacher, direct contact between teacher andhome was needed. In this way, theteacher and parent could communicate as tothe specific needs of the child as an individual (and in Head Start, one goal wastreatment of the child as an individual with personalized needs andinterests). The parent is the first step in educating the communityin child development methods.Through- out the reports of consultants ran anemphasis on the need for concomitant parent education. Consultants most often mentionparental involvement as a key a element in a successful project,and at the same time this area ismost often called out as a major projectweakness. The consultants' com- ments are summarized inExhibit III -13.These show that a particularly small percentage of parents wereincluded in center planningactivities. At least one factor contributingto what was often thought tobe a lack of parent interest, demonstratedby poor parent participation in scheduled meetings, was the heavywork schedule of parents.This problem, coupled with a lack oftransportation, was especially severe in certain rural programs.Among non-Anglo parents, the more com- mon obstacle to parentparticipation waslanguage.These points should be compared with similar onesmade by the workers (seesubsection III.C.2). III-33

I EXHIBIT 111-12 AVAILABILITY OF SOCIAL SERVICES - WORKERS' EVALUATION ' a ...... ,,, Percent FormWorkers' Item Evaluation Number GoodVery Good . Fair Poor EvaluateCannot Response No 16.15. logicalAvailabilityAvailabilityatricservice services and/or agencies of of psychi-psycho- social 21.512.5 21.930.2 13.312.9 11.7 5.9 23.334.7 5.86.3 EXHIBIT III-13 PARENTPARTICIPATION - EDUCATIONAL CON- SULTANTS' REPORTS

Consultant's Checklist 1) Question Percent( Number of Number Yes No Responses 5A. Parents' meetings are beingheld 61.3 23.3 914 53. Some parents' suggestionshave been adopted by the center 31.5 51.7 849 5C. Parents are helping to planfollow- through 23.9 59.5 843 5D. Parents are helping to plan full- year CDC's 17.0 69.8 799 8N. Teachers encourage parent parti- cipation in the classroom 73.4 4.2 896 8Q. Teachers have established adequate communication with parents 90.1 1.6 892 10B. Teachers are responsible for helping parents learn about their children 41.5 58.5 878 10C. Teachers are responsible for helping to solve family problems 73.8 26.2 881

Number of Yes Responses Programs provided for parents include: Help in child rearing 878 Homemaking education 445 Consumer education 340 Other 233

Note: (1) Remainder of responsesreported as "partial."

III-35 5. Staff and Workers Staff training for the Summer1965 program was provided by the National University ExtensionAssociation (NUEA) under contract to OEO (see Section I for backgroundinformation). The goals of the training program (Reference90)were to: Orient the trainees to the aims andactivities of the CDC; Focus on physical development andhealth problems of economically disadvantaged children; Explore some of the influences of poverty onfamiliar relation- ships, particularly acculturation andself-image; Discuss in some depth the role of the CDCstaff in their relationship to the children, parents, volunteers,and other staff; and Help the staff cope with the "concrete problemsthey are likely to encounter." A Core Curriculum was established byOEO, consisting of seven major subject areas: (1) Orientation to the Child Development Center concept (2) Medical and nutritional features of the CDC programs. (3) Relationship of CDC to other social service programs (4) Sociology of the disadvantaged (5) Educational program and activities of the CDC (6) Instructions on coping with problem situations 1 (7) Use of volunteers in the CDC The sequencing of topics and detailedcurriculum content were to remain flexible, so that the individual training institutionscould meet special local and regional needs. A later supplement tothe curriculum concerned the testing program to be carried out bythe CDC. Exhibits III-I4 and III-15 summarize severalinteresting characteristics of the trainees attending NUEA-sponsoredsessions. They show the pre- ponderance of women teachers who servedHead Start during the summer, and they also show that most of theseteachers' experience was in elementary,

1Thislist and the following materialspecific to the training program are taken from Reference 90.

III-36 EXHIBIT III-14 SELECTED NUEA TRAINEECHARACTERISTICS

Number Percent

1. Age Under 21 348 1.3 21 - 25 4,620 16.9 26-30 3,623 13.3 31 - 45 9,878 36.1 46-60 7,767 28.4 Over 60 766 2.8 No reply 325 1.2

2. Seic Male 2,100 7.7 Female 23,869 87.3 No reply 1,358 5.0 3. CDC Position Teacher 23,735 86.9 Social worker 360 1.3 Physician, dentist 6 <0.1 Nurse 164 0.6 Nutritionist F.,7 0.2 Other paid professional 2,113 7.7 Volunteer 194 0.7 No reply 698 2.6

111-37 EXHIBIT 111-15EXPERIENCE OF NUEA TRAINEES

Number Percent

1. Hi hest level of educationcompleted Less than high school 25 0.1 High school 260 1.0 College 1 or 2 years 1,131 4.1 3 or 4 years 15,744 57.6 5 or 6 years 7,723 28.3 7"or more years 1,495 5.5 No reply 949 3.5 2. College degrees held Yes No No Repiy Number PercentNumber Percent Number Percent Associate 1,855 6.8 6,944 25.4 18,528 67.8 Bachelor 21,784 79.7 1,501 5.4 4,042 14.8 Masters 5,637 20.6 6,800 24.9 14,890 54.5 Doctorate 98 0.4 7,828 28.6 19,401 71.0 3. Teaching experience Yes No No Reply, NumberPercentNumber Percent Number Percent Preschool 9,922 36.3 .6,501 23.8 10,904 39.9 Grades 1 - 3 19,312 76.7 2,247 8.2 5,768 21.1 Grades 4 - 6 11,192 41.0 4,604 16.8 11,531 42.2 Grades 7 - 9 6,458 23.6 6,453 23.6 14,416 -52.8 Grades 10 - 12 3,307 12.1 7,831 28.7 16,189 59.2 College 944 3.5 8,827 32.3 17,556 64.2 Supervisory 2,330 8.5 8,102 29.6 16,895 61.8

III-38 EXHIBIT III-15(Continued)

4. Teaching certificates or licenses Yes No No Reply Number Percent NumberPercent Number Percent 25,045 91.6 1,505 5.5 777 2.8 5. Experience with Number Percent

None - 6,449 23.6 1 - 5 years 8,005 29.3 6-10 years 3,796 13.9 11 - 15 years 2,788 10.2 16 - 20 years 2,005 7.3 Over 20 years 3,747 13.7 No reply 537 2.0 not preschool, education.Finally, although asubstantial percentage ofthe trainees (23.6 percent) had noexperience with groupsof children from conditions of poverty, alarger percentage (45.1percent) had from 6 to over 20 yearsof such experience. Ratings from the traineesof how well they believedthe training session had preparedthem for their CDC role revealthat 53.2 percent thought they were"very well" prepared;43.2 percent reported they were "adequately" prepared;while only 2,2 percentanswered that they were "inadequately" prepared.It would have beeninteresting to see the results of such a questionafter the summer CDC programs werecompleted, but such data is not available. Ma--y teachersand administrators werereported to be enthusiastic about the less structured,child-oriented approach that was tobe emphasized in Head Start.It was thought by manyconsultants that there wouldbe a .71 carryover ofthis enthusiasm into theclassrooms in the fall when the teachers returned to theirregular work. Smallclasses were strongly advocated and teachers'aides proved to be extremelyhelpful, despite initial skepticism of manyprofessionals. As a result, it wasthought that the use of teachers'aides in regular elementaryschool classrooms might be attempted as a meansof reducing the effectiveclass size and improving the learning experiencesprovided to the child. Reports of consultants favoredthe training period providedfor teachers prior to the openingof Head Start ChildDevelopment Centers. In many cases theyattributed the successfulorientation of particular programs towardmodern child development conceptsdirectly to the teacher training programs. In contrast,when teachers had notattended the training sessions, consultants werecritical of what they termed"ignorance of Head observations on staff Start Program goils.! A summaryof consultants' t training and use is shownin Exhibit III-16. Six specific training programsuggestions resultedfrom a consultants' workshop conference heldAugust 30-31, 1965.They are Programs should. be morecomprehensive. They shouldin- volve people at all importantstaff levels--administrators,

111-40 EXHIBIT III-16STAFF. AND WORKERS - CONSULTANTS' COMMENTS

Consultant's Percentage Checklist of .(1)Number Question Responses of Number Yes No Responses 2A. Formal training arranged for paid professionals 85.6 8.8 917 2B. Formal training arranged for volunteers 27.8 54.6 864 3A. Professional staff uses non- professionals well 80.8 3.2 890 3B. Professional staff uses volunteers well 77.7 6.6 781 4A. Non-professionals feel they are used well 90.9 0.8 854 4B. Volunteers feel they are used well 85.8 4.3 735 80. Staff has achieved adequate working relations among professional members 53.6 17.9 899 8P. Staff has achieved adequate working relations among non-professional members 90.8 2.8 889

Note: (1) Remainder of responses reported as"partial." '1 b teachers, teachers' aides,professionals, etc. A "team" approach should be stressed. Allteachers, regardless of past experience, need sometraining in Head Start child development concepts. Training curricula shouldplace more stress on parent in- volvement, regional problems,Child Development Center structure, staff selectionmethods, and urban versus rural approaches. Areas withoutkindergartens should put more emphasis on fundamentals. Selection of universitiesshould be improved. Some good ones wereoverlooked; others with limitedcapabilities were selected. Only one university in eachstate should make assignments. Training centers shouldprovide evaluation follow-upsessions for trainees. Demonstration schools would be ahelpful training device if the children involvedhave backgrounds similar toHead Start children. Finally, an efforwas made todetermine whether differences in effectiveness, as measured by PPVT meanD-scores, could be observed between CDC's. A sampleof CDC's was drawn atrandom from the CDC's providing the sample of matchedPPVT scores, and an analysisof variance was performed.The result was that theF-ratio was not significant.Thus, although it was not possible toidentify statistically differentcenters by this method, certaincharacteristics of the means andvariances were of interest, especially for theirimplications for future experimentsand tests. The data, analyses, andresults are discussed morefully in Appendix F. IV.RESULTS

A. Introduction In preceding sections,characteristics of the communities, chil- dren, parents, and workersaffected by the Summer 1965 ProjectHead Start were described. Alsodiscussed were center programs,which were intended toinvolve not only the participantsbut also the commu- nities.The results of these programsand an evaluation of their impact on the communities,children, parents, and workers arepresented in this section. The impact of Head Start onparticipating communities will be discussed in terms of (1) the programinvolvement of various community organizations, and (2) communityplans for programs resulting from Head Start.Information on these topics will give anindication of corn- munity concern stimulatedby Head Start. The impact of Head Start on the560,000 children is, of course, central to an evaluation of theproject. The project is evaluatedin terms of: (1) the successof the medical and dental programin locating illnesses and defects andreferring them for treatment;(2) Head Start's effect on the child's cognitivedevelopment; and (3) the extent towhich Head Start affected the child'spsychological, emotional, andsocial needs. Parental participation wasencouraged in all phases of the pro- gram. The extentof parental participation andits impact on the parents are analyzedhere. Finally, Head Startimpact is discussed in terms of the benefits of the workers'participation and their enthusiasmfor the program. B. Impact on Communities Basic to any communityaction program such as HeadStart is the coordination. ofcommunity resources to fightpoverty.In most communities served bythe 1965 Head Start program,belief in the concept impelled awide variety of organizationsand individuals with special talents to donatetheir services. As statedin a special study of Greene County,Ohio,"... the Head Start program wasdesigned to serve as astarting point for thedevelopment of a broadly-based, coordinated programembracing all of the majorservice systems and extending to all of themajor concentrationsof poverty in the county..." (Reference 2i). The key question, then,is whether the1965 Head Start program was a"community program."Available information hasbeen organized into the followinggroupings to answer thisquestion: The extent of communityorganizational participation. The community'scontinuation of Head Startand/or related programs. Total community impactwith regard to: (1) changesin par- ticipating agencies; (2)effect on school administratorsand board members; (3) roleof communications media;(4) fam- ily involvement incommunities; (5) impact ofHead Start on future county planning; and(6) measures of interagency co- operation and communication. Before these subjects canbe discussed, however,the source material for the first twoitems must be described.Information on the 1 extent of communityorganizational Participation comesfrom the obser- vations of 154 educationalconsultants and from astudy of 1965 Head Start grantees and sponsors.The analysis of thecommunity's con- tinuation of Head Start isbased upon theconsultants' observations. The measure of totalcommunity impact is 'a casestudy of a particular community. The 154 educationalconsultants visited morethan 1,000 Head Start projects. Some werevisited befcire classesactually opened; others were visited after only afew weeks of operation; and a few consultants could not call upon centersuntil after classes had formally closed. Because of these differencesin time at which centers were visited, the responses do not necessarilydescribe the programs as they were finally implemented.Organizational participation, for example, could have been more orless than that anticipated early in the program.Plans for Head Start continuationcould have been formu- lated or abandoned after theconrtultants' visits.The consultants' ob- servations, then, do not necessarilyreflect the full scope of community involvement in Head Start. Thegeographical distribution of sampled centers approximated thatof all 1965 Head Start centers. The study of grantees and sponsors wasdone by categorizing and totaling the organizationslisted in the Approved Grants forFiscal Year 1965. The three categories selected were(1) community organizations, (2) public schools, and (3) other.No additional categories werechosen, for in many cases the organizationaltitle listed in the above-mentioned volume did not permit an easycategorization. Even with thesethree simple categories, the orderingof organizations is somewhatarbitrary. "Community organizations," inthis analysis, include not only commu- nity action agencies but alsocitizens' clubs and other groupsprimarily devoted to the improvementof the community. Finally, thisstudy is only of grantees and sponsorsand not of other organizationswhich, short of sponsorship, mayhave provided substantial assistance. 1. Involvement of Community Organsanizations One measure of Head Start'seffect on the community is the extent to which variouselements in the communityparticipated in the program. Aneffective Head Start programdemanded assistance not only from public schools andcommunity action agencies, butalso from medical and dental societies,welfare agencies, andservice-oriented organizations. The Head Startexperience, despite the shorttime for project preparation, was a goodinitial test of the community'sability to organize its resources. 0 IV -3 Information exists only on the br6adth oforganizational partic- ipation.Unfortunately, the number ofcommunity-oriented organizations in a community cannotbe compared with the number thatactually partic- ipated.It is misleading to state, forexample, that there was nomedi- cal society participating inthe program. in those cases, where, infact, there was no medical societyin the community. In addition, we do not know the number of organizations orcommittees which emerged from Head Start.This would be another excellentmeasure of community impact. Finally, theavailable data does not indicate thenumber al Head Start projects whichreceived assistance from a greatvariety and number of organizations. The public schools dominatedthe 1965 Head Start program.As shown in Exhibit N-1, Boardsof Education and public school systems were over60 percent of the project granteesand almost three-fourths of the sponsors. This isfurther supported by the observationsof the educational consultants (seeExhibit IV-2). They indicated thatalmost 95 percent of the Head Start programshad some kind of school assist- ance.In many instances thepublic schools were both grantee and sponsor. Asmight be expected, this wasparticularly true in small communities with fewerorganizations. In each region thepublic schools accounted for more sponsorsthan grantees. A variety of typesof or- ganizations applied for HeadStart grants, but many of thesedeferred to the public schoolsin the operation of the program. Nonschool organizations workingtoward the improvementof the community received one-fourthof the 1965 grants. The percentage was highest inthe Northeast (36 percent ofthe regional total) and lowest in the Southwest(11 percent).Nationally, only 8 percentfinally sponsored programs. Although the percentage of otherorganizations as grantees(14 percent) or sponsors (19percent) was relatively small,they included a wide varietyof types: private schools,churches, universities and colleges, local government units,health and welfareagencies, many school-related organizations, andother public and private organiza- tions. Grants to these groups weregreatest in the Southeastand Far

IV -4 vs= 02211 fa2) IMEE1 of els r-1 Ir1. reis EXHIBIT IV-1 HEAD START GRANTEES 'I= AND SPONSORS BY CATEGORY 0 Grantees (N = 2384)(1) National Northeast Atlantic Middle Southeast Percent Midwest Southwest West Far West OtherPublicCommunity schools organizations 256114 365311 612811 232453 335710 721117 692011 522622 SponsorsCommunity (N = organizations 2474)(1) 8 12 8 11 7 6 5 7 OtherPublicNote: schools (1) "N's" may vary slightly from actual because 7319 5721 7517 of hand tabulation. 2168 7815 7915 8312 7023 EXHIBITAssistance IV-2 from: ORGANIZATIONS PROVIDING ASSISTANCE BY REGION National North-east AtlanticMiddle eastSouth- westMid- (1) westSouth- West FarWest PublicSchools Health PercentNumberNumber(2) Yes 88.089294.7888 86.811495.8118 93.795.1190187 89.612589.7126 82.210794.3106 84.818495.6180 86.37397.272 89.99996.0 PublicHospitals Welfare or Clinics PercentNumber Yes 59.281082.8882 84.478.4116109 39.615987.6186 57.080.3122114 61.584.3108104 60.477.3176164 45.87283.874 57.78788.0100 0' MedicalDental Society PercentPercentNumberNumber Yes Yes 43.4790787 9728.49532.0 44.2156 36.210539.1110 45.098100 50.0170168 40.038.670 51.19052.2 Nursing Society PercentNumber Yes Yes 27.472843.5 26.192 42.928.1146 28.196 Ii 30.19350.0 25.2151 20.070 8035.0 EconomistsDieticiansOptometrists or Home PercentNumber Yes 78074432.9 9119.493 34.7150160 25.7109101 9410239.4 16615535.5 7127.968 44.08481 Notes:Others (1) Percentage of "yes" responses to selectedPercentNumber questionsYes from consultants(2) checklist, Total number of responses (yes and no). 60.142453.5 4850.038.5 74.48675.0 52.85356.9 45,155.060 61.99753.0 40.72750.7 66.05338.3 West. These organizationssponsored proportionately more programs in those two regionsand in the Northeast. When the participationof private schools,universities and colleges, and school-relatedorganizations is added to that ofthe public schools, it appears thateducational organizationshad almost two-thirds of the grants and were78 percent of the sponsors.Colleges and uni- versities in every regionsponsored programs.School-related organi- zations with programsincluded parent-teacherorganizations, school councils, teacher associations,and administrator groups. Program sponsorshipbychurches and other parochial institutions was a verysmall percentage ofthe total (less than 1percent).It was heaviest in the MiddleAtlantic region, principallythe District of Colum- bia. Public and privateorganization participation,both as grantee and sponsor,included service clubs,sororities, unions, humanrights groups,recreation councils, women'sclubs, and youth commissions. More such groupsparticipated in the Southeastthan elsewhere. Health and welfare interests wererepresented by such units as settlement houses, guidanceclinics, mental health groups,societies for crippled and retardedchildren, and welfare agencies. Thus, although thegrants and sponsorshipscentered in thi public schools, a wide varietyof other organizationsassumed direct respon- sibility for the successof Head Start programs.The breadth of com- munity organizationalsponsorship was greater, of course,in larger communities where manysuch groups are inexistence. Many more organizationsprovided assistance toindividual proj- ects while not assumingdirect sponsorship.Information on the extent of this assistance comestotally from the observationsof the educational consultants. The specificquestion given to theconsultants was: "Are any of thefollowing organizationsproviding assistance tothe program: schools, public health,public welfare, hospitals orclinics, medical society, dental society,nursing society,optometrists, dieticians or home economists, orothers?" It is assumedthat the criterion for an

IV-7 . i

affirmative answer to this question was (1) thatthere was some type of formal commitment from theorganization listed, or (2) that the services were donated. The only possible responses were"yes" or "no." The number of responses varied,depending on the organization identified. As indicated in Exhibit IV-2, the greatestnumber of responses concernedassistance from schools and public health and welfare agencies.It is possible that the smaller number of responses to other organizationalassistance was caused by the omission of a. "don't know" response. While a greater percentage of programsreceived assistance from schools, both public and private,than from any other community or- ganization, assistance from publichealth and welfare agencies was also strong--88 and 83 percent,respectively. The consultants observed thatthere was considerably less orga- nizational assistance fromhospitals or clinics; medical, dental,and nursing societies; optometrists;and dieticians or home economists. The responses to thesequestions, however, must be interpreted care- fully. For example, in manycommunities assistance was provided by individual doctors, dentists, nurses,and optometrists, but not by their professional societies. Thereis evidence that some consultants. re- sponded affirmatively toparticipation by the societies if theprofess sionals themselvesassisted.In many of the smaller communities,of course, thesesocieties do not exist. A sampling of the consultants'narrative observations indicates that the programs variedgreatly in terms of communityorganizational participation. At the positiveend of the spectrum is a programsuch as that inLowell, Massachusetts, wherecommunity involvement was extensive. For example,social clubs and businesses provided money for picnics and trips;local merchants donated foodand equipment for play; service clubs gaveman-hours so that the childrenwould become acquainted with more men;the recreation departmentencouraged the free use of its recreationpark and health camp withwading pools and playgrounds; and the VisitingNurses Associationdonated services.

IV-8 Some consultants felt that afew prograrhs wculd have received more community assistance thanthey did if the Head Start directors had been more forceful andimaginative. At the other end of thespectrum are those programs where com- munity participation wasnegligible or nonexistent. Some consultants were criticalof the lack of assistance from theprofessional societies and/or their members. A fewcommunities had assumed that the various elements of the medicalcommunity would willingly volunteer. A consultant reported, forexample, that in one community he visited the public health and welfareagehcies and the nursing, medical, and dental societies donated veryfew services.In other communities the lack of organizationalinvolvement was attributed to poor planning effort by the sponsor. To the extent that measuresof organizational involvement are indicative of the impact ofHead Start on the communities served, the above information suggestsneither extensive community particoation nor completeneglect.In 95 percent of the projects a schoolsystem either sponsored or assistedthe program. The educational consultants also noted strong support frompublic health and welfare agencies.It would seem., however, thatthe true measure of community impact is the extent 40 which otherorganizations participated, for theygenerally had not previously had as active aninterest in the development of the deprived child. While many suchorganizations in many communities did favorably respond to theHead Start challenge, there areindications that in some areas theirparticipation was less than had beenanticipated. In these places where the program wasnot broadly-based, amuch greater burden was placed onthose units traditionally chargedwith the responsibility for assisting the deprivedchild. 2. Community Plans for FurtherPrograms Another measure of Head Start's impact onthe community served is the extent to whichelements of the community had planned to expand the program. Such expansionplans might include full-year Child Development Centers, follow-throughin the elementary schools,

Iv -9 or fall programs. Theeducational consultants wereasked to indicate whether, at the time of their visits,such programs werebeing planned or contemplated.The number of responses to mostof these questions was rather small(involving perhaps 70 percentof the centers visited). Respondents could answer only"yes" or "no," and it ispossible that when consultants did notknow or were not sure whetherplans were being made, they did not answer(see Exhibit IV-3). Over half of the 657respondents indicated that thecommunities visited planned to havefull-year Child DevelopmentCenters. The per- centage was highest inthe Far West (almost 70percent), and lowest in the West. Manyconsultants stated in theirnarrative reports that while full-year centers were bothneeded and desired, their actualoperation would depend on a varietyof factors. The availability offunds was most frequentlymentioned, followed by theavailability of adequate space, transportation,and supervisory personnel. Arelated question concerned the availability ofadequate professional staff to manthe center.In each region, more than one-halfthe respondents answered affirmatively. There were, however,almost 100 fewer responses to _. this question than to the previous one,suggesting perhaps that many consultants were not informed on this matter. With regard to Head Startfollow-through efforts in the elementary schools, the educational consultants wereasked two questions: (1) Is anything being done to adapt firstgrade or kindergarten programs to child development concepts? and(2) Have plar_s been made to transmit records to the school system?(See Exhibit IV-4. )The total number of responses to the first question wassmall, and several respondents replied that the question was not clear.The interpretation of child development concepts" caused the mostdifficulty. A review of the consultants' narrative comments suggeststhat some replies were based upon the consultants' ownunderstanding of child development concepts, while others assu,ned that the HeadStart program included such concepts by definition. A few respondentsanswered the question by describing the program in terms of childdevelopment concepts, but they did not

IV-I0 en cos ems fess ma 11223 1=11 . Inel 11111 erAl er, PM r."1 rimi r-, 43 -0 fors fe--113 i EXHIBIT IV-3 PLANS FOR FULL-YEAR. CHILD DEVELOPMENT National eastNorth- AtlanticMiddle east CENTER") westMid- South-west West FarWest yearPlans center for fun- PercentNumber(2) 51.4657 40.065 55.9136 51.693 8939.3 57.7137 6134.4 69.776 fessionalAdequateavailable? staffpro- Number 508 42 115 79 77 . 132 41 82 Notes: Percent (2)(1) TotalPercentage number of of"yell" responses responses (yes toand selected no). questions from consultants' 61.7 76.2 57.4 60.8 64.9 58.3 checklist. 58.5 65.9 EXHIBIT0 I11-4 PLANS FOR FOLLOW-THROUGH IN ELEMENTARY National eastNorth- AtlanticMiddle eastSouth- SCHOOLS(1)westMid-. westSouth- West WestFar tokindergartenconcepts?Is childadapt anything development first being programsgrade done or 40 109 76 63 121 43 76 Have plans been made PercentNumber(2) . 56.5534 75.0 42.2 67.1 54.0 61.2 46.5 61.8 tothe transmit school recordssystem? to Number . 870 111 184 120 110 169 78 97 Notes: Percent (2)(1) TotalPercentage number of of"yes" responses responses (yes to and selected no). questions from consultants' checklist. 92.0 84.7 92.4 90.0 91.8 95.3 96.2 92.8 mention whether they wouldbe applied to elementary schoolsituations. The "checklist" responses tothe question, then, must be interpreted carefully. Selected narrative responses, onthe other hand, provide useful information as to how such conceptsmight be adapted and what problems might be connected withthe adaptation.In Knoxville, Tennessee, for example, the consultantreported that the city school system had set up a teamof program evaluators,including an educator, a psychologist, a welfareworker, a health and medicalspecialist, and a specialist in child development. This teamwas to submit its recommendations to the. city school system, tobe used in planning and adaptingthe regular first grade program. Afew consultants reported that first gradeand kindergarten teachers would visitthe Head Start Centers toobserve, and that discussion meetingswould be held on the implicationsof the Head Start approach in theelementary schools. Problems connected with theadaptation of child development concepts frequently concernedteachers who attempted to apply the normal first grade andkindergarten concepts to the HeadStart situ.- ation.This is supported by a generalfeeling among consultants that neither Head Start nor publicschool teachers had a commanding grasp of child development concepts.Some consultants indicated that the effective use of child development conceptsin the public schools would depend on the size of classes.While Head Start classes were small, kindergarten and first grade classes werelikely to be large.If the adaptation of child development conceptsto regular school programs is to be an objective of HeadStart, then communities needguidance as to how thismight be accomplished. Over 90 percent of therespondents stated that plans hadalready been made to transmit HeadStart records to the school system,although some consultantsstated that they did not knowwhat plans the school sys- tem had made to usethem.In at least one case a consultantsaid that the Head Start teachers would meetwith the children's new school teachers to discuss the individualrecords.

IV-13 The existence of plans for fall programs for Head Start children is a good measure of the community's acceptance of the program. The consultants were asked whether additional programs, supplementary educational programs, medical services, social services, or parent education were planned in the fall for Head Start children. As shown in Exhibit IV-5, it appears that many programs were planned in all activities except additional programs for Head Start children.1However, in reviewing the narrative comments, it is observed that many consul- tants interpreted the question to mean the availability of programs rather than plans for their beginning.Therefore, it is probable that the re- sponses are, for the most part, indicative of the availability of such programs and services to all persons in the community. The consul- tants were not asked whether the community planned any special effort to encourage Head Start participants to take advantage of these programs and services. The available information on coin.munity plans for further pro- grams is not sufficient to permit any firm conclusions. A review of the checklist tabulations and narratives suggests, however, that in expanding the Head Start concepts most communities exploited its educational rather than its community-involvement potential. 3. Case Studof a Community A number of researchers and others have provided reports on the organizational, administrative, and operational aspectsof Head Start programs at the local level. Various elements ofinformation from them have been used where possible throughout this report. Since evi- dence about the impact of Head Start on communities is related to changes in the organization and operations of agencies and informal groups and to the development and implementation of plansand pro- grams, it is necessary to examine these communityelements in detail to evaluate effects,

It is assumed that "additional programs" means those similar to Head Start 1965.

1V-14 EXHIBIT IV-5 PLANS FOR FALL PROGRAMS(1) North- Middle South- Mid- South- Far HeadAdditional Start Childr'en Programs for Number (2) National 867 east 110 Atlantic 185 east120 west 110 west 177 West 71 West 94 MedicaltionalSupplementary Program Services Educa- PercentNumberPercent 53.86619.6 41.67917.3 51.77.0143 57.1986.7 40.2-8213.6 63.31287.9 49.1539.9 67.9788.5 ParentSocial ServicesEducation PercentNumber 72.767650.5643 71.37141.565 83.313246.4125 64.29552.599 60.09037.383 78.614057.1133 68.36343.360 84.07570.578 Notes: PercentNumber (1)(2) Percentage Total number of "yes" of responses responses (yes to and selected no). questions from 58.2639 52.267 57.6132 57.191 consultants' checklist.47.187 65.4127 48.360 74.775 Space does not permit presentation of all the available. information. However, one 'report, in which a county in a midwestern state wasstud- ied in depth, is summarized here.While it will not cover all the various problems and indications of impact found throughout the country,it does provide much valuable information for planners. Sinceit is not the inten- tion here to single out a given community, the namesof the community and its towns have been changed. Many elements ofthe study are not unique to geographical location or otherspecific factors of identity. The study has been summarized in three parts:(1) a description of Omega County social services prior to Head Startand the involvement of the poor in them, (2) a brief description co:: the originand organization of the program for over 400 children, and (3) the impactof Head Start on various elements ofthe community, particularly the welfare agencies, the poor, the school system, and the countyleadership structure. In some ways Omega County may not berepresentative of the other counties in the 1965 program.It has the highest per capita income in the state.It was mentioned earlier in this subsection thatfrom available sponsored by evidence it appears that the majority of programs were 1 school systems. However, the sponsor of theOmega County program was a social welfareorganization. a. Omega County Social Services Omega County has the usual social service agencies: health department, welfare department, childwelfare board, Red Cross, two YMCA's, and the school system.Several years ago some 50 organi- zations formed a loose umbrella agency, the OmegaCounty Council on Community Services. TheinvestigatOr states that because of its loose organizational structur, ztud the absence ofbureaucratic characteristics, it has been effective in 1:,-.:21dlingwelfare problems. More than 100 mothers from the over 300 HeadStart families were interviewed to determine the extent of their communityinvolvement and the extent to which they were servedby the social service agencies. The mothers were given a listof 12 Omega County agencies and asked

IV-16 whether they had ever heard of them before and whether they had pre- viously had some contact with the agencies. While the majority of respondents had heard of 11 of the 12 agencies, the majority had had some contact with only 2 of the12:the PTA (61 percent) and the Health Department (53 percent). The investigator concludes that if the rela- tively involved Head Start mothers are a reliable index of participation, n... the social service facilities avaiiable in Omega County areutilized by a minority of the poor ... The mothers were asked whether they knew of any organizations working to solve the problems of'the poor. Only26 percent of the respondents could mention one such organization and, significantly, only three respondents mentioned a governmentalbody as one of these organizations.Fifty-nine percent said they would be glad to join such work, while 20 percent were negative on this point.Finally, asked whether the people in power were interested in solving theproblems of the poor, 45 percent said they didn't know, 33 percent said yes,and 23 percent said no. It would appear, then, that Omega County has awell-functioning social service structure which has not, however,been entirely success- ful in reaching the poor. b. Head Start Organization Many people in Omega County felt that the successof the summer program was due to effectiveleadership and organization. When asked why Omega County was different fromthe surrounding counties which had no program, many respondentsstressed the leader- ship in their county (see organization chart,Exhibit IV-6). After Omega County entered a 6-county community.action agency, the Omega County Council on CommunityServices voted to establish a 20-member Economic Opportunity SteeringCommittee, two members of which were to be from the low-incomeneighborhoods. The.Steering Committee then set up a 32-member Head StartCommittee, composed of eight people from the school systems,four from the health professions,

IV-17 Community Action Program COMMUNITY WELFARE COUNCIL SUPPORTIVE COUNCIL ON PREVENTIVE EFFORT Epsilon Metropolitan Concerned with coordinating community develop. Area ment of Epsilon Metropolitan Area (Six counties)

COUNCIL ON COMMUNITY SERVICES 4$ Organisations in Omega County .

OMEGA COUNTY ECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY STEERING COMMITTEE 20 members, of which 10 are connected r with organizations represented in the Council on Community Services'

HEAD START COMMITTEE 32 members, of which 3 are on the OCEOSC

1 DIRECTOR OF OPERATION HEAD START

THIRTEEN LOCAL COMMITTEES Approximately 100 members SOCIAL SERVICE COORDINATOR

NURSE

VOLUNTEER COORDINATORS (1$) FOURTEEN CENTERS rrj.. 334 Families I 424 Children 4°4 29 Teachers 1 2$ Teachers' Aides VOLUNTEERS 280 used, 90 more not called on [71 I I FOLLOW-UP COMMITTEES ...1 1...... 111

EXHIBIT IV-6ORGANIZATION OF OPERATIONHEAD START, OMEGA COUNTY IV-1 8 four from Iota State University, four from private social service agen- cies, four from low-im.ime neighborhoods, and eightfrom various professions. Thirteen local Head Start committees, with a totalmembership of 100, were organized, usually throughthe school superintendents. The responsibilities of these committeesincluded identifying and en- rolling participants, acting as a sourcefor volunteers, and providing $50 to buy a food container. While many of the committees were a source of support, there wereproblems. Some committees did not cooperate; one had too many school peoplewho were not available during the summer; and another had a tendency tooverstructure its activities. From the distribution of membership onthese committees, it appears that there wasrepresentation from a broad segment of the social service population. The Council onCommunity Services had already been working for the disadvantaged,thus, the organizational channel already existed. Community representation in the planning,however, may not have been complete. Neither the traditionalsocial service agencies nor school district administrators wereheavily involved in the planning of the 1965 program. Both groups feltthat Head Start was organized by "outsiders." Some welfare personnel arguedfor a social service pro- fessionally organized, which emphasizedthe contributions of private citizens. The school administratorsof local districts felt left out It is noted that most of the localdistricts were not represented on the central committee; the administrators werepresented with a developed program through the13 local committees. Most of the schooladmin- istrators felt that if the program were tocontinue, a school-related group should perhapslead it.With regard to the 1965 effort,however, the investigator concluded that"professional procedures in mostof the agencies did not encourage orfacilitate extensive staff involvement in a voluntary communityenterprise." Organizationally, the 1965 Head Start programin Omega County was based on aperson-oriented approach. Things weredone rapidly

IV-19 in large part because of the personal relationships of the members of the Council on CoMmunity Services.Centrally planned and organized, it involved key elements of the local communities too late in the cycle. The researcher feels that Head Start must now be transferred to an institutional context unless preschool programs are to be divorced from the school systems entirely. c. Community In evaluating the impact of Head Start on the commu- nity, the researcher interviewed representatives of welfare agencies and the various school systems to determine their assessment of the 1965 program, the extent of their involvement, whether they would participate in future programs, and whether plans had been made to .develop programs or activities as a result of Head Start. In general, the people interviewed considered the 1965 program to be worthwhile and believed that it should be continued. The degree of enthusiasm depended somewhat upon the individual's or the agency's extent of involvement. For example, 4 of 14 school principals inter- viewed had been enthusiastic about the program from the beginning; these four principals and one other had participated actively.Negative attitudes, which were in the minority, emphasized hasty planning; little involvement of the traditional institutions in the planning, the money spent in relation to local money available for such purposes, and the Federal Government's involvement in the program. Significantly, no one mentioned that Head Start had not benefited thechildren. As stated above, the participation of the traditional Omega County social institutions was minimal.Staff members from 12 child-oriented social service agencies were interviewed. The majority of the tradi- tional welfare and service units were not involved in the planning, al- though they did provide some material or service. Most of them had started no additional services or programs as a result of Head Start, although all but one stated that they would participate in futureprograms. Among the 12 there were 2 relatively new agencies--the Gamma Human Relations Commission and the Alpha Area Council. The former

IV -20 was expressly formedfor Head Start, and for thelatter, a voluntary citizens' group, Head Start wasthe major project to date. While most social serviceagencies were not .involved in planning, health personnel wereintimately involved. TheHealth Department was represented on the Council onCommunity Services and the Economic Opportunity SteeringCommittee. The public health nurses,tkils closest link between the parentsof Head Start children and thesocial welfare 'agen- cies, all participated in theplanning. Inasmuch as thecooperation of the Medical Society wascrucial, members of the society werecontacted and involved in the planning.Physicians and dentists gave their coop- eration. The Omega County Superintendentof Schools participatedactively in the program, and it wasthrough him that the cooperationof the local school districts was obtained.It was stated above that 5of 14 school principals interviewed weresubstantially involved in HeadStart, and 9 had done at least someplanning. All of the principalswanted their schools and staffs to be involvedin future Head Startefforts; 4 said that the involvement shouldbe extensive. In the local communities,only one of the representativesof seven Boards ofEducation said that he hadbeen involved in the planning. In most of the districts thesuperintendents were passive.The investi- gator stated that "boththe superintendents andthe board members expressed an implicit attitudeof resentment that they were not more extensively involved in theplanning and execution of theprogram." Finally, although none of theboard members wanted toinvolve their schools and staff extensivelyin future programs, 75 percentof the superintendents said that theywould. The Omega County politicalleaders were supportive butinactive. The researcher states thatthere is no hostility betweenthe county polit- ical and social service structures,and that the poverty program wasnot considered a threat. Thepolitical leaders did not intervene,and the programdevelopers "... deliberately did notask for the endorsement orparticipation'of political officials or bodies. u.

-21 Because of Omega County's loose,"nonbureaucratic" service structure, the investigator feelsthat new leaders were uncovered in the program and that the existingleadership incorporated these elements into the structure with littlestrain.It is observed, however, thatfew disadvantaged personsparticipated in the planning; when invited,they participated in the operationof the program. The leadershipstructure, then, did not incorporate manyindividuals from the "poverty sector." It was reported thatthe reason for this might bethat, whereas the county social serviceleaders are oriented to county-wideproblems, the poor relate only totheir individual neighborhoods.The author states that the disadvantagedlack extra-county experiencesand cannot participate locally because of thepolitical dominance ofconservative elements.It will be remembered thatin the planning of theeffort, the emphasis was on central countyagencies rather than localunits. The problem of involvement ofthe various elements ofthe com- munity remains .a crucial onein Head Start.In Omega County, the traditional institutions argued thatHead Start suffered from poorplanning and that they, if invited,could be of valuable assistancein the early stages. A review of theeducational consultants' narrativecomments on other programs throughoutthe country suggests thatin some communities the traditional institutions(i.e., school systems) did notsolicit the participation of the lessstructured elements of thecommunity. There is little evidence aboutthe involvement of thedisadvantaged, although the investigator says thatthe poor in Omega County wereworkers, not planners. One measure of the impactof Head Start on a communityis the extent to which new orexpanded programs resultfrom the experience. These new programs mustemerge from the planning of variouselementw of the community. In thiscounty it would appear thatthe extent to which these groups were planningsuch efforts was related tothe extent of their involvement in the summer program. Essentially, Omega County hadthree types of groups whichmight have been stimulated enoughby Head Start to expand orbegin programs.

Iv-22 One of these, the least traditional, was the Economic Opportunity Steering Committee. Head Start was its only major project, although others are in the planning stages. The social service agencies (the second type) have been stimu- lated, it would seem, to the extent of their 1965 involvement. Of all the social service agencies, the Health Department was most involved. Since the close of the program, the researcher states, the Health De- partment has been considering applying for a grant to help meet some of the medical and dental needs uncovered by Head Start. The Depart- ment has also received a zzrant to undertake a program of homemaker training and services. Some of the active Head Start mothers will be enco,Nraged to apply. Although the Beta Township Welfare Association's initial reaction was negative, it did participate inthe provision of clothing for some children and in identifying Head Start eligibles. The researcher feels that the impact of Head Start had an influence on one newprogram--the establishment of a neighborhood center. As a staff member said, "When Head Start came along, they felt that if one can do this, let's see how far we can go." The Alpha YMCA was planning to offer selected Head Start parents a sponsored membership. The Gamma HumanRe- lations Commission was planning a summer day camp to involve Head Start children, and the Alpha Area Council was planning aday-care center. From the child's standpoint, the plans of the school systemfor future programs are probably most important.It is possible- that the limited involvement of school personnel in the planning stages,while not harming the success of the summer program,engzndeied an apparent lack of enthusiasm for continuing programsin the schools. Perhaps the greatest positive effects on the school administrators were attitudinal.Several indications of willingness to cooperate were noted. For example, after the program was over,administrators, from every district met to discussfuture county -wide cooperation on pro- grams funded by the Federal Government.The Head Start director

IV-23 said that it was the first time ir. his 34years in Omega County that all the educational agencies had been together underone roof. A second attitudinal change was observed by the Head Start director. He saw a willingness on the part of the County Board of Education to expand and initiate kindergarten programs in county schools. Feeling that preschool education will be a part of public school sys- tem by 1975, the director stated that Head Start had an impact on the Boards of Education in preparing them for the inevitable. Most of the principals interviewed supported the introduction of new programs designed to meet the needs of disadvantaged children, but they felt that they should be expansions of existing programsor new programs and should not be specifically directed towards the disadvan- taged child. Every responding Head Start teacher reported beneficial and positive gains in teacher proficiency, with 65 percent saying that their methods or curriculum had changed. The investigator feels, however, that in most cases the changes were mainly attitudinal. He assumed that if the communication among Head Start and non-Head Start teachers were extensive following Head Start, and if the non-Head Start teachers adapted or adopted some of the program's unique features, then the impact upon the school systems would be substantial. While it is too early to measure the full impact of Head Start on teachers and curric- ulum, the study suggests that the impact was minimal. Fifty-two percent of the non-Head Start teachers reported at least one conversation with a Head Start instructor on the program. All but one of the Head Start teachers said that they had had discussions; 7 out of 10 said that they had talked with six or more persons; 67 percent said that the other teachers were enthusiastic; and 63 percent said that they had discussed the program with their principals. While 12 of the 14 principals interviewed said that they had talked with one or more teachers about Head Start, only 3 of them said it had been discussed at teachers' meetings, and 6 mentioned talking about it at meetings of principals and superintendents.

IV-24 There is no indication that some of the more significant aspects of the Head Start program, such as more extensive field trips, the use of teacher's aides, or health education, had been begun in the schools. Whether the state restricts curricular innovations is not known. Finally, there were certain goalsof the 1965 Omega County pro- gram which emphasizedthe ongoing aspects of Head Start, but which were not met.It had been stated, for example, that parental- neighborhood involvement would be maximized.Plans included the establishment of a family life education program.This was quietly deemphasized. The Head Start director said,"We involved the families only incidentally and there has been alittle follow-up, but a very little, since that time in terms of the families."With so little time, Head Start was geared primarily to the children. It was stated in the county's proposalthat public health nurses' would work with families and appropriatecommunity resources to cor- rect defects discovered. TheHead Start families interpreted this to mean that there wouldbe an extensive medical follow-up program. There was, however, little follow-up. It was also stated that the Coordinator ofSocial Services would be responsible for referrals to community resourcesand for the preparation of records for sharing with the local publichealth nurse, the Welfare De- partment case worker, and thelocal school staff.These latter groups were to continue thelong-run relationships with the children andtheir families. At the end of Head Start, a 2-dayconference was planned in which the county agencies were to review the progressof the children and the involvement of their families andplan for the continuity of the prOgram. This goal proved to be toct ambitious.Since there was a general antipathy towards the forms, therecords collected were inade- quate. Some coordinates wereinadequately trained.In many cases the records were not made available tothe agencies that needed them. The conference was not held, and agencypersonnel indicated little or no post-HeadStart contact with children in the program ortheir families.

IV-25 . The investigator adds that he has sti essed someof the negative aspects of the program only to suggest areasfor improved program planning.Overall, he feels that the Head Start programin Omega County was an outstanding success. In conclusion, it seems safe to say that theprogram's success was due in large part toits leadership.It was a person-oriented approach, and the social service structure inthe county permitted it to function with ease. The schooladministrators admitted that, in the time frame, they could not have plannedand operated such a program. These administrators, however, saw HeadStart's most positive benefit as the adjustmentof the child to the school and peer situation.It will be remembered that the HeadStart director observed that the county school administrators, as a result ofHead Start, may be willing to begin and expand kindergarten programs.The investigator feels that if the goal is primarily one of schoolreadiness, then future Head Start programs must passfrom individual to institutionalleadership, and a school-related group maybe the best solution. Head Start in Omega County generallyreceived a passively - positive endorsement from the traditionalcommunity institutions. While certain important changes inoperation or behavior were noted, these institutions did not participateactively in the planning, and they have not begun or expanded many programs ar aresult of the Head Start experience. There are a numberof reasons why Z.lzge_iy have not. It is possible, for example, thattheir lack of involvementcontributed to their relative lack ofenthusiasm for follow-on efforts.In addition, it appears that, in the time frameof the program, the follow-on plans were too ambitious, or wereat least thought to be. The investigator rightfully cautions,however, that it is too early to measure the true impact onthe community: It must be assumed that the growing awarenessof a com- munity to the needs of the disadvantaged,and the commit- ment to provide services to meetthose needs, is a cumulative process in which no one programis the major contributing factor.

IV-26 Each of the 1,492 counties participating in the 1965 program is, of course, different. The problems faced by Omega County and the lessons learned there undoubtedly are not universally applicable, nor were many problems occurring elsewhere encountered in Omega County. It is suggested, however, that some of the issues raised might be explored further to provide local communities with assistance or guid- ance on effective organization, planning, and communityinvolvement. C. Impacts on Children One of the most important aspectsof an evaluative summary of the Summer 1965 Head Start programmust concern the effect orimpact of the Head Start experience onthe children who attended the program. Several major areas of impact canbe identified: health; cognitive and general development; andpsychological, emotional, and social de- velopment. All areas areintimately related to school readiness;in fact some investigators usedinstruments specifically designed to evaluate growth in schoolreadiness per se. The various schoolreadi- ness studieshave been included in the discussionsof the specific areas to which they relate. In an attemp. :o shed light onHead Start's impact, OEO specified a number ofmeasurements to be made on HeadStart children early in the program (pretests) and atthe end of the program(posttests).In order to supplement the OEOmeasurements, the Head StartResearch and Evaluation Section, underthe direction of Dr. EdmundGordon, funded 43 independent studies toevaluate and/or describe Operation Head Start.Twenty-four final reports and 15preliminary ones have been received to date. Thefour remaining reports will beavailable shortly. This subsection includes allavailable appropriate data onHead Start's impact on participatingchildren and constitutes an assessment of the Summer 1965 program.(It should be noted that anyevaluation of Head Start undertakenimmediately following the program can,in one sense,only be considered"preliminary." Researchersappreciate the necessity forlongitudinal studies to follow the progressof Head Start childr n beyond theend of the program.) Before any meaningful assessmentof evaluation data can be undertaken, some mention mustbe made of the generalproblems in testing culturally disadvantagedchildren. Several of the manypossible sources ofvariance in test scoresmentioned by Cronbach(Reference 24) seem especially to apply toculturally disadvantaged children- - factors such as ntestwiseness,"ability to 'solve problems of

IV-28 the general type presented in theparticula test, self-confidence, health, and rapport with the tester(page 128). In "Guidelines for TestingMinority Group Children "' (Reference 45), several critical issues in testadministration and interpretation are discussed.One of the points emphasizedis that "the validity of [test] interpretation is stronglydependent upon an adequate understand- ing of the social and culturalbackground of the group question" (page 130). Frequently the testperformance of children from low socio- economic backgrounds maybe affected by their being "lessverbal, more fearfulof strangers, lessself-confident..., [and] less exposed to intellectuallysc,imulating materials inthe home..." (page 132). Pettigrew (Reference 85, page7), in discussing the factorof the tester's race, pointed outthat Negro children as young as2 years of age showedrestricted verbal responseswhen tested by a white person. It would seem that evenunder the best ofcircumstances, testing Head Start children would be adifficult task.But in addition to theproblems described above, there was someevidence that a furthercomplication lay in the feelings of theteachers.In an evaluation of the programby the Head Start staff(Worker's EvaluationChecklist), some teachers expressed great frustration overthe testing requirementsand many felt that too much time wasdiverted from teaching totesting. Many of-the 450 Head Start consultantsindicated in their commentsthat the burden of testing was oftenperceived by the teacher asbeing greater or less, depending on the amountof additional administrativeand clerical help available. One of the researchers,Dr. Stanley I. Berger(University Of Rhode Island), commented onthe poor conditionsfor testing and the fact that teachersoften misinterpreted the purposeof testing (Reference 5). Some teachersapproached the testinstruments with theapprehension that their results wouldbe attributed to theeffectiveness of their teach- ing; others believedthat the results might beused to describe the"poor" children as "inferior."It was also noted that:"Children were taken away fromplaying games and snacktime, which they wereenjoying in many cases,testing was done in spiteof the teacher's help,rather than with the teacher'shelp. " Berger's observations mayapply to other independentstudies as well as to thestandard scheduled Head Start testing. The numerous problemsthat normally arisein testing "minority groups," plus themechanics and schedulingof Head Start tests, may account in large partfor incomplete and unusabletest data and must be considered in the interpretationof all test results. 1. Health Probably the greatest impactof Head Start in thehealth area was feltby those children whohad never previouslyreceived corn,- plete medical and dentalevaluations. The medical/dental datafrom the 1-percent sampleindicate that, as a resultof the Head Startexaminations, a numberof referrals were made for vision,dental, arid speech andhearing problems (see Exhibit IV-7). However, exceptin the case of dentalproblems, the referral' rate was very lowaccording to the datatabulated. The report of the ChicagoPublic Schools Head Start program (Reference 1) indicated that36 percent of the 11,553children examined percent werereferred for further diagnosisand/or treatment. Thirty of the referralsinvolved children withvarying degrees of apparent "retardation that might impairnormal school progress;"60 percent in- volved children withaggressive tendencies; and10 percent involved children who were making a pooradjustment to school. The Chicago Head Startreport by Drs. Abramsand Spaeth. (Reference 1), which detailedthe Chicago Head Startmedical program, suggested that one impactof the medical program wasinherent in the fact that medical records wereinitiated for so manychildren, and that there was time duringthe examination forparents to ask andreceive by a - answers toquestions.(In Chicago allchildren were accompanied parent to an equippedclinical facility for themedical examination.) Another impact reportedby Abrams andSpaeth involved theidentifica- tion of medicalproblems, a prerequisitefor referral of suchproblems; apparently the thoroughnessof the Chicc.g6 medicalexaminations resulted in a very highdetection-of-defect rate. IV-30 ers) cnzt on era CM C22 e"33 er-73 r""3 1'9-'1 r rte- EXHIBIT IV-7 HEALTH REFERRALS(1) Percent 32G. Mat, Visual Screening - Referral Item Number Total White Negro 33,C. NoUnsatisfactory,UnsatisfactoryReferral referral for further referred diagnosis for retesting or treatment 38.8 2.93.4 .9 43.337.7 3.2 .7 39.8 2.93.41.2 1-1 Dental Examination - Referral NoOtherReferral referral or plan for additional dental care 33.723.7 1.1 10.313.3 .6 15.910.3 .4 kik) 1 40C. Hearing - Referral Unknown 41.345.4 49.816.5 46.115.3 - ,Note: (1) N = 6,309. . UnknownOtherOtolaryngologistHearingNone and speech center 52.2 1.1 .5.8 47.1 1.21.1 .8 51.9 1.1 .1.8 The report on the Head Start programin the San DiegoCity schools (Reference 10) indicatedthat approximately 200health referrals were madefor the 20 Head Start classeswhich handled 544 children. throughout the summer. As has been indicated insubsection 11.C.3 which describes the health of the Head Startchildren as determined by the medicaland dental examinations,there is considerable evidencethat the medical services varied widelyfrom program to program. Thesevariations in quality of servicewould quite naturally affect theimpact of the ser- 1 vices on the health ofthe children.However, the potentiality of the overall health services'benefits for Head Starters wassuccinctly - described in the followingstatement made by Mrs.Gertrude Boyles and included in Dr.Allen Soule's report onNorthfield, Vermont (Reference 99): "They wereprepared not only from areadiness stand- point, but also from oneof health; they had beenchecked from top to bottom, had all, shots broughtup-to-date, aching teethattended to, and glasses where needed. Forthe first time in Northfield'shistory, all first graders could startoff on somewhat the sameequal ground."

2. Cognitive si111 There were two major sourcesof information on cognitivede- velopment during HeadStart--the independent researchstudies and the 1- percent sample. The1-percent sample was ofspecial importance because it was intended to reflect anationwide, representativepicture drawn from the total sample of560,000 Head Start childrenwho were to be

1A related impact, really morepertinent to the communities' area, probably resulted becausethis was the first time that manyschools and medical organizationshad to address themselves toorganizing a programof comprehensive medicalservices.

IV -32 pretested and posttested1on both thePeabody Picture VocabularyTest (PPVT)2and the Preschool Inventory(PSI)3. a. PPVT, 1-Percent Sample One measure of the impactof Head Start on children is the differencebetween the pretest and the posttestPPVT mean scores. This section ofthe report deals exclusivelywith those children in the1- percent sample onwhom both pretest andposttest PPVT scores wereob- tained. The totalsample size was634.The pretest raw mean PPVT score for the total sample was47.6, while the posttest raw meanPPVT score was 52.9.This difference of 5.3 raw scorepoints is statistically signifi- cant beyond the5-percent level of confidence.However, a comparison of these means with meansreported in independentstudies (see Exhibit IV-8) indicates that the total1-percent sample pre- and posttest means are somewhathigher than for these studies.Thus, there is reason to believe that there was a strongbut indeterminateselection bias produc- ing the available1-percent sample of PPVTmatched pretest and post- test scores, althoughthere could be selection biasesin the opposite

'Itshould be noted thatpretests and posttests were tobe given during the Head Start program,with at least a 4-weekinterim between tests. 2The PPVT is a testof verbal ability which does notrequire a verbal response; forexample, the examinershows the child a pagecontaining four pictures and asksthe child to point to thepicture of the "table." There are 150 possiblepictures to identify; the"raw score" is the num- ber of correctlyidentified pictures and can beconverted into a Mental Age (MA) or IntelligenceQuotient (IQ) equivalent.Because the stand- ardization sample for thePPVT was comprisedof a geographically and racially restrictedpopulation (4,012 whitechildren in the Nashville, Tennessee, area), manyinvestigators reported onlythe raw scores and expected the reader to comparethe raw score with itsapproximate Mental Age equivalentobtained from the standardizationsample. 3The Preschool Inventoryis a measure of schoolreadiness developed especially for HeadStart by Dr. Bettye Caldwelland Mr. Donald Soule at Syracuse). Theoriginal test as used in (New York State University possible the Summer1965 Head Start programcontained 161 items with a total score of 315.See Appendix A for a copyof the instrument.

IV-33 EXHIBIT IV-8 PPVT MEAN RAW SCORES (NINE STUDIES) J 1-percent SourceStudy . N Chronological Pre-.Age Mean 5-8 47.6test DeviationStandard Post- 14.7 test52.9 DeviationStandard 13.82 Difference 5.23*(1) Standard Deviation of Difference 10.02 HorowitzEisenbergJohnson 424 sample 634 7923 5-25-0(2) 34.839.632.6 12.3317.75 9.5 44.6:-9.036.8 17.3810.82 7.8 4.2*4.2*5.0* , 4,4.1.1 Fishman 37 5-2 34.7 11.67 5.79* Pierce-OzerBerlin Jones 126141 65 5-15-4 45.944.035.9 11.97 47.941.6 1.92* PorterNotes: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates reported (2) Approximation. 33 5-0(2) 48 statistically significant. direction in the independent studies' scores. A more detailed discus- sion of the possible sources of bias in the I-percent sample is given in subsection 11.F.While these considerations jeopardize both internal and external validity (Reference 15), there are interesting aspects of the data. One- and two-factor analyses were made of data in an effort to assess the effect of the treatment--Head Start experience--and its interaction with various characteristics of the children. Two types of analyses were made: analysis of differences, and analysis of covariance. The models and procedures for these analyses are given in Appendix B and will not be repeated in detail here. The results for a set of factors and factor levels similar to those presented in subsection 11.C.4 'are summarized in Exhibits 1V-9 through IV -28. Of the two types of analysis, analysis of covariance is probably the more accepted since, by holding, starting level of performance statistically constant, it eliminates a bias favoring the lower scoring groups on the pretest (cf. discussion by Lord, Reference 70).The two methods of analysis can lead to quite different conclusions. Both are included here because some comparative points of interest in the results have emerged. Results of both types of analyses for 1-factor classifications of the sample of matched pry /post test scores are summarized in Exhibits IV-9 through IV-18. The tables are compact, in order to display the greatest amount of useful interpretive information in a single area. Since they present data in a format that is not standard, a detailed description will be made of the contents of the first exhibit in the series. The other 1-factor exhibits are identical in form. Exhibit IV-9 shows the results of analyses of gains when the scores are grouped by age.Levels of this factor are named in the left-hand column. In the next column, the sample sizes for each level and for the total factor sample are shown. Next, there is a set of three columns collectively labeled "Means." Pretest means (repeated from subsection II.C.4) are given for each level in the column so labeled.In the next column, the difference (D-score) of pretest and posttest means for each level is given. An asterisk next to a difference score indicates that the

IV -35 EXHIBIT IV-9 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY AGE o Age N Pre-test DifferencePre/Post Means DifferenceRegressed Regressed DifferenceA B C Difference DifferencePre/Post Total DifferenceRegressed ' B.A. Less than 5 years 354 93 48.1737.38 4.42*(1)5.23* 16.4913.16 ...81 X ' -3.33* X -3.33* 0 -.29 .52 -2.22* 1.11* 1-4c...) C. 65 years olderyears or 187 51.42 4.47* 16.49 -.05 .76 X -.24 1.11 cr Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < .05 . 634 45.66 4.71* 15.38 Pre/Post Difference IC= C'M dl ett aNNIIItaft, ILM CM CM =3 IC=3 SCORES BY RACE EXHIBIT IV-10 COMPARISON OF PPVT Means A Difference Total 11011100 .111111111, Race N Pre-test DifferencePre /Post DifferenceRegressed Regressed Difference DifferencePre /Post DifferenceRegressed A.C.B. WhiteNegroOther 313270 8 44.950.950.8 4.8*(1)5.5*2.1 15.9416.7413.98 2.8-.7 X 3.4 X.80 .1.962.76 X -2.0 0.81.3 -1.57 1.19 .39 Ca Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) 591 48.8indicates p < ,05 . 4.1* 15.55 Pre/Post Difference 0 EXHIBIT IV-11 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY SEX Means A B Difference Pre /Post Total Regressed A.B. FemaleMale Sex 320301 48.9Pre-46.3test DifferencePre /Post 5.2*5.2*(1) DifferenceRegressed 16.0116.66 N,.. Regressed Difference X0 .65 X Difference 00 Difference -.33 .32 ilote: Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < 621 47.6 5.2* .05 . 16.34 Pre/Post Difference IEL'M 1'r:=11 Ir=1 r=3 ram O O EXHIBIT IV-12 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY FAMILY INTACTNESS Difference Intactness Family N 4111=0.1.0Pre-test DifferencePre/Post Means RegressedDifference Ites2ressedA Difference B C 1Z DifferencePre/Post Total DifferenceRegressed H A.B. iiiother Mother and fatheronly 435121 48.745.4 4.7*(1)5.4* 16.1016.12 -0.7 X .02 X 1.871.85 -2.05 -2.07 -0.3 0.4 - .06.04 ..(..4- Io D.C. Other Father only 3319 45.151.1 7.7*2.3 18.17.14.25 -3.0 2.4 -2.3 3.1 -5.4 X -3.92 X -2.7 2.7 -1.91 2.01 Note': Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < 598 47.6 5.0* .05 . 16.16 Pre/Post Difference C EXHIBIT IV-13 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY HOUSEHOLD SIZE 0 Household Means .01110,111110.Regressed A B Difference Pre/Post Total DifferenceRegressed ..=11111 A.B. More8 orSize lessthan 8 528106N Pre-48.145.6test DifferencePre/Post 4.8*5.3* (1) Difference 15.4016.56 Regressed -0.5 X Difference -1.16 X Difference -0.2 0.3 -.58 .58 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) ert..,...31i%1,10...... 634 46.8indicates p < .05 . 5.0* 15.98 Pre/Post Difference ,-..r.^...-mmpra+snrwirmowineewilmillIMPPINIMPN r-71 1'1 !I'll firT-1 EXHIBIT IV-14 COMPARISON THE MOTHER WORKS OF PPVT SCORES ON THE BASIS OF WHETHER Means A B Difference Total A.B. MotherWorks NoYes 348215N 47.8Pre-47.6test DifferencePre/Post 5.0*5.3*(1) RegressedDifference 16.4016.08 Regressed Difference .3X .32X DifferencePre/Post -.1 .2 DifferenceRegressed -.16 .16 I-41:. Note: (1) Asterisk (*) Total 563 47.7indicates p < .05 . 5.1* 16.24 Pre/Post Difference C EXHIBIT IV-15 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY URBANIZATION Difference 0 A.Urbanization Urban 383 N Pre46.1 test- DifferencePre /Post 5.3*(1) Means DifferenceRegressed 16.15 RegressedAX Difference .49B -.87 C 2.14D DifferencePre/Post 0.9 Total RegressedDifference .44 C.B. Farm 47 53.4 32* 15.66 2.1 X -1.36 1.65 -1.2 -.05 . D. Rural,Other nonfarzn.Total 611159 22 43.348.350.4 4.4*3.95.3* 15.7114.0117.02 1.4 Pre /Post Difference0 -2.1-0.7 1.4 X 3.01 X -0.5 0.9 -1.70 1.31 . Note: (1) Asterisik (*) indicates p < .05 . IMO SIM 1=3 V=2 =3 11=1 =i1 I= C r"1 Lx71- r"771 r-T-1 EXHIBIT IV-16 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY INCOME O Means- A S C D E DifferenceF G H I Total A. Less than $1,060 Income 48 N 43.5Pre-teat DifferencePre/Post 5.4*(1) RegressedDifference 15.82 X -1.13 -1.11 .02 Regressed Difference2.171.04 1.90 .77 -.54 .59 1.592.72 -3.59-4.72 -11.15-12.28 DifferencePre/Post -0.6-1.1 DifferenceRegressed -1.i2 -.69 W41. c C.B.D. $1-1,999$2-2,999 $3-3,999 150 9783 48.046.548.0 3.6*5.7*6.1* 14.7816.9316.95 -0.1-0.5 2.0 0.42.5X X 2.1 X2.15 1.88-.27 -1.58 .57 2.70 .55 -5.76-3.61 -13.32-11.17 .3.1-1.0 -2.86 -.71 E.G.F. $4-4,999 $5-5,999$6-7.999 314260 53.152.048.0 4.2*3.8*1.8 14.2316.3615.05 3.81.41.8 4.32.31.9 3.91.51.9 -0.2-0.6 1.8 -0.4 2.0 X -1.31 2.4 X 2.13X .82 -6.31-4.18-5.49 -13.871.74-13.05 -4.9-2.5-2.9 -3.41-1.28-2.59 I.H. $8-9,999 $10,000 or more .53 49.434.7 20.0* 9.0 20.5428.10 -14.4 -3.4 -13.9 -2.9 -14.3 -3.3 -16.4 -5.4 -16.2 -5.2 -15.8' -4.8 -18.2 -7.2 -11.0 X . -7.56 X 13.3 2.3 10.46 2.90 4 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < Total 519 47.0 6.7*.05 . 17.64 Pre/Post Difference SI' EXHIBIT IV- 17 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY REGION t. Means B C D EDifference F G Total Regressed B.A. MiddleNortheast Region N56 46.74Pre-test DifferencePre/Post 1.67 DifferenceRegressed 12.5(.? X .4.83 X Regressed Difference-3.58 -3.86 .97 -5.91 -4.73 .10 -5.64 DifferencePre/Post -3.62 Difference -4.08 .75 $$ C. Souk ..-Ast Atlantic 151210 65 43.0748.67'52.90 6.38*4.80*5.06*(1) 16.4516.1717.42 -4.71-3.13-3.39 -1.32 .26 -1.58 1.25 X -.28X -2.05-2.33-1.08 -1,15 -.87 -1.78-2.06 -.81 1.09-.49-.23 -.22-.50 D.F.E. Midwest SouthwestWest 101 3912 44.9355.30 5.58*6.74*6.82* 18.2317.3218.50 -5.07-5.15-3.91 -1.68-1.76 -.52 -1.94-2.02 -.78 -.44 .80 -1.24 X 1.18 X.08 -.91 X .27 1.451.53 .29 1.831.56 .65 Note:G. Far (1) West Asterisk (*) indicates p < .05 , Total 634 49.1648.68 5.29* , 16.67 Pre/Post Difference -.36 .1.16 WEL 1=1 SE=I 1=11 IZZI3 Ina =111 r-"wi rig EXHIBIT IV -18 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY FAMILY INCOME (TWO LEVELS) r-a Income N Pre- DifferencePre/Post Means DifferenceRegressed Regressed Difference A B Difference DifferencePre/Post Total DifferenceRegressed A.B. MoreLess thanthan $3,000$3,000 295224 49.2946.75 test 53,87* .83*(1 ) 15.3916.76 1.96* X 1.37 X -.98* .98* -.68 .69 U' Note: Total (1) Asterisk (*) 519 48.02 indicates p < .05 . 4.85* 16.07 Pre/Post Difference differer.ce from zero (H0: D = 0) is statistically significant at the .05 level. In the 'case of age, all three differences are significant.This is generally true for all factors and most levels.All differences are positive and most are significant. At the bottom of the column is the average pre/ post difference (D.. ) for the factor.In the next column, labeled "Re- gressed Difference," is listed the difference (cc-score) for each level when the postscores are regressed on the prescores. Again, the mean regressed difference (cc.) for the factor is shown at the bottom of the column. The final block of columns is labeled "Difference." The first column in that block is identified by the letter designations of the age factor levels.Thus, the column contains interlevel differences. The column is, in fact, a matrix. The top half, labeled "Regressed Dif- ference," contains the differences of the regressed differences of dif- ferent factor levels from each other (e.g., ccA- ccri =13.16-16.49 = -3.33).The X' s in the column are simply intended to separate the two halves of the matrix. The asterisks again indicate a significant differ- ence in the absolute regressed gains between the children of the two age levels (A. less than 5 years old, and B. 5 years old).These are main effects. As the exhibit shows, the differences, adjusted for pretest level, between the two older groups (B and C) are identical. The bottom half of the matrix, labeled "Pre/Post Difference," gives the differences of the unadjusted D-scores between levels.Thus, DA - Dc = 4.42 - 4.47 = -.05. The difference is not significant at the .05 level.The interpretation is the same as for the regressed differences. The final two columns, collectively labeled "Total," give the difference of the differences from their mean unweighted difference for unadjusted and adjusted gain scores. The unadjusted differences are listed in the next to last column, labeled "Pre/Post Difference." For each level the difference is simply the level D-score minus the total unweighted D-score. Thus, for the first level, Aj - D.. = 4.42 - 4.71 = -.29.The subscript dot signifies a mean. The absence of an

W-46 asterisk indicates thatthere is nosignificant difference betweendiffer- A signifi- ences fromthe mean unweighteddifference for the sample. cant differenceof a differencewould imply adifferential treatment effect. The group means arenot, of course,independent of the total dependence into account mean.However, the analysishas taken that of the pre/ (see Appendix B).It may be seenin Exhibit IV-9 that none is post differencesof differences wassignificant. The interpretation levels showed that, as measuredby simple gain scores,none of the age significantly greater orsmaller gains thanthe others. the In the finalcolumn, labeled"Regressed Difference," are differences of regresseddiffererices from the averageregressed A A 13.16 - 15.38 difference for thesample (e. for Level A: cc. cc = signifies a significantdif- =-2.22). As.in all other cases,an asterisk ference at the5-percent level. An overall interpretationof Exhibit IV-9 wouldbe as follows: There was asignificant differencebetween pretest and posttest meansfor each age level. There was no ageeffect observed whenchange was measured by D-scores; meanpre/post difference scores forthe three age levels werenot significantly different from eachother. For adjusted gains(Yx- scores), thereappeared to be an age effect;the gain of the youngest groupwassignificantly different from and smallerthan the regressedgain of either of the other groups. There was no observeddifferential effect (orinteraction) of the treatment(exposure to Head Startdefined by the pre/ post interval)associated with agewhen the dependent variable was measuredby D-scores. There was adifferential effect of thetreatment associated with age when aregression effect istaken into account. The youngest age group,which had the lowestpretest mean. score (aswould be expected),improved significantlyless in terms of cc - scores thanexpected when a presumed regression bias is eliminated. In Exhibit IV-10, results arepresented for a classificationof scores by race. ExhibitsIV-11 through IV-17 summarizethe results of analyses for sex, familyintactness, household size, whethermother works, urbanization, income,and regionality, respectively.Exhibit IV-18 gives results forthe income factor when only twolevels are considered. Findings in the 1-percentsample 1-factor analyses ofchange scores may besummarized as follows: Differences between posttest andpretest means for all factors and factor levels werepositive and in most cases significant at the .05 level. With the exception of ageand income (two levelsonly), there were no obtained maineffects of the experimental variables either for D-score or cc-score measuresof impact; the magnitudes of absolutedifference (D-scores) or regresseddifference (cc-scores) betweenlevels within a factor were notsignificantly different from eachother. With the exception of age andincome (two levels only), there were no obtaineddifferential effects of the program treatment (as defined byexperience during Head Start between pretest and posttest)either for differences of differences, or for differencesof regressed differences; that is, the analyses did notprovide a basis for rejecting the hypothesis that the2-way interaction (Group xtreat- ment) = 0. There was a significant maineffect of age and.income (less than $3,000 versus$3,000 and over) for both D-scoreand cc-score measures. There was a differential effectof treatment on age levelfor cc-score measures, but-notfor D-score measures; the youngest age group had asignificantly low difference of

IV-48 regressed differences(cc- scOres), but theydid not have a significantly differentdifference score(D-score) from the other two age levels. The subjects fromlower income levelshad a significantly larger D-score thanthe higher income group,and it differed significantly from thetotal group unweighted averagediffer- ence score;however, there was nosignificant difference between groupswhen regresseddifferences were used as the dependentvariable. Results for a numberof 2-factor analyses aregiven in Exhibits described in IV-19 throughIV-28. Theseanalyses, for reasons the total Appendix B, use theregressed differenceof posttest means on variable. The matched andunmatched pretest means asthe dependent factorial exhibits summarizethe results of thisanalysis of an n x m 1.-factor design in a mannersomewhat similar tothat used with the analyses. Analysesof posttest means werealso made. Consider ExhibitIV-19, which showsthe results of acomparison factor levels of PPVT scoresby income and race.The two factors and for each areidentified by name,and the sample sizeand posttest mean unweighted means are cell is listed.Row and columnsample sizes and regressed also listed. Thenumbers identified asdifferences are not the they are differences calculatedfor cells, rows,and columns. Rather, column effects. the least squaresestimates (LSE) ofthe cell row and the .05 level. An asteriskindicates that a componentis significant at column, Thus, the presenceof an asteriskindicates a significant row, differ- or row xcolumn interactionwith respect to posttestmeans _arid differences. A ences. The sameanalyses were madeof regressed asterisk, plus (+) in a cell, row, orcolumn-has the samemeaning as an posttest differ- but with referenceto regresseddifferences instead of ences of means.LSE's for regresseddifference components are not shown. substantial One point worthnoting in the2-factor tables is the further loss of scores. by race In Exhibit IV-19,data for theclassification of subjects Negro and Other and income ispresented.It was necessaryto combine

IV-49 EXHIBIT IV-19 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY INCOME AND RACE 0 Income N Mean White Difference N RACE Mean Other Difference N Mean Total Difference $2-2,999$1-1,999$1,000Less than 5221 8 49.5057.6561.29 -1.204-(1)4.24* .35+ 412834 46.8545.9351.00 .-4.24*- .35 + 1.20 + 935536 47.7254.3254.07 -5.52*(1 1.08 .83 $5-5,999$4-4,999$3-3,999 192131 54.3755.8654.71 -4.89*+-1.86+ .894- 1019 48.1158.2052.47 - .89 4.89*1.86 + + 405029 56.2851.9953.59 -1.25+ 3.04* .35 Note: $6,000 andover Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates significance atindicates .05 level significance for D-score at measures; .05 level170. forplus ce-score sign 18(4-) measure. 56.2260.1.7 2.98*2.52+ 157 6 49.1750.25 -2.98*-2.52 ÷ 327 24 53.2454.67 1.43+ it=11 t=1 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY URBANIZATION AND RACE L'73. .f.=3 E":71 Y. r 77-3 EXHIBIT IV-20 White RACE Other Total Urbani-zation N96 Mean54.45 Difference 153 N 49.92Mean Difference 3.15 i 249 N 52.18Mean Difference -1.38 RuralFarmUrban 2172 59.1063.38 -3.14-2.87 6.03*( ) 31 6 40.5054.03 -6.03*+ 2.87 103 27 56.5651.94 -1.62 3.00 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates significanceTotal at .05 level for D-score measures; plus sign indicates significance at .05 level for189 a-score measure. 58.97 5.41* 190 48.15 -5.41* 379 53.56 (+) 0 EXHIBIT IV-21 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY URBANIZATION AND FAMILY INTACTNESS. INTACTNESS 0 Urbani- zation NFather and Mother Mean Differ- ence N Mother OnlyMean Differ- ence N Father OnlyMean Differ- ence N Mean Total Differ- ence FarmUrban 179 24 58.8352.47 -3.66-0.09 56 1 48.7789.00 20.33*+-9.974) 25 40.0056.80 -16.68*+ 10.05*+ 240127 52.6862.61 -4.18 5.75* Rural 284 81 58.91 1 3.73 169 12 51.00 10.36* 6.06* 14 7 56.00 6.63 367100 56.8655.30 -1.56 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates significance atTotal .05 level for D-score measures; plus sign indicates significance at .05 level for a-score measure. 56.74 -3.12 62.92 50.93 J_ 5.93* (+) i 4,7VI17171.411rIallW"...r".....PIPMFFITOMPFIMPOPOPPrilig.11.4 EXHIBIT IV-22 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY URBANIZATION URBANIZATION AND INCOME IncomeUnder N Mean Urban Difference N Mean Farm Difference N Mean Rural Difference 188N Mean Total Difference 0.644- ana over$3,000 111210 99 49.5951.5453.49 -2.59*(1)-3.36* .59*-i- 2331 8 51.2559.9155.58 -3.69*+0.683.69*+ + 549339 58.0557.1357.59 -1.10* 2.69*+1.10* 1 334 146 54.9054.2655.54 -0.64+ Note.: Total (1) Asterisk (*) indicates significanceindicates significance at .05 level for a-score measure. of .05 level for D-score measures; plus sign (+) EXHIBIT IV-23 EDUCATIONCOMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY RACE AND RACE DEGREE OF MOTHER'S EducationMother' s N Mean White Difference N . Mean Other Difference N Mean. Total Difference 9-117-80-6 592430 56.1953.2360.00 -2.74- .50 3.69 112 3214 45.5750.1351.66 -3.69 2.74 .50 171 6238 53.1652.4452.78 - -.01 .35 .37 . High SchoolGraduate Yes 56 . 59.68 1.53 38 49.55 -1.53 9411 54.62 1.83 College No 38 54.50 - .11 3 47.67 .11 7 51.0852.66 -1.71 Yes Total 180 56.3254.33 -1.86 3 .53*(1) 203 4 49.2651.00 -3.53* 1.86 383 52.79 - .13 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicates p < .05. I 1::;3 CZ2 CM CZ C=3 r771 t"7") r-13 EXHIBIT IV-24 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY RACE AND WHETHER MOTHER WORKS Plomw an White RACE Other Total. MotherWorks Yes N 39 57.56Mean Difference. -0.45 N95 i 51.32Mean Difference 0.45 134 N 54.44Mean Difference 0.98 Note: (1) Asterisk (*) indicatesTotalNo p <.05. 137176 57.0356.49 3.57*(1)0.44 185 90 49.8948.46 -3.57*-0.45 361227 53.4652.48 -0.98 41111111.11,11111.1111111PPIMPRIMPIIPIPIPPRER4111 , 0 EXHIBIT IV-25 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY FAMILY INTACTNESS AND SEX o 1--.' Mother and Father Mother FAMILY INTACTNESS Father Total Male Sex 142 N Mean57.31 Difference 1.61 4333N 47.42Mean Difference -3.41 56.50Mean Difference 1.79 187193 N Mean53.74 Difference 1.08 Female Total 288146 54.6?.51.92: -1.62 1.96 76 49.7552.06 1 -2.91 3.39 16 53.6350.75 -1.80 047 380 52.6651.58 -1.08 . . CT.,1 CriM c e=3 1 EXHIBIT IV-26 COMPARISON OF PPVT SCORES BY RACE RACE AND SEX 0 Male Sex 106 N Mean59.70 White Difference 1.41 N95 50.24Mean Other Difference -1.40 201 N 54.97Mean' Total Difference 1.68 Note: Female(1) Asterisk (*) indicates p

IV-61 the mean performance of groups has shown a consistentincrement of the same relative magnitude. Except for age andincome variables, there has been no indication of any differentialsensitivity of subjects to the program as far as thedifference and regression models are con- cerned. Granting that the datafrom the 1-percent sample are related to a design in which a numberof factors can operate to jeopardize inter- nal validity (Reference 15) andgranting the problem of establishing or defining the validity of a summary test scoresuch as that provided by the PPVT, the results both in thenational sample and in a number of independent studies seem suspiciouslyuniform. However, the age find- ing suggests the hypothesis that, atleast in the national sample, there was some sensitivityof performance to program, as measured by the PPVT. There are, of course, manyalternative hypotheses. However, assuming for the moment that the result isindicative of a differential effect of the program with respect to age of thechildren, there are some extremely provocativefurther hypotheses of an explanatory nature that could well be the subject of futureinvestigation. For example, it was found in anexamination of characteristics of workers that asizable percentage of workers had had little or noprevious experience with preschool children. On the other hand, alarge number of the professional workers had had elementary schoolexperience.It is possible to enter- tain the hypothesis that the more removed(dissimilar) the child is from the teacher's experience, the less effectivehe or she will be in guiding psychological (especially cognitive) development orshaping language skills in a given period of time. Stated otherwise, the greater thedis- parity between the level of developmentof the child and the average level experienced by the teacher, the greater theincompatibility- of teacher's skills and child,' s abilities', By this reasoning, we are inclined to acceptthe results based on the assumptions of the regressionmodel.In the regression model, it is expected that the lower scorers will gain morethan the higher ones. Generally they did, and we are thus inclined toconclude that there was no differentialeffect. When we did not find the expected regression, as in the age factor group, we were quitepre:,ared to believe in a lack of

111- 62 effect, even though there was nopresele.ction of extreme gimps. How- ever, the younger childrendid show a gain (D-scores) that was not signifi- cantly different from that shown by the older children. Wecould just as well argue that for them tohave made such a gain indicates a far greater impact on a groupthat, by the nature of the PPVT test, should be less developed and able to copewith the requirements of the operation in the lirst place. Equal scoreintervals may not signify equal ability increments. By this argument, we may wellconclude that the treatment had a more significant impact on allthe lower scoring groups (e. g., Negroes, girls, etc.). This issue is not resolvable on apriori grounds. The conclusion drawn here is that the two analyses,based on quite different expectations, lead to quite different conclusions,In the absence of sound theory to guide a choice, the conclusions arevaluable primarily to the extent that they suggest interesting hypotheses forfurther study. b. Independent Research Studies Reports have been received to date from20 different investigators who turned their attention tostudying changes in children's general cognitive functioning ordevelopment. The test of cognitive functioning most frequently used was thePeabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). The PPVT studies constitutereplicative Itudie :... to some extent, since PPVT score changes werealso obtained in the nationwide 1-percent sample.In addition to the PPVT, the PreschoolInventory and other tests of cognitive or generalfunctioning such as school readi- ness tests, theStanford-Binet, Goodenough Draw-A-Person,Primary Mental Abilities, Seguin Form Board,and the Leiter International were used by different researchers. - Ideally, all investigations of Head Start impact wouldhave included measures of HeadStart children early in the program(pretests) and late in the program (posttests), as well as measuresof disadvantaged and nondisadvantaged non-HeadStart (control) children for comparison purposes. Unfortunately,it was often impossible forresearchers to include all the desirable groups.

IV-63 This part of the report is organized in several subsections.First, appropriate independent research studies which included thePPVT as one of their tests will bedescribed.(Of course, some of these studies also used other tests in addition to the PPVT.) Next,studies which did not report PPVT scores, but used other testsof cognitive functioning (such as the Primary Mental Abilities test), will be described. Within these two subsections, then, will be found a description anddiscussion of all available independent research studies which reported data, either of a preliminary or a final nature, on the impact of Head Start in the areas of cognitive or generalfunctioning. The third subsection will include a discussion of the relationships between various test measures and the problems of interstudy compari- sons.Finally, a summary of independent research studies in the cogni- tive area will be presented. (1) Studies Includin Use of PPVT A number of researchers included the PPVT among their research instruments. Thesestudies are of special inter- est, since data on the 1-percent nationwidesample are also available (see subsection IV. C.2). A repOrt front the Warminster Township School District in Penn - sylvania (Reference 52), reported pre-and post-Head Start PPVT scores for 15 pre-kindergarten and 13 pre first grade children. For the younger group, the pre-Head Start meanPPVT IQ of 74.0 shifted to 82. 9 after Head Start. For the older group, the preprogram mean PPVTIQobtained was 85.7, and rose to a meanof 97.2 on the posttest. Dr. Stanley I. Berger of the University of Rhode Island(Reference 5) obtained pre-Head Start PPVT scores for 59 children,ail well as pre- Head Start scores for three other tests: the Stanford-Binet, theLeiter International, and the Raven Progressive Matrices, From the59 chil- dren, a random sample of 20 children were posttested on all measures. Berger converted the raw scores to IQ points and reported thatthe pre- testmeans obtained on the Stanford-Binet,Leiter International and PPVT were respectively 92,84, and 83.The actual pretest and posttest scores

IV-64 for the201children were not presentedin the report, but the mean gains were given as +3.4 forthe Stanford-Binet, +9.75 for' thePPVT, and +10.55 for the LeiterInternational. The Raven ProgressiveMatrices scores, which werenot given as IQ scores, were4.09 for the pretest, with a -0.35 change onthe posttest.This represented a significant im- provement, in that ittook the children less time to completethe test tasks on the posttest.All gains were statistictallysignificant, but the +3.4 gain on the Stanford-Binet waswithin the standard error of measure- ment. The LeiterInternational and the PPVTreflected the most gain from the beginning to the endof this Head Start program. Dr. Leon Eisenberg of JohnsHopkins University (Reference33), used the PPVT, as well asseveral other measures, in hismajor study and several substudies. Becauseof the limited standardizationof the PPVT, Eisenberg chose to reporthis findings for the most partin raw score terms aswell as IQ points.In a sample of 424 Head Startchildren, there was an average(mean) PPVT score change from32.6 to 39.7 (raw score) from the beginning of HeadStart to the beginning of school in September. The PPVT manual(Reference 30, Page 15) indicates that for children ages 4 years, 9months, to 5 years, 5 months, this score change represents an estimatedIQ score change of 13 points(from 68 to 81). In addition to determining gross meanscore changes,Eisenberg analyzed score changes of HeadStart children in relation to theirinitial test scores.Exhibit IV-29 summarizes thisanalysis. Exhibit IV-29 shows that thegreatest gains occurredfor children with the lowest initial (pretest) score, asmight be Expected. However, there was a trend toward higherpostscores in all quartiles;in fact, 70 of the 95 children in the highestquartile either maintained orimproved their score from pretest toposttest. In order to supplementDunn's (Reference 30)standardization data on thePPVT, Eisenberg also assembledhis research data to show mean

wave 1An assumption apparently wasmade that the 20 childrenrandomly selected from the total groupof 59 children were representativeof the entire group, arid that the prescoresfor the entire group reflect the prescores forthe smaller group, IV -65 EXHIBIT IV-29 PPVT CHANGEIN. RELATION TO INITIAL SCORE (EISENBERG)

Quartile for Initial Mean Gain, Number Children With PPVT Score N Posttest Score Decrease 1 (lowest) 96 13.55 7 2 96 10.05 12 3 98 5.16 19 4 (highest) 95 .85 25 raw scores for month-by-monthchronological age intervals1between 54 and 69 months (i.e., between 4 years, 6 months, and 5 years, 9 months). The data presented in Exhibit IV-30 report initial ("naive") test resuPs for 712 culturally disadvantaged children(both Head Start and non-Head Start) and present some contrast to the original PPVT standardization group of white, Nashville, Tennessee, children. Eisenberg compared the mean raw score of 30.93 obtained by the 482 children between 57 and 65 months of age (1. e., 4 years, 9 months, to 5 years, 5 months) in his sample with the mean raw score of 50.22 reported in the PPVT manual2for the children in that age group in the standardization sample; the 20-point differential was a striking indica- tion to Eisenberg that the standardization sample, used as a comparison group, was considerably moreverbally developed, and that his own sample was indeed disadvantaged. In addition to the rather extensive PPVT data on Head Start and non-Head Start children that Eisenberg obtained andreported, he also obtained Draw-A-Person (DAP) tests on the same children (both Head Start and non-Head Start).For both tests, there were three test periods for Head Start children: early in the Head Start program(pre-Head Start); late in the program. (post -Head Start); and at the beginning of school in September 1965.All non-Head Start children were tested only once, at the beginning of school in September. Exhibit IV-31 summarizes Eisenberg's PPVT and DAP test data for his pre-Head Start children (Hy, post-Head Start children (HS2), September Head Start children (HS3), and September control, or non- Head Start, children (C). The exhibit indicates that the Head Start children's September scores were significantly higher than the scores of the control group. However, a comparison of the pre-HeadStart June scores and non-Head Start (C) September scoresyielded no signifi- cantdifferences, indicating that the Head Start children showed .about the same verbal development at a younger age (inJune) ar' the control

i 1PPVT standardization data for young children were grouped by6-month intervals rather than 1-month intervals. 2Reference 30, page 28.

IV- 6 7 EXHIBIT IV-30 DISTRIBUTIONOF PPVT. RAW SCORES BY CHRONOLOGICAL AGE(EISENBERG)(1)

Months N Mean Sigma 54 21 28.67 11.57 55 31 30.61 12.70 56 29 31.41 11.11 57 42 29.60 11.74 58 59 ;1).02 13.06 59 59 27.44 12.42 60 -46 29.13 17.58 61 77 28.73 17.42 .62 46 32.41 11.41 63 48 37.67 10.46 64 47 32.45 11.16 65 58 33.76 11.51 66 61 34.51 15.20 67 33 29.52 18.73 68 36 36.00 11.94 69 19 36.95 1'3.61

Note:(1) The sample consistedof 712 culturally disadvantagedchildren, as defined bytheir eligibility for Head Start.Thus, the sample included Head Startchildren as well as non-HeadStart, but comparabie, children. -0.1,11111PPIRPIPIPIRSPRIPI, CM1 C:=1 a CeM C=I EXHIBIT IV-31 SUMMARY OF PPVT AND DAP SCORES (EISENBERG) Mean Draw-A-Person Standard IQ N Mean PPVT Standard .14.1 IQ HS 2(post-Head Start 1 .(pre-Head Start) Group 476500 N Raw Score 9.107.71 Deviation 4.204.79 Estimate 76 423424 Raw Score 36.8332.63 Deviation 12.3310.82 Estimate 6876 1-4 HS3(Head Start, September) 435 9.75 4.41 82 413 39.74 11.34 81 %.0 1 ti C (non-Head Start, September) 420 8.91, 4.98 402 33.65 11.70 70 HS Significant.Changes1DAP - HS 2 HS PPVT1 - HS 2 HS2HS - HS 3 - C 3 HSH53 -C 22 -- HSC 3 children at an older age (in September, about 2 months later).Thus. Eisenberg speculated that possibly the Head Start children had higher verbal abilities to begin with than the non-Head Start children. Eisenberg also directed two substudies that pertained especially to cognitive development, with specific reference to the effects of spe- cial tutoring on cognitive functioning as measured by selected instru- ments. In one substudy, "Clinical Testing of Head Start Children," Eisenberg's control group consisted of 20 children who were compar- able to the experimental group of 34 Head Start children, in that their parents had also applied for Head Start.(Application had been denied because of lack of available space. ) One group of the Head Start chil- dren received, in addition to the regular Head Start experience, special tutoring in drawing people and in playing games designed to teach aware- ness of body parts. Thus, there werethree groups in the study: (1) a regular Head Start group, (2) a Head Start group which received special tutoring designed to foster awareness of the body, and (3) a control group drawn from the same population as both Head Start groups. The three tests of cognitive development were the revised Stanford- Binet Intelligence Scale, the Goodenough Draw-A-Person, and the PPVT. (A behavior profile was also used: see subsection IV. C.3. ).On,the pre- test, the 34 Head Start and 20 control children were comparable onthe Stanford-Binet, with mean (average) IQ scores of 84.17 for the Head Start group and 83.17 for the control group. The posttests indicated a +6 change in IQ points for the Head Start group (mean IQ = 90.17) and only a +1 change for the control group (mean IQ = 84.64).The difference between the Head Start and control groups' mean IQ scores on the posttest was statistically significant at the .05level.Neither the DAP nor the PPVT scores for this substudy were available at the time of this writing, but Eisenberg indicated that the drawings of the tutored HeadStart group showed significant improvement as compared with thoseof the Head Start children who received no special tutoring in body awareness. Thisfind- ing is especially interesting because the pre-DAP results wereconsidered unscorable for both groups of Head Start children.(No information was reported on the DAP for the control children. )

IV- 70 The second of Eisenberg'ssubstudies wh.ch included use of the PPVT was called "The Effectof Special Perceptual Training on the PPVT, the California MentalMaturity Scale, and the Johns Hopkins Perceptual Task." (The JohnsHopkins Perceptual Task, orJHPT, was especiallydesigned to assess cognitiveskills in language-deficient children.) For this substudythere were three groups ofchildren, all matched for age and sex:(1) 42 Head Start children whoreceived special training in perceptualand conceptual skills, (2) 42Head Start children who received nospecial training in addition totheir Head Start e:xperience, and (3) 42non-Head Start children who werematched by age and sex withthe two Head Start groups. This study did not involvepretests; all measures weremade in September at the beginningof school.Exhibit IV-32 summarizes the available results of thesubstudy. Eisenberg noted that forthe CMMS (California MentalMaturity Scale) there was a generaltrend in favor of both HeadStart groups as compared with the non-HeadStart group, although statisticalsignifi- cance wasobtained only for the HeadStart Special Training group as compared with thenon-Head Start group. In general, the resultsof Eisenberg's majorstudy and substudies, all involving use ofthe PPVT as one measureof cognitive functioning and all involvingcomparison groups of somekinds, indicate that Head Start children showedsubstantial improvement in areasof cognitive functioning and development. A locally funded independentstudy submitted by Mrs.Dorothy Zimmerman, CaswellCounty, Yanceyville, NorthCarolina (Reference 112), also studied HeadStart impact in the cognitive areavia PPVT pre- andposttesting of 38 Head Startchildren.The score changes were reported in terms of thenumber of children wholost and gained points or who had no scorechanges. Of the 38 children,2 lost PPVT raw score points from pre- toposttest, 11 registered nochange, and 25 gained points. Of the 25"gainers, " 4 gained lessthan one-half of a standard deviation, 5 gainedone-half to one standarddeviation, and 16 gained at least one fullstandard deviation.

IV-71 EXHIBIT IV-32 EFFECTS OFSPECIAL TRAINING IN PERCEPTUAL AND CONCEPTUAL SKILLS(EISENBERG)

Age inPPVT Raw CMMS Raw JHPT Raw Group N Months Score Score Score 1. Head Start (special training) 42 63.00. 37.73 39.50(1) 19.92 2. Head Start (regular) 15 62.21 37.29 21.57 3. Non-Head Start 42 63.17 34.12 30.69(1) 18.69

Note: (1) For the CMMS, theHead Start special training groupreceived a significantly higher score(1:-percent level of confidence) than the non-Head Start Group. In another study, Holmes and Holmes (Reference58) used the PPVT, the Seguin Form Board, and four Stanford-Binetsubtests to measure changes in cognitivefunctioning during Head Start.They utilized a control group for comparison purposes and wereable to obtain test scores on children some 2 months after schoolhad been in progress, as well asin August (a time corresponding to the end of the Head Start program). Unfortunately, pre-HeadStart scores were not obtained. The Holmes' study"was especially interestingbecause of the care taken to assurecomparability of the control (non-Head Start) and experimental (Head Start) groups.It had been hoped that the control population would be drawn from a Head Start waitinglist, but when this plan became impossible (because the waitinglist was not long enough) control children were recruited and selected forcomparability along the following lines: lack of previous schoolexperience, mean age of 5 years and 5 months, sex, ethnicity, parents'occupation, parents' education, presence of mother, and presenceof father.The only measure on which the two groups werenot comparable was number of siblings; the Head Start children had moresiblings in the home than the control group.The final number of children on whom twotest scores were obtained was29 for each group. Exhibit IV-33 summarizesthe results of the testings. These data bear carefulconsideration because they imply that, in general, for the program underinvestigation the 8-week summer Head Start program had an impactcomparable to approximately the first 8 weeks of regular school, and that on one measure,at least, the Head Start group was maintainingthe advantage. Another thought-provoking study wasdirected by Porter in Cambridge, Massachusetts(Reference 87). An unusual factorin this study was that all testing was"blind, " that is, the testers did notknow which children had participatedin Head Start and which had not.Un- fortunately, pretesting was not done;all tests were given at a time following the Head Start program,but before school opened. However, the 33 control groupchildren were carefully matched with33 Head Start children for: lack of previous school experience, race, sex, age,

TV -73 C STUDY RESULTS (HOLMES AND HOLMES) 0 EXHIBIT IV-33 COGNITIVE Test Score Description N August He ad29 Start November 29 N August, Non-Head Start 29 November 29 Stanford-Binet Raw score forselected subtests MeanDeviationStandardN 10.28 20.4129 31.3429 9.21 DeviationStandardMeanN 13.0329 8.03 27.2129 6.20 PPVT Raw score IQconverted score. to DeviationStandardMean 20.10 90.10 93.7017.00 MeanDeviationStandard 15.86 74.0327 91.172714.48 Seguin Form . Board NUmber of taskseconds completion for DeviationStandardMeanN 26.52 53.7027 2723.1437.44 DeviationStandardMeanN 27.86 71.30 53.3031.13 [ education and age of mother, size and intactne3Sof family, income, housing, and the fact that the children remained in Cambridge during the summer:The children were transported to Cambridge City Hospital for psychometric evaluations (PPVT, Form B, Draw-A-Person, Seguin, Geometric Designs, and language samples); in addition, the non- Head Start children received physical and audiovisual examinations.(The Head Start children had been examined earlier, during their program.) The scores obtained on the PPVT for Head Start and non-HeadStart children revealed no significant differences between the two groups. Neither the Goodenough Draw-A-Person test results nor thegeometric figures tasks snowed any meaningful distinctions between the two groups. However, on the Seguin Form Board task, Head Start children didtend to solve the problem more rapidly, both on theinitial trials and on sub- sequent trials, than the control children. Thus, while other independent studies have reported that, follow- ing Head Start, participant children did significantlybetter than their non-Head Start peers (i. e., children from comparableenvironments) on tests of cognitivedevelopment, Porter's study, in which testing was "blind, " revealed no significant differences, although theresults of the Seguin Form Board (a measure of learning rate), werefairly consistently predictive of Head Start/non-Head Start differences. While the number of children involved in the study was small, and there were no pretest scores for comparison, theresults of the study suggest that the main factor distinguishing thisstudy from other reported studies- -the factor of blind testing- -may be of greatimportance in obtaining a valid picture of program effects.There is a need for further studies in which the testing is not done by an involved teacherbut by some- one more impartial--someonewho is unaware of oruninvolved in the 1 child's preschool status. It is worth noting in passing, however, that the useof an ex post

facto analysis, N ith a quasi pretreatment equivalence ofexperimental and control groups established by matching onvariables other than the performance ones, can produce results that may be misleading(cf. Reference 15).

IV- 75 Johnson (Reference 64) of the Clavis MontessoriSchools reports several findings of interest with resp?.ct to impactof Head Start in cog- nitive areas. On the pre-Head Start PPVT test,the mean IQ for the 79 Head Start children was 72, and the meanIQ when measured by the Goodenough Draw-A-Person (DAP) test was92,Posttesting results showed an increase in the PPVT to 79 andin the DAP IQ to 111. The Johnson study also included other measuresrelated to cognitive areas. Usingthe Wide Range Achievement tests, Johnson reported sta- tistically significant gains occurring onthe oral arithmetic readiness test; the results showed anincrease in grade-placement units of 3 months for the 6-week instructional period.(It should be mentioned, however, that the Head Start children were still about6 months behind their chronological-age peers, as defined by the tests'standardization sample). On the reading test an increase of 2 months wasmeasured, but this increase was reported as notstatistically significant. The Gesell Maturation Index was one test used to measureperceptual motor development; there was evidenceof development in this area, al- though the improvement was not statisticallysignificant. Two other ob- jective tests of perceptual motor development,the Mateer Inversion Test and a test of eye-hand coordination.and dominance indicated that even after Head Start, the children would need a gr.,..deal of readiness ex- perience before formal reading experience couldbe initiated. In addition to the objective tests,teacher rating scales of growth in the "intellectual-academic" areas reflectedthe opinion that consider- able development had taken place--an opinionthat appeared to be sup- ported by the test results. In Johnson's study, all test results wereanalyzed for the Head Start group as a whole (discussedabove), and for Anglo-American (N = 17) and Mexican-American (N = 62) childrenseparately, to determine differ- ences between the two groupsof children as well as progress or develop- ment of the total group. One of the moststriking differences, which il- lustrated the linguistic handicap of thepreschool Mexican-American child, occurred on the PPVT pretests. The pre-PPVTIQ means were 91 and 55 for the Anglo-American and Mexican-Americanchildren, respectively,

IV-76 while the nonverbal DAP pretest IQ meanfor the Mexican-American children was 91-- almost the same as theDAP mean of 92 obtained by the Anglo-American children.Both groups showed mean score increases on the PPVT andDAP posttests; the increases werestatistically signif- icant for the DAP only. On the reading and arithmeticreadiness pretests, the Anglo- American children obtainedsignificantly higher mean scores; both groups made positivebut not significant mean gains from the pretest to posttest period. TheAnglo-American group scored at grade level on the posttests,in contrast to the Mexican-Americanchildren, whose mean scores wereat the prereadiness levels. On measures of perceptual -motordevelopment, both groups made progress duringHead Start, but the gains made by theMexican-American children were often significantly greaterthan those of the Anglo-American children. On the teachers' rating scales, both groupsshowed development, but the Mexican-American children were seen ashaving made more gains than the Anglo-American children in theirattitudes toward school, ad- justment in group situations, and interestin the school program. In general, then,; there is evidencein Johnson's study to support the findings of others (Eisenberg(Reference 33); Jacobs and Shafer (Reference 62); Knox County (Reference4)) that in Head Start, the lower the initial performance of children, the greaterthe measured improvement. Another investigator who included the PPVT amonghis test instru- ments was a .pediatric neurologist, Dr.Mark N. Ozer (Reference 82). Ozer used the PPVT as a subtest for aSchool Readiness Evaluation and found an increased mean raw score offrom 35.85 (pre-Head Start) to 41.64 (post-Head Start)--a gain which wasstatistically significant. Ozer's study, "The Effects of Neurological andEnvironmental Factors on the Language Developmentof Head Start Children," was es- i pecially interesting because it related typeof center (e.g., teachers

. and facilities) to gains as measured bythe School Readiness Evaluation Battery (SRE). There were65 children (with a mean Stanford-Binet IQ of 88.78) in the total groupfor whom complete test data wereobtained;

IV -?7 all children participated in a HeadStart program, but one subgroup of Head Start children met in afelicitous setting (the National Child Research Center) with teachers highlyskilled in early childhood development, while the other subgroup met in asomewhat dilapidated public school building and was taught bydedicated but nonspecialized teachers. The SRE, which consistsof six subtests yielding 14 discrete meas- ures, wasused to measure pre- andpost-Head Start change. For the total group of 65 children,the SRE reflected significant score gains in 6 of the 14 areas of measurement:repeating words, repeating sentences, comprehending terms (the PPVT), retelling astory via pictures, recog- nizing letters of the alphabet,and recognizing Arabic numerals.There was a significant scoreloss in only one area, "conversationin response to question." There were no significant differencesbetween the two centers, although they presumably reflectedconsiderable differences in certain program characteristics.Ozer further examined the scorechanges in relation to sex and age. Althoughthere appeared to be some age-specific improvement in a few SRE areas onthe posttest (i.e., older children showed significantly more improvementthan younger children on four subtests), there was considerable evidence thatthe younger children also gained generally during Head Start.Thus, significant improvements occurred on six SRE subtests. Theseimprovements were apparently not related to factors ofschool program, sex, or age, and thus may reflect a uniform effect of Head Startexperience.' Another study, by Horowitz and Rosenfeld(Reference 59) of the University of Kansas, used both the PPVTand the Preschool Inventory

11t will be recalled that forthe 1-percent sample, analyses ofPPVT score gainsby age revealed no statisticallysignificant age-specific improvementsa finding in accord withOzer's findings. However, analysis of covariance resulted inthe finding that improvement was significantly less for younger children(see the discussion on pageIV-47). r"-.25511Merlattft

(PSI) to assess the impact of Head Start. Thestudy is of special interest because it obtained data on twostandard Head Start instruments(P4PVT and PSI), and because both theexperimental and control groups (the regular Kansas University summer nurseryschool class) were pre- and posttested on the PPVT. On the PPVT, the mean raw scorefor the 16 control children re- mained about the same from pre-to posttest (50.6, pretest, and50.2, posttest), but the mean raw scorefor the 23 Head Start children jumped from 39.6 to 44.6, significant atabout the .06 level.Considering that the Head Start children were agood deal older than the control children (the mean age of HeadStart children was 5 years, 2 months, with a range of 4, years, 5 months, to 5 years,11 months; while the mean a.ge of the control children was 4 years,2 months, with a range of 3 years, 7 months, to 4 years, 8 months), itis apparent that the Head Startexpe- rience did not permit the experimentalchildren to catch up with the controls in such a short time;however, some gain was noted for the Head Start children over the 8-weekperiod, as reflected by the PPVT score changes. As amatter of fact, when the PPVTstandardization sample is referred to, it can be seenthat while the scores of the control group reflected nochange during their nursery schoolexperience and the children maintained their meanmental age of approximately 5 years, 1 month (which was nearly 1 full year morethan their mean chronological age), the Head Start groups wentfrom a mean mental age of about 3 years, 10 months, to about 4 years, 4months, as reflected by the PPVT--again of about 6 "mental agemonths" for the 2 actual months thatpassed (i. e., the approximately 2 monthsbetween pre- and posttesting).Although their mean mental age of about4 years, 4 months, at the endof Head Start was still about a year lessthan their mean chronological age,their PPVT gains can be consideredfairly substantial. Although the original unrevised PSI wasadministered to each child, the results, reported were based on80 items which appear in ashorter form as revised byCaldwell.'The 80 items were grouped intosix content areas: Basic Informationand Vocabulary (12 items);Number Concepts and Ordination (21 items); ConceptsI--size, shape, motion, andcolor (17 items); Concepts IItime,object, class, and social (14 items); Visual Motor (4 items); andFollowing Instructions (12 items). Head Start children werepretested during the third and fourth weeks of the program andposttested during the eighth (final) week.The control children were testedonly once on the PSI, at the same timethe Head Start children werepretested. The tests were scoreeby tabulating the number of scoredpoints for each child in each ofthe six content areas. Exhibit IV-34summarizes the results. Tests of significance for the meansin Exhibit IV-34 indicated that in all six content areas, thenondisadvantaged (control) children were significantly superior to the HeadStart children at the time of the first testing. When the scores of thecontrol children were compared with the posttest Head Start scores,the control children were significantly superior in only three content areas:Concepts I, Visual. Motor, and Following Lnstructions. The twr groupsperformed comparably on Basic Information and Vocabulary,Number Concepts and Ordination, and Concepts II.Significance tests between the pre-and post-Head Start means indicated nostatistically significant changes in anyof the six content areas, althoughthere was improvement in all areas except "Following Instructions." At the University of Texas,Pierce-Jones (Reference 86) directed several Head Start investigations, oneof which studied "Cognitive Func- tioning of Preschool Childrenin Relation to Duration of Head StartEx- perience." Using 126 Head Startchild::en, he -obtained pretest raw scores on theSeguin Form Board (SFB), aswell as the PPVT and the

'Inconsistentscoring methodsof the PSI among differentinvestigators make meaningfulcomparisons between PSI resultsobtained in various independent studies virtuallyimpossible.

IV-80 czn, c=r mim t=.3 r=2 EXHIBIT IV-34 PRESCHOOL INVENTORY RESULTS (HOROWITZ AND Pre-Head Start ROSENFELD) Post-Head Start (N = 22) NumberBasic Information Concepts and and Ordination Vocabulary Content Area MeanControl Deviation (N13.419.6 = 20) Standard 4.05.7 Mean 10.717.1 (N = 22) Standard evip.tiGn. 4.4 Mean Deviationll..017.7 Standard 4.65.7 FollowingVisualConcepts Motor III Instructions 11.917.317.918.2 4.46.26.13.8 15.112.512.3 7.4 4.65.35.23.7 14.914.512.9 7.7 6.15.95.25.0 PSI. He then varied the number of days before administeringthe post- testso so that some posttests were given afteronly 10 days in Head Start, while other posttests were given as many as 29 daysfollowing Head Start experience. The mean pre- and posttestgains for the total sample of 126 children, without regard tothe time interval between tests, were significantbeyond the .001 level of confidence for the PPVT and the Preschool Inventory; for the Seguin FormBoard there was not a signif- icant change for the total group.Exhibit IV-35 summarizes the PPVT, PSI, and SFB pre- and post-HeadStart test results.(It should be noted that a shortened version of the PSI wasused, which contained only 69 items rather than 161.) For the specific time periods between pretestand posttest (10 to 13 days, 14 to 18 days, 19 to 22 days, and 23 to29 days) the pre.- and posttest score change was significantlyhigher in two cases for the PPVT (14 to 18 days and 23 to 29 days), in three casesfor the Preschool In- ventory (10 to 13 days, 14 to 18 days, and 23 to29 day's), and in one case for the Seguin Form Board (19 to 22days). In addition to test measures of cognitivedevelopment, Pierce-Jones also included a rating scale measure of school adjustment.After 4 months of school (in December, 1965), 100 first-gradeteachers (50 percent of whom had taught in the summer program) from five Texasschool systems were asked to compareHead Start and non-Head Start first-grade pupils. The results showed that teachers mentioned HeadStart children more Often as "proficient learners" and "intellectuallycurious." And, whether or not the teachershad been involved in Head Start, they all tended to believe that Head Start children were generally superior tonon-Head Start children. A report of PPVT scores obtained in October1965 on 37 children who had participated in Head Start was receivedfrom Mr. Robert Fishman (Reference 34) of Project HOPE(Kentucky). Although pre- Head Start scores were not available, these .scores areof interest tig"a, causethey illustrate the wide range of scores obtained on the PPVT. The contrast between a group post-Head Start raw score meanof 34.70

1.3/-82 arz:1 czi u C77771 r".1 r-71 77i r- EXHIBIT IV-35 PPVT AND PSI TEST RESULTS (PIERCE-JONES) O AllTime Subjects Intervals Subjects/ 126 N Pretest 45.93 PPVT posttest 47.85 *(2) PretestMean43.79 Scores PSI Posttest 45.56 p* Pretest28.56 SFB 28.15ostsi---1 23-2914-1810-1319 -22 days days 35263134 44.7447.9448.5241.69 49.5148.0647.9445.23 ** 46.4042.0342.3144.00 48.4944.1644.2445.04 ** 28.7125.5927.2332.77 26.8328.6926.0031.06 * t-Aco Notes: (1) The SFB scores, (2) Asterisk (*) indicates ment,of seconds in that required the taskwhich was completedare intended more to measurefor task completion;statistically significant thus a decreased scorequickly. difference, beyond .05 learning rate, are reflects improve- reported inlevel terms of confidence. (presented here) and 52.9 (from the 1-percent sample), for children of quite similar ages, is noteworthy. The group studied consisted of 37 children with a mean chronological age of 5 years, 2 months. Their raw score mean of 34.70 was converted to the equivalent mean mental age of 3 years, 7 months, or IQ = 69.51. Further discussion of the meaning and use of the PPVT results appears in subsection IV. C. 2.b. (3) below.

. IV-84 (2) Studies Using Measures OtherThan PPVT In Tompkins County in upstateNew York, Harding (Reference 47) obtained pre-and post-Head Start Draw-A-Personand PSI 1 scores for68 Head Start children.Both DAP mean raw scores were 3.8, thus indicating thatthe DAP test did not reflectchanges in cognitive functioning.(A raw score of 3.8 represents amental age of about 4 years.) However, Hardingreported significant meangain scores on the Preschool Inventory total score(excluding the DAP which was used as asubtest) in each of five classesstudied. For the 68 HeadStart children in five classes the pretest PreschoolInventory mean score was196, and the posttest score was 209,with a mean gain ofabout 13 points. Harding mentionedthat the gains, as measuredby the Preschool Inventory, werefairly evenly distrib- uted between classes,although the teaching styles ofthe five teachers var- ied widely; thus, "at presentit seems that there werefew if any significant differences in effectiveness amongthe... programs," despite theconsider- able differences in thesize of the staff, approachesof the teachers, and methods used. This findingof Harding's was consistentwith Ozer's finding that two groups of Head Startchildren made comparablegains (on the School Readiness EvaluationBattery), even though the two programsreflected rather large differencesin program quality in termsof staff and facilities. Allerhand (Reference 2) notonly measured PSI changesmade by Head Start children duringthe program, but he alsostudied and compared con- cept developmentof 76 Head Start"graduates" and 126 non-HeadStart chil- dren, all of whom hadbeen attending kindergartenin the same classrooms for about 2months. As a means of establishingcomparability of the two groups,Allerhand administered thePreschool Inventory to allchildren, and found no significantdifference between the preprogramHead Start mean of 151.33 (SD =35.79) and the non-Head Start meanof 150.42 (SD = 34.36). (These reported HeadStart scores were obtainedduring the first 2 weeks

1TheDAP scores wereobtained by separate scorings ofItem 3 on the unrevised PSI. For Item3, the tester said:"Draw me a picture of a man, awhole man, not just part of aman.'r of the summer program, but the time period duringwhich the non-Head Start scores were obtained was not actually reported.It is assumed in this report that their PSI scores were obtained at the sametime). On all six concept measurements (color,form-space, grouping, ordering, time sequence, and time duration),the Head Start group did better than the non-Head Start group, and theiradvantage was statisti- cally significant in the four conceptspertaining to grouping, ordering, time sequence, and color. In addition to the concept measurements,Allerhand also reported data on the post-Head Start PSI resultsfor the 76 participants. He found that the mean gain of 17 points(median and mode gains of 13 and 11, respectively) from the pretest meanof 151 was statistically sig- nificant beyond the .001 level of confidence. Allerhand plans follow-up studies of the two groups.To the ex- tent that the non-Head Startchildren were actually comparable to the Head Start children early in the summer,before Head Start, his future results should yield meaningful comparisonsof the school progress of the Head Start children in relation to the progressof their cultural peers. Eisenberg (Reference 33), whose PPVT studies werediscussed earlier, also did a substudy concerning conceptdevelopment, "The Ef- fect of Tutoring on Perceptual andConceptual Skills." The three groups, each of which contained 42 children, wereall matched for age and sex and represented (1) Head Start childrenwho received special training in perceptual and conceptual development,(2) Head Start children.who re- ceived no special training in addition tothe regular Head Start program, and (3) non-Head Start children. At the end of the Head Start program, thechildren were tested in- dividually for (1) ability to find similar stimuli amongdiffering geometric forms; (2) ability to select objects accordingto labels of color, size, and shape; (3) ability to name color, size, andshape; and (4) ability to ab- tract color, size, and shape (a conceptualtask).Results to date (for all ex- cept 27 of the regular Head Startchildren) indicate significant advantages for the Head Start special tutoring group incomparis:n with the regular Head Start and non-Head Start groups in two perceptualareas--selection and naming of objects. However, when the twoHead Start groups werepooled and treated as one group, there were nosignificant differences in the per- ceptual areas between the HeadStart and control groups. In the concep- tual area, however, there were nosignificant intergroup differences on the rate of solution of the concepttasks. Eisenberg's tentative conclu- sion was that neither theHead Start rogram nor specialtutoring signifi- cantly facilitated conceptformation, as reflected by the measuresused. 'Thus, the results obtained by Allerhandand Eisenberg do not appear to be fully consistent inregard to the impact of Head Start onconcept for- mation, although use ofdifferent tests is a confoundingfactor. Chesteen (Refe Tence 18) ofLouisiana State University used the Science Research AssociatesPrimary Mental Abilities (PMA) test to assess developmentin the area of cognitive functioning.Three groups of children were tested andstudied: 86 Head Start children were given pre-and posttests. Of these 86, 81 were retested athird and fourth time, in Sep- tember (early in the fall schoolterm) and 4 months later. Their mean IQ scores for the four testperiods were as follows: 86.35, 90.99, 93.32, and99.11. 28 middle-class non-Head Start controlchildren who were not assigned to the sameclasses as the Head Start children, but who were similar in age andgeneral geographic locality, were tested twice,first in September, and again 4 months later.Their mean IQ scores were 105.68and 112.36 for the two test periods. . 126 non-Head Start children who werein the same school classes as the Head Start' "graduates" (and thus were pre- sumed to be a mix of culturallydisadvantaged and nondisad- vantaged children) were tested once,4 months after school had been in session.Their mean IQ was found to be98.37. The results of the tests aresummarized in Exhibit IV-36. EXHIBIT IV- 36 PMA. TEST RESULTS (CHESTEEN) Time Periods ControlHeadControl Stzkrt (middle class) Groups (mix)(1) 126N 8628 Pre-Head Start 86.35 Post-Head Start 90.99 105.6893.32 (N=81)Preschool 112.3699.1198.37 (N=81)Postschool Note: (1) Culturally disadvantaged ... and nondisadvantaged, .combined. Chesteen found that, within the Head Start group, there Was a statistically significant IQ mean score change from the pre-Head Start test period to the preschool (September) test period. However, in Sep- tember the middle-class control group had a significant IQ score ad- vantage over the Head Start children.Thus, while Head Start children were better prepared for school than they were before their Head Start experience, they still were not on a par with their middle-class peers. It is in light of the above findings that Chesteen's second control group of 126 children isof special interest, because theynot the 28 middle-class children--were the actual classmates and peer-competitors of the Head Start "graduates" in school.It seems clear, then, that after 4 months of school the Head Start children were successfully competing with their actual classmates on the PMA, but continued to be at a disad- vantage when compared with middle-class children.It is unfortunate that early summer test results were not obtained on the 126 control children. Another aspect of Chesteen's study was an analysis to determine on which of the four PMA subtests (Verbal Meaning, Number Facility, Perceptual Speed, or Spatial Relations) the Head Start children would make the most improvement. With pre- and postscores for all 86 Head Start children, he found gains on each of the four subtests significant beyond the .001 level of confidence. However, the greatest mean gains were not in Verbal Meaning (91.14 to 95.35), as one might have pre- dicted in light of large PPVT score gains reported by other investiga- tors, but in Perceptual Speed (83.41 to 92.50) and Spatial Relations (79.55 to 87.02).These findings tend to support Porter's findings that performance on the Seguin Form Board (a measure of learning rate) seemed to be the most consistent indicator of impact of Head Start experience. Kerns (Reference 66) of the West Virginia Department of Mental 'Health is directing a study in which all West Virginia children who par- ticipated in Summer 1965 Head Start were tested on the Culture Fair In- telligence Test at the beginning of the program.One-third of the children

IV-89 were posttested.At present, preliminary pre- and posttestresults are available for only one countycomprised of 50 Head Start children,al- though 52 counties will eventuallyprovide data.There was a mean gain of 20 points in the pre-and post-Culture Fair .test scoresfor the sample. Although statistical analyses werenot reported, it appears that thegains may be significant. Jacobs and Shafer(Reference 62) of the University of Oklahoma studied "Some Effects ofProject Head Start on 178 Children in Cleveland County, Oklahoma" on grantfunds supplied by the ExtensionDivision of the University ofOklahonia.This study was of special interest because it compared the impact ofHead Start on "high-risk" and"low-risk" sub- groups of HeadStart children.The "risk" factor was defined by pre- Head Start IQ's, as determinedby the Goodenough Draw-A-Person test; "high-risk" children were considered tobe those with an obtained IQ on the DAP of 85 or less.The high-risk group of Head Startchildren was tested with the Stanford-Binet testin addition to the DAP. For the total group (based on apopulation of 162 children for whom pre- and posttests wereobtained), the mean Goodenough IQincreased from an initial 89.41 to 94.45 atthe end of the program; this increase was significant atthe .001 level of ,confidence. In addition tothis anal- ysis of total group scorechange, the investigators determined thatthe children whose pretest scores werebelow the total group mean of 89 had a mean gainof 11.46 IQ points, as comparedwith a mean gain of only 1.89 IQ points for the childrenwhose initial DAP scores were above the total group mean of 89.This finding substantiates theHorowitz-Rosenfeld (Reference 59) finding that developmentalchanges for middle-class chil- dren are not reflected over ashort time period on some cognitive measures. One especially pertinent findingstressed in the Jacobs andShaer study was that 37 percent of theHead Start children obtained anIQ of 85 or less onthe first Draw-A-Person test.This percentage is far in excess of normal expectancy, so it canbe assu.ned.that the HeadStart program (in Cleveland County) actuallydid reach many children who werelagging developinentally.

IV-90 Two of the analyses performed by Jacobsand Shafer were purely descriptive and related children's performance onpre-DAP test scores to other variables such as theincome of the family or the educational level of the parents.Their descriptive findings are summarizedin Exhibits IV-37 and IV-38. A second study, in addition to that ofJacobs and Shafer, which es- sentially divided the Head Startchildren into high- and low-risk groups (e. g., groups less likely and morelikely to succeed in school) was "An Evaluation of the Knox County,Kentucky Project Head Start," by Psy- chological Associates, Ltd.,(Reference 4).The two subgroups (total- ing 42 children) represented a veryisolated and deprived area (Dewitt) and a considerably less isolatedand deprived area-environment (Knox Central). Although most of the measuresdealt with psychological, emotional, and social assessment,there were a few cognitive measures (Draw-A-Person and Draw-A-Circle, Square andDiamond). There were no significantdifferences reported from pre- to posttesting on any of the cognitive tasks for either groin). Berlin (Reference 6), University ofWashington, obtained Pre- school Inventory pre- and post-HeadStart measures on 148 children and found significant gains on each ofthe seven subtest areas taken sepa- rately, as well as on the combined areas.Of a possible score of 304, the pretest mean was 197 and the posttest mean was215.These score changes support Caldwell's expressed hopethat the PSI scores would be sensitive to change rather than resistant tochange. Silberstein (Reference 97) directed a study inwhich 61 Head Start children were pre- and posttested on several measures,both objective and subjective.I The Ammons Full Range Picture VocabularyTest (AFRPVT) and the DraW-A-Person (DAP) test wereused to determine development in

1Silberstein's data havebeen submitted on a preliminaiy4wei's; more complete analyses are currently under way onthe data presented here, as well as onfollow-up Head Start and control dataobtained in Decem- ber 1965.

W-91 EXHIBIT IV-37 SUMMARY OFPRE-DAP SCORES--GROUPED BY FAMILY INCOME LEVEL (JACOBSAND SHAFER)

Family Income Number of Subjects Mean Pre-DAP IQ A.59000+ 49 95.33(1) B.3,000 to 4,999 62 91.16 C.0 to 2,999 46 83.00(1)

Note:(1) The differences in IQ betweenA-C and B-C were significant.

EXHIBIT IV-38. SUMMARY OFPRE-DAP SCORES -- GROUPED BY . PARENTS' EDUCATION (JACOBSAND SHAFER;

Highest Le vet of Education Attained by Either Parent Number of Subjects Mean Pre-DAP IQ A. College 30 98.07(1) B, High School Graduate 53 90.75. C.9th to 11th Grade 60 89.23 D.0 to 8th Grade 25 80.40(1)

Note:(1) The difference in IQ between Aand D was significant. terms of mental age.A mean mental ageof 58 months (4 years,10 mental months) was obtainedfor both tests.The post-Head Start mean ages for theAmmons and theDAP respectively were72 months and 63 marked. months, 'thus indicatingthat verbal skill improvementwas very The MetropolitanReadiness Tests forreading and arithmetic were given, with raw scoreresults as follows:

Pretest Posttest Possible Score Reading 30.5 35.2 66 Numbers 5.4 6.5 24

On the PSI, thechildren's scares shiftedfrom a pre-Head Start in which the mean of128.0 to a post-meanof 142.1, in a scoring system PSI highest possible scoreobtainable was 210.Exhibit IV-39 details the results reported .1 On the Bender-GestaltReproductions, a measureof perceptual-motor development, the HeadStart children obtained apreprogram mean age- 43 months. equivalent of 40 months(3 years, 4 months)and a post-mean of Silberstein-commented thatwhile this 3-monthgain over the 6-week pro- was not on gram isprobably not significant,the focus of the program motor skills but onverbal skills. Two other studies,bOth conducted by publicschools and locally funded, reported pre-and post-Head Startdevelopment in terms of Metropolitan ReadinessTest scores. In a report entitled"Growth of Pupils inProject Head Start as 17), the author Measured by theMetropolitan ReadinessTest" (Reference children fell in of a local studyindicated that a group of654 Head Start week the tenth percentile onthe readiness testsadministered in July, 1

1There discrepancy, betweenthe reported pre-. and is a slight unexplained and post-means (128.0and 142.1) and theadded totals-of-subtest pre- post-means(132.2 and 146.9).

W-93 EXHIBIT IV-39PSI TEST RESULTS (SILBERSTEIN)(1)

Concept Area Pre-Mean. Post-Mean Possible Score Personal orientation 7.4 9.2 15 Body image 15.6 17.0 20 Number concepts 13.6 15.1 25 General information 17.9 24.1 36 Visual discrimination/ association 21.0 21.1 36 Relationships: similarities/ differences 13.0 15.0 19 Following instructions 32.8 34.1 43 Comprehension of social roles 10.9 11.3 16 Total 132.2 146.9 210

Note:(1) N = 61. after the program began. In September,1 week after school had begun, the same children were retested andfell in the twenty - eighthpercentile. Exhibit IV-40 summarizes some ofthe results reported. Another local study (Reference55) reported Metropolitan. Readiness Test score shifts for 47 HeadStart children and comparedthe results with scores obtainedby a control group composedof all first-grade children in the town. Exhibit IV-41summarizes the reported results. Tests of significanceindicated that the post-Head Startand control scores werecomparable, but that there weresignificant differences be- tween the pre-HeadStart and control group mean scores.

(3), Relationships Between Tests andStudies The large number ofindependently designed and operated research projectswhich studied Head Start duringthe Summer 1965 program can be considered" amixed blessing. On onehand, infor- mation was obtained on testinstruments a; well as onchildren; not only did different investigatorschoose different kinds of measures tosupple- ment the regular Head Starttest program, but alsoseveral investigators obtained a variety of test results ontheir samples of children--apractice which provided good informationfor determining how the samechildren performed on different testswhich were designed to measuresimilar kinds of functioning. Onthe other hand, the widemixture and range of experimental conditions ofteninhibits the making of validcomparative or differentialassessments of Head Startin relation to certain factors of interest (e. g., programvariables, sample characteristics,and test variables). First, let us look at theresearch studies which includedseveral different tests to assess cognitive orgeneral functioning of a given group of children-. Because of the many variationsin sample characteristics(such as age) and methods of reporting scores,only data which were converted to and reported in terms of IQ scorepoints have beenincluded in the fol- lowing discussion. For many purposes,raw scores are verydesirable,

IV - 9 5 EXHIBIT IV-40 METROPOLITAN READINESS TEST RESULTS(A)

Poor Risk Low Normal Average High Normal Superior N % N % N % N % -N To-...... Pre-Head Start 254 39 270 41 89 17 35 5 6 1 Early September 120 18 265 41 172 26 70 11 27 4

EXHIBIT IV-41METROPOLITAN READINESS TEST RESULTS (B)

Test Area Group Reading Numbers Total Pre-Head Start 1.77 2.11 1.74 Post-Head Start 2.79 3.36 2.89 Control (first grade) 2.91 3.49 3.06 meaningful and interpretable, butfor the present purpose, the variation in children's age is reasonenoufa to look at converted scores which, by definition, take age factorsinto account.Thus, those studies which re- ported intelligence testresults in terms of subtest scores only, raw scores, or partial scores,or in any irregular manner,have been omitted from the presentdiscussion. Bergerts research study(Reference 5), discussed earlier, used four tests--the PPVT, theStanford-Binet (S-B), the Leiter International, and the Raven Progressive Matrices1--to obtain intertest correlations. For 59 Head Start children, preprogram meanIQ scores of 83, 92, and 84 were obtained on the PPVT,Stanford-Binet, and Leiter International, respectively.The PPVT, then, yielded the lowest meanIQ score, and the Stanford-Binet the highest, onthe same group of children, with all tests given during the same testperiod. The mean IQ scorechanges2 (from pre- to post-Head Start) were +9.75,+3.4, and +10.55 on the PPVT, S-B, and Leiter.While the mean gains were statistically sig- nificant for all tests, it is evidentthat the PPVT and Leiter reflected the most change, while the S-B scoreremained more stable or constant. The development of vocabulary anti verbalskills during Head Start may haveaccounted for the large increase in PPVT scores.Since the Leiter Internatioral is a nonverbal testwhich requires the child to match objects (for color, size, etc.), onemight speculate that in this particular Head Start program, special emphasis mayhave been given to concept development. In any case, the posttestPPVT and Leiter scores were much closer to the post-Head Start S-B scores. Eisenberg (Reference 33) obtained both PPVT andDAP scores on one group of about500 children (including Head Startand non-Head Start children). When the mean scores wereconverted to IQ estimates, the

1 The Raven scores were not given as IQ scores. . 2Score changeswere based on posttestingof 20 children selected randomly from the total sample of59.

IV - 9 7 PPVT yielded a somewnat lowerIQ than the DAP at the outset of Head Start.Exhibit IV -42 shows the IQ scores. Eisenberg found an intertest(DAP-PPVT) correlation of .39, and suggested that the two tests "aresampling different aspects of intelligence:' In order to demonstrate the extent towhich the PPVT and DAP were "instruments sensitive to the deficitsin the culturally disadvantaged child rather than... accurate measures ofhis overall cognitive functioning," Eisenberg (Reference 33) tested subsamplesof culturally disadvantaged children with other measures.Exhibit IV-43 summarizes his results, It can be seen that, inEisenberg's studies, whenever more than one IQ estimate wasobtained on a given sample of culturallydisadvan:- taged children, the 'PVT IQestimate was lower than otherestimates, while the S-B and CM1',IS providedsomewhat higher estimates. These findings agree with those ofBerger, who also obtained lower PPVT IQ estimates for Head Startchildren, as compared with estimates obtained from two other IQ tests. Johnson (Reference 64) obtained IQestimates on 79 children using both the PPVT and the DAP. Hisfindings are shown in Exhibit IV-44. In this study, while the pre-PPVT meanIQ score was considerably below the pre-DAP mean score,the greatest gains during Head Start were reflected on the DAP. This apparentinconsistency (i.e., the DAPreflect- ing more change) is not toosurprising in view of the factthat most of the children in Johnson's study wereMexican-American (62 out of 79).There was, infact, a 36-point differentialbetween the estimated PPVT andDAP IQ scores on the pretests forthe Mexican-American group. Ozer (Reference 82) obtainedboth PPVT and S-B test results onhis sample of 65 x;:hildren, but he reportedthe PPVT results in raw score points. For purposes of the presentdiscussion wherein IQ estimates ob- tained from different sources forthe same children are compared, wehave roughly converted Ozer's PPVT raw scoresto IQ scores. Thechildren's approximate mean chronological ageof 5 years, 1 month, placesthem in thePVT standardization age groupof 4.-9 to 5-5.The IQ estimates are shown in Exhibit IV-45.It can be seen that. in Ozer' 3study,: also the

IV -98 illitagemgre

EXHIBIT IV-42IQ COMPARISONS,'DAP AND PPVT(EISENBERG)

,IQ Estimate DAP PPVT .... Pre-Head Start 76 (N= 500) 68 (N = 424) Head Start, September 82 (N = 435) 81 (N = 413)

TESTS EXHIBIT IV-43IQ SCORES OBTAINEDON DIFFERENT (EISENBERG)

Mean IQ Time of (1) Sample/Subsample DAP Description Test N PPVT CMMS S-B Total Sample Head Start Pre-Head Start 500 68(2) 76

Subsample

a Non-Head Start, 91.3 disadvantaged NR(3) 23 65.5 83.9 Non-Head Start, disadvantaged Sept. 48 70.5 85.9 Head Start Sept. 48 76.7 94.8 Pre-Head Head Start 69.1 84.8 84.2 Start 34 Head Start Post-Head Start 72.8 88.2 90.2

Notes:(1) Columbia MentalMaturity Scale. (2) N = 424; basicallyfrom the, first 500subjects. (3) Not specified inreport.

IV-99 EXHIBIT IV-44 PPVT AND DAP IQ RESULTS(JOHNSON)

Mean IQ Estimate N PPVT MP- Pre-Head Start 79 72 92 Post-Head Start 79 79 111

EXHIBIT IV-45 PPVT AND S-B IQESTIMATES (OZER)

t PPVT PPVT Estimated IQ for S-B N Raw ScoreCA = 4.9 to 5-5 IQ Early in Head Start 65 35.85 74 88.78 Midprogram .65 Late in program 65 41.64 P.,

. IV-100 considerably belowthat ob- meanpre-Head Start PPVT IQestimate was tained with the S-B. (Reference 5), Eisen- In addition to thefour investigators--Berger (Reference 82) berg (Reference 33),Johnson (Reference64), and Ozer obtain IQ esti- who used the PPVTand at least oneother instrument to DAP to examine mates, Jacobs andShafer (Reference62) used the S-B and obtained S-B IQ the problem of intertestconstancy ofmeasured IQ. They of 85 or below on the scores on56 children whohad obtained IQ scores between the pre- DAP, in order todetermine thecorrelation coefficient Stanford-Binet. DAP and theStanford-Binet, andthe post-DAP and the administered only once.) (It should be notedthat theStanford-Binet was Although the pre- andpost-DAP mean scoresyielded significant gains, with the Stanford- both the pre- andpostscorescorrelated significantly +.49 and +.64 Binet at the .01level of confidence.(The correlations were respectively. for the Stanford-Binetwith the pre-DAPand.the post-DAP, for whom The correlations werebased on a totalpopulation of 56 children, obtained a all three testresults wereobtained; i.e., all56 children had pre-Head Start DAPIQ score of 85 orless.) The rangeof the pre-DAP 71.5, and the rangeof IQ scoresextended from 40 to85 with a mean of The Stanford.. the post-DAP scoreswas 53 to117 with a meanof 84.09. Binet range was46 to 118 with a meanof 85.27. of actual It is also interestingto observe theshift in the ranges (from scores. Thelower score limitof the DAP shiftedfrom 40 to 53 Follow- pre- toposttest) and the upperlimit was raisedfrom 85 to 117. for the DAP wasmuch ing Head Startexperience, the IQ score range more likethat for the S-B. reported, in There were, then,five independentresearchers who cognitive terms of IQ scores,the results of morethan one test of

IV - 101 :4411inftwistruilwilowir

functioning for a given group.Without exception, all S-B testresults yielded higher mean IQ scores.1 These findings areinterpretable in several ways.Whereas the 2 PPVT is recognized pimarily as a test of verbalability,and cultur- ally disadvantaged chiidren tend to haveless verbal skill, the S-B pre- sumably covers a wider range ofabilities, so the child has more op- portunities to demonstratecapability.It may be that, for somechildren, tests which areintended to be simple,in terms of limiting the complexity of required responses, arereally a handicap.If the child is unable to make the particularkind of response needed,he has no opportunity to demonstrate whateverability he does have on atask more compatible with his abilitiS. It is also possiblethat the inconstancy of IQ scoresobtained from the PPVT another tests is a function ofthe nature of thestandardization samples of the tests.The PPVT was standardized on afairly limited sample of 4,012 whitechildren in and aroundNashville, Tennessee. Since PPVT raw scores areconverted to IQ scores inrelation to the perfor- mance ofthe white Nashvillechildren, the low PPVT IQ scoresof the children in the HeadStart studies may meanmerely that the culturally disadvantaged childrenhave a considerably differentvocabulary from that of the Nashvillechildren. A final point to bementioned concerns the raw scoreincrease over expected to time,and particularly thequestion of the gains in raw scores particular. significance 0 C Cr "naturally" overtime.This problem takes on

It is interesting to notthat the S-B means inall cases were higherthan the mean IQ of 80.7(S.D. = 12.4) obtainedby Kennedy, Vazi deRiet, and White when they usedthe Binet, Form L-M, toobtain normative data on 1,800 Negro elementaryschool children in fivesoutheastern states (Ref- erence65). 2See Dunn's discussion of validity (Reference30). See also an interesting critique of the PPVT typeof test by Church,(Reference 26, pages 172ff).

IV-102 when (1) the time intervalis fairly short, (2)expected gains aresmall, and (3) the reliability ofthe test is uncertainfor the populationand ad- ministration conditionsinvolved. Unfortunately, there are toofew data to deal withthe question ade- quately at this time.The Expanded PPVTManual (Reference 30),cites three studies with fifthgrade, seventh grade,and preschool children showing that IQ pointchanges averaged lessthan two points over a 2- to 7-day interim betweentest periods.Inspection of the Manualindicates that, in general, inthe raw score rangeof 23 to 52, one raw scorepoint usually accounts forabout 1 month of mental age.Thus, over an 8-week period, one mightreasonably expect anapproximate gain of two raw score points on the PPVT.Allerhand (Reference2), in studying thefeasibility administer the PPVT andPSI of using indigenousHead Start parents to (Preschool Inventory),obtained test-retest scoreswith a 5-day interim. Allerhandrs study did notreport actual scores,but it did indicate thatthe mean test-retestraw scoredifference on the PPVT was2.1, which was not statisticallysignificant./ It appears thatthe evidence concerningtest-retest reliabilityof the PPVT, especiallywith culturallydisadvantaged children, islimited and inconclusive, but there are someindications that it is afairly stable measurewhen the test-retestinterval is small. It seems fitting toclose this subsectionby quoting the final para- graph in "Guidelinesfor Testing Minority.Group Children." Theprob- lems of test purposes,interpretation, and uses aresimply and succinctly put (Reference45, page 144). In testing the minority groupchild it is sometimes ap- propriate to comparehis performance withthat of advantaged children to determine themagnitude of thedeprivation to be overcome. Atother times it isappropriate to comparehis test performancewith that of otherdisadvantaged childrento

1Howeveri PSI test-retest scores werestatistically he did find that the different significantly different.Pierce-Jones alsoobtained significantly PSI results over atest-retest interimof about 11 days.

IV-103 determine his relative deprivation in coraparisonwith others who have also been denied good homes, goodneighborhoods, good diets, good schools and goodteachers. In most instances it is especially appropriate to comparethe child's test per- formance with his previous testperformam:e.Utilizing the individual ch;ld as his own controland using the test norms principally as "bench marks," we arebest able to gauge the success of ourefforts to move the minority group child for- ward on the long, hard roadof overcoming the deficiencies which have been forced uponhim. Many comparisons depend upon tests, butthey also depend upon our intelligence, our good will, and our sense ofresponsibility to make the proper comparison at the proper time and toundertake proper re- medial, and compensatory action as aresult.The misuse of tests wah minority groupchildren, or :In any situation, is a serious breach of professional ethics.Their proper use is a sign of professionaland personal maturity. (4) Summar} In summary, 20 investigators, most of whom were funded by the Office of Economic Opportunity, assessedthe impact of Operation Head Start on participating children during the summer of 1965 in the areaof cognitive an.d general development. A number of instruments were used, including thePeabody Picture Vo- cabulary Test, the Stanford-Binet, the GoodenoughDraw-A-Person test, the Seguin Form Board, the PrimaryMental Abilities test, the Caldwell-Soule Preschool Inventory, and the Metropolitan Readiness Tests. Although there was wide variation in experimental design, and research considerations often were somewhat strained due to practical considerations, all studies without exception indicated some degree or kind of positive changes in the Head Start children. The results of studies which were able to compare Head Start groups with non-Head Start,middle-class, or non-culturally deprived children, tended to suggest that, while the Head Start children them- selves made significant gains during the prcgram (that is,with each child considered as his own control), they were still somewhat(often significantly) below their middle-class peers, with whom they would be competing in school. However, when the control group wasdrawn from the same sort of population from which the HeadStart children were drawn, the post-Head Start children's scores werehigher than those of the control children, indicating that the Head Startchildren were indeed more school-ready thantheir disadvantaged peers, although not as school-ready as their non-deprived peers. The veryfew studies that obtained comparative test data on both Head Start andnon-Head Start children who had been in school (kindergarten) for about2 months suggested that by the end of about 2 months of school,the 2 groups of children were again comparable on some measures. It may be noted that there is some indicationin the research literature that differences between Negro'and whitechildren are much less noticeable in the early years. Osborne(Reference 81), using

IV-105 intelligence and academicachievement tests,reported.widenig gaps between Negro and white children asthey grew older.In Deutsch's study (Reference 28),white-Negro intelligence testcomparisons were analyzedin relationship tograde in school (first orfifth) and partici- pation in some preschoolactivity. He reported"presence or lack of preschool experience atgrade 5 more highlydifferentiates intelligence. test scores than itdoes at grade 1" (p.33).Obviously, long-range follow-up studies are neededto evaluate suchdifferences. Because of the widevariation in data collectionand reporting, it is difficult to makesophisticated interstudycomparisons of test results.But despite the manyproblems discussedconcerning the meaning and interpretationof the various reports, it seemsclear from the independentstudies that children whoparticipated in Head Start changed somewhat in the courseof the program.It would seem, as manyinvestigators pointed out,that changes can and doin fact occur to some measurableextent, even during such ashort (6 to 8 weeks) program asHead Start. The degreeand kind of change seem to vary at least with the typeof measurement madeand the initial level of the . group, butchanges suggestingimprovement in cognitivefunctioning are quite consistentlyreported. 3. Emotional and Social Areas The data concerningimpact in areas ofemotional and social development appeared to be soclosely interrelated thatfor this report all information availablein these impact areaswill be included in the present section, ratherthan separately. In this section, theavailable evidence relating tothe impact of Head Start on children'sschool behavior, schouladjustment, personal adjustment, emotionalhealth, fantasy life,self-identity, personality traits or characteristics,social behavior, andsocial experiences will be discussed. One of the Head Startinventories designed to assesschanges in "emotional adjustment," "social maturity,"and "cognitive activity"

IV-106 was the Behavior Inventory (BI), which was scheduled for administration both early and late in the Head Start program. Berlin (Reference 6) in- cluded the BI among the several measures he used to evaluate Head Start.For 131 children, he found significant pre- and post-Head Start improvement in only one area, "cognitive activity." Significant changes in "emotional adjustment" and "social maturity" were not reflected by the Behavior Inventory. The study in Knox County, Kentucky, conducted by Psychological Associates, Ltd. (Reference 4), was particularly concerned with changes in noncognitive areas. Seven measures for assessing certain psychologi- cal and emotional characteristics before and after the Head Start program were used. These included: (1) interactions (e.g., smiling at the exami- ner and eating proffered candy); (2) interview (10 simple questions, such as: "What is your name?"); (3) sociometric choice (the child scores if he can identify any friend by name); (4) three wishes (an index of goals and desires); (5) draw-a-man (an index of self-esteem); (6) three com- missions (directions for three simple tasks given in one instruction); and (7) drawing (a circle, a square, and a diamond figure). Results of the pre- and post-Head Start evaluations of Knox County Head Start children were analyzed for the total group as well as for two subgroups representing levels of environmental isolation and deprivation. For the subgroup analyses at the beginning of Head Start, the two groups differed significantly on all measures except sociometric choice, self- esteem, and drawings (circle and diamond figures).The less deprived and isolated children (Knox Central) scored better than the more deprived Dewitt children, who appeared to be (1) more socially inept, (2) less aware of their peers,. (3) less concerned with specific goals and desires, and (4) less able to integrate and respond to stimuli from their environ- ment. At the end of Head Start, however, the two subgroups were com- parable on all measures except "three wishes, " where the Knox Central group continued to express more concern with goals and desires than the more deprived Dewitt group. The total-group analyses (i. e,,both programs or subgroups combined) indicated statistically significant

IV-107 &-

pre- and post-HeadStart gains on all measuresof psychological, emotional, and social development. Silberstei.n's study (Reference97) included a pre- and post-Head Start behavior rating scale, aswell as the measures of cognitive develop- ment discussed earlier.The observations were madeby a trained ob- server andby the child's teacher; a5 - point scale was used to rate cooperation with adults,aggressive reactions, ability topostpone gratification, restraint of motoractivity, type of motor activity,verbal skills, and quality of speech.Although analysis of the data has not yet been completed and statisticalsignificances have not been computed, the mean scale-point gainsshow a consistent trend toward morepositive ratings during the latter partof the program.1 A provision for ratings ofbehavior changes for Head Start chil- dren was included inEisenberg's studies (discussed earlier inthis section). Although detailed enumerationand analyses of the ratings were notavailable at the time he submittedhis preliminary report, Eisenberg did comment that HeadStart children showed positive(fa- vorable) changes in their orientation toclinical testing.In the report on one ofhis studies, Eisenberg stated: It was very clearly observedby the examiners that Head Start youngsters were much moreable to re- spond to adult instructions than werethe controls. Their attention span was definitelylonger and their ability to comprehend complexinstructions was superior. Eisenberg's subjective observations weresupported by a similar statement made by Jacobsand Shafer (Reference62) concerning quali- tative changes in Head Startchildren during the program: In a large number of cases, agreater degree of organization, a greater accuracyof perception, and, in general, better formquality were found (for the DAP drawings). On thebasis of clinical

fell somewhere . AOn a 5-point scale, all pre-and post-mean ratings in the range of 3.0 to 3.8.The mean differences rangedfrom .1 to .4.

Iv-108 impression, it could be inferred that the child had grown in his ability todifferentiate himself and his world, and had formed afiner sense of iden- tity during his participation in this (Head Start) program. A report of the San Diego Cityschools' Summer 1965 Head Start program included theresults of two questionnaires which related to the psychological and emotionalwell-being of the children (Reference 10 The first questionnaire wasdirected to 29 parent helpers and indi- cated a unanimous acceptanceof the program. In response to the question, "How were children helped?"the following were listed: better school ad- justment, growth in social adjustmentand cooperation, self-control, and security and independence. The second San Diego questionnaire wasdirected to present (Fall 1965) kindergarten teachers of children who hadparticipated in the Sum- mer 1965Operation Head Start.Of the 23 teachers who responded, 92 percent said that the HeadStart children seemed to be more readyfor school than non-Head Start children; oneteacher said that she noticed no difference.Eighty-seven percent indicated that in theiropinions, prekindergarten experience positively(favorably) affected their classes, while 13 percent noticed no apparenteffect of prekindergarten experience. Specific characteristics of children withpreschool experiences were given by the 87 percenfas betterlanguage development, better social adjustment, better conformity to groupbehavior and routine, and longer attention span with better listeningability. Zimmerman's study (Reference 112) also included a measureof school readiness.In September, a rating questionnaire wasgiven to kindergarten teachers of 59 children whohad participated in Head Start and 59 children who had not participatedin the summer 1965 program. The children, whose median IQ's weresimilar, reflected significant differences in school behavior; HeadStart children did better (at a.001 level of confidence) in all four ratedcategbries: Individual Behavior, Group Behavior, Mental Alertness,and Physical Traits.

IV-109 Two independent studies, one from Warminster Township (Refer- ence 52), and one directed by Harding (Reference 47), reported their analysis of the item on Head Start's Paid and Voluntary Worker's Evaluation Form ("I feel that, in general, children attending the Oper- ation Head Start program were changed in the following ways ..."). Exhibit IV-46 summarizes the responses reported in the two studies and presents a picture of considerable improvement in some behaviors, in the opinion of the Head Start staff in these programs. Johnson.'s study (Reference 64) of the Clavis Montessori schools reported significant changes in children. during Head Start, as deter- mined by teacher ratings of social-emotional characteiistics such as "attitude toward school," "adjustr.lent to group situations," "independence from mother," and "child's interest in program." From Hawaii, Er lith Doi (Reference 29) reported that 250 kinfler- garten teachers rated children after hey had been in the kindergarten classes for 2 months. The majority of teachers rated the Head Start .children as similar to non-Head Start children on all items of school adjustment. However, not all investigators found such enthusiastic response by kindergarten teachers to the behavior of their "Head Start graduates." In interviews with 22 kindergarten teachers (representing 17 classrooms) where Head Starters were enrolled in the fall of 1965, Van Egmond (Reference 105) found a somewhat cooler view of the children's classroom behavior. A summary of the interviews is shown in Exhibit IV-47. It is clear from Exhibit IV-47 that some kindergarten teachers did not see the Head Start children as easy to manage. However, Van Egmond included Lae following comment in his report. Although the teachers did not tend to view this (unruly) behavior in a favorable light, their comments provided ample indication that chil- dren who participated in the summer program were inclined to actively explore the environ- ment of the classroom, to try out materials and equipment, to feel at ease in the kindergar- ten situation, to be more outgoing, and to ver- balize and request help or information from the teacher to a much greater degree than children who did not have the Head. Start experience. IV-110 EXHIBIT IV-46IMPACT ON SOCIAL BEHAVIOR (HARDING, WARMINSTER TOWNSHIP)

Percent Teachers Rating Much rho Much Social Behavior Item Better Better ChangeWorseWorse Getting along with other children A(1)69 31 WIN

B(2)55 44 MMI Self-confidence A 56 44 B 44 55

Speaking ability A 56 44 OM MID B 33 66 ME, ME, OM Can do things on his own A 56 44 B 33 66 ft&

Everyday manners A 50 38 12 N1D NI* B 66 33 MEI

Interest in new things A 38 62 MMI B 55 44 - ea Does what he is told A 31 56 6 6 .1 B 44 55 - *es

Finishes what he starts A 19 69 12 MMI ORD B 33 44 22 111 MMI

Notes:(1) Group A. refers to Harding's study, and represents 16 Head Start staff members. (2) Group B refers to Warminster County, and represents 9 Head Start staffers. EXHIBIT IV-47 IMPACT ON CLASSROOM BEHAVIOR (VAN EGMOND)

Question or Item Number and Description of Responses 1. Any items in which Head 13 No 4 Yes 5 Cannot Start children seem to be Gener- better prepared for school alize as compared with non- Head Start children?

2.Degree to which children 16 With 1 Better 5 No Dif- accept classroom routine Diffi- ficulty and limits culty

3.Participation in learning 22 No Difference or Cannot Generalize task set by teacher 4.Social relationships 8 No Dif-8 Better 6 Cannot with other children ference Gener- alize

5. Any clisacivantag. e_ 6 No 16 Early Management attributable to Head Problems Start? In addition to the teacher interviews, Van Egmond's study in- cluded classroom observation and rating of behavior using Bellack's formulation in which school is viewed as a game, and the players (chil- dren) are either "with it" or "not with it." Head Start children who were enrolled inkindergarten in the fall of 1965 and non-Head Start children were matched by sex and presence in the same classroom. There were 50 Head Startboys and 50 non- Head Start boys, as well as 54 Head Start girls and 54 non-Head Start girls involved in the study. Van Egnxond found a substantial similarity in the extent towhich both Head Start and non-Head Start kindergarten childrenresponded to task requirements and classroom socialsituations.Consistent with findings in child development studies, he found girls (Head Start and non-Head Start) more adaptive to the class;:oom than boys. Porter (Reference 87) reported observations of 31 Head Start and 35 non-Head Start children made on the first or secondday of the fall 1965 school year.Lii.e the psychometric evaluations done for the study, all ratings were made by trained observers whodid not know which children had participated in Head Start and which had not. In general, Head Start children found it somewhatdifficult to fit into the structured first 2 days of class; they showedresistance to being led from one activity to another at the behest of theteacher. Porter warned that the findings of differences between the two groups weretoo meager, despite the generalizationsabove, to permit conclusions; it did seem evident, however, that the Head Startexperience did not pre- pare these children for anexceptionally well-adjusted "first day in school." Holmes and Holmes (Reference 58) reported two studieswhich shed light o1 the areas of emotional and socialdevelopment. A series of observations on 32 Head Start childrenearly and late in the program gave evidence of somesignificant favorable changes in goal orientation, goal attainment, expressedself-esteem, reaction to frustration, and emotional reaction.It should be noted that the changes were observed duringthe Head Start program, and not in kindergarten. IV-113 The second study used a projective technique (similar to the TAT, or Thematic Appelception. Test) inwhich the children were asked to tell a story about each of three pictures.This study used both Head Start and non-Head 5t4 1, J. children (with 29 children in each group) so that comparisons could be made between the two groups to determine the effect of Head Start cn the children's fantasies about their peers, adults, and play, ingeneral.'The projective test was administered twice to all children; the first time period was in August after the Head Start pro- gram and the second was inNovember after both groups had been in kindergarten for about 2 months. Unfortunately, no pre-HeadStart' measurements were made in this study.However, extensive compa- rability checks indicated that the two groups would havebeen fairly similar in June (pre-Head Start).The results of the scorings are summarized in some detail in Exhibit IV-48. The detail presented in Exhibit IV-48 will permit carefulinspection of the results, in regard to the provocativefindings reported by Holmes and Holmes. They f-)und that in August the children whohad participated in Head Start were significantly more positive (or adjusted)in all four categories, but in November the two groups were comparable in all four categories.Thus, it would appear that while Head Start had an impressive impact on the children in emotional areas asmeasured by the TAT-like projective technique, this impact wasabout the same as the impact of 2 months in school on the same areas innon-Head Start children. An interesting clinical finding reported by the investigators re- sulted when they compared the projective test resultswith the behavior- observation ratings fOr the Head Start children.(Non-Head Start chil- dren were not rated on behavior, as those ratings weremade only during the Head Start program. ) On the ratingsof actual behavior, the Head

1 There was substantial comparability between the childrenin the two - groups on the followingcriteria: lack of previous school experience (none for either group), age (mean age of 5 years, 5months, for each group), sex, ethnic background, presence of parents,and education . and occupation of major parental wage-earuer. IV-114 C.73 C."M C=21 IC= C:=3 C7.3 C:=1 C:771 r"--1 C77.1 r-11 Nieman.irrprimmESIP 1)-1 ' EXHIBIT IV-48 SUMMARY OF PROJECTIVE TEST RESULTS (HOLMES AND HOLMES) r"-1 rn T-1 Head Start (N = 29) Non-Head Start (N = 29) November August November j August A.Category/Characteristic mentio.i.edmainactionQuality characters between of in inter- 36+ 40 - 380 Total 114 25+ 35 - 340 Total 94 28 22 - 59 0 Total109 30 32 - 30 0 Total 92 B. ' mentofDegreestory main on the ofcharacters invest- part 75 39 0 114 60 34 0 94 55 54 0 109 55 37 0 92 C. Effectinactivity story with was which invested the 34 45 31.35 114 34 42 18 94 30 2127 5450 107 3130 3132 3032 9294 D. tivetivityDegree or was destructiveto whichconstruc- ac- 48 35 114 35 42 17 94 . 34 109 Start children showed a considerable shift to less aggressive social be- havior over the course of their Head Start involvement (in July and August), but the projective technique indicated significantly more aggres- siveness in their fantasy life, as compared with the non-Head Start children (who were very similar to the Head Start children on several criteria) in August.It would appear, according to Holmes and Holmes, that during a socializing experience like Head Start (or kindergarten), the children become more able to bring hostile impulses and behavior under control. . The largest reported independent study concerned with social development was directed by Lamb at the University of Delaware. His study involved more than two-thirds of the Head Start children in that state.There were both experimental (Head Start) and control (non- Head Start) groups; both groups received two tests. The 770 Head Start children were given a shortened version of the Self-Social Sym- bolsTasks'before and after their program participation; the 100 control group children, who were somewhat comparable to the Head Start group on many variables, including general socio-economic back- grounds, were tested in their homes during the same two time periods that the Head Start children were being tested.Exhibit IV-49 sum= niarizes some selected sample characteristics. Two of the primary purposes of the study were: (1) to determine to what extent the development of self-social constructs of the Head Start children would differ from that of a somewhat comparable control group, and -(2) to determine to what extent,if at all, Head Start children would develop "appropriate social trust."2 Statistical analyses revealed significant differences between the Head Start and non-Head Start children on only 2 of the 11 tasks: (1) the Head Start children increasingly identified with the teacher and saw

1The Self-Social Symbols Tasks were developed by Ziller, Alexander, and Long in 1964, at the University of Delaware to measure self and self-other relationships. 2For discussitut of other aspects of Lamb's study, see subsection IV. E.

IV-116 EXHIBIT IV-49 SELECTED SAMPLECHARACTERISTICS (LAMB)

Male Female Age WhiteNegroWhiteNegroTotalFourFiveSixseven Head Start N 129 239 106 296 770 25 325 385 35 % 16.7 31.2 13.7 38.4 100 3.2 42.250.0 4.6 ControlN 27 40 12 21 100 19 46 31 4 % 27.0 40.0 12.0 21.0 100 19.046.031.0 4.0

IV-117 her as less threatening, and(2) Head Start childrenshifted significantly toward a perception of self assimilar to others (as opposed todifferent from others). Other comparisons betweenthe two groups, while notyielding statistically significant results, were seenby.the investigator as evidence of substantial andinteresting trends. For example,there was a tendencyfor Head Start childrentoward a balanced power per- ception of the policeman, aswell as the teacher. With respectto variables of sex and race,white females appeared togain the most from their Head Startexperience in the developmentof self and self- other constructs. Asstated by Lamb, "thosechildren who changed little, if at all, in theirself and self-other constructs wereNegro males. White male andNegro female children fellsomewhere between those two groups in theirdevelopment." A further finding wasdescribed in the following way:"Head Start children initially hadunrealistic perceptions of thesharing situation. As a result ofthe daily interpersonal giveand take in Head Start, more nearlynormal, realistic perceptionsand attitudes were developed with regard tosocial trust." In addition to the 15independent researchers whosestudies con- cerned the impact ofHead Start in areas of thechildren's psychological, emotional, and socialdevelopment, there were severalother sources of information pertaining tothe same areas. Head Start staff and workers'opinions of the effects on thechil- dren of participationin the program were sought on an0E0 form, the Worker's Evaluation Form. Exhibit IV-50 shows thedistribution of opinions in a.sample of 6,320 paid and voluntaryworkers in 423 ChildDevelopment Centers across the country.These opinions concerned waysin which the chil- dren changed as a resultof participating inHeadStart.'For each item,

1The general nature of thesample of workers isdiscussed inSection II and will not be repeatedhere.

IV-118 EXHIBIT IV-50 CHANGES IN HEAD START CHILDREN (WORKERS' OPINIONS)1 Workers'sItem Number Evaluation BetterMuch Better Change No Percent Worse WorseMuch Response NV-- 19(1) Getting along (2)(3) Self-confidence Speaking ability with other children(2) 41.242.545.968.6 49.147.349.230.7 4.22.91.30.7 0 .1 0 5.35.5 %C:)* (5)(4) EverydayFinishing mannerswhat he starts 38.528.2 60.252,7 4.92.6 .2.1 .1 5.96.5 - (9)(8)(7)(6) CanDoingInterestedChances do what things of in hesuccess new on is toldhis things inown kindergarten 37.858.757.5 33.335.053.4 2.61.71.0 00 0 .1 7.06.75.7 Notes: (1) N = 6,320. (2) Percentages for this item based on "No Change" includes Worse and Much Worse. N = 3,653. The number (.7) shown as #F"--E-trIPtr'Sffedllegektsgatz-,

a percentage of the total samplemaking a given response is shown. The percentages in the column entitled "No Response" wereobtained by subtracting the total number of responses to the itemfrom the total sample size and converting the difference to a pei:centage.The items are numbered according totheir number on the evaluation form. The exhibit shows the following points ofinterest.Generally, the- workers in the sample believed that the children hadimproved in a num- ber of ways.In particulars a majority felt that thechildren were much better in getting along with other children and weremuch more inter- ested in new things.Otherwise, the modal opinions *.are that the chil- dren were at least better in the otherbehavior patterns shown in Exhibit IV-50. Chi-square tests were made of the distributionsof responses to each item by each of the four types of CDCworkers (paid and volunteer professional, neighborhood paid, etc.). The statistic wassignificant beyond the .05 level in each case.This was generally because of the much greater proportion of "Much Better" responsesby the profes- sionals.Interestingly, the modal response of the professionals was "Much Better" for Items 1, 2, 3,4, 7, and 9 and "Better" for Items 5 and 8. It might be supposed that staffmembers would feel that the pro- gram should beprolonged to support the desired changes in thechildren. Out of 2,857 comments written on a sampleof 6,433 Worker's Eval- uation Forms, 340 urged a continuingand/or expanded Head Start pro- gram, or establishmentof a kindergarten. However, a number of workers (45) commented that the lengthof the school day and/or the program was too long.Statements in this category were to theeffect that after 6 weeks children of preschool agelost interest, slacked off in attendance, and generally grew weary.Some felt that shortening the program day or thelength of field trips would help and notedthat the children they observed characteristically grewtired or restless in the afternoon.

IV-120 The largest portion (751 out of 2,857) of the voluntarily appended remarks on the Worker's Evaluation Form were that the children benefited. However, no one volunteered to define a "good" program other than tc cite either the emotional benefits that the children derived or the imaginative, whole-hearted efforts ofthe staff. Some workers (54) felt the value of the program for the children might be lost if there were no follow-up, especially in the areasof medical, dental, or psychiatric treatment, as well as in cultural and educational areas. With respect to the latter, some workers who commented expressed the reservation that the program would be of value only to children going on immediately to schoolin the fall. Three of the items.on the National Opinion ResearchCenter (NORC) parent interview concerned the parental opinions of the effect of Head Start on their children, and were. identical to thequestions directed at Head Start workers and summarized in ExhibitIV-50 above. Exhibits IV-51, IV-52, and IV-53 summarize the NORC results for these items iZor the total NORC sample and for white and Negro parents separately. An asterisk next to an item in ExhibitIV-51 indi- cates a significant difference at the 5 percent level betweenwhite and Negro responses. Exhibit IV-51 indicates that, in general, the parents believed that their children had improved in a number of areas as a resultof Head Start.Of the parents. interviewed, 95 percent said that Head Start had had good effects on their children, while only 2 percent statedthat Head Start had had bad effects (see Item 5).The exhibit indicates that most of the parents felt that Head Start provided a number ofsocial experien- ces for their children, such astrips into the community .a.nd play activ- ities (see Item 3). Item 6 in Exhibit IV-51 shows that the majority of parentsbelieved that their children, as a result of Head Start, were much better or some- what better in the eight characteristics listed (getting along, self-confidence, speaking, manners, completing tasks, obedience, interest in new things, and independence).

IV -121 EXHIBIT IV-51 CHANGES IN HEAD START CHILDREN - PARENTS' OPINION (TOTAL)(1) NORC Item Number , Yes Don't IfPercent YesMuchVery Right AllWas It Worthwhile? of WasteTime KnowDon't SignificantDifferencesvs. White Negro 3. 4. B.A.As part of the Head HaveHave a medicala dental ex....amination? examination? Start Program did (child): 876984 No251310 Know 326 767059 131110 <1<1 <1 61 ** E.D.C. GetTakeGet to anyto know see trips orany inhear new the a community?toys lotstories, or and music? of books, games? 9188 25 66 7480 1114 <1<1 <1<1 ** G.F. GetHave any. a individualchance to take.attention thepartactivity teacher with ? other children? in groupfrom 6293 13 1 25 5 8254 11 8 <1 0 <1 2 * 5. A.B. Did Head Start have anyon the child? badgood effects effects 95 2 61 2 <1 1 About Somewhat Much Don't 6. .What wasA.childC.B. the witheffect regard of Head to the following: SpeakingSelf-confidenceGetting along ability with other children Start on the BetterMuch 403850 Somewhat Better .363027 Same 3022 Worse <1 Worse Know<1 0o <1<1<1 ** Note: (1) N. = 2036. H.E.D.G.F. EverBeingDoingFinishing /day ableinterestedwhat manners what to he do is he inthingstold startsnew onthings his own 4830293555 3229333536 3533291419 <1<1 01 <1 000 <1<1<1 ** 1 L -3 HEAD START CHILDREN - PARENTS' OPINION (WHITE) (1) EXHIBITNORC Item IV-52 Number CHANGES IN Yes No KnowDon't Percent-VeilIfMuch Yes Right of AllTimeWas ItKnow Worthwhile?. Waste Don't 3., 4. A.AsB. part of the Head Star:. Have a dentalmedical examination? examination? Program did (child): 866180 321712 631 734761 131417 . 21 <1<1 F.EeD.C. GetTakeGet to any know.anysee individualtrips or hear in new the a attention lottoysstories, or games and music? community? of books, from ? 835390 15 72 31 79 467866 1117 7 <1<1 <1<1 5. A.G. HaveDid a Head chance Start to havetakeactivityon thepartany the teacher?withbad child? other children? in group effects 90 3 95 1 8 .79 11 <1 <1 6. B.Whatchild was with the effect regard of to Head the Did Head Start haveon any the good child? following: Start on the effects BetterMuch 39 93 SomewhatBetter2 29 AboutSame 29 Somewhat Worse <1 WorseMuch KnowDon't<1 A.D.C.B.F. EverydaySpeakingSelf-confidenceGetting along abilitymd.nners with other children 33222533 29353235 42363628 <1 2 <1<1 <1<1 21 E. Finishing what he starts <1 0111 Note: (1) N = 832.H.G. DoingBeing what ableinterested heto isdo told inthings new onthings his owns 374518 353.233 252045 1 1 21 EXHIBIT IV -53 CHANGES IN HEAD START CHILDREN - PARENTS' OPINION (NEGRO)(1) NORC Item Number Percent If Yes, Was It Worthwhile . ? 34,, 4. A.As part of the Head Start Program did (child): Yes86 No10 Know Don't 35 MuchVery 76Right All10 8 of Time KnowWaste <1 Don't 1 E.D.C.B. HaveGetTake toa any dentalmedicalknowsee trips or any examination?hear examination?in newthe a lot community?toys of orbooks, games? 74929188 20 39 255 78678180 121010 <1 IND <1 1 F.G. GetHave any a individualchance to attentiontakeactivity partthestories, teacher fromin withgroup and ? othermusic? children? 6795 12 1 20 3 8559 10 9 <1 <1 3 5. A. Did Head Start haveon any the bad child? effects good effects 96 1 98 1 <1 About Somewhat Much Don't A.Whatchild was with the effectregard of to Head the following: Start on Getting along with other children the BetterMuch 4557 Somewhat Better 3526 Same1817 Worse <1<1 Worse 110 Know<1 E.D.B.C. Self-confidenceFinishingEverydaySpeaking abilitymannerswhat he starts 334142 383429 2724 <1 <1 - <1 Note: (1) N = 1,129.H.G.F. BeingDoingBeing interestedwhat able heto dois intold things new thingson his own 556237 292632 *112915 <1 <1.<1 Since both the parents and the Head Startstaff respanded to the same eight questionsin Exhibit IV-51 (Item 6) relating to impact, it is interesting to compare the opinionsexpressed by the two groups. Comparison of the data (Exhibit IV-50 andIV-51) shows that for all items a much higher percentageof parents believed that their children were "about the same."Some possible explanations for this outstanding discrepancy might be that the parentssimply did not notice the change, or the childrenbehaved differently at home, or the parents did not appreciate the change in "speakingability" and "everyday manners," or the Head Startteachers had some bias (enthusiasm) which led them to see more change, thanactually occurred. The apparent discrepancy between the ratingsof the Head Start teachers and the Head Start parents isespecially interesting because it is somewhat consistent with thediscrepancies between various ratings of social behavior made by Head Start teachersand the kindergarten teachers who received the Head Startchildren into their fall 1965 classes. It appears that the children's socialbehavior may have improved only for the actual Head Start program and may nothave improved much at home or at regular school. Follow-upstudies would be needed to deter- mine lasting (or latent) effectsof Head Start on behavior. An analysis of the NORC items describedin Exhibit IV-51 was also made whereby the parents weregrouped by race (white and Negro) and comparisons were made to determine towhat extent different respon- ses were made to the samequestions by parents of different races. The results of the analyses are shown inExhibits IV-52 and IV-53, although the occurrences of findings ofsignificant differences are indicated in Exhibit IV-51 with asterisks. The exhibits show that for Items 3 and5, Negro parents responded "Yes" more frequently for dentalexamination, new toys and games, indiv- idual attention, and group activities;they responded "No" more frequent- ly for bad effects of Head Start,All tests for differences for subitemsin Item 6, regarding the effect of i-leadStart 'on the child, were significant

IV -125 with the Negro parents responding that their children had improved more than the white parents felt that their childrenhad. In summary, there were 15 independent researchers who studied Head Start children in relation to the effects of the program on their psychological, emotional, and social development. In addition, impact data were obtained from more than 6,000 Workr's Evaluation Forms and nearly 1,750 Head Start parents. Although considerable variations, and even inconsistencies, appear to exist amongthe several data sources, some findings emerge. Head Start teachers tended to rate the childrenconsiderably higher in most areas relating to behavior than didHead Start parents or kinder- garten teachers who had not been involved in HeadStart but in whose classes the Summer 1965 children are now enrolled. But evenwhen the kindergarten teachers express their opinions about thealertness, curiosity, independence, and self-awareness of their "HeadStart graduates" (as contrasted to opinions about behavior perse), they frequently indicate that Head Start children are superior to non-Head Start children. One implication is that regular school teachers admirequalities of alertness, curiosity, and independence but frown upon thebehaviors that often accompany the qualities.1At any rate, positive developments or changes in at least somepsychological, emotional, and social areas were reflected to some extentby all sources of data. A major question to be considered in the interpretationof all studies which compar:,d Head Start children with non-HeadStart chil- dren is who the non-Head Start children were.The finding that some kindergarten teachers saw no significant differencesin behavior or school readiness characteristics between their HeadStart and non-Head Start pupils might at first lead one to assume thatthe Head Start impact was negligible. However,'Oils finding could be considered highly favor- able to the Head Start program if the comparison groupof non-Head

1=0%1 1Research supporting this point is reviewed byStern (Reference 101).

IV-126 Start children were culturally advantaged children.Indeed, one of the aims of the Head Start program was to place the deprivedyoungsters on an equal footing withtheir nondeprived or advantaged peers. Unfor- tunately, while Head Start and non-Head Start groups were oftenmatched for age, sex, presem- in the same classroom, or other variables,they were not matched forlevel of family income or other indices of cultural and economic deprivation. Therefore, to the extent that Head Start children werecompared unfavorably with nonculturally deprived children on somecriteria, it must be considered that their school adjustment mayhave been attri- butable at least in part to their life-long environmentaldeprivation, rather than to the experiences in the Head Start program,which some teachers believed encouraged unruly, nonconformist behavior. There is certainly considerable evidence attesting to someposi- tive (favorable) effects of Head Start.There is, however, much need for further definition and measurement of criterionvariables. Pro- vision for careful and systematic follow-up seems most urgentlyneeded in future evaluation programs.

IV-127 D. Impact of Head Start on Parents The three primary sources of infoimationconcerning the impact of Head Start on parents of Head Startchildren were (1) the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) interviewsof parents, (2) the Head Start staff's and workers'opinions concerning Head Start's effects on parents, and (3) the independentstudies. Exhibit IV-54 summarizes therelevant data for the total NORC sample (1,742 parents), andExhibits IV-55 and IV-56 show the results of those data separately forwhite and Negro parents, respectively. Exhibit IV-54 indicates that a verylarge percentage of parents had an opportunity to talk withtheir child's teacher; 31 percent were able to become acquainted withother parents; and 23 percent had some opportunity to talk with a social worker orcounselor. These Head Start-related activities were judged asfairly or very much worthwhile by almost all parents to whom theitems applied. The data in Exhibit IV-54 (Items1D-J) do not indicate what percent of parents attended any kind (unspecified) ofspecial meeting, so one cannot determine to what extent the percentagesof the parents in Items 1D-H and ZD -H are represented more than once.It is clear, however, that the large majority of parents who did attendspecial meetings considered them very much worthwhile. The samefindings appear for the remaining categories--of the few parents whodid see films and go on trips, a largemajority considered the activities very muchworthwhile. Item 7 of Exhibit IV-54 (referring to Item7 on the actual NORC form--see Appendix A) indicates that a verysmall percentage of the parents themselves received help,improved their job status, or planned to continue their own education as aresult of Head Start. Item 8 indicates that an overwhelmingmajority (88 percent) of parents interviewed expressed a morehopeful outlook for their child's future as a result of Head Start; 80 percentexpressed a new awareness of community concern for their problems. Exhibits IV-55 and D/-56 show the percentagesofresponses to the same questions discussed above forwhite and Negro parents, separately.In general, the Negroes participated morein the program

IV-128 EXHIBIT IV-54 EFFECTS ON PARENTS - TOTAL (PERCENT)(I) 1., 2. Ac part of the Head Start Program, did you: NORC Item Number Yes No MuchVery Right All (If Yes, was It worthwhile) of Time Waste KnowDon't Significance Difference(White vs. Negro) 4,(2) A.C.B. GetTalk to know with any socialafnew the parents? child'sworkers teachers? or counselors? 233178 79756621 66152218 11 359 <1 - <1 1I * . F.E.D. Attendskills? any specia'special -sleeting'meetings aboutabout housing child care? homemaking It, 47 9390 36 1 - 2I * H.G. opportunities?Attendconditions? any special meetings about job personal 3 94 22 <1 1 212 .42 7. AsJ. a res.1t of Head Start, did you (or your AttendGoSeeproblems? onany any any films special group in connectionmeetingstrips in the about withcommunity? yourthe program? husband): 1510 828793 12 8 32 < 1 - 2 * 8. B.A.AsD.C. a result of Head Start: GetMake helpanya job any medicalof or any plansswitch kind or to dentalto continuefrom a better anyattention? yoursocialjob? education? agency? 2328 958994 * A.C.B. Didfuture?Do you you feellearnmake any anythingany more new hopeful youfriends? didn't about know (child's) about 3988 5810 ei.* Note: (1) N = Z,036. (2) AsteriskD., (41) indicates significance Doyouraising you and feel children? your that problems? the community cares about beyond the 5-percent level. 8026 1471 * 0 (PERCENT)(1) EXHIBIT1., 2. IV-55 EFFECTS As part of the Head Start Program, NORC Item Number ON PARENTS - WHITE did you: Yes No MuchVery (If yes, was it worthwhile) RightAll of TimeWaste KnowDon't D.C.B.A. TalkGet to with know any any ofsocial newthe child'sworkersparents? teachers or counselors? ? 20102782 78857018 . 171866 7 15 93 < <1 1 I <1<1 - E.F. Attend anyconditonsmaking special skills? ? meetings aboutabout housingchildhome- care? 12 9394 11 < 1 1 -- -- H.G. Attend anypersonalopportunities special problems meetings ? ? about yourjob < 1 73 919295 <1 23 <1 31 < 1< 1 1 t-.(J.). 4 I 7. .. AsJ.I. a result of Head Start, did you (or your GoSee onany any films group in connection trips in the withcommunity? the program ? husband): 19 76 14 5 - - 0 C.B.A.D. MakeGet helpanya job medical plansof or any switch to kind orcontinue dentalto from a better yourattention?any social job?education? agency? 2313 95949393 ------8. AAsB. a result of Head Start: Do you feelfuture? any more hopeful about (child's) 8132 6516 -- - ..- Note: (I) N =C. 832.D. DidaboutDo you you you raisinglearnmake feel and anythingthatany children?your newthe problems? community youfriends? did not reallyknow aboutcares 7618 17,80 - - - r r-t3 rTi EXHIBIT IV-56 EFFECTS ON PARENTS NORC Item Number NEGRO (PERCENT)(1)\ (If yes, was it worthwhile) 1., 2. A.AsB. part of the Head Start GetTalk to knowwith anyany ofnew the parents? child's teachers? Program, did you: Yet 3576 24I...No64 MuchVery 202.766 Right All 10 69 of TimeWaste - KnowDon't 21 E.D.C. AttendAttendTalk withanymaking any specialany special socialskills? meetings meetings workers about about or child counselors? housinghome- care? 261025 887474 22 95 32 - 332 $.4 G.F. AttendAttend anyconditions any special special meetings? meetings about about job 56 9392 4 2 1 - 3 -.1,-,"4.4t H.J.I. SeeAttendGo any on opportunities?any filmspersonalany special group in connection problems? meetingstrips in the with community? the 'about your program? 1213 4 868595 1111 3 21 -. 33 (Or your husband): MI .1111 ell OP 7. As a result of Head Start, did you gO A. agency? 3 96 ODen MPob fa OD Get help of any kind from any social 2 96 OW B.D.C. GetMake anya jobany medical orplans switch to or continue dentalto a better attention? your job? education? 12 3 9587 OD OD .NB 8. 011, As a result of Head Start: MD MD Ala A. Do you feel any more hopeful about (child's) 94 6 OW OD ii future? MD ODa OD Note: (1) N zB. 1,129.D.C. DoDid youyou feelmakelearnandraising thatyour anythingany the children?problems?new community you did not cares know friends ? about you about 334584 655412 and felt that the program was more worthwhile. The 5-percent level of significance was used to indicate those items for which theremight be real differences between the two groups. As far as participationin the program is concerned, significant items indicated that the Negroes talked more with social workers and counselors and attended more special meetings on child care, homemaking skills, and job opportunities. The white parents participated more in group trips in the community. This was one of two items forwhich the white parents responded more affirmatively. There were three items to which the Negroes responded "more worthwhile" than thewhites.These were knowing new parents, attending meetings about homemaking skills and housing conditions, and viewing films in connection with the program. The white parents indicated that the group trips inthe community were more worthwhile. Five items which show significant differences in the impact on the parents (Items 7 and 8) are making plans to continueeducation, feeling more hopefulabout the child's future, making new friends, learning more aboutchild-raising, and feeling that the community cares. For all five items, the Negro parents 'responded more affirmatively. Opinions of the workers in the national sample concerning the extent to which parents of Head Start children were affected orchanged in various ways as a result of the program are shown inExhibit IV-57. The question asked on the Evaluation Form was: "As a result of their contact with Operation Head Start, the parents are "The items shown in the exhibit are the dimensions rated. The data show that the majority of workers felt that parents had benefited in all ways more than not.The percentage of "no change" responses (and indeed of "noresponse") is higher in all cases for this question thanfor the question concerning improvement inchildren. It is also higher than for the same category of opinion aboutchange in the workers theMselves (see Exhibit IV-58 in subsection IV.E). This trend, along withthe fact that the percentages of "much better" responses are generally lower than for the other two questions, suggests a numberof possibilities. Among these are that many workers felt less able to

IV-132 EXHIBIT IV-57 EFFECTS ON PARENTS - WORKERS' OPINIONS (1) Worker's Evaluation Much No Percent Worse WorseMuch Response No 20(1) (2) Concerned Involvedappearanceeducation with child' s Item Number about own Better 22.036.0 Better 46.450.2 Change21.4 6.0 0.00.1 0.00.1 10.1 7.7 1-1 (3) Paiticipating runty activities in corn- 20.1 49.055.4 20.412.8 0.10.0 . 0.10.1 10.210.3 co)-c....) (5)(4) AwareEffective of enlightenedin interpersonal child-rearing practices 21.515.6 56.7 15.3 . 0.0 0.1 12.3 Note: (1) N(6)* = Knowledgeable6,320. .relationscommunity resources about . 19.3 55.9 13.9 0.0 0.1 10.8 comment on changes in parents or that the programs lacked sufficient parental involvement to have an effect on them. It is true, nonetheless, that the modal response for each item in Exhibit IV-58 is "better." An examination of opinions classified by position of the workers in the CDC showed that the distributions for each item differed signifi- cantly at the .05 level.In each of the six items, the nonprofessionals expressed highly favorable opinions about effects on parents to a greater extent than did the professionals.In particular, neighborhood volunteers tended to be particularly favorable in their estimates of impacts. The interpretation of these data raises some questions. The items imply that the respondent had substantial opportunity toobserve or talk with a sizable number of parents during the course of the program. There is no way of telling to what extent such was the case. One inter- esting cross-check on the workers' opinions can be made bycomparing them with the parents' opinions collected by NORC (see Exhibit IV-54). Item 8C in that interview was: "As a result of Project Head Start, did you learn anything you didn't know before about raising children?"

This item has some similarity to Item 20(4) in Exhibit IV-57:"As a - result of their contact with Operation Head Start, the parents are aware of enlightened child-rearing practices: much better, etc." Seventy- seven percent of the sample of workers said that theparents were better or much better in this respect.In contrast, 71 percent of the parents interviewed said that they had not learned anything new aboutraising children. These and the above considerations make this question and the obtained data difficult to interpret unambiguously. As a final point here, it may be reported that a number of workers did make written comments on the Evaluation Form about the ways in which parents benefited from the program. Eighty-two comments, or about 3 percent of the total comments, were to the effect that parents had been helped or benefited. However, this spontaneous response is a relatively small part of theoverall percentage (76 percent) of favorable comments made about the program and its .effects. Examination of the comments appended to the Consultant's Check- lists of Head Start Educational Consultants indicated that parental IV-134 involvement was seen by all as a necessary ingredient to asuccessful Head Start program. Examples of inadequate orlacking parental parti- cipation were cited, as well as examples of activeparental involvement. It should be noted that lack of parentalparticipation was often not because of a lack of interest, but moreoften because of a lack of resources, feelingof mistrust, or fears of a school-like system. One of the independent studies(Reference: 2) is of special interest because it involved seven Head Start parents(four Negro, three white) in an attempt to determinethe effectiveness of indigenous parents as test administrators.The parents, whose own educational levelranged from 9th to 12th grades, participatedin an intensive training session to lea:m to administerthe PPVT and the Preschool Inventory.The comparison group of testers was a groupof graduate students who were sophisticated in the use o-s: the PPVT andthe PSI. The intertester (parents/graduate students) correlations were obtainedfrom 57 children who were tested by both groupsof testers and were significantly high for both tests (PPVT, r =.64, and PSI, r = .64).The implications of this study for future parentinvolvement were summarized by Allerhand: "The effectiveness revealed by the parentsin this study leads us to believe that there is apotential corps of untrained people who maybe utilized for services requiring some areasof testing skill.Highly motivated individuals indigenous tothe particular setting may very well provide the traits needed to negatingthe handicaps inherent in lack of professional training. It is likely that the qualitiesdemonstrated by the parents as testers mayalso be utilized in other aspects of currently viewed professionalservices such .as observers, handlers of data, and teacher-aides." While Allerhand's project was theonly, one that reported such a systematic involvement of parentsthemselves as subjects, other independent studies commented aboutparents' participation. Van Egmond (Reference105) found that only a few of the 17 teachers included inhis study implemented contact with parents. The teachers generally did not seem torecognize the important socialization role of the parents, or wish toextend their own role definition to include a "partnership" with the parents.

IV-135 A report from the SouthernConsumers' Education Foundation (Reference 114) commented that parentmeetings held in small, groups in one another's homes seemed mosteffective.The wide variety of topics discussed included mealplanning, budgeting, and the War on Poverty itself. A report from WarminsterTownship (Reference 52) mentioned a number ofopportunities that were providedfor parents to participate in Head Start, includingclassroom observation days andspecial meetings for fathers inaddition to individual conferences. In a report fromChicago (Reference 19) it waspointed out that often the Head Start families camefrom such divergent backgrounds-- Appalachians, Negroes,Mexicans, Puerto Ricans,Hungarians, etc.-- that parents were strangersin their own neighborhoodsand welcomed, if timidly, the opportunity toget acquainted. Anexample of these dynamics, extracted fromthe report, follows: "In one center, a Mexican womanoffered to bring the refreshments to one of the afternoonmeetings. She brought the makingsfor tacos and as she began to skillfully prepareher native dish, her neighbors gradually began to see herin a different light. She was nolonger just. another Mexican, but a capable,charming hostess and a motherwho, like the others, shared manyof the same responsibilities andproblems. Barriers began to break down,recipes were exchanged, andthe once quiet room was filled withattempts to communicate." There are several instancescited in other independent studies where impacts on parents mayhave been implicit in participation, although the impact has not beenassessed.In Chicago, all medical examinations for Head Start weredone in an equippedclinic, and in the presence of atleast one of the child's parents.Time was allowed for parents to ask questionsand receive answers; it seemslikely that these discussions had apositive effect on the Chicagoparents. In Montez' "Evaluation ofHead Start BilingualChildren" (Refer- ence 74),mention was made of a nursewho, upon sensing among parents an interest in mattersrelating to family planning,held a meeting for parents. There was aturnout of 80 percent forthe meeting.

IV-136 Porter commented that "the parents of Head Start children were made welcome in the center classrooms for the first 2 days in shortened sessions. Teachers were encouraged to confer with each set of parents, home visits were made in many cases, and evening meetings were held. ,It is patently clear from the attendance of parents at these meetings that the schools, in the case of Head Start centers, at least, were places that thesefamilies could trust and respect" (Reference 87). There remains much more to be learned about impact on parents and the role of parent participation in Head Start programs. Some of the available data seem to raise more questions than they answer. Considering the vital role that parents play in the development of the children, it is to be hoped that more correlated data in this realm will be developed in future programs.

le

'111 -137 E. Impacts on CDC Staff Workers In Section II we presented someof the known characteristics of the 184,000 professionals andsubprofessionals, discussed how some became involved in the summer program,explored Head Start teacher styles and attitudes, and indicated someof the duties of the subprofes- sionals.It was suggested that not only werethese people vital to the success of the programthrough their instruction and assistance, but that the experience contributed toincreasing their understanding of the deprived child.In this section we shall discuss:(1) the impact of the children and the program onthe CDC workers; (2) the effect of the innovation of using a variety ofsubprofessional workers; and, most important, (3) the impact of theteacher styles and attitudes on the development of the child. Exhibit IV -58 shows ways in whichthe 6,320 responding staff members of over 420 Child DevelopmentCenters in the national sample felt they had benefited from theirHead Start experience.The exhibit slows percentages of the workers inthe sample who checked a given scale level for a given item.The question asked on the form was:"As a result of my contactwith Operation Head Start, I am...." The vari- ous items arelisted on the exhibit. Most of the respondents indicatedthat they had learned much from their Head Start experience.These findings are of interest,particularly in light of the data on their previousexperience (see Exhibit 11-65, sub- se section II. E).About 44 percent of the sample hadhad no previous exper- ience with preschoolers or withchildren from conditions of poverty. There were significant differencesbeyond the .05 level in the distributions of responses by the fourmain categories of workers ana- lyzed1for each item shown. There wereabout 3,500 workers, all told, who answered these items, of whichabout 40 percent were prOfessionals and 35 percent were paid neighborhoodresidents. On Items 21-1, -2, and -3, a larger proportion of professionalsthan other categories of workers indicated they had benefitedmuch. more from their Head Start

1.Paid and volunteer professionals,neighborhood paid, neighborhood volunteer, and other paid andvolunteer. IV-138 ern* EXHIBITIV-58 BENEFITS OF WORKERS' PARTICIPATION - WORKERS' RATINGS can cza E-773 c"--1 (1) O f*--1 Worker's Evaluation Item Number MoreMuch Somewhat PercentLittle A At All Not Response No teachingKnowledgeableAwarechildren ofpre environment -school about 60.460.5 28.126.1 4.75.2 1.4 .8 6.06.8 withAbilityforAcquiredthatexperience dealing theseHead to newchildren dealStart effectively techniques with children other 43.148.0 37.234.3 9.77.8 2.21.6 7.88.3 Note: professionaldevelopmentconcerned with workers child's(1) N = 6,32.0. experience. On Items 21-3 and -4, somewhat disproportionate numbers of paid neighborhood residents felt they benefited little or not at all. How- ever, the great majoritydid indicate they thought they had gained sub- stantially.In written comments on the evaluation form, many volunteers and aides stated that their experience had helpedthem to decide upon a career in teaching, socialwork, etc. These positive expressions are generally supported by several in- dependent research studies.In Greene County, Ohio, Cohnstaedt (Ref- erence 21) statedthat two-thirds of the interviewed Head Start teachers reported significant attitudinal changeswhich they attributed to Head Start.These included a better understanding of the child's needs as an individual, a greater sense of the importance of creativeself-expression, and a greater ability to relate to the "wholechild." Miller and Cassileth (Reference 73) asked both the subprofessionals and the teachers in their 30 sample centers to determine theeffect of the program on the subprofessionals.When asked about the benefit of Head Start to these aides, the plurality of teacher and subprofessional response suggested a gain of understanding and insight intochildren.1Eleven sub- professionals and ten teachers believed that the aides were better able to work with children. Ten subprofessionals and sixteen teachers cited self-accomplishment, improvement, and gratification. More teachers than aides thought that the latter had a better understanding of educational processes; more aides than teachers sawthe former as benefiting through a new insight ini;e. deprivation.Increased self-confidence was mentioned by 13 teachers, but by only one of the subprofessionals. When asked the ways in which the subprofessionals enjoyed the work, the majority of both groups stressed enjoyment of the children. An equal number of teachers and subprofessionals (21 each) suggested that the latter enjoyed the personal gratification from their own activities.

1In this subsection, response frequencies arepresented. Eighty-six sub- professionals and forty-three teachers were interviewed; the latter com- mented on two subprofessionals each. Therefore, it is assumedthat the frequencies represent effective responses from 86 teachers.

IV-140 Both groups were asked whether the work changed thesubprofee- sionals in any way. Twenty-four subprofessionalsand sixteen teachers reported no change.Both groups also stressed that the subprofessionals were better with and had agreater understanding of children.In self- evaluation, the aides then emphasized moresympathy with and under- standing of poverty problems and a broadenedoutlook and experience toward children.The teachers stressed that the aides hadchanged in self-confidence and self-respect. As a final bit of evidence aboutthe impact of Project Head Start on staff members, percentagesof responses to questions of satisfaction with the experience and anticipations offuture participation are presented in Exhibit PT-59. The distributions speak forthemselves, Furthermore, it is noteworthy that these were the only twoquestions on the evaluation form in which the distributions of responsesby position of worker in the CDC were not significantly different (p > .40in each case). This expression of enthusiasm, made at the end of the program, can be considered inconjunction with the attitudes expressed by a sam- ple of Texas teachers (reported by Pierce-Jones, et al., seesubsection II. E). Expectations were high at the start of the program;judging from the data in Exhibit IV-59, the enthusiasm did notdiminish. It was the general, although not unanimous,view of the program participants and observers that the use of subprofessionalpersonnel contributed to the success of the program. The usualcriticisms were that the authority and duties between teacher andaide were not always clearly delineated, and this created some confusion.A few teachers did not enjoy having another person, even an aide,in the classroom. A very few comments speak to the abnormalcircumstances of an aide in the classroom- - unlike the situation thechild is to find in his regular

school class.- . Available studies, however, suggest that theoverwhelming major- ity of participants and observers were positive.Miller and Cassileth asked the subprofessionals and the teachers the extent towhich the aide

IV -141 4111N111111, -.1114.11111! V1111111.1.1! .111111.11111 41111111=MIV AMIIIIMI, I C EXHIBIT IV- 59 WORKER ENTHUSIASM - WORKERS' OPINION (1) 22. Worker's Evaluation EnjoyedItem Number duties Great Deal 90.0 A Somewhat 5.4 Percent Little .5A At All Not.1 Response 4.0No Note:23. (1) N = 6,320. participatingyearLook forward next to 83.1 9,8 1.7 1.1 4.3 ,I0 was ofassistance.'Seventy percent of the subprofeszie_,.nals andeighty- one percent of theteachers indicated that the aides had helped the chil- dren either extremely or verymuch. No one from either group said that the children were nothelped at all by the aide.Two-thirds of the aides and almost 80 percent ofthe teachers said that the former had helped the teacher either extremely or verymuch. Only one teacher mentioned that one aide was no help atall. In measuring overall effectivenss,the great majority of both groups were verypositive towards the subprofessionals' role. The two groups stated thatthe most effective areas were in educationalplay, supervisory (children), and affect-relationalactivities. Many workers considered themselves to be very effectivein social shaping (discipline), but few teachers agreed. Theauthors stated: "Subprofessionals con- sidered themselves to be least effectivein the area of clerical work and testing,2and as community resource persons;teachers felt subprofes- sionals to be least effective in the rolesof disciplinarian and information giver, and in work with individualchildren." Looking to future programs, the workers in thesample were asked how the program might make even better useof the subprofessionals. Both the subprofessionals and teachers stressedincreased orientation or special training.Finally, both groups thought that the subprofessional should be hired again. One independent study, conducted for the Centerfor Urban Education in New York (Reference 69), focused on the useof 10 teen-aged aides in , a program in oneof New York's most depressed neighborhoods.It appears that an objective of the program was to trainhigh school graduates as preschool teachers' assistants. Because it wasbelieved that the children needed to relate to adult males, it washoped to employ males in this capacity.

1A.5 -point scale (Not at All, A Little,Moderately, Very Much, Extremely).

. 2According to the authors, such tasks wen)in fact rarely performed by these 86 workers. IV-143 The investigators reported,however, that the anticipated success was not achieved.They state: Observation of the teacher'sassistants' activities did not reveal evidence of theirhaving benefited from the two week intensive training programdesigned to fit them for their work as teachers' aides. Theirrole in the program was largely confined to "moppingup" operations and offering general custodial careto the children. Moreover, all but one of the trainees weregirls, although several young men had been engaged as assistants atthe outset of the program. Only one boy remained and he wassurrounded by female teachers and female assistants. Hehad no one with whom to identify. It seemed evident from the way in whichall the boys crowded around the older brothers of some ofthe chil- dren, that they were willing and evenanxious to have male supervision. The absence of maleassistants was a serious shortcomingof this program. As for the teachers themselves, they seemedcompetent. They had a planned teaching program whichthey attempted to follow faithfully.They appeared to guard their role as educators jealously and were hesitant to share the respon- sibility of teaching with the teenage assistants. While the program appeared to be goodfor the children, the trainees received only moderatestimulus. The pro- gram failed to clarify therole of the assistant; to make it more responsible educationally, and to foster aspirit of team work between teacher and trainee. These obsta- cles can and must be overcome before teenageassistants can participate effectivelyin pre-school programs through- out the city. While this study was of but one aspect of one programand may or may not representsituations experienced in other projects, it does suggest some of the problems to be facedin fully utilizing the teenage volunteer. Even with the problems raised, it would appearthat the subprofes- sional worker has become an integral part of theHead Start program. While his duties and responsibilities havevaried from center to center, his mere presence has encouraged aconcentration on the individual child. As indicated in the 86-worker sample,he has been of great assistance to the children, to the teachers, and, asimportantly, to himself. Per- haps the greatest tribute to his effectiveness,however, would be the

IV-144 consideration of the inclusion of this Head Start conceptin the traditional educational structure. There seems little doubt thatteacher attitudes and styles affect the learning progress of the child.However, the exact nature and extent of this influence is notclear.'It is an i aportant issue for Head Start, for if different teaching stylesand attitudes do significantly affect the child's development, there may beimportant program implications in teacher training and recruitment. At least two independentresearch studies have attempted to relate the teacher to the child. As theinvestigators indicate, their results should not be interpreted as conclusive.However, they do serve to illus- trate the important areas offuture research. Eisenberg (Reference 33) is currentlyevaluating data concerning teacher behavior and attitudes in relation tochanges in the children. Progress reports have been submitted;because the final report has not been completed, his results must be regarded astentative.In his study, Eisenberg investigated several teacher characteristics.Thirty-eight teachers were rated on warmth,restrictiveness, activity, and a variety of other behaviors. Each teacher also completedthe Minnesota Teacher Attitude Inventory (MTAI). Teachers wereranked and grouped on each of the variables. Analyses of variance wereperformed on three sets of PPVT change scores for 15 children ineach teacher's charge to dis- cover if each teachercharacteristic affected an IQ change. Eisenberg reports the following results: (1) teachers with a moderate degree of 'communicative acts' producedsignificantly more improvement in IQ during the Head Start period.(2) Teachers with a moderate degree of 'management' producedsignificantly more improve- ment in IQ during the Head Startperiod.(3).Teachers who were viewed as warm, active, varied,and permissive produced significantly more IQ change during the entire study.(4) There is no significant effect of

1See, for example, reviews of the literatureby Getzels and Jackson (Reference 39) and Withall and Lewis (Reference109).

IV-145 fierIrrJEM='_

'encouragement' on IQ.(5) There is no significantrelationship between teacher attitudes on the MTAIand IQ changes.(6) Teachers who engage in 'intellectual' behaviorsproduced significantly morepositive IQ changes." While Eisenberg lookedat change asillustrated by the IQ, Lamb (Refe::ence 68) studied"The Development of Self-OtherRelationships During Project HeadStart." In focusing on theteachers,1Lamb asked the develop- two questions:(1) Would theteacher& cognitive styles affect . ment of self-socialcontructs?and (2) Would theteachers' perceptions of Head Start childrenaffect the development ofself-social constructs? To answer thesequestions, Lamb concentrated onthe state of Delaware: 28 CDC's,92 teachers, and1,400 children. Almost 800of these children weretested using the Self-SocialSymbols Task instrument and a sharing taskinstrument. Eighty-fourof the teachers were avail- able for analysis. In order to classifythe teachers according tocertain attitudes and styles, a series of tests wasused. One, an EssayProblem Test developed by Lamb, was given todetermine the conceptualstyle or level of teachers. The subjects wereasked to discuss the topic"Rules" according to guide- lines which increasedin conceptual complexity.Second, measure self-complexity teachers wereasked to select!'in a list of 90those adjectives which bestdescribed them. Teachers werethen given a test to measure theirperception of dislikedstudents. Finally, the HeadStart Workers Attitude Scale wasgiven before and aftertraining and several times during the operationto measure teacherattitude towards the poor. The teachers, onceclassified, were comparedwith the development of their students. Lamb found that in-general,"...students of abstract andcomplex teachers gained inself-esteem, identified moreclosely with mother, developed a morebalanced power perceptionof teacher and policefigures students and perceivedthemselves as[more] similar to others" than did

1He also looked atself-other relationships ofHead Start and non-Head Start children.This is discussedin subsection IV. C.

IV-146 of concrete and less complex teacheis. Lambhypothesizes that one of the reasons for this result maybe that the abstract and complex teachers are better able to provide avariety- of environmental alternatives,"... thus generating greaterbehavioral freedom to explore self and self- other relationships." The effect of teachers'perceptions (e. g., perception of a disliked person and generalattitude toward the poor) produced someinteresting results. Lamb found thatteachers' perceptions had the most effect on Negro male students andlittle or no effect on female white students. Lamb states: Male Negro students of teacherswho are relatively less positive in their attitudes about the poorand in their perceptions of disliked students identifyless closely with their teachers, feel moreassertive with respect to au- thority figures and are lowerin -their self-esteem. The converse holdsfor male Negro students with relatively more positiveteachers. There is a tendency for males in general to be affected byteacher attitude.Self-esteem, balance of power, and self centralityincrease for males under positive teachers while decreases arefound for males under less positive teachers.This suggests that those students typically distantfrom the institutional con- current norm are moresensitive to teacher attitude with a defense ofselfsocial constructs.Those students typ- ically within the norm are more secure and opento change under a wide variety of attitudinal pressures. The investigator concludes,then, that the responses to both his questions are affirmative:teachers' cognitive styles and teachers' per- ceptions of Head Start childrendo affect the development of self-social constructs. It is important, of course, to putthe results of the Eisenberg and Lamb studies in perspective.It has been suggested throughoutthis re- port that many factors cameto play in the Head Startchild's develop- ment. These include thechild's environment, his family situation,his physical and mental health, a numberof program variables, and worker attitudes and styles. When someof these factors were related to the child's development during the 8-week summerperiod, some significant differences emerged.

IV-147 Behind the various resultsthat have been reportedthroughout this and other sections, there isthe wealth of data that wasobtained during the Summer 1965 ProjectHead Start. These data,admittedly shaky in some cases,nonetheless are of greatimportance.. No one can rightfully say that allimplications and potentialshave been exploited in them. They provide, collectively, amajor body of informationof extraordinary scope. Much wasaccomplished in Project HeadStart; of equal impor- tance in the long run,much was learned. V. SUMMARYAND RECOMMENDATIONS

This concluding sectionsummarizes the currentinformation on the Summer 1965 ProjectHead Start.It is to be emphasizedthat the obser- vations made here arebased on the availableinformation which has been analyzed. The summary isorganized into six key elementsof Head Start: the magnitude of the project;the communities served;the people involved; and the programemphasis; community, parent,and worker reactions; perhaps most important,the impact on the child. We finallyprovide a set of recommendationsfor future, planning, pro- based grams, andresearch. Theserecommendations are, in some cases, on a bestjudgment from a detailedexamination of the data;they are not necessarily the outcomesof arguments developedin the text. A. Summary

1. Project Magnitude a. Early Projections (1) 100,000 culturallydisadvantaged children (2) 200 communities acrossthe nation (3) 12,000 teachers b. Results (1) 560,000 children in 50 states,District of Columbia, Puerto Rico, VirginIslands, Guam, andArmrican Samoa were served: s 12 percent fromNortheast 20 pe.rcent fromMiddle Atlantic . 20 percent fromSoutheast 15 percent fromMidwest 16 percent from Southwest 6 percent from West 7 per"ent from FarWest 3 percent fromTerritories (2) 184,000 workersparticipated: 22 percent professionals(child/professional ratio:13/1) 47 percent professionalsand nonprofessionals 53 percent voJ,Anteers served (3) 47 percent ofcountry's 3,142 counties were by one or more HeadStart Programs: 71 percent of countiesin Northeast 66 percent of counties inMiddle Atlantic 57 percent of countiesin Southeast 38 percent of countiesin Midwest 51 percent of countiesin Southwest 27 percent of countiesin West 51 percent of countiesin Far West

V -2 (4) 67 percent of nation'sspecial-target counties (300 with the lowest percapita income) were served by one or moreHead Start Programs; 9 percent of HeadStart children came fromthese counties. (5) Total cost was $94.6 million($168 per child). $82.7 million Federal share $11.9 million local share 13 percent localshare/total cost

2. Communities Served a. Early Plans (1) Head Start should concentratemore heavilyin communities with a largepercentage of lowincome families. (2) Special effort would bemade to includespecial- target counties. b. Results

(1) Per population size,there was greater percentage participation of children fromthree lowest 'family income regions (Southeast,Southwest, Middle Atlantic). (2) There was less relativeconcentration in fourth lowest family incomeregion (West). (3) The least concentration wasin the highest family income region (FarWest). (4) Per population size,there was greater percentage participation of childrenfrom special-target counties than from othercounties with programs. (5) Comparing special-targetcounties with programs to those without programs,little or no significant differences were apparent.Those with programs had a slightly higherpercentage of low income

V-3 families; those without programstended to have larger nonwhitepopulations.

3. People Involved a. Children (1) Expectations (a) Children would comefrom economically and culturally deprived families(85 percent with incomes under$3,000 per year). (b) The children would havehigh rates of un- detected medical disorders. (c) The children would bebehind other children of their age in cognitive,social, and emo- tional development.

(2) Results (a) Description Age: About 13 percent were 4 years old or younger; 42 percent were 5 yearsold; about 39 percent were 6 or more(es- timated average age: 5 years, 10 mos.). Sex: Slightly more boysthan girls participated. Race: About equal percentagesof white and Negro children were served. Lingual Different groupsrepresented subgroups: included Puerto Rican and Mexican American,French, Indian, and Eskimo.

V-4 (b) Family Background Family At least 68 percent of the structure: children lived with both mother and father. Household Nearly 60 percent of the chil- size: dren came from households with six or more people. Income: About 38 percent probably came from familieswith income of $3,000 per year or less. When the family income poverty level is measured in relation to household size, almost 50 percent of the fam- ilies earned less than the pov- erty line. EmploymentLess than half worked outside status of mother: the home. Educational Less than half finished-high level of mother and school. father: Father's Over 30 percent reported occupation: as laborers.

(c)Socio-Cultural Environment A national sample of parentsof Head Start children was interviewed.Some findings about thechildren of these parents were: 35 percent had no pets. 40 percent had no toys or games. 48 percent had no books ormagazines. 43 percent had no crayons,paints, or paper.

V-5 Differences were found between whiteand Negro children with respect to habits, activities, andpossessions. (d) Health Many medical and dentalevaluations were completed, and health statistics werecompiled.Findings of interest included: Nearly 15 percent of children werenot born in a hospital. Over 75 percent reportedgeneral health as good. In some outstandingmedical programs, the incidence of somedisorders was higher than in thenational sample. (e) Cognitive Functioning and Social/ Emotional. Development Studies mliformly showed Head Startchildren scored lower on tests ofcognitive functioning and general mentalability than the norms for the testswould predict. In national pretests ofcognitive ability, results were that: Males scored higher thanfemales. Negroes scored lower thanwhites. Urban children scored lcwerthan rural farm and nonfarm children. No differences in standing werefound with respect to the educational level of the mother,household size, or whether or not themother was employed. Scores tended to increasewith increasing family income levels. Studies using both PPVT andStanford-Binet (S-B) tests consistently found higher IQ's whenmeasured by the S-B. This disparitysuggests the culturally disadvantagedchildren tested had poorly developedverbal ability.

. (f) Social and Emotional Development Very few children with severepsychological disturbances reported. Among thosewith such disturbances, the following symptomsiwere most frequentlynoted:

V -6 Restlessness Anxiety Selfishness No major differences among ract...s wereobserved. b. Staff and Workers (1) Expectations (a) A wide variety of specialistsand non- specialists would be involved. (b) Both paid and volunteer personnelwould. be used.

(2) Results (a) Staff structures included teachers,social workers, nurses, psychologists,physicians, dentists, housewives, NeighborhoodYouth Corps and other youngsters, and parentsof participating children. (b) Over 30 percent of the workers hadhad no previous experience withpreschoolers or with children from disadvantaged neighborhoods. (c) Workers of many ethnic andcultural back- grounds participated. (d) Ages of staff members and workersranged from under 14 to over 60. (e) Staff workers were predominantly women. (f) There was a large volunteerturnout. 4. Program Emphasis a. Expectations child and (1) Medical/dental evaluations for each treatment as required wouldbe provided through the CDC.

V-7 (2) Daily programs consistent withmodern concepts of early childhood developmentwould be provided. (3) Social services would beavailable. (4) Programs for parental involvementwould be established. b. Results (1) Data from educationalconsultants indicates that 88 percent of the CDC'svisited had arranged for medical examinations and 85 percentfor dental examinations. (2) NORC interview data showsthat 84 percent of the respordents indicated theirch;.dren received a medical examination as partof Head Start and 69 percent received a dentalexamination. Parents felt that these examinations wereworthwhile in 70 percent and 59 percent ofthe cases, respectively, (3) Public health or welfare agencies wereproviding some assistanceto over 80 percent ofthe projects visited by educationalconsultants. (4) Some services in themedical/dental field were provided by medical and dentalsocieties in about 43 percent of the programsvisited by the edu-

cational consultants. a (5) Seventy-eight percent of theworkers expressed the opinion that theavailability of medical/dental services was good or verygood. (6) Workers were generallyenthusiastic about the daily program content.In over 90 percent of the cases, they reportedthat morale of the staff was good or verygood, 9-Lifri4wer 85 percent that relevance of thecurriculum to the child was good or very good.

V-8 (7) Educational consultants were less enthusiastic about the content of the daily programs of some centers visited.The cccurrence of highly struc- tured programs (about 16 percent of those ob- served) was criticized by these consultants. (8) Parent participation was a weak element in CDC programming as reported by educational consul- tants.In only about 30 percent of the cases had some parents' suggestions been adopted, and in less than 25 percent of the cases were parents active in planning follow-on activities. (9) Considerable enthusiasm was reported by many teachers and administrators about the training program provided and the use of teachers' aides. to implement more effective classroom activities. A majority of workers sampled believed that: (a) The daily schedules were realistic. (b) Cooperation from the parents was good or very good; however, about 15 percent thought it was fair or poor. (c) The amount and quality of play material were good or very good; however, over 10 percent thought they were only poor or fair. (d) The research instruments were good or very helpful; however, over 14 percent thought they were only poor or fair. (e) Children benefited greatly from the.programs, Many complaints or suggestions were offered concerning: Training and selection of teachers' aides. Too much testing and paperwork. Problems in organization, planning, and administration. The need for more participation of parents and/or communications between parents and teachers.

V-9 5. Community, Parent, Worker Impacts I a. Community (1) Expectations

(a) Many elements of the community would Oft contribute the necessary resources to the centers. (b) As a result of Head Start the community would see the need to establish on-going programs.

(2) Results The educational consultants' observations reveal: (a) School systems, boards of education, indi- vidual schools, etc., sponsored the majority of programs. While many community organi- zations, including community-action agencies, were project grantees,few of them spon- sored programs. .., (b) Some sponsors received extensive assist- ance; others did not.Consultants' obser- vations were that after the public schools, the public health and welfare agencies pro- vided the most assistance. a (c) In over 90 percent of the projects, arrange- ments had been made to transmit theHead Start records to the school system. b. Parents (1) Expectations (a) The parents would participateextensively in the Head Start programs. (b) The parents would have a new awareness of community concern for their problems.

11- I 0 (2) Results

(a) The degree of parent participation varied greatly from center to center. (b) 88 percent of the parents in the NORC survey reported they were more hopeful about their child's future. (c) 80 percent reported that as a result of Head Start they felt the community cared about them and their problems. (d) 78 percent became acquainted with their child's teacher. (e) 31 percent became acquainted with other parents. (f) 26 percent said they learned 'something new about raising children. (g) 23 percent talked with a social worker or counselor. (h) In general, Negroes participated more in the program and considered it more worthwhile. c. Staff and Workers (1) Ex ectations

(a) There would be increased experience with preschool children and children from dis- advantaged areas. (b) There would be increased opportunity to learn new concepts and techniques of child development and education, and to gain interdisciplinary experience. (c) The program would be effective.

(2) Restrlts (a) Over 70 percent of the sample of workers believed they had benefited' in each case

V-11. by (1) becoming more awareof the nature of the environment ofculturally deprived children, and of the waysof preschoolers, (2) acquiring new conceptsand skills, and (3) gaining increased ability towork with other professional specialistsconcerned with child development. (b) Over 80 percent wouldbe interested in participating again.

6. Impact a. Expectations (1) There would bereferral and treatmentof medical and dental problems. functioning and social (2) Improvement in cognitive and emotionaladjustment would berealized. b. Results

(1) While referral Yates weregenerally low, there have been reportedreferrals for vision, dental, speech, and hearingproblems, among others. In some individuallocal programs, as high as 40 percent healthreferrals for the population handled were reported.However, data suggest that there was inadequateprovision for follow-up in many programs. substantial variations in the adequacy (2) There were of the medical evaluationsfrom program to pro- gram. Therelationship of programcharacteristic or quality toimpact on child's healthis clear. (3) Studies almost unanimouslyfound positive and statistically significantdifferences in PPVT mean raw scoresbetween pretests andposttests.

V-12 ',..'-,."4,--"es..fte.ovagnorommlwasowevare&rare.rig.simamwellar.c.rearlimamoviwiteimArWxroolimaelftrarggidtEis.,Alegfk.

(4) Mean raw score gains were generally on the order of 4 to 6 points on the PPVT. (5) Studies using various tests comparing Head Start with comparable non-Head Start children at the end of the summer usually found that the groups weret significantly different. However, the results are not conclusive. (6) At the end of the summer, tests of Head Start children versus non-Head Start children who were not culturally deprived suggested that, while the Head Start children made significant gains, they still performed less well than the non- disadvantaged groups. (7) There is some evidence that non-Head Start chil- dren with socio-economic characteristics similar to the Head Start population caught up in perform- ance on certain test.;; after 2 to 3 months of attend- ance in school. However, the available follow-up test results comparing Head Start and non-Head Start children in school are limited and based on only a small sample of children.In effect, reli- able evaluation of long-term effects of Head Start cannot be made at this time. (8) Some results found in the 1-percent nationwide sample of PPVT scores were: (a) no differential effects of Head Start related to factors such as race, sex, region, family structure, levelof mother's education, employment of mother, and urbaniza- tion were found; and (b) there was an indication that younger children (i.e., under 5) mayhave bene- fited less than expected in terms of the PPVT. (9) Effects or gains observed in different studies depended in part on the tests employed. For .-"..41112igalliettgoirigabotibliTrarmira.sion. fr-onwormirammlasirremiorm...... 0.rwitrkirarsorsimersow affergommaimistIEIMIXIIN

example, one study found a measureof rate of learning to be more sensitivethan other tests of cognitive ability. While thereis little reason to doubt that significant changesoccurred in large numbers of Head Start children,the changes are apparently complex, and variablesaffecting changes were identified only verygrossly in the 1965 Project Head Start. Nofindings to date in- dicate a reliable relationshipbetween improve- meilt and programmaticvariables. (10) A majori4y of parents andworkers surveyed on a national basis rated the HeadStart children as having improved in self-confidence, ability to do things on their own, ability totalk, and in every- day manners. (11) In one study, a relationshipbetween teacher variables and pupil behavior wasobserved. The effect varied withthe sex and race of the

pupils. ,_, B. Recommendations Finally, we shall present some recommendations for future pro- grams and research.We have throughout the text drawn attention to implications of findings in both of these areas. These recommendations, which are by no means the only ones possible, arise from two types of problems--those encountered in the actual process of analyzing the data, awl those suggested by the analyzed data, either directly or upon consid- eration. We have not assigned priorities to the recommendations, nor will we attempt to discuss each in detail.

Although one of the Head Start objectives was to provide adequate medical and dental examinations for every participant,with follow-up treatment as desired, there were in fact wide variationsin the quality of the programs. Recommendations

1.There is a need to establish procedures for following up on individual case findings. 2. There is a need for a continuing effort to collect and report reliable prevalence data, since these are necessary for guidingfuture plans and programs; they are inputs to determining requirementsfor personnel, facilities, etc.Special attention should be directed towards assuring that examinations of all groups (e. g., white and Negro) are of equal quality. 3. There is a need to define strictly the disorders to be looked for (e.g., vision defects, anemia), and to establishcriteria for the re- porting of disorder prevalence in order thatrecommendation No. 2 may have the greatest possible effect on future plansand programs. 4. There is a need to iecognize that there arevariations among regions and communities in the prevalenceof certain disorders and in the cost and availability of medical resources. 5. Because of the need presented in Nd. 4 above,there is a need for 0E0 to prepare more specificguidelines defining the types of examin- ations and services which should or can beprovided. These guidelines

V-15 should suggest means fordeveloping medical programsin accordance with screening examinationand follow-up prioritieswhich take into ac- count (a) the localprevalance of certaindisorders, (b) the costand availability of medical resources,and (c) the medicalservices a child will receive whenhe enters the publicschool system. personnel to carry 6. There appear to havebeen shortages in for vision out certain facetsof the examinations,such as screening tests plan- and hearing problems.Either more effortis required in program if they are ning andimplementation to obtainand use such personnel, or, really not available, aconcerted effort must bemade to find, train, and use them. Problem Z There were numeroussources of error in obtainingand collecting the national assessmentdata. Recommendations In future evaluations,primary attentionshould be paid to quality rather than quantityof data to becollected; specifically: Special provisionneeds to be made to assurethat data can be cross-correlated;that is, all data relevant to achild in a sampleshould at least includethat child's identification number or CDC classnumber, as appropriate. All forms usedshould be reevaluatedand redesigned toin- crease theirability to produce usableand interpretable data in satisfyingthe informationrequirements of the 1966 program. Consideration should be givento having certainaspects of the testing and datacollection carried outby persons not involved in the operationof the CDC's; in anyevent, all data collected should bedirected, monitored,and/or supervised by persons notdirectly involved in theoperation of the Head Start programs.

V-16 Problem 3 There were many difficulties incomparing the results of inde- pendent research studies. Recommendations 1. Certain baseline datadescribing the samples should be re- ported in uniform terms, as agein months, numbers of boys and girls, level of economicdeprivation, ethnic characteristics, etc. 2. Test results should be scoredand reported in some uniform way (such as rawscore), in addition to whatever waythe reporter chooses to present his scores(e. g.,in terms of IQ or mental age). 3. Intervals between pretestsand posttests should be uniform, or, wherethis is not possible, should atleast be clearly stated. 4. Careful descriptions of control groupsshould be reported, including how they wereobtained. Problem 4 There is evidence that manychildren, unquestionably eligible for Head Start, did notparticipate.In some cases, thisappeared to be due to inadequaterecruiting procedures. Recommendation More emphasis is neededin planning and implementingthe recruit- ing phase of the program, sothat more of the targetpopulation is served. Problem 5 There is no way of assessingthe long-range effects ofHead Start without carefully plannedand implemented follow-upstudies. Recommendation At least as much effortshould be put intolongitudinal studies as into short-term assessmentand evaluation. It was not possible withavailable data to determine thosefactors in the programs that wererelated to improvements in thechildren. Recommendations 1. More attention must begiven to defining reliably observable characteristics or variablesof programs. 2. Pilot-testing of rating scalesand questionnaires should be undertaken where possible. Problem 7 A number of findings wereprovocative and warrant further research emphasis. Recommendations

1. The finding that Head Startboys apparently performed signifi- cantly higher than girls on thePPVT is contrary to findings reportedelse- where and warrants furtherinvestigation. 2. The finding that there appearedto be no differentialsensitivity, in terms of PPVT gain,associated with any of the statedsingle classifi- cations of children (with theexception of age and possiblyfamily income), or with CDC'swarrants further investigation. 3. The finding in theindependent studies that IQ scoresobtained from the Stanford-Binet wereconsistently higher than IQ scoresobtained from the same children on thePPVT is contrary to resultsof studies re- ported elsewhere, and wart antsfurther investigation. 4. The finding of an apparentdifferential effect of the program associated with age seemsparticularly important in viewof its implica- tions for program planningand practices, and warrantsfurther investi- gation. 5. The finding that different measuresappeared to give different indications of level ofperformance and improvementdeserves much more attention.

V-18 Problem 8 It was difficult to identify comparable costelements between pro- grams. Recommendation There is a need for clarification of costelements and standardiza- tion of reporting procedures topermit analyses of relationships of cost to program effectiveness. LIST OF REFERENCES

1. Abrams, Irving, and Ralph Spaeth, Chicago's Head Start Program, Summer of 1965.(Final unpublished non -OEO- funded Head Start study.For further information, contact Dr. Irving Abrams, or Dr. Ralph Spaeth, Board of Education, 228 North LaSalle Street, Chicago, Illinois 60601. )

2. Allerhand, Melvin E.,Head Start Operational Field Analysis, Progress Reports No. 1 and 2.(Unpublished Head Start Study, OEO Contract No. 512.For further information, ..ontact Dr. M. E. Allerhand, Department of P.;ychology, Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio 44106. ) 3. Ausubel, D., and Pearl Ausubel, "Ego Development Among Segre- gatea Negro C ildren, " in A. H. Passow (ed.), Education in De- pressed Areas. New York: Teachers College Press, 1963.

4. Baldwin, I. T.,and Joan C. Lee, An Evaluation of the Knox County, Kentucky Project Head Start, August 1965. (Non-0E0-funded Head Start study. For further information, contact Mr. I. T. Baldwin or Dr. Joan C. Lee, Psychological Associates, Ltd.. 115 East Maxwell Street, Lexington, Kentucky. )

5. Berger, Stanley I.,Development of Appropriate Evaluation Tech- niques for Screening Children in a Head Start Program -- A Pilot Project.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. _515. For further information, contact Dr. Stanley I. Berger, Department of Psychology, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, Rhode Is- land 02852.)

6. Berlin, Irving N.,Report of King County, Washington Regional Research Project.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 532. For further information, contact Dr. Irving Berlin, Division of Child Psychiatry, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98105.)

7. Bloom, B. S.,"Testing Cognitive Ability and Achievement, " Chapter 8 in N. L. Gage (ed.), Handbook of Research on Teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co. ,1963.

8. Bonney, Merl E. and Ertie Lou Nicholson, "Comparative Social Adjustments of Elementary School Pupils With and Without Pre- school Training, " Child Development, March 1958, Vol. 29, pp. 125-133. 9. Boston Project Head Start, (Local Head Start study, non-0E0- funded. )

R-1 10. Boyer, Donald N. , Evaluationof Project Head Start, Summer, 1965 -- A Report to the Board ofEducation, San Diego City Schools, November 1965.(Final Head Start study,non-0E0-funded. For further information, contact SanDiego City Schools, Park and El Cajon Boulevards, SanDiego, California.)

11. Bruner, Jerome, "Education asSocial Invention, " Saturday Review of Literature, February 19,1966. 12. Bruner, Jerome, "TheCognitive Consequences of Early Sensory Deprivation, " in Philip Solomon(ed. ), Sensory Deprivation. Cambridge: Harvard UniversityPress, 1961, pp. 195-207.

13. Caldwell, Bettye, and DonaldSoule, The Preschool Inventory. (Unpublished Head Start study,OEO Contract No. 514. For further information, contact Dr. BettyeCaldwell, Department of Pediatrics, State University of NewYork, Syracuse, New York 13210.)

14. Cambridge Health DepartmentReport on Head Start.(Final non - OEO- funded study. For furtherinformation, contact Dr. Clara Waldinger, Department of PublicHealth, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139. )

15. Campbell; D. T. , and J. C. Stanley,"Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Designs for Research onTeaching, " Chapter 5 in N. L. Gage (ed. ), Handbookof Research on Teaching.Chicago: Rand McNally and Co. , 1963.

16. Chandler, Marvin,' Community DepthStudy of 3rd and 7th Wards and its Effects on Head Start Children.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No.536.For further information, contact Rev. Marvin Chandler, Council ofChurches of Rochester, 130 Main Street East, Rochester, NewYork, 14604.)

17. Chesapeake Public Schools, Growthof Pupils in Pxoject Head Start as Measured bthe Metro olitan Readiness Test,November 1.73-63: Non -OEO- unded Head Start study.For urther information, contact Department of Research,Guidance and Testing, Cheapeake Public Schools, Chesapeake,Virginia.)

18. Chesteen, Hilliard, Robert A.Perkins, Edwin 0. Timmons, Jack B. Parker, and William P.Addison, M. D. , The Effectiveness of the Head Start Program inEnhancing School Readiness inDifar= ent Cultural-Socio-EconomicGroups. (Unpublished HeadStart stu y, OEO Contract No. 570. Forfurther information, contact Dr. Hilliard Chesteen, Jr. , Schoolof Social Welfare, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana70803.)

19. Chicago Committee on UrbanOpportunity -- Project Head Start. (Private: non-0E0-funded Head Startstudy.

R-2 20. Church, Joseph, Language and the Discovery of Reality.New York: Random House, 19 16. 21. Cohnstaedt, Martin, and Peter H. Irons, A Study of OperationHead Start -- Greene County, Ohio.(Unpublished Head Start study,OEO Contract No. 572.For further information, contact Dr. Martin L. Cohnstaedt, Sociology Department, Antioch College,Yellow Springs, Ohio 45387.) 22. Colon, Manuel, Sociological Study of Head StartChildren of a Public Housing Project in the Virgin Isles.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 523. Forfurther information, contact Mr. Manuel Colon, L. C. 19, Caparra Terrace, SanJuan, Puerto Rico.) 23. County and City Data Book. Bureau of theCensus, 1962. 24. Cronbach, L. J. , Essentials of PsychologicalTesting. New York: Harper and Row, 1960. 25. Curwood, Sarah, A Surve and Evaluation ofProject Head Start as Established and Operated in Communities of theCommonwealth of Massachusetts Durin the Summer of19-65. (Unpublished Head Start Study, OEO Contract No.551. For further information, contact Dr. Sarah Curwood,Massachusetts Committee on Children and Youth, 9 Newberry Street, Boston,Massachusetts, 02159.) 26. Davis, Allison, "Society, the School, and theCulturally Deprived Student, " in Improving English Skillsof Culturally Different Youth. U. S. Department of Health, Education andWelfare, OE Bulletin No. 0E-30012, 1964, pp. 10-21. 27. Deutsch, Martin, "The Disadvantaged Child andthe Learning Process," in A. H. Passow (ed.), Education in DepressedAreas. New York: Teachers College Press, 1963. 28. Deutsch, M. and Bert Brown, "Social Influencesin Negro-White Intelligence Differences, " Journal of Social Issues,April 1964, Vol. 20, pp. 24-35. 29. Doi, Edith, Hawaii Head Start Project.(Final non-0E0-funded study.) 30. Dunn, Lloyd, Expanded Manual, PeabodPicture Vocabulary Test. Minneapolis: American Guidance Services, 19 5.

31. Edwards, Thomas J., Program Proposalfor Culturally Disadvantaged Preschool Children.Chicago: Science Research Associates,1965. 32. Edwards, Thomas J. , "The Language-ExperienceAttack on Cultural Deprivation," The Reading Teacher, Vol. 18, No.7 (1965), pp. 546-551.

R-3 33. Eisenberg, Leon,A. Change in Verbal Intelligence as Measured by the Peabody picture Vocabulary Test; B.Teacher Behavior and Attitudes in Relation to Chan es inthe Children; C. The Effects of Tutoring on Perceptual andConceptual Skills; D. The Effect of Special Perceptual Training onthe Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, the Columbia MentalMaturity Scale and a New Test, the Johns Ho skins Perce tualTask; E.Clinical Testing of Head Start Children; F. LanguageFunction in a Sample of Head Start Children.(Substudies from unpublished Head Start Study, OEO Contract No. 510.For further information, contact Dr. Leon. Eisenberg, Children'sPsychiatric Service, The Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore,Maryland 21205. ) 34. Fishman, Robert, A DemonstrationProject in the Development of Rural Child Care and Project Hope.(Final and preliminary reports, respectively, OEO ContractNo. 437-1D. For further information, contact Dr. RobertFishman, Director of Research, Department of Child Welfare,Frankfort, Kentucky 40601.) 35. Foley, Walter, Quarterly Progress Report onFollow-Up of Head Start Participants in the State of Iowa,No. 1, September 1965; No. 2, January 1966.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 522. For further information,contact Dr. Walter Foley, Educational Information Center,University- of Iowa, Ames, Iowa 50012. )

36. Frazier, Alexander, "A ResearchProposal to Develop the Lan-. guage Skills ofChildren with Poor Backgrounds, " in Improving English Skills of Culturally Different Youth.U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, OEBulletin No. 0E-30012, 1964, pp. 69-79. 37. Friedlander, George, A Study of theArticulatory and Intelligibility Status of Socially DisadvantagedPre-School Children. Final, unpublished Head Start study, OEO ContractNo. 536. For further information, contact Dr. George Friedlander,Yeshiva University, 55 Fifth Avenue, New York, NewYork 10003. )

38. Gage, N. L. (ed.), Handbook ofResearch on Teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1963. V 39. Getzels, J. W.,and P. W. Jackson, "TheTeacher's Personality and Characteristics, " Chapter11 in N. L. Gage (ed. ), Handbook of Research on Teaching. Chicago:Rand McNally and Co.,063. 40. Gordon, Edmund W.,"Characteristics of Socially Disadvantaged Children, " Review of EducationalResearch, December 1965, Vol. 35, pp. 373-388.

R -4 1"'".3Iimmermarriminob

41. Gordon, Edmund W.,"Relevance of Behavior Theory to Directed Learning in Culturaly and Socially Disadvantaged Children" (paper presented at40th Annual Meeting, American Orthopsychiatric Association, March1963). 42. Gordon, Sol, Irnct of Curriculum on Readin Readiness in Tosecoena and Sharkey County, Mississii.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No.57. For further information, contact Dr. Sol Gordon, Rutgers State University, Highland Park, New Jersey 08903. ) 43. Greater Anchorage Head Start Project (Local Head Start study, non - OEO- funded. ) 44. Grotberg, Edith, "Learning Disabilities and Remediation in Dis- advantagedChildren, " Review of Educational Research, Vol. 35, pp. 413-425. 45. "Guidelines for Testing Minority Group Children, " The Journal of Social Issues, April 1964, Vol. 20, pp. 129-145. 46. Guidelines: Special Programs for Educationally Deprived Chil- dren.U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, OE Bulletin No. 0E-35079, 1965 47.Harding, John, A Corn sarative Studof Various Pro ect Head Start Programs, Progress Report.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 545. For further information, contact Dr. John Harding, Department of Child Development and Family Relationships, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14851.) 48. Har:ris, Chester W. (ed. ), Problems in Measurin Chan e. Madison, Wisconsin: University of Wisconsin Press, 19 3.

49. Harris, Dale B., Goodenough Draw-a-Man Test. (Unpublished so Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 684. For further information, contact Dr. Dale B. Harris, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802.) 50. Havighurst, Robert, "Who Ara the Socially Disadvantaged? " Journal of Negro Education, Summer 1964; Vol. 33, pp. 210- ns. 51. Headstart Program in the Alexandria City Schools.(Final, non- 0E0-funded Head Start study. For further information, contact Alexandria City Schools, Alexandria, Virginia.)

R-5 52. Head Start Iteiort, Summer 1965, Warminster Township School District, Warminster, Pennsylvania.(Final report, non-0E0- funded. For further information, contact Warminster Township School District, Warminster, Pennsylvania.) 53. Health Supervisicsirl yoniECL..._,ildren in California, Findings of the 195Child Healt12SLuyj.ve State of California, Department of Public Health. 54. Hess, Robert D., Study of Assessment (Progress report, unpublished Head Start study, 0E0 Contract No. 519. For further information, contact Dr. Robert Hess, University of Chicago, Urban Child Center, Chicago, Illinois 60637. )

55. Higgins, Edward L., West Warwick Head Start - 65 HS 0069. (Non-(CE0-funded Head Start study. For further information, contact Dr. Edward L. Higgins, Director of Guidance and School Psychologist, West Warwick School Department, West Warwick, Rhode Island 02893.) 56. Hines, Ralph, "Social Expectations and Cultural Deprivation," Journal of Negro Education, Spring 1964, Vol. 33. 57. Hollander, Cornelia, Eight Weeks of Art.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 577. For further infor- mation, contact Mrs. Cornelia Hollander, 3502 Macomb Street, N. W.,Washington, D. C. 20016. ) 58. Holmes, Douglas and Monica Holmes, Evaluation of TwoAssociated YM-YWHA Head Start Pro rams. Start study, Contract No.5u.For further information, contact Drs. Douglas and Monica Holmes, YM-YWHA' s ofNew York, 33 West 60th Street, New York, New York 10023. ) 59. Horowitz, Frances and Howard M. Rosenfeld, Comparative Studies of a Grou of Head Start and a Grou of Non-Head Start Pre-Schooi. Children.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 521. For further information, contact Dr. Frances Horowitz, Department of Family Life, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas 66045. ) 60. Hunt, Y. McVicker, "How Children Develop Intellectually," Children, May -June 1964, Vol. 11, pp. 83-91. 61.Illness Among Children, Data from U.S. National Health Survey. Children Ts Bureau, U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1963.

R-6 62. Jacobs, Mildred, and James Shafer, Some Effects of Project Head Start on 178 Children in Cleveland County, Oklahoma.(Final, unpublished Head Start study, non-0E0funded. For further information, contact Mildred Jacobs or James Shafer, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma.) 63. John, Vera, Analysis of Story Retelling as a Measure of the Effects of Ethnic Content in Stories, Progress Report.(Un- published Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 577.For further information, contact Dr. Vera John, Yeshiva University, 55 Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003.) 64. Johnson, Henry S., The Effects of Montessori Educational Tech- niotles on Culturally Disadvantage,; Head Start Children.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 631. For fur- ther information, contact Dr. Henry S. Johnson or Dr. James Slaven (Director), Clavis Montessori Schools, 2025 East Chapman, Fullerton, California 92631.) 65. Kennedy, W.A., V. Van de Riet, and J. C. White, "Use of the Terman-Merrill Abbreviated Scale on the 1960 Stanford-Binet Form L-M on Negro Elementary School Children of the South- eastern United States," Journal of Consulting Psychology, Vol. 27 (1963), pp. 456-457. 66. Kerns, Robert D., West Virginia Statewide Impact on Children Served.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 541. For further information, contact Dr. Robert Kcrns, West Virginia Department of Mental Health, 1721 Quarrier Street, Charleston, West Virginia 25311.) 67. Krider, Mary A., An Evaluation of Head Start Pre-School En- richment Pro rams as The Affect the Intellectual Abili the Social Adjustment, and the Achievement Level of Five-Year-Old Children Enrolled in Lincoln, Nebraska.(Progress report, unpublished Head Start .study, OEO Contract No. 543. For fur- ther information, contact Dr. Mary Krider, Psychological Clinic, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, Nebraska 68508.) 68. Lamb, Howard, Project on the Development of Self-Other Con- cepts During Project Head Start.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 511.For further information, contact Dr. Howard E. Lamb, School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware 19711. ) 69. Loomis, James, Review and Evaluation of the Orientation Program . for Project Head Start Conducted by New York University, New York University, Division of General Education and Extension Services.

R-7 70. Lord, F.M., "Elementary Models for Measuring Change," Chapter 2 in C. W. Harris (ed. ), Problems in Measuring Change. Madison, Wisconsin: University of Wisconsin Press, 1963. 71. McGaughy, Robert H., Operation Head Start, Newark, Ohio, August 1965.(Final study, ron-0E0-funded. For further information, contact Mr. Robert McGaughy, 19 North Fifth Street, Newark, Ohio.) 72. Meyer, Henry J., et al.Girls at Vocational High: An Experiment in Social Work Intervention. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1965. 73. Miller, Martin B., and Barrie Cassileth, The Sub-Professional Worker in the Head Start Program. (Preliminary report, 0E0-: funded study. For further information, contact Dr. Martin Miller, Yeshiva University, New York, New York.) 74. Montez, Philip, Evaluation- -Head Start- - Bilingual Children. (Progress report, unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 547. For further information, contact Mr. Philip Montez, Foundation of Mexican-American Stucli-es, 1216 West Second Street, Los Angeles, California 90026.) 75. National Opinion Research Center, Collection of Social History, Experience Information and Parent Evaluation of Head Start on 1% Sample Nationwide.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 544. For further information, write NORC, 5720 Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, Illinois 60637.) 76. Nelson, W. E. (ed. ), Textbook of Pediatrics, 8th Edition. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1964. 77. Newark, New Jersey, Project Head Start. (Final non -OEO- funded study. For further information, contact United Community Corporation.) 78. operation Head Start, Charlestown, Massachusetts.(Local Head Start study, non-0E0-funded.) 79. Orshansky, Mollie, Counting the Poor: Another Look at the Poverty Profile, Social Security Administration, January 1965. 80. Ortiz, Alfonso, Pro'ect Head Start in a New Mexico Indian Com- munity.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 539. For further information, write Mr. Alfonso Ortiz, Depart- ment of Anthropology, University of Chicago, 1126 East 59th Street, Chicago, Illinois 60637. )

R -8 81. Osborne, R. 11., "Racial Differences in Mental Growth and School Achievement: A Longitudinal Study," Psychological Reports, Vol. 7 (1960), pp. 233-239. 82. Ozer, Mark, The Effects of Neurological and Environmental Factors on the Language Development of Head Start Children: An Ev4luation of the Head Start Program.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 528. For further information, write Dr. Mark Ozer, Children's Hospital, 2125 13th Street, N. W.,Washington, D. C. 20009. ) 83. Palomares, Vvaldo H., and Henry S. Johnson, A Study of Some Ecological, Economic and Social Factors Influencing Parental Participation in Project Head Start and A Pilot Study of Com- munity Opinion and Understanding of the Coachella Valley Project Head Start.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 534. For further information, write Mr. Vvaldo Palomares or Dr. Henry S. Johnson, University of California Extension, Riverside, California 92507. ) 84. Passow, A. Harry (ed. ), Education in Depressed Areas. Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, 1963. 85. Pettigrew, J.F., "Negro American Personality: Why Isn't More Known?" Journal of Social Issues, April 1964, Vol. 20, pp. 4-23. 86. Pierce-Jones, John, S. Thomas Friedman, and William E. Barron, Outcomes of Individual and Pro rammatic Variations Amon- Pro- ject Head Start Centers, 1965.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 508. For further information, contact Dr. John Pierce-Jones, Personnel Services of Research Center, University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712. )

87. Porter, Philip J.,Evaluation of Head Start Educational Program in Cambridge, Mass.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 529.For further information, contact Dr. Philip J. Porter; Chief, Pediatric Service, Cambridge City Hospital, Howard Medical School, Cambridge, Massachusetts. ) 88. Prather, Misha S., Richard D. Alter, O.J. Harvey, James K. Hoffnieister, and B. Jack White, Project Head Start Teacher- Pupil-Parent Interaction Study.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 517. For further information, contact Dr. Misha S. Prather, University of Colorado, 1165 Broadway, Boulder, Colorado 80304. ) 89. Project Head Start One-Percent Sam le Summarization Wei:hted), U.S. Bureau of the Census, Demographic Operations Division, November 1965.

R-9 90. Project Head Start Staff Trainina Program for 1965, August 196'5. (Final, unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 170. For further information, contact National University Extension Association, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland. ) 91. Project Head Start, Summer 1965: A Summary Report.(Arch- diocese of Chicago School Board, non -OEO- funded final report. For further information, contact Archdiocese of Chicago School Board, Chicago, Illinois. ) 92. Raph, Jane B., Language Research Study--Project Head Start. (Final, unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 535. For further information, contact Dr. Jane Raph, Department of Educational Psychology, Rutgers State University, New Brunswick, New Jersey.) 93. Rice, Robert R., Housing and Home Environment from Head Start Population.(Unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 583. For further information, contact Mr. Robert R. Rice, University of Missouri, 1003 Hickory Hill Drive, Columbia, Missouri 65202. ) 94. Riessman, Frank, The Culturally Deprived Child. New York: Harper and Row,19-62. Reissman, Frank, "The Overlooked Positives of Disadvantaged Groups, " Journal of Negro Education, Summer 1964, Vol. 33, pp. 225-231. 95. Scheffe, Henry, The Analysis of Variance. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1959.

96. Sigel, Irving, Comparative Stud -:Head Start Children and Con- trol.Relationship of Language to Classification and Behavior. (Final, unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 542. For further information, contact Dr. Irving Sigel, Palmer In- stitute of Home Development and Family Life, 71 East Terry Avenue, Detroit, Michigan 48202. ) 97. Silberstein, Richard, Comparative Study: Head Start and _3 Control Classes (2 Disadvantaged and 1 Middle Class).(Final, unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 516. For further information, contact Dr. Richard Silberstein, The Staten Island Mental Health Society, 657 Costelton Avenue, Staten Island, New York 10301. ) 98..Smiley, Marjorie, "Research and Its Implications," in Improving English Skills of Culturally Different Youth.U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, OE Bulletin No. 0E-30012, 1964.

R-10 99. Soule, Allen, Northfield, Vermont: A Community Depth Study. (Final, unpublisiled Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 621. For further information, contact Dr. Allen Soule, Central Vermont Community Action Council, Montpelier, Vermont 05602.)

100. Sprig le, Herbert, Outline for Preschool Education.(Final, un- published Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 745.For futher information, contact Dr. Herbert Sprig le, Learning to Learn School, 1936 San Marco Boulevard, Jacksonville, Florida. )

101. Stern, G. G.,"Measuring Noncognitive Variables in Research on Teaching," Chapter 9 in N. L. Gage (ed. ), Handbook of Research on Teaching.Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1963. 102. Summary of Head Start Observations, Chicago.(Local Head Start study, non-0E0-funded.) 103. Swift, Joan W., "Effects of Early Group Experience: The Nursery School and Day Nursery," in Hoffman and Hoffman (eds. ), Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Found- ation, 1964. 104. Tomlinson, Ethel, "Language Arts Skills Needed by Lower Class Children," Elementary English, May 1956, Vol. 33, pp. 279- 283. )

105. Van Egmond, Elmer, George Miller, Sandra Jackanicz and George Cheong, Operation Head Start: An Evaluation.(Final unpublished Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 537.For further information, contact Dr. Elmer Van Egmond, Lesley College, 29 Everett Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138.)

106. Wallen, N. E.,and R. M. W. Travers, "Analysis and Investigation of Teaching Methods," Chapter 10 in N. L. Gage (ed. ), Handbook of Research on Teaching.Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1963. 107. Wax, Murray, and Rosalie Wax, Indian Communities and Project Head Start and An Appraisal of Possibilities for a Head Start Program Among the Potawatomi Indiaw of Kansas.(Final, un- published Head Start study, OEO Contract No. 520. For further information, contact Drs. Murray and Rosalie Wax, Department of Sociology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas 66045.) 108. Wirtz, Morvin A., "Increased Concern for Disadvantaged Children and Youth: Implications for the Education of the Mentally Retarded," Education and Trainin of the Mentall Retarded, February 1966, Vol. 1, pp. 8-10.

R-11 109. Withall, J., and W. W. Lewis, "Social Interaction in the Class- room," Chapter 13 in N. L. Gage (ed. ), Handbook; of Research on Teaching.Chicago: Rand McNally and Co.,1963.

110. Wolff, Max, Effect of Pre-School Education on Adjustment and Achievement in Elementary School and study of Recruitment Methods Used by Head Start Centers. Factors Contributing to Success or Failure, Parent Motivation and Response to Recruit- ment Efforts.(Final, unpublished Head Start study, 0E0 Con- tract No. 577. For further information, contact Dr. Max Wolff, 59 Stephenson Boulevard, New Rochelle, New York 10801. )

111. Woolman, Myron, Cultural Asynchrony and Contingency in Learning Disorders.Washington, D. C, :Institute of Educational Research, 1965 (Multilith).

112. Zimmerman, Dorothy, Gains Made by 38 Head Starters on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test of Mental Maturity, Percentile Ranks of Head Starters and non-Head Starters on Readiness Inventory the Second Week of School, and Traits of a First Grader.(Non- 0E0-funded Head Start study. For further information, contact Mrs. Dorothy Zimmerman, Supervisor of Instruction, County Schools, Caswell Action Committee, Inc., P.O. Box 566, Yanceyville, North Carolina 27379. )

113. Gerard, Roslyn, Dunbar Center, Syracuse, N.Y.(Final study, non-0E0-funded. )

114. Evaluation of Head Start (1965), Southern Consumers' Education Foundation, Lafayette, Louisiana.

115. Jewett, Arno, Joseph Mersand, and Doris V. Gunderson (eds.), Improving English Skills of Culturally Different Youth in Large Cities.U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, OE Bulletin No. 0E-30012, 1964.

116. Moore, Alexander M., "Personal and Professional Qualifications for Teachers of Culturally Different Youth," in Arno Jewett, et al (eds. ), Improving English Ski of Culturally Different Youth in Large Cities.U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, OE Bulletin No. 0E-30012, 1964.

117.Economic Opportunity Council of the Ozarks, Inc., Mountain Home, Arkansas 72653.Paul F. Mummey, Director.

118. West Valley Child Development Center, E. 8818 Grace, Spokane, Washington 99206. Marvin D. Smith, Director. 119. Cincinnati Public Schools, 608 E. McMillan Street, Cincinnati 6, Ohio Anna M. Evans, Director of Early Childhood Education.

120. Pennsylva!,ia Avenue School, Project Head Start, Atlantic City, New Jersey 08401.Jane M. Flipping, Project Supervisor. in. Snyder Consolidated Independent Schools, Snyder, Texas 79549. Dr. Robert L. Clinton, Superintendent. 122. Resnick, Melvyn, Completed File Copy of Reports on Head Start (7,enier.,-. with Non-English Speaking Children, Summer, 195.(Final unpublished Head Start consultants' study. For furthe information, contact Melvyn Resnick, Department of Languages and Linguistics; University of Rochester, Rochester, New If ork 14627.) Reilf, Donald G., The Language Situation in Project Head Start Censers 1965.(Final unpublished Head Start study, 0E0 Con- tract No. 93Z.For further information, contact Dr. Donald .Reiff,aeiff, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627.)

2 1. Cochran, W. G.,"Analysis of Covariance: Its Nature and Uses," Biometrics, 13, Z61-281, 1957.

125. Siegel, S.,Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956. 126. Cou-ey, 111.W., and P.R. Lohnes, Multivariate Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1962. CD Lc-N cv 00 RESULTS OF THE SUMMER 1965 ti--f CD PROJECT HEAD START Ca 141

PRC R-795

VOLUME II APPENDIXES

9 May 1966

Prepared for

OFFICE OF ECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY 00 DIRECTOR, PRO JECT HEAD START RESEARCH ANDEVALUATION

PLANNING RESEARCH CORPORATION 0PoiLOS ANGELES, CALIFORNT WASHINGTON, D.C. C/) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION &WELFARE

OFFICE OF EDUCATION

THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRODUCED EXACTLYAS RECEIVED FROM THE

PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIGINATINGIT.POINTS OF VIEW OR OPINIONS

STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENTOFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDUCATION

POSITION OR POLICY. RESULTS OF THE SUMMER 1965 PROJECT HEAD START

PRC R-795

VOLUME II APPENDIXES

9 May 1966

Prepared for

OFFICE OF ECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY DIRECTOR, PROJECT HEAD START RESEARCH AND EVALUATION Under Contract 0E0-753

By H. Russell Cort, Jr. William D. Commins, Jr. Kenneth L. Deavers Ruth Ann O'Keefe James F. Ragan, Jr.

PLANNING RESEARCH CORPORATION LOS ANGELES, CALIFORNIA WASHINGTON, D.C. 0 Cf) ..."...... "'",'...... T.I.P...... "M.,rM"....."IMMRrMwl-..",'"'.nsm

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page APPENDIX A Data Collection and Test Instruments, Project Head Start 1965 A- 1 APPENDIX B Statistical Models and Analyses B- 1 APPENDIX C Height and Weight Tables C- 1 APPENDIX D Further Definitions of the Population Served D- 1 APPENDIX E IQ Scores of Culturally Disadvantaged Children Who Were Tested on More Than One Intelligence Test E- 1 APPENDIX F Analysis of CDC Differences F-1 APPENDIX G Worker Evaluation Responses G- 1 APPENDIX H Additional Head Start Studies H- 1 APPENDIX I The Preschool Inventory (PSI) and the Behavior Inventory (BI) I- 1

iii APPENDIX A DATA COLLECTION AND TEST INSTRUMENTS Pa0 =C T HEAD START 1965

Following is a list of tests or questionnairesadministsired in con- necticn with the summer 1965 Head Start program.1 Copies of these tests are appended herewith. A. Children Medical/Dental and Family Information Preschool Inventory2 Behavior Inventory Psychological Screening Procedure

B. Staff Staff Member Information Sheet Paid and Voluntary Workers' Evaluation Workers' Attitude Scale2

C. Parents Parent Evaluation Parent Participation Record (completedby staff)

D. Other Consultants' Checklist National Opinion Research CenterParent Interview (replaced the Social History/Social ExperienceInventory)

1Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test has notbeen included. 2Administered pre- and post-Head Start as a measureof impact.

A-1 C A11, HS 31 PAGE 1 t. - 1 BUDGETCOPYFORM APPROVEDBUREAU NO. JUNE 116.6503 30. 1966 canfidentlal.gcglithjilAll information which would Will fig thoOrtd griCt1t11; _ MEDICAL/DENTALOFFICE OF ECONOMICAND 'FAMILY INFORMATIONOPPORTUNITY PROJECT HEAD START I Card 0 02 (1'2) 1.I IDENTIFICATION Child Development Center No. OM- ..__ 1 (4-14) 2. Child's Identification No. (State Code) I (15-21) 3a. Child's Home (Last) (22-36) (CrantNo.) - (First) (37-47) (48)(11I) (49-51)(Suffix) 4. Date of this Report" (moat's)(72-73) (any) (74-75)(76-77) (year) .../..,___/- 3b.5. County II.Child's GENERAL of Address: Residence - CHILD (52-71 6. How old is child? 7. Birthdote: - 8. Sex: Male Card ft 03 (1-2) (72) (Street and Number or RFD) (22-41) (Years) (Months) (Month)(66-67) (Da) (Yar)(68-69) (70-71) / / 01 Female 02 (City of Place) (42-61) (State Abbr.) 02:1511. 9. Is the child (Check one) AsianWhite 03 Eskimo 04 1 Negro 2 UnknownAmerican 05 Indian 73) 10. Is the child (check one) Puerto Rican 01 Other 3 Mexican American 02 I (74) T11. a. Has the child been hospitalized (overnightMEDICAL/DENTAL or longer) at -any HISTORY time Whatduring oYes problem 01 made the hospital stay necessary? No Ei 2 Don't Know 03 the past year? ((75) b. j6 If Yes, how many times? DI b. What was specific reason? 2 or 3 Ej2 II 4 or more 03 (76) 13. Was the child born in a: HospitalAn illness 01 01 At home 02 Surgery 02 injury from nn accident 03Other D3 Don't Know 04 Tonsillectomy/Adenoidectomy 04 (78) 14. About how many pounds t:id the child weigh at birth ? ,(Give to nearest pound) Under 5 rit Other 05 5-7 02 8 - 10 0 3 Over 10 04 Cecd aE a4 (t -it /DXWLat. 16.15. WhatWas there was theanything matter unusual (diagnosis)? about the child or was anything wrong when Vas 01 No 02 Don't Know 03 /DXB/ he(she) was born? (22)1 18. If general health is fair or,poor, what is the t'ouble? /PX2//PX1/ 19. Which17. How best is the describes child's general the eaIC-activity? heelth?a. Not able to take part in ordinary play Very good 01 with other children Di Good 02 Fair 3 12a Poor 20. How long has it been In the past since the child1 -2 has visited a doctor? 2 YearsMore than Never 0 4 /PX3/I (25), e.I. AbleNot limitedto play in with any other of these children, ways but limited in amount or kind of 03 play 23. What was the illness or injury? 21. How long has it been 12In months 12.the pastmonths DI Di since theYears child1 - 202 has visited a dentist? 2 YearsMore than 03Eh Never Cal I (2s) 22. Has the child had any serious illness or occident in the past year? Don't Know 3 IMO /DXL/ 24.IV. IMMUNIZATIONa. Hwy, many baby HISTORY shots (DPT) has the child received? Yes r-71 No 02 404 505 Can't remember 06 (28) b. Series is: Complete 111 Inc:Kai:let., 09 Series is: Uncertain L. j3 r__,1 (29) 27.25. a.Any Any Salk measles poliovirus vaccine vaccine received? received? NonaYes 00 01 101 202 303 No 02 Don't Know 03 33 b. If Yes, type3 of vaccine: (30 26. a. Now many oral poliovirus vaccine? None 00 1 Q1 Live, attenuatodg 2 2 02, 4 3 or 4 03 Can't Remember 04 I (34) 21'. Any tuberculosis vaccine (BCG) received?(31) b. Complete 01 Incomplete 0 2 Uncertain Don't Know 104. 35 3 3 30.29. a. HasIs vaccination the child received scar resent? a tuberculin test before YesYes 01 No D1112 I No 2 Don'tNot Know sure 03 12_62 b. How many years since last smallpox vaccination Live, with Gammathis Globulin Summer? Within last 3 years 01 Killed 01 3 Combination of these F787:1 b. Did the Nurse or Doctor soy it was More than 3 years 02 Never 03 Yes Di Don't Know 04 No 02 (39) Yes 01 No 02 Don't Know 3 ,...... Positive DI Negative 02 Can't Remember 03 44 .. 40 31.a- Has the child been exposed to someone with active Yes - Probably No r-t 2 on t Know tuberculosis? 3 [±sg b yes, whet is that person's relationship to chi PLEASE DO NOT US COPY MEDICAL/DENTALOFFICE AND OF FAMILYECONOMIC INFORMATION OPPORTUNITY PROJECT HEAD START Name ChildV. EXAMINATIONDevelopment Center No. 65H5-... (41-43) (44-46) (47-49) (50-52) (Last (First) (53) (MI) (SO Childs Identification No. 52. (a)(b)Vision Acuity Were screening glasses - Right worntest ey.:_/_ for test: (53-36) Yes111Left eye:-/-;_Abnorrnal 1(57-51) Not]2 None usually wom 3 Test normal 2 Not don 3 3 (59)(54) 33. (a) PerformedDental Examination by - Dentist (to be 1completed Physician by 02 a dentist Nurse or [J his assistant or from the dentists examination report.) (48) (d)(c) Hyperopia,Cover test, plusNear lens - Right (60) Right LeftLift(61) ____; ; Distant - RightAbnormal 1 (62) Test normal *2 Not don Lek(63) (b) Findings: Caries Ti (69) Infection _Ji (72) Malocclusion Number of carious teeth 1 (75) (73) Peridentel..--... Disease 1 (74) Other 4 (70-71) (e) Worth(g) Results Dot Test of screening: Unsatisfactory Pass1 Abnormal 1 Referral for further diagnosis or treatment Fall 2 (65) 01 Unsatisfactory, referred Test normal 1ili2 (f) Other abnormality Dascribe:--. for retesting 02 No referralNot don 03 3 noted (67) (66)(64) (c) Disposition: No Referral NoDescribe: Disease 3 OtherOtherDescribe: 03 Di (76) Referral or plan for edditional Dental Care 02 34. Child's Weight Lbs. (22-24) 35. Child's4 Height -; 1 Inches . Card N OS (1.2) (25-26) 36. Given:Tuberculin test given at this Center: Type of test (Check one): Mantoux 1 Tine 2 Patch 1(27-33) 37. b. a.Area of induration at test sitePositiveResults,of tuberculin test don at the Center - 1 Negative 2 Uncertain 3 (34)mm. (35-36) 39. b. a.UrineSugar: tests: Albumin: Positive (month) / (day) / (year) 1 Negative 2 (311)2 (37) TestTest used used and and result result 39. BloodMethod determination: Test used and result - MicrohematocritHemoglobin L(32) P (40-41) %Gm. 40. a. Screening test for hearing: Positive (42) b. Results: 1 (43) Referral: --. (44) Not done 01 Audiometry 02 Voice.011=111111M11106, or other D3 Unsatisfactory Normal test 0 2 Abnormal test 03 OtolaryngologistriNone Ell Hearing and 3 Other Speech Center 02 4 2 VI. DIAGNOSES AND IMPRESSIONS Check these diagnoses below that represent the physician's impression of the child's Present Suspect medical status. Present Suspect rl Present SuspectI(45 -65) 41.42. Astiums Nayfever Present Suspect 0 2 48.47. AnemiaTonsil and/or Adenoid Disease Di 22 53.55.54. Other FrequentUrinary Orthopedic Tract Gastrointestinal Infection Problems 0 1 1 0 2 60.59. KidneyConvulsive Disease Disorder O 2 2 43.45.4A. Other FrequentSkin Allergies Infection Respiratory or Ober Illness Disease 0 2 2 51.50.49. Hernia,ChronicHeart Discos. InguinalOtitis Media D i 0 2 2 57.56. SpeechEnuresis Abnormality Upset O 2 2 63.62.61. MentalCerebralNutritional Retardation Palsy Disorder Di 0 2 22 65.dd.66. BronchitisEnter /DX1/ the item number of the diagnosis(s) of Write Out Diagnoses if Not in Above List, Enter One To A Line: 0 2 which the parent has52. Orthopedic Problem Feet, Lower Extremities been aware: DDi i (69-70) 2 58. Parasitic(71-72) Infestation (73-74) (75-76) 2 64. Other Problem(s) 1Di 2 67. /DX2/ 411011NO 70.69.6$. Was/DX3//DX4/ the parent aware of any of the additionalWrite Outdiagnoses? Other impressions Below: Yes 0 1 No 02 (77) 72.71. /IM 1/ 1.42/ VP 73. /1M3/ 1110011111111111miwarridellielliilillillialerrruricarilsill ORIGINAL1.11=1=110111.1111/1111111111111110, - To ks returned MEDICAL/DENTAL.11.1 AND FAMILY INFORMATIONFICE 0 EC PROJECT HEAD START 0 OF2 'cation o. ChildYll. Development FAMILY CenterHISTORY N.. 65HS, I, Name (Last) 4...,,=1, aufhc) .601111 Tirratiti 111 74.75. Who Mother's deem Nome the child live with: Mother and Father? I Mother 02 Father 03 Other (Last) (First) (MI) 04 (sal) If Other, what76. isAge: the relationship of that person: Years 77. Is the Mother deceased? Yes 01 No 02 Don't Know 03 1 78. If the child lives with Mother only 79.80. (e) (a) What How was many the full highest tern: pregnanciesschool gradeIs thehas completed0011-6E12 Motherthe Mother nowby the had? Mother? 7-803 9-1104 Widowed 1 Divorced 2 Separated 03 Other 4 , Not sure 1. (b) No. of children(b) High living?School graduate? Yes Ell No 2; (c) Any college? Yes 1 No 2 Don't know 1 (d) Don't Knew level completed CM $1. (a) Does the Mother usually work (d)(c)(b)outside IfIfWhat thethe the MotherMothertype home, of usuallyusuallywork or is is workssite worksthis? looking Chockwhatdoes workforshe One: a work:doesjob now? she do when she Is employed? ServiceProfessional, or Private Technical, Household ManagerialFull worker time Di 4 Ell Farmer OS Part time 1:232 Once in a while 1:::13 Yes 01 No []2 Not Sure []3 Clerical, Sales =12 Laborer 06 Craftsman, foreman, Operative 03 $3.112. About Was shehew out long of waswork she and out looking of work forTwe anda lob Months looking at any orfortime Less a duringlob ED in thethe pastpast year?year? 3 -6 Months 02 7 -9 Months 03 10 -12 Months 04 Yes Ell No 2 Don't Know OS Can't Remember 3 Student 7 V.84. If Father's the child Name lives with Father but withoutraTiMt wasthe Mother, the highest Is the school Father grade - completed by the Father? (Lest) .---( ritit)--. Widowed Di Divorced 2 Separated E33 Other (M/) (Son (b) High School graduate? I ir 7r= (Years r Yes (c) Any college? 4 06. Is the Father deceased: []l No 02 (d) Don't Knew level completed pi Don't Knew 3 119. (a) Does the Father usually either(e)(b) IfIfwork01 the the Fatheroutside 1.402Father usually the usually home, 7-803 works, works,or iswhat he 9-1104 lookingworkdoes does he for work ahe lob do now?- when Full timehe Is employed?l Part time 2 Once in a while 03 Yes Ej 1 Yes Di No 02 No Eji Net Sure 03 Yes Ell No 02 90. Was the Father cut if work and looking(d) What for a jobtype at of any work time Is this?during Check the past One. year? ServiceProfessional, or Private Technical, Household Managerial Worker Eji 04 Yes 01 Clerical,Farmer ESsales 2 No C:32 Laborer 06 Craftsman, foreman,Can't Remember Operative 3 3 Student 01 91.92. About Has hewthe family long was received the Father any public out Twoof assistance work Months and lookingor or ADC Less for payments 01 a job? In the past 3 -6 Nooks 02 7 -9 Months 3 yoga 10 -12Yes Months Di Ne 0 2 4 Don't Knew 03 Don't Know 05 ^=f c=1:4 es (Cent'd) (711) 94.93. AboutWho usually hew much took income care of from the childall sources duringLess thandid the your day $1000 familylast 1year? have$1000.$1999 Parent during 2 $2000-12999 0 3 $3000.$3999 El the past twelve months? (Include public assistance04 $4000.$4999 checks) 05 0500045999Nursery 6 $6000 Scheel -$7999 7 $11000.$1999 8 $10,000 Plus 2 Baby Sitter[] 3 Sibling 4 Oth.or Adult Relative* Os 09 Dee't Knew 0 Other 6 TY.96.95. OfHew Location this many number, of people home how live amany child: together are Urban children in the and household? under Suburban 16 years 1 of age? arstir Farm []2 f...=fothes= Rural, Non-Farm 3 FI:=1:13 Other 04 For-I:rent =1 00=1 .-.-C=1-1=1---7_71--.'L iNere: ;:r.e rr-erri exr:-ss *:

PUSGIO0 ;NIVNTORY

Time started Time 'iinished

Chi!dts. name Date of test

Picce of test Development Center ID number

Cnilc-s ID number Exammer's staff ID number

motor languace

Lar,,-,-7,:aga in which tes'.- was given

CAP -HS 7CRM 42 JUN 65

Ilorlos . PRESCHOOL INVENTORY 11,!!in try asking Ow child the fo:lcwing questions: KNOWS: YES NO

- . V o

. .ast r.ar.-e. For ex:::;4:e, 2'..:onnny ';Vnotts your last name?" 2. 3. Give the child a sheet of plain white paper and a crayon and say, "Draw me a picture of a man. .. awhole man, not just part of a man." Then do the same with Now draw a picture of a woman... awhole woman, not just part of L. woman." 3. 4. How old are you? 4. 5. When is your birthday? (Score yes for month or date) 5. 6. Where do you live? (Address, location of housing project, etc.) 6. 7. Whca school will you go to? 7. S. What is your teacher's name? 8. 9. Who are some of the children in your group?" (Probe for four names. If child says first name only, probe for last name; e.g. "Tommy? Tommy who?") Circle number of first names given. 9.0 1 2 3 4

10. Circle number of last names given. 10.0 1 2 3 I. Point to the following parts of the examiner's body and say, "What's this?" For all items missed in 11-20, go through again, say, "show me your .11 I. II. Wrong Gives Or Name Wrong D.K. Shows D.K. 11. U. Ear ".""''.1 12. Finger 12. 13. Neck 13. 14. Back 14. /.1.8 15. Eye 15. 10111 .NIND 16. Elbow 16. IN. 17. Heel 17. 1110 18. Shoulder 18. =11i, 19. Eyebrow 19. 20. Knee 20. N."7/ tro. c:ueslions: Y do you hove?" ht /so "-

29.

. 2Z. 25.

26. Lt. nn 4.: . 29. Necks nC e'' ;n orals (o- somethi-c: else the chido by curly doesni; have, to o.:si' "none" orno;- cny)

Now ask, "How many wheels does a have ?"

1. Cc- n ..)!CVC:0 3z. -.r cycle (or baby bicycle) nn

. 32.. 25. 7.:*.owoc!-

36. "Let's :Icor you count ou: !ouct". responses, start chi!ci by saying, . Circle numper &wen, up five. 36. 37. ('-lo;d quo niece "Do you know what a corner :s? ,r1^:,'1/ MC . :37. con Cr"il --low many corners does this sheet of paper have ?" 38 :cnows doesn't For ''he next few items taki- out the Sox of 12 checkers, all the come color, tho child the opportz..r.ity to rrani::.uic*-e '-hem briefly.

Seeing all the checkers touch one another and occupy moreor less the sc-1?*. arc.sn, (all flat on table), out ;re checkers in two groups in front of the as fo;:ows and ask (pointing first to one, then fhe other):

Cr-ups of checkers Right Wrong 39. 2 7., 3 "Which one has more checkers in it?" .40. 5 C 6 "Which one has more checkers in it?" 40.

41. 6 one has more checkers in it?" 4 1. 42. Recombine and make two groups, 8 and 2. Say, pointing; Which group has fewer/less?" 42. Take away all but 5 of the checkers.Instruct the child as follows: "Put these checkers next to each other in a line/row." See to it that a half-inch space is made between each two checkers. Give whatever guidance is needed to yield a fairly straight row. Say:

Right Wron2ILI 43. "Give me the middle one." (Note: Credit first-last hi terms of 43. a child's choice; i.e. either end of 44. "Give me the first one." 44. the row of blocks. All subsequent 45. "Give me the last one." choices should be consistent with 45. that choice, however.) 46. "Give me the second one." 46. 47. "Give me the next-to-last one." 47. Next, line up the checkers in a row, all touching. Take out the two black checkers and stack one on top of the other at one end to make an engine. Say, "Let's pretend this is a train. You know what a train is, 1 don't you? You know, it has a lot of cars, one after the other, like this." 48. "Do you know what we call the first car, the one that pulls the train? (Probe to elicit engine.) 48. 49. "What do we call the last car on a freight train?" If no correct response 49. is given to either of the above: 50. "What pulls the train, the engine or the caboose?" 50. 51. "What do we call the last car on the freight train, the engine or the caboose? 51. Detach the page with the line, triangle, circle, and square drawn on it. Give it to the child. Ask him: I. Gives D,K, Points Gives/Similar or Name Object Wrong Yes N.) 52. "What do we call this? (Circle) 52. 53. (Line) 53. 54. (Square) 54. 55. (Triangle) 55.nremr If child cannot name shape, ask him to point to ones missed. (Column II). Using the same sheet, say to the child, "Now I'd like you to make some drawings. Make one like this," (and point to):

lJ i..

Recognizable Linrecognizc':,:c

56. Line 56. 37. Circle 57.

...,vg o Square 58. 59. ,,.-,,Tr'angle Now ask the child to point to "the onewhich is most like a it Right Wron- 60. Wheel 60. 61. Window 61. 62. Piece of string 62. 63. Tent or teepee 63. 64. Ice cream cone 64. 65. Plate/dish 65. 66. Stick 66. Take the paper from the child and continuewith: ?u "Which is bigger, a or a 67.i.':c...:i or bicycle 67. 68. Tree or flower 68. 69. Telephone or television 69. 70. Man or boy 70. 71. Mosquito er grasshopper 71. 72. Fly or butterfly 72. )ii "Which usually goes slower, a or :3 73. Horse or dog 73. 74. Ccr or bicycle 74. 75. Train or rocket 75. ?ti "Which is li-eavier, a or a 76. Butterfly or bird 76. 77. Brick or shoe 77. 78. Feather or fork 78. 5.

Say, "Good. Now let's try something different. "I want you to do some things for me." Right Wrong 79. Close your eyes. 79. 80. Raise your hand. 80. 81. Show me your teeth. 81. 32. Show me your fingernails. 82. 83. Wigg!e. 83. 84. Say "hello" very loudly. 84. 85. Say "hello" very softly. 85. 36. Standup. 86. 87. Turn around(all the way around). 87. 88. Face the door. 88. 89. Jump. 89. 90. Sit down. 90. Thank the child and continue with, "I want you to think of all the things your mother gives you to eat at mealtime, and the things she gives you to eat with, 91. Name 7311File things you can think of." (Copy 91. verbatim, if possible, in this space: 0 1-3 4-6 7-9 10+

ing117----111says not ing after 10 seconds, Say "youknow like bread and forks." Stop after 30 seconds if child says nothing. Let him continue if he appears to be still thinking. Now place the 8 crayola crayons (or any similar high intensity crayons of red, orange,yellow, green, blue,purple, brown, and black) on the table. Mix them up and line them up about 1/2 inch apart. Say "What color is this" for each (Column I).If child does not name all correctly, for those missed, say, "Give me the one," (Column 11).Replace the one he hands you each time. I. U. Names Gives Right Wrong 'Right Wrong 92. Red 92. 93. Yellow 93. 94. Orange 94. 1 95. Green 95. 96. Blue 96. 97. Purple 97. 98. Brown - 98. 99. Black 99. 6.

With the crayons stillonthe te'o.:e ask him the folowing questions.If he gives cn incorrect crswer or indicates he doesn't know, have him show you or give you the co:cr.If he still misses, score wrong. 3e certain 'there is a sheet of white paper in sght for the snow rues'-ion.

Says IL Pointed "Vtirrat color is ?" il. "Show me. Right Wrong R1ght Wror- 'AAiv.). Fre ;red, orange, or yellow) 100. 101. Grass 101. 102. Snow 102. 103. Ca-rot 103. 104. The sky (blue) 104. 105. Night (blue, black) 105. "Have yeu ever been on a swing? You know how a swing goes -- up and down and back and forth? (Accompany with gesture). Says Shows

Right Wrong Right Wronc: 106. Which way does a saw co? 106. 107. Which wcy does an elevator go? 107. 108. Winich way does a Ferris wheel go? 108. 109. Which way does a'phonograp'n record go? 109. 110. Which way does a waterfall go? 110. Wr'.i-c down in the blank exactly what the child says. Code responses as 2 (clear, correct), 1 (approximation), 0 (wrong). Mark D.K. if no response is given or the child says, "I don'tknow." 1112fi '111110 Wrong D.K.

111. When do we eat breakfast? 111. 112. What day do people go to church? 112. 113. What day is today? 113. 114. When your mother says it's time to go to bed, what is it like outside? 114. 115. What do we cell the time of year when it's hottest?115.1 116. What do we call the time of year when it's coldest? 116. 117. What time of year is it now? 117. 118. If your mother wanted to call up.and talk to a friend, 118. what would she use? wow 7.

Wrong 119. If you want to find a lion where would you look? 119. 120. If you wanted to buy some gas, where would you go? 120. 121. If you were sick, who would you go to? 121. 122. If you wanted to find a boat, where would you look? 122. 123. If you wanted to read some what would you do? 123. mmoom.0. Take out the three c-irs, red, yellow, and blue; take out the three boxes,black, white and green. Be sure theblack box is 6attoms up. After each item, make sure all cars and all boxes are visible and available; 'i.e., do not leave a car in a box, etc. Giveeach instruction only once. Make sure he is looking and listening, and say the wordsslowly. 124. Put-a car on a box. 124.ON 125. Put a car in a box. 125. IN 126. Put a car under a box. 126. UNDER.

127. -Put the red car on Ihe black box. 127. RED BLACK ON E, 128. Put the blue car on the green box.. 128. BLUE ON GREEN

129. Put the yellow car on the little box. 129. YELLOW ON LITTLE

130. Put one car in the middle-sized box. 130.ONE iN M!D -S 131. Put all the cars on one side of the table and all the boxes on the other side. 131.ALL CARS ONE ALL BOXES OTHER 132. Put 3 cars in the big box. 132. 3 IN BIG 133. Put 2 cars behind the box in the middle. 133. 2 BEHIND MIDDLE 134. Give everything to me. 134.All cars and all boxes In this section, write down exactly what the child says. Also mark category, as indicated inthe manual. FUNCTION ASSOCIATION WRONG D.K. SupportiveRestrictive

135. What does a doctor do? .135. .1=111014.. 136. What does a policeman do?

136.

137. What does a dentist do?

137. 8. FUNCTION ASSOC;ATION WRONG D.K. SupportiveRestrictive I 133. Wrict does a teacher do? .)0

139. What does c father do? 139.

140. What does a nurse do? 140.

141. What doet.. a mother do? 141. 1 1 : i 1 ! i 142. What does c soldier do? 142. I I

Detach the printed sheet of pictures and give it to the child. Say, "See these pictures?lira going to drew a line from the boy to the cake, like this." Draw a !ine with the pencil. Hand crayon to child and say, "Now you do it." Take his hand and help him trace it, if necessary. Yes No 142. ":"races successfully. '143. After you have ascertained that the chi!d draw a line, scy, "I want ycu to draw fcme more iines for me, one at a time. a line from the to the .1/ 144. Bird to wagon 144.

145. Ciock to cake 145.

146. Dog to boy 146.

147. Girl to ball 147. 148. Bird to other bird 148. 0

TEACHER REPORT ON CHILD (Need not be done at some timeas rest of test)

YES NO 149. Con put on jacket or shirt without help. 150. Can zipor button jacket. 151. Wears shoes. 152. Can put on shoes (if correct shoe is identified). 153. Can put on correct shoes without help. 154. Con tie shoes. 155. Can carry out simple c:rbal instructions pertaining to clothing, food arrangements, etc. ("Go put on your jacket." "Pass the cookies to the children.") 156. Can go about immediate home and/or school neighborhood.- unattended. Can get to school alone (attendant provided at major street crossings). Rural: can get to bus stop and wai!. without supervision. 157. Knows meaning of red-green traffic lights. (Permissible to ask child if there is no opportunity to observe on this).

15C. Can wash hands. 159. Can wash and dry hands and face. 160. Notifies teacher of his toilet needs. 161. Can care for himself in the bathroom without help: Pushes, cleans, fixes clothing.

"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS Bettye M. Caldwell, Ph .D. COPYRIGHTIED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED Syracuse, New York All rights reserved BYEth,Lcat%opRk Testi f4q SeRuice 0 1%1 b Edt.t.cfalic4.0 TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING 1 -Fes+, ee.vice. UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U.S. OFFICE OF EDUCATIOII. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REQUIRES PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIC7 OWNER." Construction of the throe boxes reap:red in items 124-134 can be a relative y simple matter. A diagram is provided below for patterns of cutting.. Fold c'or:-.... the dotted lines and cut along the solid lines. We suggest the foliow:ng dimensions for the size of the paper:

Black paper box 7 1/2 inches square Green paper box 9 inches square White paper box 11 inches square,

Use construction paper, which you may have to purchase.

1

"NNW ..IIIMMIP i I IIININ 111/Mb Mani, 1

1

Zt% r.... r I

1 1 1 r____ 11111.1110 011 IMMIN. 11.1 IMMD 41.11110

41 I= I =2 E"7:3 L r E I 1-1 = , ....."....17 N.J.. ,

F OPERATION HEAD START BEHAVIOR INVENTORY D'S NAME: SCHOOL: CHILD'S IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER EXAMINER IDENTIFICATIONNUMSER

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 S 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 5 7 S

I 5 7 O I 2 3 4 5 it 7 9 0 2 3 4 5 s S

O I 2 3 4 5-----s -7 9 0 I 2 3 5 7---- 9

5 7 '0 I 2 3 4 5 --.5 7 9 0 I 2 3 4 9

O I 2 3 4 5 5 7 5 5 0 I 2 3 4 5 7 S

5 5 7 O I 2 3 4 5 5 7 5 II 0 I 2 3"' °'4 S

CENTER IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER BUDGET BUREAU NO. 116504 APPROVALEXPIRES3-316

O I t 3 4 5 5 7 9 INSTRUCTIONS

0 I 2 3 4 5 5 7 * 9 PLEASE DESCRIBE AS ACCURATELY AS POSSIBLE HOW THIS CHILD BEHAVES BY .-. . MARKING, WITH A NO. 2 LEAD PENCIL,ONE OF THE FOUR REgONSES TO O I 2 3 4 5 5 7 9 EACH QUESTION: O I 2 3 4 3 5 7 5 II VEkY MUCH LIKE SOMEWHAT LIKE VERY LITTLE LIKE NOT AT ALL LIKE 0 I 2 3 4 5 _5 7 5 9

PLEASE GIVE A RESPONSE TO EVERY ITEM AND BASE YOUR RESPONSE UPON 0 I 2 3 4 9 YOUR PERSONAL OBSERVATION AND EXPERIENCE WITH THE CHILD.

0 I 2 3 4 5 -5 7 S VERY SOME- VERY NOT MUCH WHAT LITTLE ATALL, LIKELIKE LIKE LIKE' IS USUALLY CAREFREE; RARELY BECOMES FRIGHTENED OR APPREHENSIVE.

IS SYMPATHETIC, CONSIDERATE, AND THOUGHTFUL TOWARD OTHERS.

IS EASILY DISTRACTED BY THINGS GOING ON AROUND HIM. .

IS VERY SUGGESTIBLE; LETS OTHER CHILDREN BOSS HIM MOUND.

TALKS EAGERLY TO ADULTS ABOUT HIS OWN EXPERIENCES AND WHAT HE THINKS. 2.ak.

IS UNDULY UPSET OR DISCOURAGED W HE MAKES A MISTAKE 04 DOES NOT PERFORM WELL.

OFTEN KEEPS ALOOF FROM OTHERS BECAUSE HE IS UNINTERESTED, SUSPICIOUS, OR BASHFUL.

DEFENDS OR PRAISES HIS OWN EFFORTS. -......

. IS CONFIDENT THAT HE CAN DO WHAT IS EXPECTED OFHIM. k. IS JEALOUS; QUICK TO NOTICE AND REACT NEGATIVELY TO KINDNESS AND ATTENTION BESTOWED UPON OTHER CHILDREN.

VERY SOME- VERY NOT MUCH WHAT LITTLE MALLu LIKE LIKE LIKE LIKE IS METHODICAL AND CAREFUL IN THE TASKS THAT HE UNDERTAKES. r

IS RARELY ABLE TO INFLUENCE OTHER CHILDREN BY HIS ACTIVITIES OR INTERESTS.

. TRIES TO FIGURE OUT THINGS FOR HIMSELFBEFORE ASKING ADULTS OR OTHER CHILDREN FOR HELP.

. GREATLY PREFERS THE HABITUAL ANDFAMILIAR TO THE NOVEL AND THE UNFAMILIAR.

.APPEARS TO TRUST IN HIS OWN ABILITIES.

. HAS LITTLE RESPECT FOR THE RIGHTS OFOTHER CHILDREN; REFUSES TO WAIT HIS TURN, USURPS TOYS OTHER CHILDREN ME PLAYING WITH, ETC.

. SEEMS DISINTERESTED IN THEGENERAL QUALITY OF HIS PERFORMANCE.

. RESPONDS TO FRUSTRATION ORDISAPPOINTMENT BY BECOMING AGGRESSIVE OR ENRAGED.

. IS EXCESSIVE IN SEEKINGTHE ATTENTION OF ADULTS. ----- b. STICKS WITH A JOB UNTIL IT IS FINISHED. I. DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE PRESENT WEEK OF CENTER'S OPERATION "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED :A:::A:::A:: :A::;I:::Al:::2:: ::t :; BY EAuJACtt ciItr 7111111b

OVER TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U.S. OFFICE OF EDUCATION. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE by Edward ZIgler,1965 CAPNS FORM 37, JUN. '65 MiuMM THE ERIC SYSTEM REOUIRES PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER." 11111111110111111MM=IMIN"--- OPERATION HEAD START BEHAVIOR INVENTORY

VERYSOME- VERY NOT MUCHWHATLITTLE ATALL LIKE LIKE LIKE LIKE il. GOES ABOUT HIS ACTIVITIES WITH A MINIMUM OF ASSISTANCE FROM OTHERS.

I O. IS CONSTRICTED, INHIBITED , OR TIMID; NEEDS TO BE URGED BEFOREENGAGING IN ACTIVITIES.

23. IS EVEN-TEMPERED, IMPERTURBABLE; IS RARELY ANNOYED OR CROSS. p. IS RELUCTANT TO TALK TO ADULTS; RESPONDS VERBALLY ONLY WHEN =ED.

,n.WORKS EARNESTLY AT HIS CLASSWORK OR PLAY; DOESN'T TAKE IT LIGHTLY.

26. IS OFTEN QUARRELSOME WITH CLASSMATES FOR MINOR REASONS.

27. DOES NOT NEED ATTENTION OR APPROVAL FROM ADULTS TO SUSTAIN HIM IN HIS WORK OR PLAY.

28. WHEN FACED WITH A DIFFICULT TASK, HE EITHER DOES NOT ATTEMPT IT OR GIVES UP VERY QUICKLY.

29. DOESN'T LIKE TO BE INTERRUPTED WHEN ENGAGED IN DEMANDING ACTIVITIES, E. G., PUZZLES,PAINTING, CONSTRUCTING THINGS.

30. WELCOMES CHANGES AND NEW SITUATIONS; IS VENTURESOME, EXPLORES, AND GENERALLY ENJOYS NOVELTY.

VERY SOME-VERY NOT MUCH WHAT LITTLE AT ALL LIKE LIKE LIKE LIKE 3E. CALMLY SETTLES DIFFICULTIES THAT ARISE WITHOUT APPEAL TO ADULTS OR OTHERS.

32. IS RELUCTANT TO USE IMAGINATION; TENDS NOT TO ENJOY "MAKE-BELIEVE" GAMES.

33LIKES TO TALK WITH OR SOCIALIZE WITH TEACHER.

34. OFTEN WILL NOT ENGAGE IN ACTIVITIES UNLESS STRONGLY ENCOURAGED.

35. IS EAGER TO INFORM OTHER CHILDREN OF THE EXPERIENCES HE HAS HAD.

36. EMOTIONAL RESPONSE IS CUSTOMARILY VERY STRONG; OVER-RESPONDS TO USUAL CLASSROOMPROBLEMS, FRUSTRATIONS, AND DIFFICULTIES.

37. IS UNCOOPERATIVE IN GROUP ACTIVITIES.

38. IS USUALLY POLITE TO ADULTS; SAYS "PLEASE," "THANK YOU," ETC.

39. ASKS MANY QUESTIONS FOR INFORMATION ABOUT THINGS, PERSONS, ETC. (EMPHASIS HERE SHOULD BEON QUESTIONS PROMPTED BY

GENUINE CURIOSITY RATHER THAN BIDS FOR ATTENTION.)

40. USUALLY DOES WHAT ADULTS ASK HIM TO DO.

VERY SOME- VERY NOT MUCH WHATLITTLE ATALL LIKE LIKE LIKE LIKE 4L REQUIRES THE COMPANY OF OTHER CHILDREN; FINDS IT DIFFICULT TO WORK OR PLAY BYHIMSELF.

42. RESPONDS TO FRUSTRATION OR DISAPPOINTMENT BY BECOMING SULLEN, WITHDRAWN, ORSULKY.

43. DEMONSTRATES IMAGINATIVENESS AND CREATIVITY IN HIS USE OFTOYS AND PLAY MATERIALS.

44. INSISTS ON MAINTAINING HIS RIGHTS, E. G., WILL NOT YIELD HIS PLACE AT PAINTING, OR ATTHE CARPENTRY BENCH, ETC.; INSISTS

ON GETTING HIS TURN ON THE SLIDE OR IN GROUP GAMES, ETC.

45. IS WANTED AS A PLAYMATE BY OTHER CHILDREN.

45. IS LETHARGIC OR APATHETIC; HAS LITTLE ENERGY OR DRIVE. 47. HAS A TENDENCY TO DISCONTINUE ACTIVITIES AFTER EXERTING A MINIMUM OF EFFORT. -- 48. IS GENERALLY A HAPPY CHILD.

49. APPROACHES NEW TASKS TIMIDLY AND WITHOUT ASSURANCE; SHRINKS FROM TRYING NEWTHINGS.

50. WHAT HE DOES IS OFTEN IMITATED BY OTHER CHILDREN. DO NOT MARK INTHIS SPACE NNW

WM

NM 1-

- . IIIII.II

11111111 r-- SUOSET SORE AU NO. 11643041 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE. APPROVAL EXPIRES 6-3044 a.. DI NAME: WIMP NUMBER =WM CHILD'S IDENTIFICATION NUMBER EXAMINER'SIDENTIFICATION MIMI IN=1.F 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

l.., 5 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 5 9 elm/P 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1111 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 5 5 o I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 5 0 al ,M=M 1 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 5 - -- 9 0- I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 5 IN=1.L 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 S 5 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 5 10 IN=1. 5 9 0 I 2 1, 4 S 6 7 8 IN=1. CENTER IDENTIFI ATIONNUMBER ,M=M 6 9 PRESENT WEEK OF CENTER'S OPERATION IN=1. O 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 IM=1.L..... :Alt:::1c: 7 S 9 ::!::: ::3::: :t: : :5:: :it:::7:: O 1 2 3 4 6 10=1, 9 PLEASE USE A NO.2 LEAD PENCIL TO MARK THIS FORM O I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURES ARE MUCH LIKE GENERAL PHYSICAL .11 5 6 7 8 9 O 1 2 3 4 Imma EXAMINATIONS, EXCEPT THAT THEY AXE INTENDED TO REVEAL MENTAL RATHER THAN 5 6 7 8 9 O 1 2 3 4 PHYSICAL SYMPTOMS. TWC SUCH PROCEDURES HAVE BEEN INCORPORATED INTO 7 8 9 OPERATION HEAD START, A SYMPTOM CHECKLIST AND A CHILD DESCRIPTION O 1 2 3 4 5 6 CHECKLIST. BOTH SHOULD BE COMPLETED BY THE HEAD TEACHER AFTER SHE HAS SEEN mim. 5 6 7 8 5 MIMI O : 1 2 3 4 ACQUAINTED WITH HER PUPILS FOR AT LEAST FOUR WEEKS. I] =MEM THIS SECTION WHICH HAVE CHARACTERIZED THIS CHILD'S BEHAVIOR -- 1. SYMPTOM CHECKLIST MARK EACH OF THE BEHAVIORS LISTED IN THROUGHOUT HIS ATTENDANCE IN YOUR CLASS. IF ABEHAVIOR OCCURRED ONLY ONCE OR TWICE DO NOT MARK IT. "I's ..... I. SELFISH OR GREEDY HOARDING OF OWN A.!1D OTHERCHILDREN'S PLAYTHINGS OR CLASSROOM MATERIALS. D

2. REFUSES TO EAT OR DRINK.

3. HOLDS BREATH UNTIL LOSES TYPICAL COLORING ORUNTIL DIZZY OR FAINT.

4. TEMPER TANTRUM IN WHICH THROWS SELF ABOUT OR DOWN,CRIES, SCREAMS, HITS FLOOR, ETC.

5. TEMPER TANTRUM IN WHICH VIOLENTLY ATTACKSOTHER CHILDREN OR ADULTS OR DESTROYS PROPERTY.

6. BANGS HEAD AGAINST HARD OBJECT, BITES HIMSELF,SCRATCHES HIMSELF, PULLS OUT OWN HAIR, OR OTHERWISE ABUSES SELF.

7. BITES OTHER CHILDREN OR ADULTS IN ANGER.

8. PLACES FOREIGN OBJECTS IN SOME BODY OPENING OTHER THANTHE MOUTH;FOR EXAMPLE, ROCKS IN EARS, PENCIL IN NOSE.

9. STUTTERS OR STAMMERS TO POINT THAT IT IS DIFFICULT TOUNDERSTAND HIM.

10. FAINTS OR PASSES OUT.

II. COMPLAINS OF PAINS IN HEAD OR STOMACH. OR ACTIVITIES. 12. INTERESTED IN ONLY ONE OR TWO OBJECTS OR ACTIVITIES.REFUSES TO PARTICIPATE OR SEEMS DISINTERESTED IN OTHER THINGS

13. CRIES EXCESSIVELY OR BECOMES VERY ANXIOUS OR WITHDRAWNWHEN MILDLY REPRIMANDED.

14. FREQUENTLY WANDERS OR RUNS AWAY FROM NURSERY.

15. WILL NOT FEED SELF.

16. ALMOST CONSTANT THUMB - SUCKING.

17. EXCESSIVE CLINGING TO SOME OBJECT(BLANKET, CLOTH,SOFT ANIMAL, OR OTHER TOY).

18. ASKS TO BE CALLED BY SOME NAME OTHER THAN OWN ANDREFUSES TO ANSWER TO OWN NAME.

BECOMES VERY ANXIOUS IN NEW SITUATIONS. 19. NEEDS EXCESSIVE PROMPTING AND CONSTANT REASSURANCE TOTRY SOMETHING NEW; a 20. CONSTANTLY CRITICIZES SELF AND OWN PRODUCTIONS. a

21. OFTEN CRIES OR LAUGHS SUDDENLY FOR NO APPARENT REASON.

22, SHOWS NO INTEREST IN PLAYING WITH OR BEING ACCEPTEDBY OTHER CHILDREN.

23. CANNOT COMMUNICATE WITH SPOKEN LANGUAGE. a

24. OFTEN SITS ROCKING BACK AND FORTH. a MOM 25. SAD OR FRIGHTENED FOR MOST OF THE DAY.

26. AUDIBLE CLAMPING OR GRINDING OF TEETH.

27. FEAR OF URINATING OR MOVING BOWELS.

28. COMPLETE INABILITY TO INTERACT WITH STRANGERS. DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE

29, UNABLE TO REMAIN SEATED FOR MORE THAN FIVEMINUTES AT A TIME ( AS WHEN EATING OR BEING READ TO). VIM OVER

30. SEVERAL WEEKS AFTER INITIAL PARTICIPATION INOPERATION HEAD START, STILL CRIES OR BECOMES DEPRESSED WHENMOTHER LEAVES. CAP-HS FCrItM 40 JUN. '65 7L) 2221 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCREENING PROCEDURE

CHILD DESCRIPTION CHECKLIST READ EACH DESCRIPTION CAREFULLYAND PUT A MARK BESIDE ANY THAT FIT THIS CHILD REASONABLY WELL. IT IS RECOGNIZED THAT DESCRIPTIONS OF THIS SORT DO NOT DO JUSTICETO THE WHOLE CHILD AND THAT NO CHILD WILL FIT ANY DESCRIPTION EXACTLY. JUST PLACE A MARK BESIDE THE DESCRIPTIONS THAT FIT THISCHILD REASONABLY WELL.THESE DESCRIPTIONS ARE NOT MUTUALLY EXCLUSIVE. IT IS POSSIBLE THAT SOME CHILDREN WILL FIT TWO OR MOREOF T. ALSO, THERE WILL BE MANY CHILDREN WHO DO NOT FIT ANY OF THESE DESCRIPTIONS. IT IS POSSIBLE THAT IN SOME CLASSES THEREWILL BE NO CHILD TO FIT ANY OF THE DESCRIPTIONS. FEW PROFESSIONAL PEOPLE, NO MATTER HOW WELL TRAINED,CAN MAKE THISKIND OF RATING WITH ABSOLUTE CERTAINTY ANDCOMPLETE COMFORT. DON'T SPEND TOO MUCH TIME WORRYING WHETHER A PARTICULAR CHILDREALLY DOES OR DOESN'T FIT THE DESCRIPTIONS. MAKE YOUR BEST JUDGEMENT FOR EACH CHILD ON EACH DESCRIPTION AND THENGO ON TO THE NEXT.

I. THE DISRUPTIVE CHILD THE DISRUPTIVE CHILD IS ONE bvii0 DISTURBS THE ACTIVITIES AND PLAY OF OTHER CHILDREN. HE MAY DO THIS

BY PUSHING OR TEASING CHILDREN WHO ARE ENGAGED IN ACTIVITIES OR BY SNATCHING OR OTHERwiSE DISTURBING THEMATERIALS nITH WHICH

OTHER CHILDREN ARE PLAYING.

2. THE PROVOCATIVE CHILD THE PROVOCATIVE CHILD IS ONE OHO DELIBERATELY TRIES TO IRRITATE THE TEACHER. HE ATTEMPTS TO SECURE

THE TEACHER'S ATTENTION BY DOING THINGS WHICH ARE PROHIBITED OR WHICH HE SHOULD KNOW THAT THE TEACHER DISLIKES. HE MAY REFUSE

TO GO ALONG WITH GROUP ACTIVITIES, HE MAY CURSE OR OTHERWISE INSULT THE TEACHER, HE MAY DAMAGE OR DESTROY CLASSROOM MATERIALS,

ETC.THIS CHILD DOES NOT RESPOND TO PUNISHMENTS SY "BEING BETTER."

3. THE ISOLATED CHILD THE ISOLATED CHILD NEVER SEEMS TO PLAY WITH OTHER PUPILS. HE DOESN'T SEEM TO BE ABLE TO INITIATE

CONTACT WITH OTHER CHILDREN, THEY SEEM TO IGNORE HIM AND HE THEM. OTHER CHILDREN DO NOT INCLUDE HIM IN GROUP ACTIVITIES AND

HE DOES NOT SEEM TO CARE.

4. THE FEARFUL OR TEARFUL CHILD THE FEARFUL CHILD IS EXCESSIVELY TIMID. HE CRIES MORE OFTEN THAN THE OTHER CHILDREN. OFTEN

HE CRIES FOR NO APPARENT REASON. HE SEEMS TO WANT TO PLAY WITH OTHER CHILDREN AM) DO THE THINGS WHICH ARE"FUN", BUT HIS

FEARFULNESS GETS IN THE WAY. HE MAY BE SOMETHING OF A "TATTLE TALE," A "WHINER," OR A "MOTHER'S BOY (GIRL)."

5. THE SILENT CHILD THE SILENT CHILD NEVER TALKS. HE WILL USE GESTURES OR SIGNS RATHER THAN WORDS. HE SEEMS TOUNDERSTAND

WHAT OTHER PEOPLE SAY, BUT HE WON'T RESPOND VERBALLY UNLESS REALLY URGED.

TAUGHT. HE MAY 6. THE CHILD WHO DOESN'T LEARN THE CHILD WHO DOESN'T LEARN NEVER SEEMS TO GET ANY BETTER AT WHAT HE IS BEING

TRY HARD, BUT HE DOESN'T SEEM TO IMPROVt. HE MAY HAVE DIFFICULTY UNDERSTANDING WHAT HE ISTOLD, AND MAY HAVE TO HAVE THINGS

REPEATED A NUMBER OF TIMES. HE DOESN'T SEEM TO BE AS QUICK OR ALERT AS THE OTHER CHILDREN. OFTEN,HE SEEMSIMMATURE FOR HIS AGE.

THE CHILD WITH SEPARATION PROBLEMS SEEMS TO GET ALONG WELL MOST OF THE TIME, BUT HE HAS 7. THE CHILD WITH SEPARATION PROBLEMS

GREAT DIFFICULT. EARLY IN THE SCHOOL DAY. HIS DIFFICULTIES MAY BE MOST MARKED DURING THE FIRST DAYSOF NURSERY SCHOOL AND AFTER

WEEKENDS OR VACATIONS. EARLY IN THE DAY, HE MAY SAY THAT HE DOESN'T WANT TO LEAVE HIS MOTHER OR THATHE WANTS TO GO HOME TO HIS

MOTHER. LATER ON, HE SETTLES DOWN AND SEEMS TO DO FINE. THIS CHILD'S MOTHER MAY COME TO THE CLASSROOM WITH THECHILD MORE

FREQUENTLY THAN OTHERS MOTHERS AND MAY,TALK TO THE TEACHER QUITE OFTEN ABOUT HOW DIFFICULTTHINGS ARE FOR HER CHILD.

AND SEEMS TO LACK A "JOY 8. THE UNHAPPY CHILD THE UNHAPPY CHILD IS ALWAYS "DOWN-AT-THE-MOUTH." HE DOESN'T SMILE VERY OFTEN

FOR LIFE." HE MIGHT NOT CRY VERY OFTEN, BUT HE DOESN'T APPEAR TO ENJOY HIMSELF OR THE THINGS THATARE GOING ON AROUND HIM.

ROOM.IF HE IS DISRUPTIVE 9. THE HYPERACTIVE CHILD THIS IS A CHILD WHO JUST CAN'T SIT STILL. HE MAY ROAM AIMLESSLY ABOUT THE

OF OTHER CHILDREN'S ACTIVITIES IT IS MORE AN ACCIDENTAL RESULT OF HIS RUNNING ABOUT, THAN ADELIBERATE AGGRESSIVENESS. SOME

HYPERACTIVE CHILDREN DON'T ROAM AROUND A GREAT DEAL. RATHER, THEY OCCUPY THEMSELVES WITH STRANGEMOTOR ACTIVITIES SUCH AS

SHAKING THEIR HANDS OR WAVING THEIR FINGERS BEFORE THEIR EYES, PULLING AT THEIR EARS OR OTHERBODY PARTS, ROCKING BACK AND FORTH.

THIS TYPE OF CHILD IS OFTEN EXTREMELY DISTRACTIBLE. II .REFERRAL OR TREATMENT REPORT ON THE BASIS OF THEBEHAVIORSNOTED ABOVE OR ANY OTHER FACTORS, WAS THIS CHILD REFERRED TO, TREATED AT,OR PLACED INTO ANY OF THE FOLLOWING: YES NO YES NO 1.CHILD GUIDANCE CLINIC B. HOME FOR DEPENDENT CHILDREN

2. MENTAL HEALTHCENTER 9. OTHER (SEE BELOW)

3.PUBLIC HEALTH NURSE OR PHYSICIAN 10.IF REFERRAL WAS MADE, WAS THE CHILD DIAGNOSED AS AINORMAL?

4. HOSPITAL OR MEDICAL CLINIC 11.IF REFERRAL WAS MADE, WAS TREATMENT INITIATED? DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE into 5.STATE SCHOOL FOR THE MENTALLY RETARDED IMO 6. HOSPITAL FOR THE EMOTIONALLY DISTURBED MO 7. FOSTER HOME PLEASE SPECIFY OTHER , REFERRAL OR TREATMENT MIMI

UM OPERATION HEAD START STAFFMEMBER INFORMATION SHEET

SUOSET-11UREAU NO.1111-110211 APPROVAL EXPIRES4-1 -SS

CENTER IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER STAFF IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 $ 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 5;:$

0 I.-..-2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 S_----7 6 9

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 11 7 9 0 1 2 3 4 5 S----7 6 9

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 II 9

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 II 9

UNDER NI 111-21 21-30 31-45 411-60 OVER GO 1. AGE PLEASE USE A NEGRO WHITE ORIENTAL NO. 2 2. RACE AMERICAN PUERTO CULTURAL FRIO INDIAN MEXICAN RICAN CREOLE ESKIMO OTHER PENCIL BACKGROUND TO MARK

THIS FORM MALE FEMALE 3. SEX

4. HIGHEST LEVEL OF EDUCATION COMPLETED GRADUATED NUMBER OF YEARS COMPLETED 1-4 5-7 YES NO ELEMENTAAV SCHOOL

1-2 3-4 HIGH SCHOOL

1-2 3-4 COLLEGE

GRADUATE SCHOOL MA,

Ph.D

5. YOUR POSITION IN THE CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTER PROFESSIONAL (TEACHER, NURSE, PSYCHOLOGIST, ETC.) NEIGHBORHOOD RESIDENT PAID VOLOIRTI OTNER VOLUNTEER

6. WHAT IS YOUR USUAL FAMILY INCOME PER YEAR ?

UNDER $1000 NON TOON MOON um $3000T0$3111 $4100T0$4111 $5000T0$5* ONO TO NON $1000100111 $1000 PLUS

NOT AT F 1-3 3-5 OVER ALL 7. PREVIOUS TO HEAD START, HOW LONG HAD YOU WORKED WITH GROUPS OF CHILDREN FROM CONDITIONS OF POVERTY? YEARS YEARS SITARS

1-3 3-5 OVER NONE TEAM YEARS 5 YEARS S. PREVIOUS TO HEAD START, HOW MUCH EXPERIENCE HAD YOU HAD WITH PRESCHOOLERS?

YES NO 9. DO YOU SPEAK FLUENTLY ANY LANGUAGE OTHER THAN ENGLISH?

YES NO 10. IS THIS LANGUAGE USED WITH THE CHILDREN IN THE PROGRAM?

YES NO 1. DID YOU ATTEND AN NUEA TRAINING SESSION?

CAP-H3 FORM 41 JUN. 415 IBMH92224 PAID AND VOLUNTARY WORKER'S EVALUATION OF OPERATION HEAD START

CENTER IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER PLEASE USE A NO. 2 LEAD PENCIL TO MARK THIS SHEET. WORKER'S IDENTIFICATIONNUMBER 0 ----2---- 3---:4 5 6 7 9 9

0 2 3 '4 5 6 7 "9 9 0 I 2 3 4

0 2 3 - :4 5 6 7 9 9 0 I 2 3 6 ----- 7 9 9 --:::

2 - "3 -4 - -- 5 6 -7 9 9 0 I 2 3 4

9 G. --- 2- 3 4 6 7 9 9 0 I 2 3 4 -:- 7 -:

0 I - 2 . 3 -::4 7 -9 9 0 I 2 3 4 6 7 -9

2 --3 -- 2---- 3 4 5 6 T -! 9 OMIT SUMO NO. 116-611011 VERY CANT APPROVAL CRP! -30- GOOD G000 FAIR POOR EVAL 1.THE GENERAL MORALE OF TEACHER, TEACHER'S AIDES, AND VOLUNTEERS WAS: :::.::::::_ -:

2.HOW REALISTIC WAS YOUR DAILY SCHEDULE?

3.WERE THERE ENOUGH WORKERS TO CARRY OUT GOALS OF THE PROGRAM?

4.WAS THE CURRICULUM WITH WHICH YOU WORKED RELEVANT TO THIS AGE CHILD?

5.COOPERATION FROM THE PARENTS WAS

6.THE FACILITIES (SUCH AS LIGHTING, SPACE, WATER, STORAGE, BATHROOMS, ETC.) OF THE PRESCHOOL AREA WERE.

7.THE AMOUNT OF AVAILABLE PLAY MATERIALS WAS:

8.THE QUALITY OF AVAILABLE PLAY MATERIALS WAS:

9.HOW ADEQUATE WAS THE PHYSICAL MAINTENANCE OF THE PRESCHOOL AREA?

10.THE QUALITY OF THE TEACHING SUPERVISION WAS:

II.HOW HELPFUL TO YOUR TEACHING WERE THE PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASURES AND RATINGS YOU COMPLETED?

12.HOW ADEQUATE WERE THE TP.ANSPORTATION ARRANGEMENTS, WHERE NEEDED, FOR CHILDREN AND PARENTS?

13.HOW AVAILABLE WERE MEDICAL AND/OR DENTAL SERVICES?

14.HOW AVAILABLE WERE SPECIAL-EDUCATION FACILITIES?

15.HOW AVAILABLE WERE PSYCHOLOGICAL AND/OR PSYCHIATRIC SERVICES?

16.HOW AVAILABLE WERE SOCIAL SERVICE AGENCIES? ::::1

17.HOW ADEQUATE. WAS THE AMOUNT OF TIME SET ASIDE FOR PARENT-TEACHER CONFERENCES? :

18.IN RESPECT TO THE PHYSICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL HEALTH OF THE CHILD AN() HIS EDUCATIONAL DEVELOPMEt4I, Mr ATTITUDE:. Tc..i DIntl IOI LC dINGAhl

VERY MUCH OCCASIONALLY WASTE NOT WORTHWHILE WORTHWHILE WORTHWHILE OTINE APPLICABLE I.MEDICAL EXAMINATION .--:: ......

2.DENTAL EXAMINATION

3.OPPORTUNITY TO ATTEND SCHOOL AT AN EARLY AGE

4.INCREASED EXPERIENCE WITH A VARIETY OF TOYS AND GAMES

5.INCREASED EXPERIENCE WITH A VARIETY OF BOOKS, STORIES, AND MUSK

6.TRIPS INTO THE COMMUNITY

7.INDIVIDUAL ATTENTION GIVEN TO EACH CHILD BY TEACHER AND AIDES

8.OPPORTUNITY TO PARTICIPATE IN GROUP ACTIVITIES WITH OTHER CHILDREN

"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS 'COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED DO NOT MARK -IN THIS SPACE BY EjwAl2A VIM ITO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING. 'UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U.S. OFFICE OF EDUCATION. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REQUIRES PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER." by Edward Viler, 1965 CAP-H$ FORM 39 JUN..'65 MIR PAID AND VOLUNTARY WORKER'S EVALUATION OF pPERATIONHEAD START

19.I FEEL THAT, IN GENSAL, CHILDREN ATTENDING 'ME FILIATION HEAD START MOCI1AM Wel CHANG. IN THE FOLLOWING WAYS:

0111111111 SETTER CHOU WORSE WORE 1. GETTING ALONG WITH OTHER CHILDREN . 1 ig.....4 2. SELF-CONFIDENCE I

3. SPEAKING ABILITY . ---- .

4. EVERYDAY MANNERS s:

3. FINISHING WHAT HE STARTS

6. DOING WHAT HE'S TOLD .

7. INTISKTED IN NEW THINGS ...._ I. CAN DO THINGS ON HIS OWN - ---

9. CHANCES OF SUCCESS IN KINDERGARTEN ARE:

20. AS A RESULT OF THEIR CONTACT WITH OPERATION HEAD START, THE PARENTS ARE: . MUCH ND MUCM SUTTER SITTER CHANGE WORSE WORN I. INVOLVED WITH CHILD'S EDUCATION

2. CONCERNED ABOUT OWN APPEARANCE

3.PARTICIPATING IN COMMUNITY ACTIVITIES

I 4. AWARE OF ENLIGHTENED CHILD-REARING PRACTICES

-,-. 5.EFFECTIVE IN INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS

6. KNOWLEDGEAILE ABOUT COMMUNITY RESOURCES

21. AS A RESULT OF MY CONTACT WITH OPERATION HEAD START, NOT MUCH A AT MORE MORE LITTLEALL I.I AM KNOWLEDGEABLE ABOUT TEACHING CHILDREN OF THIS AGE ..... 2.I AM AWARE OF THE ENVIRONMENT THESE CHILDREN EXPERIENCE ...... 3.I HAVE ACQUIRED NEW TECHNIQUES TO INTERACT EFFECTIVELY WITH THESECHILDREN

4.I AM KNOWLEDGEABLE ABOUT AND HAVE THE ABILITY TO DEAL WITH OTHER PROFESSIONAL WORKERSCONCERNED WITH THE

CHILD'S PHYSICAL, PSYCHOLOGICAL, AND SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT

A - NOT GREATSOME- A AT DEALGNAT LITTLEALL 22. HOW MUCH DID YOU ENJOY YOUR DUTIES WITH OPERATION HEAD START?

23. WOULD YOU LOOK FORWARD TO PARTICIPATING IN OPERATION HEADSTART NEXT YEAR? t24. ADD HERE ANY COMMENTS THAT YOU WOULD LIKE TO MAKE: OPERATION HEAD START WORKER'SATTITUDE SCALE BUDGET BUREAU NO. 116-6504 WORKER'S IDENTIFICATION NUMBER APPROVAL EXPIRES 3-31-66

5 6 7 9 9 o 1 2 3 4 PLEASE USE A NO. 2 LEAD PENCIL

O I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 9 9 PART I O I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS HAVE NO RIGHT OR WRONGANSWERS. ALL O I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 *--- 9 THAT IS REQUIRED IS THAT YOU GIVE YOUR HONESTREACTION TO EACH G I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 NO. 2 QUESTION. AFTER READING EACH STATEMENT, SIMPLY MARK, WITH A I CENTER IDENTIFICATION NUMBER OWN OPINION. AFTER 9 LEAD PENCIL, THE ALTERNATIVE THAT INDICATES YOUR 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 READING EACH STATEMENT, MAKE ONE OF THE FOLLOWINGFIVE CHOICES: 9 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 s- 5 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 7 5 STRONGLY AGREE, AGREE, NOT SURE, DISAGREE,STRONGLY DISAGREE 7 6 9 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 9 9 STR I 2 3 4 5 6 7 SIR NOT 0 AGR AGR SURE OISA DISH I.I WOULD ENJOY WORKING WITH POOR PEOPLETO HELP THEM SETTERTHEITLIVES.

2. POOR PEOPLE TEND TO BEHAVE INCHILDISH WAYS.

3. POVERTY IS LARGELY A FUNCTION OF BADLUCK, INJUSTICE, OR DISCRIMINATION.

4. I WOULD BE EMBARRASSED TO INTRODUCE A POORPERSON TO MY FRIENDS.

5. POOR PEOPLE ARE LESS TRUSTWORTHY THAN PEOPLEWITH MORE MONEY.

6. IN GENERAL, POOR PEOPLE LACK INTELLIGENCE. PEOPLE SETTER THEIR LIVES. 7. THE CITY, STATE, AND FEDERAL GOVERNMENTSHOULD DO ALL IT CAN IN TRYING TO HELP POOR

8. POOR PEOPLE TEND TO SE AS INTERESTED INTHER CHILDREN AS ARE PEOI'LE WITH MORE MONEY.

POOR. AGRNOT SIR 9. VIOLENT BEHAVIOR CHARACTERIZES THE AGR AGR SURE OISA PISA 10. MOST POOR PEOPLE DO NOT KNOW V''.AT THEY WANTOUT OF LIFE.

11. POOR PEOPLE DESERVE AS MUCH RESPECTAND CONSIDERATION AS ANYONE ELSE.

12. MOST POOR PEOPLE ARE POOR BECAUSETHEY ARE LAZY.

13. ITS HARD FOR AN ABLE - BODIED MAN TORESPECT HIMSELF IF HE DOESN'T WORK.

14. IMMORAL PRACTICES ARE MUCH MORE COMMONAMONG THE POOR.

15. WE SHOULD TRY TO HELP ONLY THOSE WHOAPPRECIATE OUR HELP.

16. JUST ABOUT EVERY TYPE OF PERSONALITYCAN BE FOUND AMONG THE POOR.

17. POVERTY IS A SIGN OF FAILURE IN LIFE.

AHEAD. 18. POVERTY IS QUITE OFTEN DUE TO LACKOF SELF CONTROL ,WILL - POWER,OR THE DESIRE TO GET

STR 19. POOR PEOPLE WOULD IMPROVETHEMSELVES IF THEY WERE GIVEN ADDITIONAL OPPORTUNITIES. STR NOT AOR AGR SURE DISH DIU 20. HOW MUCH MONEY A PERSON MAKES IS USUALLYA GOOD INDICATOR OF 1115 CHARACTER.

DONE TO HELP THE POOR TO SETTER THEMSF VES SHORT OF TAKINGCARE OF THEM OR GIVING THEM MONEY. 2i . THERE IS LITTLE THAT CAN BE

22. MOST POOR PEOPLE ARE WILLINGTO WORK HARD IF GIVEN THE OPPORTUNITY.

23. IN GENERAL, THE BEHAVIOR OF POORPEOPLE TENDS TO SE ERRATIC AND UNPREDICTABLE.

24. POOR PEOPLE DONT CARE HOW THEYLOOK.

WHO ARE WELL OFF TO HELP POOR PEOPLE BETTERTHEMSELVES. 25. IT IS THE RESPONSIBILITY OF PEOPLE

26. POOR PEOPLE TEND TO BE LOUD,VULGAR, AND IMPOLITE.

27. POOR PEOPLE WILL TAKE ADVANTAGE OF YOUIF YOU GIVE THEM THE OPPORTUNITY.

78. IT WOULD BE ALL RIGHT WITH ME TOHAVE A POOR PERSON AS A CLOSE FR TEND.

29. POOR PEOPLE ARE INHERENTLY DIFFERENTFROM PEOPLE WHO HAVE MORE MONEY.

TO SAY ABOUT HOW THE GOVERNMENTSPENDS MONEY TO HELP THEM. 30. POOR PEOPLE SHOULD HAVE SOMETHING DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE PRESENT WEEK OF CENTER'SOPERATION ::I:::1:::3:: i :4":5ERNiSSiOrio iiiipiiehlie: COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED

BYEALNI Aita-2.10er

TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U.S. OFFICE OF Edward 2Igler,19sq EDUCATION. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REOUIRES PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER." i I OPERATION HEAD' START WORKER'S ATTITUDE SCALE

PART II

AGAIN THERE ARE NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWERS TO THE FOLLOWING ITEMS. WE ARE INTERESTED IN FINDING OUT WHAT YOU

FEEL CHARACTERIZES OR DOES NOT CHARACTERIZE YOUNG CHILDREN. FOR EACH Of THE ADJECTIVES BELOW, SIMPLY MARK WHETHER OR

NOT YOU FEEL THE DESCRIPTION IS CHARACTERISTIC OR NOT CHARACTERISTK OF MOST CHILDREN OfTHIS AGE, AND THEN DO THE SAME

FOR THE CHILDREN IN THE OPERATION HEADSTART PROGRAM. SINCE WE ARE INTERESTED IN YOUR FIRST OVERALL IMPRESSIONS, IT IS Ali

RIGHT TO GO TI4ROUG., THE ITEMS 1:ELATIVELY QUICKLY.

MOST CHILDREN OF THIS AGE ARE:OPERATION HEADSTART CHILDREN ARE: CIMIACTERISTICNJCNAIACTEMSTIC C:IMIACTEMSTICiffCIAIACTERISTIC I. ALERT

2. AMBITIOUS

3. ANNOYING

4. ANXIOUS

5. CALM

6. COMPETITIVE

7. CONFIDENT

8. CONSIDERATE

9. CREATIVE

10. CRUEL MOST CHILDREN OF THIS AGE ARE: OPERATION NEAOSTART CHILDREN ARE CIAIACTERISTIC MOT CMAIACTEMSTIC CIIMACTENISTIC SOT COARACTEMSTIC 11. DEF IAI" T

12. DEMANDING

13. DEPENDABLE

14. DESTRUCTIVE

15. FEARFUL

16. FRIENDLY

17. HAPPY

18. HOSTILE

19. INQUISITIVE

20. JEALOUS MOST CHILDREN OF THIS AGE ME:OPERATION HEADSTART CHILDREN ARE ...... CNARACTEMSTIC12ICNAIACTEMSTIC CINACTEMSTICRICIIMIACTEMSTIC 21. MEDDLESOME --I 22. NAGGING

23. PATIENT

24. POSSESSIVE t i 25. SELFISH

26. SELF-SUFFICIENT

27. SPOILED

28. STABLE

29. WELL-MANNERED

30. WITHDRAWN aMI DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE BAN a c. SUOSET SUIRERU NO. 1111-4310 PARENT EVALUATION OF OPERATION' HEAD START APPROVAL EXPIRES Si

CENTER IDENTIFICATION NUMBER TO THE PARENTS: WE WOULD LIKE TO KNOW WtiETHER YOU FEEL THAT YOU AND YOUR CHILD NAVE`PROFITED FROM PROJECT HEAD START. WE ARE PARTICULARLY INTERESTED 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 s 9 IN KNOWING WHAT THINGS YOU LIKED AND WHAT THINGS YOU DID NOT LIKE. THE 5 -._6 7 e INFORMATION FROM THIS FORM WILL BE USED TO MAKE FUTURE HEAD START PROGRAMS

0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 S 9 MORE EFFECTIVE. THIS EVALUATION IS TO BE DONE WITHOUT PERSONAL IDENTIFICATION. r, 0 I 2 4 5 -. -. -6 7 5 9 PLEASE 00 NOT WRITE YOUR NAME ON THE FORM. THE ONLY IDENTIFYING MARK 0 I 2 3 4 6 7...-.9 9 SHOULD IL THE CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTER NUMBER, WHICH THE WAD START CENTER STAFF WILL PROVIDE. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 PLEASE USE A Na 2 PENCIL TO MARK THIS SHEET 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 COMPLETE MOTH SIDES OF THIS FORM

1. FORMAL CONTACT AND PARTICIPATION vety NMI OCCASIONALLY WASTE NIT II TIE MITIMILE VOITIMILE 111111111111 V TINE MINN MY REACTIONS TO THE FOLLOWING WERE:

I. TALKING WITH CHILD'S TEACHERS.

2. MEETING WITH OTHER PARENTS.

3. SPEAKING WITH PARENT-COUNSELOR OR SOCIAL WORKERS.

4. SPECIAL EVENTS:

DISCUSSION ABOUT a. CHILD CARE

b. HOMEMAKING SKILLS

c. HOUSING CONDITIONS

d. 2.6PLOYMENT OPPORTUNITIES I4

s. PERSONAL PROBLEMS

5. GROUP TRIPS IN THE COMMUNITY

6. FILMS SHOWN IN CONNECTION WITH THE PROGRAM

7. OTHER (PLEASE SPECIFY ACTIVITY IN LOWER LEFT CORNER OF FORM)

11. THE CHILD

MY REACTIONS TO THE EXPERIENCES MY CHILD HAS HAD viii INIGI OCCASIONALLY WASTE NOT IN THE IINTIMNLE INITIMMLE =TIME OF TINE PROM IN THE OPERATION HEAD START PROGRAM ARE:

1. MEDICAL EXAMINATION

2. DENTAL EXAMINATION

3. OPPORTUNITY TO ATTEND SCHOOL AT AN EARLY AGE.

4. INCREASED EXPERIENCE WITH A-VARIETY OF TOYS AND GAMES.

5. INCREASED EXPERIENCE WITH A VARIETY OF BOOKS, STORIES, AND MUSIC.

6. TRIPS INTO THE COMMUNITY.

7. INDIVIDUAL ATTENTION GIVEN TO EACH CHILD BY TEACHER AND AIDES.

8, OPPORTUNITY TO PARTICIPATE IN GROUP ACTIVITIES WITH OTHER CHILDREN.

AS A RESULT OF ATTENDING THE OPERATION HEAD START PROGRAM, MUCH NO MUCH SETTER SETTER CHANGE WORSE WORSE MY CHILD WAS AFFECTED IN THE FOLLOWING WAYS:

1. GETTING ALONG WITH OTHER CHILDREN "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS 2. SELF-CONFIDENCE COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED 3. SPEAKING ABILITY BY Edward 251er 4. EVERYDAY MANNERS

S. FINISHING WHAT HE STARTS TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U.S. OFFICE OF 6. DOING WHAT HE IS TOLD .EDUCATION. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE 7. INTERESTED IN NEW THINGS THE ERIC SYSTEM WNW PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT °Mtn 8. CAN DO THINGS ON HIS OWN OVER OTHER FORMAL CONTACTS AND P RTICIPATIONS DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE

MEM

RIM

by Edward ZIalr, 1155 ISM MUltit CAP -HS FORM 41 JUN. '65 PARENT EVALUATION OF OPERATION HEADSTART

. THE HOME (6)

CHECK ANY OF THE FOLLOWING WHICH APPLY AS A RESULT OF YOUR CONTACT WITH

OPERATION HEAD START:

I. MY HUSBAND OR I HAVE BEEN HELPED SY SOME SOCIAL AGENCY.

2. MY HUSBAND OR I RECEIVED MEDICAL AND/OR DENTAL ATTENTION.

3. A CHILD OTHER THAN THE ONE(S) EMIOLLED IN OPERATION HEAD START) RECEIVED MEDICAL AND/OR DENTAL ATTENTION.

4. MOVEDTO OEM LIVING QUARTERS.

S. REPAIRED OR ADDED THINGS, ....,FURNITURE, NEW CURTAINS, ETC., TO MYPRESENT LIVING QUARTERS.

6. A FAMILY MEMBER GOT A JOS OR SWITCHED TO A UTTER j011.

7. MY HUSBAND OR I PLAN TO CONTINUE OUR OWN EDUCATION.

S. MY HUSBAND OR I HAVE SOUGHT LEGAL AID AND/OR FINANCIAL ASSISTANCE.

MM1 AUttlf Mt MME IMIE Am. III. THE HOME (I')

I. AM AWARE OF NEW THINGS THAT MY FAMILY AND I CAN DO IN THE COMMUNITY.

2. FEEL THAT THE COMMUNITY CARES ABOUT ME AND MY PROBLEMS.

3. HAVE LEARNED NEW THINGS ABOUT RAISING CHILDREN.

4. HAVE BEEN GIVEN NEW IDEAS ABOUT HOW TO TAKECARE OF MY FAMILY.

5. FEEL HOPEFUL ABOUT MY CHILDREN'S FUTURE.

6. FEEL UTTER ABLE TO HANDLE FAMILY ARGUMENTS THAT ARISE.

7. MADE NEW FRIENDS.

DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE

111111111 Budget Bureau.No. 116-6507 PROJECT HEAD START Approval expires March 31, 1966 PARENT PARTICIPATION RECORD (To be kept separately for each class) CARD 21 (1-2) CHILDDEVELOPMENT CENTER NO. (4-14) CLASS (15) S-

...... (17.18) 1.NUMBER OF CHILDREN IN THE GROUP (Pleaseput responsesin these areas. . 2.NUMBER OF MOTHERS OF THESE CHILDREN EMPLOYED AS FULL-TIME PAID WORKERS IN . OPERATION HEAD START (employment in an Operation Head Start program that meets for only half a day (19-20) should be considered as full-time)

3.NUMBER OF FATHERS OF THESE CHILDREN EMPLOYED AS FULL-TIME, PAID WORKERS IN (21-22) OPERATION HEAD START

4.NUMBER OF MOTHERS OF THESE CHILDREN EMPLOYED AS PART-TIME, PAID WORKERS IN (23-24) OPERATION HEAD START

5.NUMBER OF FATHERS OF THESE CHILDREN EMPLOYED AS PART-TIME, PAIDWORKERS IN (2526) OPERATION HEAD START

6.NUMBER OF MOTHERS OF THESE CHILDREN WHO PERFORMED SOME VOLUNTEERSERVICE (27-28) (unpaid) FOR THE PROGRAM

7.NUMBER OF FATHERS OF THESE CHILDREN WHO PERFORMED SOME VOLUNTEER SERVICE (29-30) (unpaid) FOR THE PROGRAM

S.HOW MANY FORMAL PARENT MEETINGS WERE HELD? ' (31)

9. IF FORMAL MEETINGS WERE HELD, HOW MANY PARENTS ATTENDED EACH MEETING?

s, NO. OF NO. OF , . , NO OF NO OF , s., ,4 ,,' , , MOTHERS FATHERS MOTHERS FATHERS

1ST FORMAL MEETING (32.33) (3445) 5TH FORMAL MEETING (48-49) (50-51)

2ND FORMAL MEETING (36-37) (38-39) 6TH FORMAL MEETING (52-53) (54-55)

3RD FORMAL MEETING (40-41) (42-43) 7TH FORMAL MEETING (56-57) (58.59)

4TH FORMAL MEETING (44-45) (46-47) 6TH FORMAL MEETING (60-61) (62-63)

CARD 22 (1-2) 10. WHAT WAS THE TOTAL NUMBER OF FORMAL PARENT-TEACHER CONFERENCESSCHEDULED? (17)

11. IF FORMAL PARENT-TEACHER CONFERENCES WERE SCHEDULED, HOW MANY PARENTSATTENDED?

NO. OF NO. OF NO. OF NO. OF NO. OF NO. OF CONFERENCES MOTHERS FATHERS CONFERENCES MOTHERS FATHERS

NONE (18-19) (20-21) 3 (30-31) (32-33)

1 (22.23) (24-25) 4 (34-35) (36-37)

2 (26-27) (28-29) 5 OR MORE (38-39) (40-41)

12. IN ADDITION TO TEACHER INITIATED CONFERENCES, HOWMANY.::"

B. FATHERS REQUESTED CONFERENCES? (44-45) A. MOTHERS REQUESTED CONFERENCES ? (-42-43)1

13. HOW MANY TRIPS OR SOCIAL EVENTS FOR PARENTS WERE HELD? (46)

14. IF PLANNED TRIPS OR SOCIAL EVENTS WERE HELD, HOW MANY PARENTSATTENDED THEM? PLEASE SPECIFY IN THE PARENTHESES, THE NATURE OF EACH EVENT, E.G., FILM, LECTURE, RUMMAGE SALES,ETC. NO. OF MOTHERS NO. OF [FATHERS NMEEMEMISIMEMEINIMINI (47.48) (49.50) 1ST EVENT ( ) (53.54) 2ND EVENT ( ) (51.52) (5546) (57-58) 3RD EVENT I ) (59-60) (61.62) 4TH EVENT ( ) (6566) 5TH EVENT I ) (6344) CAP-HS FORM 45 JUN 65 (Continued) (Please check one box for each line.)

15.IN GENERAL, ENTHUSIASM AND PARTICIPATION AT GROUP VERY VERY CAN'T 011 FAIR LOW HIGH LOW EVALUATE PARENT.TEACHER MEETINGS WAS: , 1 3 4 5 6 MOTHERS (67)

3 4 S 6 FATHERS (68)

. . , VFW , 16.IN GENERAL, ENTHUSIASM AND PARTICIPATION AT SPECIAL &.,;;.: A.1.. % , % . .., WAS: EVENTS(e.g., trips, lectures, films, etc.) ,.f:. c ',,:-4iOt. S.:, C. r ,,, , 4.;,',.:,. .;.':.;r.;' 4 S 6 MOTHERS (69)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FATHERS (70)

. :.,...,0. A...:.;:. ,,. , --, v000 x/4 ,05w0 ,::.:'5,1,r.'.:..- , .sf..w.00V$ :(0x 0:4,.A ,' , . IN GENERAL, PARENTAL ENTHUSIASM AND INTEREST IN THE ::",;::..., ,>,:.' .., .)w.v., 17. :.: ,,::;, ,..rf::>;' : , :. .. ''S ' OPERATION HEAD START WAS: W.?::::,,,, 4

1 2 3 4 5 6 MOTHERS (71)

1 3 4 5 6 FATHERS (72)

ACTIVE PARTICIPATION AND ENTHUSIASM WAS: 16.IN GENERAL, OVER THE COURSE OF THE PROGRAM,

DID NOT GREATLY CAN'T ,.',.?:::'.:..:41.,:. ,., GREATLY DECREASED INCREASED CHANGE DECREASEDEVALUATE ':' %:.., INCREASED

1 2 3 4 5 6- MOTHERS (73)

1 2 3 4 5 FATHERS (74)

BY THE CARD 23(1 -2) 19, IN GENERAL, THE ENTHUSIASM ANDINTEREST IN THE OPERATION HEAD START PROGRAM SHOWN P.ARC.111 I a ,jr c.m...n .m.1116.10 ii,I %/UM%.1..Ptia in rt.a. (Please check one box for each participating parent.) . ..:,.-^,. ,4 % .. VERY VERY CAN'T '.:".% $.""..%; HIGH FAIR LOW (Enter child's ID No. for those parents participating.) HIGH LOW EVALUATE . ,,,.';' 5 6 1. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 3 4 MOTHER (22)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FA TM ER (23) . 3 4 5 6 2. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 MOTHER (22)

5 6 _ 1 2 3 4 FATHER (23) _ 3 4 5 6 3. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 MOTHER (22)

5 6 _ 1 2 3 4 _ FATHER (23)

2 3 4 5 6 4. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 MOTHER (22)

6 _ 1 2 3 4 5 _ FATHER (23)

2 3 4 5 6 S. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 MOTHER (22)

5 6 _ 1 2 3 4 FATHER (23)

3 4 5 6 6. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (1641) 1 2 MOTHER (22)

5 6 _ 1 2 3 4 FATHER (23)

2 3 4 5 6 7. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 1 MOTHER (22)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

(Continued) CAP.HS FORM 45 JUN 65 111. (Continued) CARD 23(1.2)Continual

VERY VERY CAN'T , e' HIGH FAIR LOW i't. HIGH LOW EVALUATE < $ so

B. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 3 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

1 4 6 FATHER (23)

9. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 1 4 6 MOTHER (22)

1 2 4 FATHER (23)

10. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 1 2 3 4 5 MOTHER (22)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

11. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 3 51 6 MOTHER (22)

2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

12. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 2 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

2 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

13. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 2 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

14. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 3 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

15. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 2 3 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

2 5 6 FATHER (23)

16. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 3 MOTHER (22)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

17. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16-21) 1 2 3 4 5 6 MOTH ER (22)

1 2 3 4 FATHER (23)

IL CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 5 MOTHER (22)

1 2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

19. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 1 2 3 4 5 MOTHER (22)

(2() 2 3 4 5 6 FATHER

20. CHILD'S FULL ID NUMBER (16.21) 1 2 3 4 5 6 MOTHER (22)

2 3 4 5 6 FATHER (23)

GSA DC 64 39 5 CAPHS FORM 45 JUN 65 MO. CONSULTANT'S CHECKLIST CARD1(14) CODE (3.5) 'I.;UL TART

GRANTEE No. (8.13) GRANTEE 6511S DATE OF LAST DAY OF VISIT(Month andday) (1316) NO. OF DAYS (17)

(NOTE: Numbers in pecenthesee ere fee Office Use Only.) NEIGHOC11814000 OTHER I. WHAT NUMBER OFNON.PROFIESSIONALS ARE OILING USED IN THE FOLLOW. ING CATEGORIES? PAID VOL. . VOL. A. CUSTODIANS (16-19) (20.21) (2243) O. FOOD SERVICES (24.25) (26.27) (wpm C. TRANSPORTATION (3041) (3343) (3445) 0. TEACHER AIDE (36.37) O&M (40.41) E. HEALTH AIDE (42.43) (4445) (46.47) F. NEIGHBORHOOD WORKER (41I47) (50.51) 1 (53.53) G. BABY-SITTER (5445) (U47) ;5149) H. OTHERS (identify on "Comments Sheet") (60111) (62.43) (64.45) YES NO PARTIAL columnforeachquestion. Do not mark shaded areas) (Check applicable (1) (2) (3) (66) 2.HAS ANY FORMAL TRAINING BEEN ARRANGED FORPAID PROFESSIONALS? VOLUNTEERS? (67) 3.DOES THE PROFESSIONAL STAFF USE NON-PROFESSIONALSWELL? . (68) VOLUNTEERS WELL? (64) (70) 4.DO NON-PROFESSIONALS FEEL THEY ARE BEING USED WELL? VOLUNTEERS? Oil

S.ARE PARENT MEETINGS BEING HELD? (72) HAVE ANY SUGGESTIONS BY PARENTS BEEN ADOPTED BY THE CENTER? (73) ARE PARENTS HELPING TO PLAN FOLLOW - THROUGH PROGRAMS? (74) FULL-YEAR CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTERS ? - (73) .,, i. ARE ANY OF THESE ORGANIZATIONS PROVIDINGASSISTANCE TO THE PROGRAM? "-,i-'4. CARD 82(1.2) '' SCHOOLS (13) ; '''-'. , A''' (141,, PUBLIC HEALTH . '. *NM PUBLIC WELFARE (15) Tr''''''`'` '4..M.51* HOSPITALS OR CLINICS (16) ,": Ms.,, :.; MEDICAL SOCIETY 071 6: :,44;sf, e; '; :V ,,Ag:: DENTAL SOCIETY (ll) (19) . , NURSING SOCIETY (20) ' ":.$. k OPTOMETRISTS . :, ,,, . .: -f#A 5%2 ,.,.: 0,,`%<.1.:t +' .:t.,.. DIETITIANS OR HOME ECONOMISTS (LI/ MA i: v.,,...t.w, , , OTHERS(Identify on "Connate Skeet") (so/ ,:e.x.\1--\t-- . - -,$.1 Af , ,, W, 7.HAVE THESE THINGS BEEN DONE? A. MEDICAL EXAMINATIONS SCHEDULED FOR ALLCHILDREN (23) . B. DENTAL EXAMINATIONS SCHEDULED FOR ALLCHILDREN (24) C. IMMUNIZATIONS ARRANGED FOR (25) D. NEEDED MEDICAL TREATMENT ARRANGEDFOR (26) E. NEEDED DENTAL TREATMENT ARRANGEDFUR (27) (28) F. GLASSES PROVIDED AS NEEDED 'G. RESPONSIBILITY ASSIGNED TO SEE THAT CHILDRENGET NEEDED MEDICAL an OR DENTAL TREATMENT

0 >4. :. : }6 .,t, f; BASED ON YOUR OBSERVATIONS, PLEASE CHARACTERIZETHE OVERALL ACTIVITY OF IL .. . -- ,,,,,, ,;.,.', THE STAFF P-CCORDING TO THESE ITEMS: . 4'4 (30) A. TAILORS PROGRAM TO THE NEEDS OF THE INDIVIDUALCHILD . 8. APPEARS TO HAVE ESTABLISHED RAPPORTWITH liaS,,,; (s)THE CHILDREN AS A GROUP Oil 12) CHILDREN WITH SPECIAL PROBLEMS (32) C. CELIEVES IN AND IMPLEMENTS A RELATIVELY UNSTRUCTUREDPROGRAM OF ACTIVITIES (33) D. BELIEVES IN AND IMPLEMENTS A HIGHLY STRUCTUREDPROGRAM OF ACTIVITIES (34) CAP HS FORM 47 JUN SS (Pelle 1 3) (pratinsed . YU NO PARTIAL r.(Cos.::nard_) (1) (2) (3) E. BELIEVES ALL CHILDREN SHOULD BE EXPECTED TO ACHIEVE ASPECIFIC LEVEL OF (.15) GOALS DURING THE PROGRAM ._F. BELIEVES THAT GOALS SHOULD BE ADAPTED TO THE INDIVIDUALCHILD (36) G. EMPHASIZES SELFDICIPLINE AND SELFCONTROL (37) H. ENCOURAGES FREE PLAY AND EXPRESSION (31) I. PROVIDES A WIDE CHOICE OF LEARNING ACTIVITIES FOR THECHILDREN THROUGH (39) ACTIVITIES INVOLVING: MUSIC (40)MIN '.: :. .:.2 ART (41) : : SCIENCE (42)::, '.":1 CARE OF PETS (43) ::. %. NATURE STUDY 14411,;.-;..;:1',..s.:f:i.,-.-.L:i.::-.: .:: -:.i NUMBER CONCEPTS NV .iq';:::*;:...:!-! :I.:. : ,-;;;'*:::;-::.:-:' -" ''71:, I . PRINTED WORD (467k::::..:::; ":c::.;::": :'''::-:.::: .::;;;,,:;,,, ;:.... PERSONAL AND BODY NEEDS (47):::53P:i.:4:!:

MODERN SPACE AND COMMUNICATION DEVELOPMENTS MO';::::;k::::.:"i::.*: :.:?A1.7:.:... *i , ADULTS OF VARIED BACKGROUNDS ANDABILITIES (0)Wit! .ii;;' 4; :-:,.:W ,::;.:::::i7;?:: ..,: : THE LARGER WORLD ABOUT THEM (50)..:;.:`;Ya'::::;:::,:.V,;,:::::;:.*:: 'i J. MAKES EFFECTIVE-USE OF MATERIALSAND EQUIPMENT, ADAPTED TO THE PRE-SCHOOL (61) CHILD K. PROVIDES A WIDE VARIETY OFOUTDOOR PLAY ACTIVITIES (52)

1 (53) L. PROVIDES FOR SIGNIFICANTFIELD TRIPS M. PROVIDES TOTAL GROUP ACTIVITIESTHAT ARE OF SHORT DURATION AND INTERESTING (54) TO THE CHILDREN (55) N. PERMITS AND ENCOURAGES PARENTInenstaffi PARTICIPATION IN THE CLASSROOM

0. HAS ACHIEVED ADEQUATE WORKING RELATIONSHIPSAMONG OTHER PROFESSIONAL 1561 MEMBERS OF THE STAFF

P. HAS ACHIEVED ADEQUATE WORKING RELATIONSHIPSAMONG NON-PROFESSIONAL (57) MEMBERS OF THE STAFF (58) Q. HAS ESTABLISHED ADEQUATE COMMUNICATIONWITH THE CHILDREN'S PARENTS r. R. PLAN AND IMPLEMENT PROGRAM WHICH EMPHASIZESLANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT (59)

S. IS AWARE OF THE CREATIVE AND LEARNINGPOTENTIAL OF CHILCREN'S PLAY .(601 ACTIVITIES S

T. ENCOURAGES THE CHILDREN'S CURIOSITY, SPONTANEITY,AND EXPRESSION OF (61) FEELINGS

U. CAN ACCEPT THE BEHAVIOR OF CHILDRENFROM A DIFFERENT SOCIAL STATUS THAN (62) THEIR OWN '(63) V. HELPS CHILDREN FEEL ACCEPTED AND GOODABOUT THEMSELVES . W. EXPLOITS THE WORLD OF THINGS ANDIDEAS TO HELP CHILDREN GAIN NEW CONCEPTS . (64) AND UNDERSTANDINGS AND CREATE AN AVENUEFOR COMMUNICATION SKILLS

X. ENCOURAGES THE CHILDREN AND GIVESTHEM TIME TO MAKE CAREFUL OBSERVATIONS (65) OF THINGS THAT INTEREST THEM

Y. CAPITALIZES ON THE DIFFERENCESBROUGHT TO THE CENTER BY CHILDREN OF (66) DIFFERING BACKGROUNDS AND CULTURES

Z. RESPONDS TO CHILDREN IN A WAYTHAT HELPS THE CHILD TO SEE HIMSELF AND HIS (67) INTERESTS AS WORTHY

AA. PLANS EXPERIENCES FOR THE CHILDREN THATHELPS THEM APPRECIATE THE (61) SERVICES OF POLICEMEN, FIREMEN, DOCTORS, DENTISTS ANDOTHERS WHO SERVE THE NEEDS OF THE COMMUNITY CATEGORY AND ALSO CHECK SOX AT LEFT a,\ 9. PLEASE INDICATE SEPARATELY FOR EACH '4 < CARD 63 (14) WHICH CHARACTERIZES THE MAJORITY OFTEACHERS IN THE PROGRAM. ',..,.,. ,s.Nr ' A. PROFESSIONAL TRAINING IN (22) .7Ws177, igP.:jse: CHILD OR HUMAN GROWTH AND DEVELOPMSNT (1) EARLY CHILDHOOD EDUCATION (2) (14).:.s0::%:g:i V ::*:::51;:..':t1 ELEMENTARY EDUCATION (3) 0 (10, b.<- - fii)..;,L, ''' s s CHILD PSYCHOLOGY (4) 0 s, rv 071.?;zelat PEDIATRIC NURSING (5) CAP. HS FORM 47 JUN SS .13) (Continued) - YES NO PARTIAL 9:' (Con:in.N.,11 (22) (1) (2) (3) _ SOCIAL WORK ';'.,...... (S) El (1$) ' OTHER (7) 0 (10) i?;.:f::1": .1. MIXED (8) 0 (70) ,''::'ts:',":

ONE .(T) (27)*.A;;:,::,,'. ..-,,. B. ONE OR MORE YEARS EXPERIENCE IN : (32) ;k;9 ,,e;-(i.m'.:."t-..:,,wi:: . :. NURSERY SCHOOL 0) aniq5 e..fi::`:Z::ys::.,1 KINDERGARTEN (2) (24) i',.., %:';. 2'i1:

ELEMENTARY SCHOOL (3) 0 no-5....:::,.::;:::T; ,.:. NURSING (4)C) no.5.ttr.q:in3:';f ,, SOCIAL WORK (5) (37) ',...q:A;Me:::. v RECREATION (6) (21) g.;:;;01:0> MIXED (7) 0 (29) W. '1:if;?::V:ez.q:k:'::,.:-$;. OTHER ($) 0 (io) ,?-:.-!txU5(ZA-;..,*(.. NONE (9 ) 0 (31) 0' 444V::4''

10. ARE TEACHERS ASSIGNED ANY RESPONSIBILITIES FOR ,-.,: ..-',A.M.n. 44::4."#,,k?' ',IV* i / A. SEEING THAT THE CHILD GETS MEDICAL TREATMENT (33) ,f,'- ' '''4 s..

B. HELPING PARENTS TO LEARN ABOUT THEIR CHILDREN (34) :''''4''' .. C. HELPING TO SOLVE FAMILY PROBLEMS WHICH REQUIRE SOCIAL SERVICES (35)4:1;i:Y Air-A9,4)A-g.. 11. PLEASE CHECK THE APPROPRIATE BOXES IN EACH CATEGORY...... ::":,1,,. k4et-U-Ra,R.esiegs A. CENTER PROVIDES (1) 111 1 ",117:17'F'" 7'''%.r.Z14:1WVIV), BREAKFAST (36) SNACK Li (37) LUNCH an ."..'AP:c''''',,IC,s;,,,? 0.."' , ) 9,$,., ..:, -e.....- .'..q!...';!. .): B. QUALITY, VARIETY, AND SERVICE OF FOOD APPEAR ow;?'1".'';'''r''.--,r :.64::::,,,, ,.. ,r 4.- ADEQUATE (1) INADEQUATE En (2) MIXED 0(3) ':f., ' ::fVfn 12. ARE THERE ANY SIGNIFICANT PROBLEMS IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN GRANTEE AND <, 0: 4,:... .,.:..s., (40) ..Y- 14:<..4V. SPONSOR? (Please &en** owlGontoments 51ms") ,: 7''::,te;41.* ,*,...*..... 13.IS PRESENT SPACE ADEQUATE? (41) - . :, -3.";7 A '' M. a X %, VS.. SIZE? (42) ? ze k0.?:KV CONDITION? (43) .13,k! :Xi!...W. (If not,describe on "Comments Sheet") .;..77:40M,:4,hss,.. .ttalL.A:: 14. HAVE PLANS BEEN MADE TO TRANSMIT RECORDS TO THE SCHOOL SYSTEM? MM:',W ,143:.%:. .) (44):iw,.'. .:,: 4:44f .< 15. PLEASE CHECK THE KINDS OF PROGRAMS WHICH ARE BEING PROVIDED FOR PARENTS ',".!Cir-c.',;;e; 7 '42tSZMU HELP ON CHILD REARING (45) 'N's' .- 'c:P.0 .vga.t HOMEMAKING EDUCATION (46) s:- ""4,:.e. 4''P*xkot. s --: S 'c;, CONSUMER EDUCATION . . (47); f,44' ' Menial er ...:,, OTHERS (Please list on "Comments Sheet") . (4$) 18. ARE THE SERVICES OF SOCIAL WORKERS AVAILABLE TO THE PROGRAM? (0) 17. ARE THE CHILDREN SUPPLIED WITH ADEQUATE CLOTHING TO ATTEND THE CENTER? (50) 18. DOES THE CENTER HAVE ENOUGH CONSUMABLE SUPPLIES AND MATERIALS TO OPERATE EFFECTIVELY? (Please describe deficiencies on "Comments Sheet") " (51) --7.--7 . 19,11S THERE ANY EVIDENCE.OF DISCRIMINATION TOWARD CHILDREN, STAFF OR PARENTS? (If so, please dcicribe on "Conuiiinti Mee.) (52

20.IS THE CENTER HAVING ANY SIGNIFICANT PROBLEM IN MAINTAINING ATTENDANCE? (If so, please describe what is bang dose to overcome the problem ea " Comments Shoot".) : (53)

21. ARE PLANS BEING MADE TO PROVIDE ADDITIONAL PROGRAMS THIS FALL FOR a4'5't4 . .''<, : .: HEAD START CHILDREN? (54)"sb,,,V;:ssw,_ss ,'4;'...",'Vf. ,` '',' A. SUPPLEMENTARY EDUCATIONAL PROGRAM (55)Fs's 4; '4...' '-?"N 8. MEDICAL SERVICES (so',:t.:4t.R.,.,sauz,', C.SOCIAL SERVICES (57)j'Er'7r07,(-, D. PARENT EDUCATION (5$)i:',7. -'Qlsnaiik.. 22. DOES THE COMMUNITY PLAN TO HAVE A FULL -YEAR CHILD DEVELOPMENT CENTER? (59)

A. FOR APPROXIMATELY HOW MANY CHILDREN? (6043) . ' , . . . , ., .. s.::::,. ., ... ;',,,,. t' B. IS ADEQUATE SPACE AVAILABLE? .. (64) . ------. C. IS ADEQUATE PROFESSIONAL STAFF AVAILABLE? (R) 23.IS ANYTHING BEING DONE TO ADAPT FIRSI:04AQE OR KINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS TO (SS) CHILD DEVELOPMENT CONCEPTS? (PleaseW "eak on "Comenesneed) . CAHS FORM 47 JUN IS (Pile. 3 a 3) SSA RC Si. 480 Budget Bureau No. 116-6510

Approval expires June 1, 1966

PROJECT HEAD START PARENT INTERVIEW

NAME OF HEAD START CENTER CHILD'S NAME:

ADDRESS OF HEAD START CENTER CHILD'S ADDRESS:

(street address) (street address)

(city and state) (city andstate)

NUMBER OF HEAD START CENTER CHM'S 1D. NO. TELEPHONE NUMBER:

INTRODUCTION

Hello. I'm (your name) from Project Head Start.We are interviewing some of the parents of Head Start children, and I_ understandthat (name of child) was registered for the program. May I speak with the child's mother (or stepmother)?

IF MOTHER IS NOT HOME, FIND OUT BEST TIME TO CALL BACK FOR INTERVIEW.

IF NO MOTHER OR STEPMOTHER IN HOUSEHOLD, ASK:

May I speak with the female head of the household?

IF NO FEMALE HEAD OF HOUSEHOLD, ASK:

May I speak with the child's father (or, if nofather, male head of the household)?

RECORD OF CALLS

CALL DATE TIME OUTCOME YOUR NAME

1

,

2

3

4

L_5

CAP-HS 41 (Rev. Aug. 1965) replaces CAP-HS Forms41 and 46 dated June 1965 which may be used. -2-

TIME INTERVIEW BEGAN: A.M. ----P.M.

First, I'd like to list all the persons who live inthis houmehold.

A. 1. Let's see. The child who was in Head Start is(name of chi1J). WRITE CHILD'S NAME ON THE FIRST LINE OF TABLE ON PAGE ?.

2. IF YOU ARE INTERVIEWING MOTHER ORSTEPMOTHER: And you are the child's mother?May I have your name, please?WRITE MOTHER'S (OR STEPMOTHER'S) NAME ON SECOND LINE.

IF YOU ARE INTERVIEWING SOMEONE OTHERTHAN MOTHER OR STEPMOTHER: Does (child's) mother live in thishousehold? IF YES, ASK "What is her name, please?" AND WRITE NAME ON SECOND LINE. IF NO, WRITE IN "ABSENT" ON SECOND LINE.

3. Does (child's) father (orstepfather) live in this household? IF YES, ASK "What is his name, please?" AND wRrrE NAME ONTHIRD LINE. IF NO, WRITE IN "ABSENT" ON THIRD LINE.

4. What other members of (child's) immediatefamily live in the household with (child)? WRITE NAMES OF BROTHERS,SISTERS,GRANDPARENTS, AUNTS, UNCLES, AND OTHER FAMILY MEMBERS WHO LIVE IN THIS HOUSEHOLDON SUBSEQUENT LINES.

5. What other persons live in the household with(child)?WRITE IN NAMES OF PEOPLE WHO LIVE IN HOUSEHOLD BUT ARE NOT RELATEDTO THE CHILD.

CHECK THE LINE TO THE LEFT OF THE PERSON YOU AREINTERVIEWING. THEN RECORD THE FOLLOWING INFORMATION FOR EACH PERSON LISTED.

B. What is (each person's) relationship to(child)? Write the one code 4, 5, 6, or 7 (use the legend at the bottom of page 3) to describe the correctrelationship under "B." If unrelated to child, write the one code 8, 9 or 0(use the same legend at the bottom of page 3) which describesthe person's role in the household.

C. CIRCLE CODE 1 or 2 FOR MALE OR FEMALEUNDER "C" FOk EACH PERSON IN HOUSEHOLD.

D. How old was (each person) on his(her) last birthday? WRITE AGE UNDER "D." Ile INFANT UNDER 1 YEAR, WRITE IN "0."IF RESPONDENT DOESN"T KNOW EXACTAGE,ASK FOR A"BEST GUESS."

E. What is the highest grade in school that(each person) completed? WRITE IN NUMBER OF SCHOOL YEARS: WRITE IN "E." (2 years high school = 10, completed high school = 12, completed college = 16). FOR CHILDREN UNDER AGE 6, WRITE "0" WITHOUT ASKING.

F. ASK FOR EACH PERSON AGES 5-20:Will (person) be going to school this fall.? CIRCLE THE CODE FOR "YES ," "NO ," OR "DON'T KNOW"UNDER "F."

G. And in what state was (each person)born? WRITE NAME OF STATE UNDER "G." IF BORN OUTSIDE' U.S., WRITE IN NAME OF COUNTRY. HOUSEHOLD ENUMERATION

CIRCLE LINE NO. TO INDICATE RESPONDENT

A. B. C. D. E. F. G. RELATIONSHI' IEARS OFGOING TO SCHOOLSTATE OR COUNTRY NAME SEX AGE TO CHILD SCHOOL THIS FALL? OF BIRTH

Yes 1 Child No 2 F...2 D.K 3

Mother Yes 1 (or step- No 2 mother) F...2. D.K 3 f Father M...1 Yes 1 (or step.- No 2 Father) F...2 D.K 3

M...1 Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

M.. Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

M.. Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

M.. Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

M.. Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

M...1 Yes 1 No Z F...2 D.K 3 :

M... Yes 1 No 2 D.K 3

M...1 Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D.K 3

. M...1 Yes 1 No 2 F...2 D K 3

LEGEND

4 Brother/Sister 8 Roomer 5 Unele/Aunt 9 Friend 6 Grandparent 0 Other 7 Relative -4-

Now, we're ready to begin the mainpart of the interview. 1. As part of the Head Start program, did you Yes No

A. Talk with any of (child's) teachers? 1 2 r-

B. Get to know any other parents you didn't know before? 1 2

C. Talk with any social workers or counselors about (child)? 1 2

D. Attend any special meetings about child care? 1 2

E. Attend any special meetings about homemaking skills? 1 2 fl

F. Attend any special meetings about housing conditions? 1 2

G. Attend any special meetings about job opportunities? 1 2

H. Attend any special meetings to talk about your own personal

problems?. . . 1 a.

I. See any films or movies shown in connection with the program? . . . 1

J. Go on any group trips in the community? 1 2

FOR EACH ITEM CODED "YES" IN QUEJTION it_ ASK:

How worthwhile did you feel it was to (each item below which you coded "Yes" in Question 1)--Was it very much worthwhile, was it all right, or was it a waste oftime? Very All WasteDont Much Right of Time Know

A. Talk with teachers? 3 4 5 6

B. Get to know parents you didn't know before? 3 4 5 6

C. Talk with social workers or counselors about (child)? 3 4 5 6

D. Attend special meetings about child care? 3 4 5 6

Attend special meetings about homemaking skills?. . . 3 4 5 6

F. Attend special meetings about housing conditions? . . 3 4 5 6

G. Attend special meetings about job opportunities?. . . 3 4 5 6

H. Attend special meetings about personal problems?. . . 3 4 5 6

I. See films or movies in connection with the program? 3 4 5 6

J. Go on group trips in the community? 3 4 5 6 non ' ( Yes NoKnow S. As part of the Head Start program,did (child)...

A. Have a medical examination? 1 2 3

2 3 B. Have a dental examination? 1

3 C. Take any trips in the community? 1 2

3 D. Get to know any new, toys orgames? 1 2

3 E. Get to see or hear a lot ofbooks and stories ari music? . . 1 2

2 3 F. Get any individual attentionfrom the teacher? . 1

G. Have a chance to take part in groupactivities with other children? 1 2 3

4. FOR EACH ITEM CODED"YES" IN Q. 3 ABOVE, ASK:

How worthwhile did youfeel it was for (child) to (each itembelow which you coded "Yes" in Q. 3)--verymuch worthwhile, all right, or was it a wasteof time?

Very All Waste Don't MuchRightof TimeKnow

6 A. Have a medical examination? 3 4 5

5 6 . 4 B. Have a dental examination? 3

4 5 6 C. Take trips in the cemmunity9 3

5 6 D. Get to know new toys ar..4games? 3 4

3 4 5 6 E. Sec and hear the books, storiesand music? .

. 3 4 5 6 F. Get individual attentionfrom the teacher? .

G. Take part in group activitieswith other 4 5 6 children? 3

on the child, asfar as you 5. A. Did the Head Start program have anybad effects can tell? Yes 1

No 2

Don' t Know . . . 3

effects on the child, as far as you cantell? B. Did the program have any good Yes 1 No 2

Don' t Know . . . 3 -6-

6. Since the beginning of the Head Start program, would you say that (child) is much better, somewhat better, about the same, somewhat worse, or much worse as far as:

Much Somewhat About Somewhat Much Don't better better sameworse worse Know

A. Getting along with other children is concerned' 1 2 3 4 5 6

5 6 B. How about in self-confidence? . . 1 2 3 4 B

C. As far as speaking ability is con- cerned' 2 3 4 5 6

D. How about everyday manners" 1 2 3 4 5 6

E. Finishing what he starts" 1 2 3 4 5 6

F. Doing what he is told? 1 2 3 4 5 6

6 G. Being interested in new things? . . 1 2 3 4 5

H. Being able to do things on his own? 1 2 3 4 5 6

7. As result of Project Head Start, did you (or your husband). .

Yes No

A. Get help of any kind from any social agency? .1 2

B. Get any medical or dental attention? 1 2

C. Make any plans to continue your own education? 1 2

D. Get a job or switch to a better job? . . 1 2

Yes Na

[1 8. As a result of Project Head Scart

A. Do you yourself feel any more hopeful about (child's) future" ,1 2

B..Did you yourself make any new friends? .. .. . 1 2

C. Did you ream anything you didn't know before about raising children? 1 2

Do you feel that the community cares at allabout you and your problems? 1 2 -7-

9. Has (child) ever attended a Day Care Center?. Asummer camp?...ASettlement House Programa...Sunday School?...Any other suchprograms?... CIRCLE CODE FOR EITHER YES OR NO FOR EACH ITEK. Don't Yes No Know

A. Day Care Center 1 2 3

B. Summer camp 1 2 3

C. Settlement House Program .. 1 2 3

D. Sunday School .OOOO . 1 2 3

E. Other (SPECIFY) 1 2 3

10. Do you own or rent your place here?

1 Own apartment . . GO' TO Q.11 2 Own house . . . GO TO Q.11 3 Rent house. . . . ASK A

Rent apartment . ASK*A 4 Other (SPECIFY)

ASK A . . . 5

A. Imo: ais your place here partof a public housing project?

Yes 1 No 2

WRITE IN NUMBER. 11. How many rooms do you havehere?

rooms

12 How many other children does(child) share his bedroom with? One other child 1 Two 2 Three 3 Four 4 Five or more children 5

f. . 6 None .. Don't Know 7 B. AM many adults doeschild) share his.bedroom with? One adult' 1 Two 2 Three ., OOOO OOO 3 : Four 4 Five or more OOOOO 5 6 Nona . .. OOOOOOOOO Don't Knave. OOOOOOOO OO 7 -8-

home? CIRCLE ONLY ONE 13. Row long has your familybeen living here in this CODE. 1 Less than six months ASK A . . . .

Six months to lessthan 1 year ASK A 2

3 1 year to less than2 years . ASK A

4 2 years to less than3 years . . 60 TO Q. 14

. . . 5 3 years to less than5 years . . GO TO Q. 14

. . 6 5 years to less than10 years . GO TO Q. 14

. 7 10 years or longer GO TO Q. 14 . .

. 8 All my life GO TO Q. 15 . .

have you moved duringthe A. IF"LESS THAN TWO YEARS: How many times AS ONE MOVE. last two years?.COUNTMOVEIO PRESENT HOME

Once

Twice 2

0 Three times 3

.. 4 Four t i m e s . . . . OOOOOOO

Five to eight times 5

Nine to twelve times 6

More than twelvetimes 7

moved here . . . Did you live in this 14. Where did you livejust before you different part of-thiscity (town), inanother sameneighborhood, in a state? CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE. part of. this state,or in a different 1

. SaMe neighborhood

Different part of city ortown 2

3 Another part of thesame skate: ". 4 Another state 5 Other (SPECIFY)

, 15. How many people in your family are employed right now?

One' ASK A I

Two ASK A 2

Three ASK A 3

Four ASK A 4

Five or more . ASK A 5

None GO TO Q. 16 . 9

A. IF ANYONE EMPLOYED: Who is (arc) employed?CIRCLE ALL THAT APPLY.

Mother 1

Father 2

Older brother(s) 3

Older sister(s) 4

Grandmother 5

Grandfather 6

Other (SPECIFY) 7

16. Who usually takes care of (child) during the day? CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE.

Father 1

Mother 2

Aunt 3

Grandmother 4

Sister . . . 5

Brother 6

Friend of family 7

Baby sitter 8

Other (SPECIFY) 9 Don't Know 0

17. Aside from when (shill) is at the Headstart school, is (he/she) usually taken care of in your home or someplace else? (Where?)

In own home 1

Nursery school or Day Care Center . 2

At home of relative 3

At home of baby sitter 4 Other (SPECIFY) 5

Don't Know...... , . . . . 6 -10-

18. Are there any languages spoken at home otherthan English? Yes ASK A AND B No GO TO Q. 19 IF YES: A. What language is spoken? (Any others?) CIRCLE ALL THAT APPLY.

Arabic 1 Norwegian

Chinese 2 Polish

French 3 Portuguese German 4 Spanish

Greek 5 Swedish

Hebrew 6 Yiddish

Italian 7 Other (SPECIFY) Japanese 8

B. By whom is itspoken? CIRCLE ALL THAT APPLY Mother Father Grandfather

Grandmother Sisters and/or brothers Others (SPECIFY)

19. Do you yourself belong to any clubs or organizations, such as church groups, or a labor union, or political organization? .Yes ASK A 1

No GO TO Q 20 2

A. IF YES: How often do you attend club or organization meetings more than once a week, about once a week, several times a month, or less often than that? More than once a week 1 About once a week 2 Several times a month 3 Less often than that 4

20. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD: Does your husband belong to any clubs or organizations of any kind? Yes ASK A 1

No . GO TO Q. 21 . . .. . 2

A. IF YES: How often does he attend club or organization meetings -- more than once a week, about once a week, several times a month, or less often than that?

More than once a week 1 About once a week 2 Several times a month 3 Less Often.thadthat 4 21. Do you have a radio in your home?

Yes . . ASK A AND B 1

No . . GO TO Q. 22 2

IF YES:

A. About how, many hours a day is the radio on in your home?CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE. 1 hour 1

2 - 3 hours 2

4 - 5 hours 3 6 - 7 hours 4

8 hours or more 5

Less than one hour a day 6

Not every day -- few hours a week 7

Never 8

Don't know 9

B. Who usually listens to it?CIRCLE AS MANY AS APPLY.

Respondent 1

Respondent's husband 2

Child in Head Start 3 Others in Household 4

22. Do you have a TV set in your home?

Yes . . ASK A AND B 1

No . . GO TO Q. 23 2 IF YES:

A. About how many hours a day is the TV on in your home?CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE. 1 hour 1

2 - 3 hours 2

4 - 5 hours 3 6 - 7 hours 4

8 hours or more. . 5

Less than one hour a day . . 6

Not every day -- few hours a week. 7

Never. 8 Don't know 9

B. Who usually watches it?CIRCLE AS MANY AS APPLY.

Respondent 1 Respondent's husband 2

Child in Head Start 3 Others in household 4 -12-

23. A. About how often do you go to the movies?CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN A BELOW.

B. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD, OTHERWISE GO TO C. How about your husband--how often would you say he goes to the movies? CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN B BELOW.

C. And how often does (child) go to the movies? CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN C BELOW.

- _ A B C Respondent Husband Child

Twice a week or more. . 1 1 1

Once a week 2 2 2 Once every two or three weeks 3 3 3 Once a month 4 4 4 Once every two or three months 5 5 5

Two or three times a year 6 6 6

Once a year cr less . . 7 7 7 Never 8 8 8

Don't know . - 9 9

24. A. About how often do you attend religious services? CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN A BELOW.

B. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD, OTHERWISE GO TO C. How about your husband--how often would you sayhe attends religious services?. CIRCLE ONE CODEIN COLUMN B BELOW.

C. And how often does (child) attend religious services?CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN C BELOW.

A B C Respondent Husband Child

1 Once a week or more . . 1 1 Twice a month 2 2 2

Onc every one to three months 3 3 3 Two or three times a year 4 4 4

Once a year or less 5 5 '5

Never 6 6 6 Don't know - 7 7 25. A. And how often do you usually read a newspaper -- everyday, about every other day, once a week, or less than once a week. CIRCLEONE CODE IN COLUMN A BELOW.

B. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD. OTHERWISE GO TO Q. 26. How often does your husband read a newspaper --almost every day, about every other day, once aweek, or once a month or less? CIRCLE ONE CODE IN COLUMN B BELOW.

A. B. Respondent Husband

.

1 Almost every day . . 1

2 Every other day . . 2

Once a week 3 3' Less than once a week 4 4 Don't know - 5

I'll 26. We're interested in how often youusually do a number of other things. read the things we want to knowabout and you just tell me for each one whether you never do it atall, you do it once every threemonths or less., about once a month, twice amonth, or once a week or more. HAND RESPONDENT BLUE CARD. This card has on it what I justread to you and it will help you remember the answer groups. ASK ONE ITEM AT A TIME AND CIRCLEONE CODE ON EACH LINE. Once Once a Twice Once Every Week or a a Three Never More Month Month Months or Less

.

2 1 0 A. Go to a sports event? 4 3

2 1 0 B. Take part in sports event? 4 3

2 1 0 C. Read a book or magazine ? 4 3

D. Go to see friends or 4 3 2 1 0 relatives?

, . E. Have friends or relatives 4 3 2 I at your home?.

3 2 1 0 F. Eat in a restaurant? 4

play G. Go to a concert or a 4 3 2 1 0 or a museum ?

H. Go on a picnic, for a 4 3 2 1 0 ride, or swimming ?

I. Meet and talk withfriends 4 3 2 1 0 on the street? -14-

27. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD. OTHERWISE GO TO Q. 28. How often would you say your husband does these same things?ASK ONE ITEM AT A TIME AND CIRCLE ONE CODE ON EACH LINE.

. Once Once a Twice Once Every Don't Week or a a Three Never Know More Month Month Months or Less

A. Go to a sports event? 4 3 2 1 0 X

B. Take part in sports event? 4 3 2 1 0 X

C. Read a book or magazine? 4 2 1 0 X

D. Go to see friends or 4 3 2 1 0 X relatives?

E. Have friends or relatives 4 3 1 0 X at your home? .

F. Eat in a restaurant? 4 3 2 1 0, X

G. Go to a concert or a play 4 3 2 1 0 X or a museum?

H. Go on a picnic, for a 4 2 1 . 0 X ride, or swimming? 3

I. Meet and talk with 4 3 2 1 0 X friends on,the street?

28. Were you registered to vote in the election for President last November or didn't yoti get a chance to register? Registered . . .ASK A 1

Not registered . .GO TO Q. 29 . 2

A. IF REGISTERED: Do you remember whether or not you actually voted? Voted 4

Did not vote 5

Don't remember 6

29. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUSEHOLD. OTHERWISE GO TO Q. 30. Was your husband registered to vote that election?

Registered . . .ASK A 1

Not registered . .GO TO Q. 30 .

A. IF REGISTERED: Doyou remember whether or not he voted last NOvember?

Voted 4

Did not vote 5 Don't remember OOOO 6 -15-

30. Have you ever taken a trip outside your owncity (town) in; ASK ONE ITEM AT A TIME, AND CIRCLE CODE FOR YES OR NOFOR EACH.

Yes No

A. A car 1 2

8. A bur 1 2

C. A train . . . , 1 2

D. An airplane . . 1 2

E. A boat . . 1 2

IF ANSWER IS "NO" TO ALL, GO TOQ. 32.

31 A. Now think about just the last year. How many times have you gone to any place that's 50 miles away or more,aside from moving? CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE IN COLUMN A BELOW.

B. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES INHOUSEHOLD. OTHERWISE GO TO (421. About how many times during thelast year would you say your husband has gone to any place50 miles away .or more? CIRCLE ONLY ONE CODE IN COLUMN B BELOW. A. B. Respondent Husband

None 0 0 One 1 1

Two 2 2 Three 3 3 Four 4 4 5 Five to eight . 5 6 Nine to twelve . . 6

7 More than twelve .

things. do you sometimes do in your sparetime? ASK ONE 32. Which of the following ITEM AT A TIME, ANDCIRCLE CODE FOR YES OR NOFOR EACH. Yes No

2 A. Justsit and relax? 1

B. Grow flowers or vegetablesin a garden? 1 2

C. Sew things, such asdresses 1 2 or curtains? . .

D. Play a musical instrument orsing with a choir? 1 2

. . 1 2 E. Play cards (=A7 othergames? . .

F. Any other spec:;..:1 thing youdo in yo..r spare time? 1 2

G. IF YES TO F: What? WRITE IN WHAT RESPONDENT SAYS: 1 -16-

33. ASK ONLY IF HUSBAND LIVES IN HOUS'(OLD. OTHERWISE GO TO Q. 34. Now I'll read a list of things and you tell me which ones your husband sometimes does in his spare time.ASK ONE ITEM AT A TIME, AND CIRCLE CODE FOR YES OR NO FOR EACH. Don't Yes No Know

A. Fishing or hunting? 1 2 3

B. Work with cars or just tinker with things? 1 2 3

C. Sit and relax? 1 2 3

D. Play cards or other games? 1 2 3

E. Do carpentry just for himself? . . . . 1 2 3 . F. Play a musical instrument or sing with a choir? 1 2 3

Grow flowers or vegetables in a garden? 1 2 3

H. Any other special thing he does in his spare time? 1 2 3

I. IF YES TO H: What? WRITE IN WHAT RESPONDENT SAYS

34. A. Does (child) have many toys or games, some but not too many, oronly a few? Many 1

Some 2

A few 3 None 0 Don't Know 4 B. How about children's books or magazines, does(child) have many, some but not too many, or only a few books or magazines?

Many 1

Some 2

A few 3 None Don't Know 4 C. And crayons, paints, and papers, does (child)have many,some but not too many, or only a few crayons, paints,and papers? Many 1

Some 2 A few 3 None 0 Don't Know 4 -17-

35. Has (child) ever had any pets?

Yes

No 2

Don't Know 3

36. How often during the past year has (child) gone to the library -- never, once, a few times, or many times?CIRCLE ONE CODE IN LINE "A" BELOW AND THEN ASK SAME QUESTION FOR OTHER ITEMS ONE AT A TIME.

I A Fewi Many Don't Never I Once Times I Times Know

A. Library? 0 1 2 3 4

B. Supermarket? 0 1 2 3 4

C. Small grocery or food store? 0 1 2 3 4

D. Post Office? 0 1 2 3 4

E. Playground or park? 0 1 2 3 4

F. Zoo? 0 1 Z 3 4

G. Airport or railroad station? 0 l 2 3 4

H. Fire station? 0 1 2 3 4

I. Department store? 0 1 2 3 4

J. Parade, circus, or fair? 0 1 2 3 4

K. A restaurant to eat? 0 1 2 3 4

L. Beach, lake, or pool? 0 1 2 3 4

M. Ride in a car? 0 1 2 3 4 -18-

37. Just one last question. Here is a card. (HAND RESPONDENT WHITECARD.) Would you please tellmewhich one of those is presentlyyour main source of income?CIRCLE ONLYONE.

Wages, salary 1

Business or profession 2

Social Security 3 Government pension 4

Private pension 5

Old Age Assistance 6 General welfare assistance . . ASK A . . . 7 Aid to Dependent Children (ADC) ASK A . 8

Interest, dividends, insurance 9

Rent 0 Cash contributions X No money income

A. IF WELFARE OR ADC: How long have you been receiving thistype of assistance?

Less than six months 1

Six months to less than 1year 2

1 year to less than 2years 3 2 years to less than 3years 4

3 years to less than 5years 5

5 years to less than 10 years 6

10 years or longer 7

INI=E1,

That's all the questionsI have. Thank you very much foryour time and help.

CJ

TIME INTERVIEW ENDED IMMEDIATELY AFTER LEAVING RESPONDENT, FILL OUT ITEMS ON NEXT PACE. A.M.

P.M. -19-

FILL OUT FOLLOWING ITEMS IMMEDIATELY AFTER LEAVING RESPONDENT

38.

A. RESPONDENT'S RACE: F. CIRCLE ONE CODE TO INDICATE RESPONDENT'S ALERTNESS AND YOUR White...... 1 ESTIMATE OF INTELLIGENCE: Negro 2 Dull, couldn't understand . . 1 Other (SPECIFY) 3 Slow, had to explain a lot. 2

Average intelligence 3 B. From what you could see, would you rate this household as -- Above average intelligence. 4 CIRCLE ONE CODE

G. LANGUAGE USEE IN INTERVIEW: Very clean and orderly . . 1

Fairly clean and orderly . . 2 English, no difficulty. . . . 1

Clean but not too orderly . . 3 English, some difficulty. . . 2

Not clean but orderly . . . . 4 English, but very difficult 3

Not clean and not orderly . . 5 Spanish 4

Other language ( SPECIFY). . 5 C. How much trouble did you have getting the respondent to start the interview? H. INTERVIEWER'S SIGNATURE:

No trouble 1

Some trouble 2

A lot of trouble . . . 3 I. DATE INTERVIEW COMPLETED:

D. Did respondent at any time try to break off the interview?

J. WRITE HERE ANY REMARKS YOU WOULD Yes 1 LIKE TO MAKE ABOUT THIS RESPONDENT No 2 OR INTERVIEW;

E. CIRCLE ONE CODE TO INDICATE RESPONDENT'S BEHAVIOR DURING INTERVIEW:

Nervous most of the time . . 1

Occasionally nervous ; . 2

Mostly, relaxed 3

ISA Di.fib. 2 1N / APPENDIX B STATISTICAL MODELS AND ANALYSES

A. The PPVT ScoresUnmatched With ChildCharacteristics Three kinds of scores maybe discussed:(1) the scores called "pre" under the rules discussedin subsection II.Fof the first volume of this document, (2) a subsetof these for whichthere is a matching postscore, and (3) the (matching)postscores. We shallspeak of prescores, matched prescores, andpostscores. Weshall also speak of adifference score, which is, of course,the excess (posPildynegative) of a postscore over its matched prescore. Weshall consistently use theletters X,X(m),Y, and matched prescores, postscores,and difference scores, D for prescores, A respectively. We shallintroduce a "regresseddifference" score,cc, below. The means of these scoresand their import arereported in the appropriate sections.Possible selection biases arediscussed in sub- section II. F. We shallhere report on some ofthe other parameters for the benefit of readerswho wish to do someanalysis of their own.First, we shallgive two frequencytables (Exhibit B-1).They are of a slightly different set of matchedPPVT scores than the634 we have been and shall be discussing; they arefrom a tape which is one run awayfrom the final tape and theydiffer from it primarilyby being composed of only 621 scores. Weintroduce this further(but relatively mild) noise factor because the frequencycounts on the early tape arethe only ones and we nowhave of matched prescores.Asterisks will denote names and symbols associated withthese scores.(Frequency tables, sums, sums of squaresfor the unstarred scores areavailable, but are not reproduced here.) We shalldenote estimates of parameterscoming from frequency tablesby a subscript f.Estimates of momentsfrom these frequency tables,using midpoints ofthe class intervals, are nonfrequency estimates. smaller thalr g. the * From Exhibit B-1 we cancalculate both anestimate rfof the correlation betweenmatched prescores andpostscores and estimates of the regressionparameters cc and pin the model:

E(Y) = cc + 13X orE(D) = cc + (f3 - 1)X B-1 or80 75-79 70-74 65-69 60-64 55-59 50-54 45-49 40-44 35-39 30-34 25-29 20-24 15-19 10-14 5-9 0-4 Class over Intervals EXHIBITB-1 17 16 44 85 89 106 103 64 38 15 17 Class scores Matched Number 7 6 9 2 1 2 thein Pre- of FREQUENCY 107 107 121 thein 28 7 15 34 60 59 37 23 10 5 4 2 0 2 Number Class Postscores of DISTRIBUTION - - -20 -21 OF 30 25 - 20 - 15 - 10 - 5- 0- 5 --. -10-.- 15 -+ --* or Class PPVT or 29 24 19 14 -11 -16 Intervals Difference more 9 4 1 6 below the SCORES 12 18 31 73 123 230 93 26 Scores Number 5 5 1 4 Class in Difference of Exhibit B-2 displays these estimatesalong with the projection of the least squarei line onto the D axis atthe midpoints of the X class intervals.It indicates that sere was moreimprovement by those whose pretest score:. were low.Under the linear model, the hypothesis that improvement declines with ability isthe hypothesis that(3 < 1.Since the variance of Y is not verymuch smaller than the average sum of the A* squares of the X scoresabout this mean, the standard deviation ofof is on the order of 1/EEF,or about .04.The hypothesis that13 < 1 would be accepted, then, at a lowlevel of significance. Finally, we shall give the estimatesof the standard deviations of the prescores, postscores, anddifference scores.They are calculated from sums and sums of squares ofobservations and not from frequency tables.In order to estimate the standarddeviation of the matched pre- scores, it was necessary to usethe frequency table in Exhibit B-1, in order to avoid underestimating thestandard deviation of the matched scores, we used as ourestimate:

(SDZ +634 urn 2 - S2+ ZriSx,* * iSy,* fur)634 1/2

Exhibit B-3 gives the estimates of thestandard deviations of pre- scores, matched prescores,postscores, and difference scores. We shall later use these estimates as ifthey were known standard deviations and hence label them in Exhibit B-3 asa(X), a(Xm), a(Y), and a (D), respectively. EXHIBIT B-Z ESTIMATES OF CORRELATION(rf) AND REGRESSION A* 11* PARAMETERS («f 'pf ) OF PRE- AND POST-PPVT SCORES

Correlation Regression Parameters * rf= .7896 15.67

.7663

Midpoint of the ",:, Pre* Score Class Ef 1 D 1 15.67-.2337 (Midpoint) Interval

2 15.20 7 14.03 12 12.87 17 11.70 22 10.53 27 9.36 32 8.19 37 7.02 42 5.85 47 4.69 52 3.52 57 2.35 62 1.18 67 .01 72 -1.15 77 -2.32 83 -3.49

EXHIBIT B-3 ESTIMATES OF STANDARDDEVIATIONS OF PPVT SCORES

a(X(m)) a(X) a(Y) a(D) 14.78 14.57 13.82 10.02

B -4 B. The 1-Factor Tables A factor is a possible cause of variation.It appears at levels such as male and female.The 1-factor tables are the result of categorizing PPVT scores by the levels of a single factor.The appropriate level of a factor for a child wastaken from the Medical/Dental and Family Infor- mation Form. Inability to find amedical/dental form with the same child ID number as the PSI form resultedin the loss of some PSI forms. More- over, PPVT scores werelost because questions on a found medical/dental form were unanswered. These losses ofPPVT scores were not severe, especially when compared with thosefrom unreturned PSI forms and re- turned PSI forms with no PPVT score. The analysis of the 1-factor tables isdesigned to compare levels of a factor.This is the only thing to do with the matched prescoresand the postscores.The difference scores can in additionbe compared with zero. We know, however, that difference scoresaveraged across the levels of a factor are positive; the99 percent confidence interval for the meanof the 634 uncategorized differences is 4.20 -6.26.The main question is: do the levels differ among themselves? Thequestion of whether the low levels are yet positive is subsidiary to whetherthere are low levels. We are not, then, primarily testing thepositiveness of differences. The comparison of means is usually doneunder the assumption that the corresponding variances are equal. We assume,in addition, that the common value is theappropriate value in Exhibit B-3--i. e., thatthe score variances are known and equal to 14.78 for prescores,etc.Exhibit B-4 gives estimates for the standard deviationsof X, Y, and D scores for six factor levels chosen randomly.It also gives the natural logarithmof the estimate of the variance which hasapproximately a normal distribution with mean equal to the natural logarithmof the variance and variance equal to twice the reciprocal of thenumber of observations going into 2 Hence, the entries for log S,. . in Exhibit the estimate. e tjj (3) B-3 provide a very rough way ofcomparing the estimates. Foi example, 2 2 one might take asthe standard deviation of the differencelogeS(i)- logeS,..u) 1.27;----1,.andIT/7 . This gives a rough comparability the larger of 11) (J)

B-5 EXHIBIT B- 4 ESTIMATES OFSTANDARD DEVIATIONSOF SOME FACTOR LEVELS

X Y D i

Urban/Suburban (1) S(1) 1328 12.52 10.77 5.173 5.054 4.754 .043 .072 .072

Lives with mother only (2) S(2) 14.89 13.10 10.55 logeS(2)2 5.401 5.144 4.711

4-2 / .--7-12 .085 .129 .072

Lives with father only (3) S(3) 14.03 8.58 6.77 logeS(3)2 5.282 4.299 3.824 .177 .324 .324 7/11.737/ 15.18 13.40 9.94 Mother works (4) S(4) logeS(4)2 5.439 5.191 4.593 11-27Tnm .062 .096 .096

Family income $3,00043.999 (5) S(5) 13.78 13.10 7.81 logeS(5)2 5.246 5.145 4.111 .093 .155 .155 477711(5) Family income $4,000- $4,999 (6) S(6) 14.42 12.73 21.93 2 logeS(6) 5.337 5.088 6.176 .106 .182 .182 J2/m(6)

B-6 indeed, for in addition to approximating the sum of variances, we are ignoring the correlation between estimates across factors.Nonetheless, it is fairly clear that, on the one hand, the variances are not equaleither within or between factors and, on the other, that this matters verylittle. The robustness of our procedures for comparing level meanswill depend largely on the unequal variances within a factor having an average nottoo far from the assumed value of the putative commonvariance.There are no obvious pitfalls pointed outby Exhibit B-4, and it is hard to believe that we are any worse off with an assumed known. variancethan if we estimated the variances anew for each factor in the usual methodof anal- ysis of variance. We shall compare means, then, with a method equivalent in a sense (to be made precise below) to using a chi-square statistic insteadof an F statistic, since we have replaced the usual Fstatistic denominator with a known variance which results in aX2statistic (divided by its de- grees of freedom). Weshall also make another slight modification of the procedure which first tests the hypothesis that thelevel means are equal (usually with an F-test) and, having rejected it, looksfor the ex- ceptional mean differences. We shall instead examinedirectly the con- trasts which might be of interest were they differentfrom zero, and if we find one differentfrom zero we shall know that theX2test would have rejected the hypothesis of equal means.This method, which some readers will by now have recognized as theX2modification of Scheffe's S-method of multiple comparison, allows us to miss contrastswhich may be signifi- cant, but if the investigation of the datathemselves does not suggest these contrasts they are probably difficult to findand interpret even if a signifi- cantX2told us they were there. That the S-method can be adapted from F toX2is apparent from Scheffe's arguments. on page 69 of his book, The Analysisof Variance. For if we define a contrast as something ofthe form:

c Eam. JJ j=1

where the 11.1J .1. are the c level orcell,means and ' ' B -? ..- ..,

OM

i=1 varying al, ... , ac and hencethe corresponding contrastswill span a c-1dimensional linear space.Denote a basis for the spaceof contrasts by

and let

f_=

is-1

estimates of t --i. e., replace g by X.. be the vector of least square 3 is the cell mean.If the X.. are normal with mean p , where X.. J then t hasJ a c-1 variatenormal distribution with mean tand co- variance matrix we shall callEAt.It follows that

A (t .-t)IEC 1(t-t)

has a chi-square distributionwith c-1 degrees of freedom,and that

(1) (t -t)tE"t 1(t-t)sX2 c-1

8-8 ,is the upper cc percent point of theX2 distribution whenX2cc; c-L is a 1-e: confidence ellipse for t .But Equation (1) holds ifand only if

I ht(tt)ls iht(X2 EA)h (2) cc;c -1t

That is, if and only if t isbetween all parallel supportinghyperplanes of the ellipse of Equation(1). We can now choose h to give any con- trastEaJ. 11Jof interest and its estimate,and Equation (2) becomes

It- soc2 Ea2./n.)a2 (3) cc ; C J J

ci2 .For, if variance X.. =nn.

= Varby

= VarlEa.X.J ) J EJa

Thus, Equation (3) holds forall t simultaneously withprobability 1-cc and we have our confidencebounds and a powerful tool forwhat &IL:a:fie calls "data snooping," sincethe number of contrasts we testdoer. not change the overallsignificance level, which we haveset at 5 percent. By choosing the basis tto be the c-1 linearlyindependent con- trasts pi - 42 , - 43 , , 41 -pc , thehypothesis 41 = = is tested by whether or notthe ellipse Equation (1)contains the origin. But for these contrasts,

-.x)2 =E (4) J=1 a In.

B-9 which is the usualX2statistic for testing the equality of means. More- over, the ellipse will containthe origin if and only if all the parallel supporting hyperplanes do, thus the equivalent of theX2test to confi- dence intervals for all contrasts containing zero. 9 Since under the null hypothesis a contrast is zero,the constant subtracted from and added to the estimated contrast toform the confi- dence interval is a bound in the sense that thenull hypothesis is rejected D if the estimated contrast exceeds it in absolute value.We have calculated bounds for all contrasts of certain kinds forcertain scores, and, when D the contrasts have exceeded the bounds, we havestarred (*) them. Be- fore naming the kinds of contrasts, we mustintrociuce another score. G Because the prescore to postscore gains in PPVT scores may vary between levels of a factor primarily becausethe prescores vary, an anal- t D ysis of covariance model might be appropriate(see the discussion in Section IV).The model we have selected is D

El D..I= ex.-.2337 X1.. 13 3 3 D the ith indi- when X..i)is the (now assumed non-random) prescore for vidual at the jth level.-.2337 is, of course, the estimated slope for D the regression of D or X when no factor isconsidered, and here, rather than estimate a common slope for all thelevels of a factor, we Q assume the commonvalue is known and equal to - .2337. By a regressed difference score«jwe mean the estimateD..D. + .2337 X.X..ofex.. If ' 3 D would be that of D.D..,and we use Xii were a constant the variance of i Variance ID. 1 in setting the bounds onna,but we use the smaller var- i i iance, 88.9, about the regression. D The constrasts, the estimates for which we havecalculated bounds, are:(i) all mean level differences p.i - pk, and (ii) alllevel effects D (i) matched .p. ii.-. The scores whose level means the g. represent are: J prescores, (ii) postacores,(iii) difference scores, and (iv) regressed u difference scores. o

B -10

I That is, we have calculated all bounds appropriate to all contrasts of the forms (1) and (ii) for each of the scores (i), (ii), (iii), and (iv). The bounds, in the sense that if the estimated contrast does not exceed them in absolute value and the confidence interval contains zero, are a.r...... , 1 Nke IL.4 17. I (14.57) .05; c-1 1 for matched prescores, etc.Finally, a caution that X.. is the unweighted average of the cell means and not the overall averageX of Equation (4). Thus X.. - X..is the least squares estimate of the level effect. 3

B -11 C. .The 2-Factor Tables Pre- and post-PPVT scores,but-not difference ormatched pre- scores, w.;recategorized by level combinationsof two factors, again by matching PSI forms withmedical/dental forms. The analysis of these tables is a relativelystraightforward extension of the1-factor analysis. Again we are primarilylooking for differences withinlevels of a factor pair and are onlysecondarily concerned with theabsolute size of the smaller or largercells. Let Xii (or Yij. ) bethe cell average for theijth cell, i = 1, 2, , r ;j = 1, 2, ... ,c. Wearediscussing then, a 2-waylayout with one factor at rlevels and another at c .LetN..ii be the number of and let n.. be the numberin Y....N. is, of PPVT scores in X..13. 13 13 13 course, muchlarger than n13 ... We had to form a measureof impact fromprescores and post- scores.The linear model

1= cc.. + (3X.. E 1Y..13. 13 13.

where 13is assumed equal to.7663, the regression coefficientof post- scores onmatched prescores inExhibit B-2, provides a measure cc..ij which leaves something tobe desired as an absolutenumber, but which may form asound basis for comparingthe impact of Head Startcells. For X..includes N.. - n.. scores, manyof which are not likematched 13, 13 13 prescores.They are too high formatched prescores, probablybecause they were obtained toolate in the program. Now,the assumption of a common cellslope in an Analysis ofCovariance is common andworks out well; if, in addition, weassume that the commoncell slope is the overall slope, we havearrived at our p of.7663.If there is a hooker in this, it is hard to see.If, moreover, the differencebetween X.1j. and X.(m)is only that the first is adisplacement of the secondby a ran- 1g . dom amount that isapproximately independent ofthe cell effect, then contrasts among our cciireflect accurately contrasts amongthe inter- cepts of an Analysisof Covariance with postscoresregressed on matched We have estimated the prescores, eventhough our cc..ijare smaller. 0:.. by Y.. = Y..-.7663 X..and looked for cell differences ,in theY. 13 13 13. 13. 13. assuming that Var Y.. = Var Y..=(13.82)213/n... We were forced tothis 13 13. at best labored model by a last-minutebug in a computer program that deprived us of average differences. So much for what was analyzedand why. The method of analysis was chosen, as inthe 1-factor case, to getsimultaneous confidence intervals on cell contrasts.The analyses for X, Y, and Y scoresdiffer only in the standard deviation used.For, again we assume standard de- viations known and equal a (x) to14.78, a(Y) to 13.82 and a(Y) to 13.82. , ,and its variancea2 /nib. The We shall write about its meanF'Lij contrasts (of all means) of interest areprimarily the interactionsPij- - + ,where dot denotes average overthe subscript.Their .3 least squares estimates are

X.. - X .+ X... 13.-X.1.. .3.

µi.- p, and the column effects In addition, the row effects . - are contrastsof the int..,and confidence intervals for them .3 .. are also found andreported in the form of bounds on theabsolute value of the least squares estimates.Note that the estimates X. - X...of, say, a row effectin the 2-way tables differ fromthe estimate of the corresponding level effect in the 1-waytable, because in the 2-way table it is possible to estimate thecontribution to all means made bythe row treatment and distinguish itfrom the contribution made by thecolumn and the cell itself.The row contribution maystill, however, be con- founded with something that a 3-way or moretable would be required to discover. Similarly, X... is anunweighted average of all meansand estimates something differentfrom the weighted average or theunweighted avarage of the 1-waytable. It is possible to get an interaction sumof squares but we do not. It is of no use to us.The confidence limitsassociated with it bound not the interactions but contrastsof interactions. Moreover,the confidence limits for the row and columneffects would have to comefrom another

B-13 simultaneity. Wehave source, thusrequiring somedodge to preserve equivalent chi-square chosen to workdirectly with cellcontrasts. The test statistic is

(x..3r2) E -T----a /nib which has rc -1 degrees offreedom. Finally, to greatlysimplify thecalculations, we haveassumed that replacing

i,iEa2../n..13 13

with

v l..V/..... La a. rc Z. n. i,j. 1j ij.. 1j

level and other preservesapproximately thesimultaneous confidence properties of theintervals.

B -14 D. Statistical AnalysisNORC Data EstimatingWhite-Negro Differences results, In the descriptiontables made fromthe NORC survey between Negro items are starred(*) if there issignificant difference The an- and white responses.The stars werearrived at as follows. nearest one-half waschosen swer forwhich the overallresponse was then grouped as as "head."(All other answersand ronanswers were "tail.") Let pw.A bethe estimate ofthe proportionof whites that were "A the fact the "head" and let pNbe the same forNegroes. Ignoring and dichotimization into"head" and "tail" wasdone using the data, of the ignoring the finitepopulation corrections,and the randomness total numbers ofNegroes and whites,

A A 1 i2[niwpiwqm +18.49n2wp202w1 Var (p....w - D-N- ) = (n + 4.3n2W' 1W (5) 1 /7a + 18.49n2Np = (n2N + 43nm-*[1N1NP(11N 2N -2N]

subscript 2 = the where p = the trueproportion of "head," q =1 - p, the subscript N = Negro, rural midwest,subscript 1 = the restof the country, subscript W =white. We take

n1N =1,129 n1W =479 = 0 n2w =82 n2N

enough todifference We now assumethat Equation (5)is insensitive in strataproportions that we canassume PlW = P2W = PW and PIN =P2N = pN .Then,

A A 10-3n-A -pN) = 2.8850 x10-3pq+ .8857 x -11 Var. (pW WW s 3.7708 x10-3pq

B-15 to one-halfand q = 1 - p where p iswhichever of pwand pN id nearer is boundedby So, thestandard deviationof the difference

alpw-ON)=.061411;TI

2a. The tablebelow lists somevalues of

EXHIBIT B-5VALUES OFa

2ai

.50 .061 .40 .060 .30 .059 .20 .049 .15 .044 .10 .037 .05 .027 .01 .012

value nearerone-half ofPw We thenentered ExhibitB-5 with the A A significant if itexceeded and pNA (or qwA and qN) anddecided pw - pN was conditioned by the answerto a the value inthe rightmostcolumn. Items not tested. previous item("if yes," items) are

B-16 APPENDIX C HEIGHT AND WEIGHT TABLES

Centiles for weight andheight were calculated fromfrequency dis- tributions for males and females, asshown in Exhibits C-1 through C-4. The data were obtainedfrom the 1-percent nationalsample. No systematic attemptto analyze thesetables has been made. A brief comparison of the centilespresented here with those reported by Stuart and Stevenson(Reference 76) was made.While no attempt to interpret the data is made here, one ortwo points are noted that seem curious. Weight centiles for theHead Start children, for example, gen- erally run higher for the age rangeof 3-1/2 to 5, and lower for the range 5-1/2 to 6-1/2 years. Thisrelationship applies both to males andfemales. The differences are notlarge, but they are quite consistent.In the case of height, the Head Start centiles areslightly, but consistantly, higher than the others for both sexes. EXHIBIT C-1 WEIGHTCENTILES - MALES

Contiles(1) Age Range (Years and Months) 3 25 50 75 97 N 3/6-3/11* 31.4 38.8 40.4 45.9 53.2 55 4/0-4/5 30.8 35.0 38.2 42.3 50.6 68 4/6-4/11 33.0 38.1 41.3 44.6 57.6 326 5/0-5/5 35.5 39.9 43.1 46.5 54.2 380 5/6-5/11 36.2 41.6 -44.9 48.4 58.1 716 6/0-6/5 37.8 42.9 46.7 51.3 60.9 679 6/6-6/11 36.8 43.0 47.5 52.1 63.8 197 7/0 and over 35.9 43.0 46.5 52.2 63:7 89

Note: (1) Figures are weightsin pounds. EXHIBIT C-2 WEIGHT CENTILES -FEMALES

Centiles") Age Range (Years and Months) 3 25 50 75 97 N 3/6-3/11 29.2 34.0 36.9 40.3 50.2 45 4/0-4/5 27.9 34.8 37.5 41.5 54.1 48 4/6-4/11 31.4 36.1 39.7 43.5 51.4 311 5/0-5/5 33:0 37.7 41.2 44.9 58.6 384 5/6-5/11 34.0 40.0 43.1 46.5 57.5 702 6/0-6/5 36.0 41.4 45.2 49.6 60.3 619 6/6-6/11 34.7 42.2 45.7 49.7 60.8 179 7/0 and over 34.3 40.1 45.3 51.2 63.3 73

Note: (1) Figures are weights inpounds. EXHIBIT C-3 HEIGHTCENTILES- MALES

Gentiles(') Age Range (Years and Months) 3 25 50 75 97 N 3/6-3/11 34.8 41.8 44.5 46.1 48.1 49 4/0-4/5 38.5 41.1 42.6 44.8 46.1 61 4/6-4/11 40.7 43.0 44.3 45.7 48.6 321 5/0-5/5 41.9 44.0 45.4 47.0 49.4 374 5/6-5/11 42.5 45.3 46.7 48.2 50.9 719 6/0-6/5 43.4 46.2 47.8 49.3 52.3 670 6/6-6/11 41.4 46.8 48.5 49.8 52.9 195 7/0 and over 39.6 46.5 48.1 49.9 54.2 87

Note: (1) Figures are heights in inches. EXHIBIT C-4 HEIGHTCENTILES - FEMALES

Centiles(1) Age Range (Years and Months) 3 25 50 75 97 3/6-3/11 36.6 41.6 43.3 44.6 49.8 46 4/0-4/5 34.7 40.9 42.6 44.1 48.1 48 4/6-4/11 39.9 42.3 43.9 45.4 48.2 306 5/0-5/5 40.8 43.5 44.8 46.1 49.3 382 5/6-5/11 41.5 44.7 46.3 47.7 50.7 691 6/0-6/5 42.3 45.7 47.4 49.0 51.7 615 6/6-6/11 42.5 46.1 48.0 49.2 52.9 181 7/0 and over 42.2 45.4 47.9 50.3 52.5 73

Note: (1) Figures are heights in inches.

C-5 APPENDDC D FURTHER DEFINITIONS OF THE POPULATION SERVED

Consideration can be given to the proposition that the family income level used to identify poverty depends on at least two factors: (1)the num- ber of persons in the household, and (2) whether the family is"farm" or "nonfarm." These factors, shown in the table below, have been taken into account in granting funds for 1966 Head Start projects.OEO hopes that 90 percent of the families to be served by Head Start will have incomes below these levels.

Family Income Level by Household Size Nonfarm Households Farm Households Persons Family Income Persons Family Income

1 $1,500 1 $1,050 2 $2,000 2 $1,400 3 $2,500 3 $1,750 4 $3,000 4 $2,100 5 $3,500 5 $2,450 6 $4,000 6 $2,800 7 $4,500 7 $3,150 Above 7 $5,000 Above 7 $3,500

If we apply the nonfarm scale to Head Start1965,1we get a clearer picture of Head Start's success in reaching the target population.Exhibit D-1 compares the Head Start Families (using the scale above)with the low income families throughout thecountry.2The 1-percent :ample

1Data is not analyzed for nonfarm and farm households. Seven percent of the nation's families are "farm"(1963 Census Astimates). 2This scale is slightly different from that now used byOEO. See foot, note (6) to Exhibit D-1 for explanation.

D- EXHIBIT D-1 LOW INCOME FAMILIES IN THE UNITEDU.S. STATES Population(') Poverty Head Start(2) Poverty Total Families 47,440,000 Total (%) 7,210,000 Poor ( 3 )The (%) (ika)0) 15 5,036(5) Total Poor(6)The2,462 Rate(%)47 ti Number of Persons in Family 423 202132 131434 101116 (%) 13 51 (%) 9 1 473548 e, N/A675 or more 13 - 77 1713 - 9 351915 - 421617 6 541715 634152 Notes: (3)(2)(1)- BasedThe Based SSA 'upon upon (Social _Bureau 1963 Security estimates,of Orshanaky,Poverty Profile, loc. cit. Social Family Security income Census 1-percent Head Start Administration)sample. pOverty index-economyOrshansky, Mollie, Counting the Administration, 1965. level by household size. Those Poor: Another Look at the level (nonfarm). From coming below this percentamount lessare consnet cash eyed than impoverished. Family size: 2 45 persons 3persons persona persons a nonfarm family of the It is assumed $3,700$3,100$2,400$2,000that same size and composition. a farm family would need 40 IMO 32119 L !vizi rioa figatvzli 1222,3 IC I -s.14 itos-ine CSC:;... i ammo saga EMI a I M SHWA EXHIBIT D-1 (Continued)Family size: 6 persona 7 or more persons $4,100$5,100 (4)(6)(5) Poverty OEO26 percent poverty rate of indexthe theindex.One OEO other index, problem In 1963, it was (To) = Impoverished 34.5 million5,036: not answeredis(presented orcomparisons: and discussed estimated that the SSA (Orshaneky, loc. cit). families/total families. the OE0 index unknown. in text) differsindex included identified poverty levels slightly from34.6 the millionSSA as their income in in $500impoverished; in- poverty wasadjustmentmidpoint.$1,000crements.line).$2,000 arbitrarily classes and wasFor $2,900, (tonecessary,example,Head the it was assumed that the median Start,$6,000 it was level;only necessary thereafter, for theassumed 20.8 percent that 10.4therefore, percent to apply the income of families of the families ofthe classes were thatOEO families index indicate to theearned Head less than in each $1,000 class was three earning between$2,000 intervals). Some $2,500 (the poverty Start data. the It indicates that 47 percent of the Head Start families had incomes below the poverty line (compared with 38 percent when$3,000 was used as the line). Over 70 percent of the families so identified had six or more per- sons in the household(see subsection II. C. 2, Exhibit 11-23).This is to be compared with the total number of low income families in the country, where only 26 percent have six or more persons in thefamily.' While Head Start did not reach its goal of 85 percent participation of "poor" families, there was a concentration on the economically de- prived. The nation's impoverished families account for 15 percent of the national total; Head Start's low income participants constituted at least 47 percent of the total families served. There was substantial success in reaching asizable portion of the target population.

'One-third of the nation's impoverished families have no children. Of the two-thirds with children, only 12 percentlave six or more.

D -4 ft, vow..

APPENDIX E IQ SCORES OF CULTURALLY DISADVANTAGED CHILDREN WHO WERE TESTED ON MORE THAN ONE INTELLIGENCE TEST

Five investigators had research designs which called for their sub- jects to be tested with more than one measure of intelligence.For the convenience of the reader, Exhibit E-1 summarizes the intelligence test results in terms of IQ ecoreo.1 Results are presented for all groups who were Head Start or non-Head Start,but comparable (i. e., all groups who were assumed to be more or lessdisadvantaged), and on whom more than one IQ score was obtained.

1Because of the wide variations in ages, only results which were con.; verted from raw scores to IQ scores were used, since the IQ score 'is derived from chronological age.

E -1 EXHIBIT E-1IQ SCORES OFCULTURALLY DISADVANTAGED CHILDREN TEU ED ONMORE THAN ONE INSTRUMENTS11

Investigator N PPVT DAP CMMS S- B Leiter Berger 59 83 92 84 Eisenberg 500 68(2) 76 Eisenberg 23 65.5 83.9 91.3 Eisenberg 48 70.5 85.9 Eisenberg 48 76.7 94.8 Eisenberg 34 69.1 84.8 84.2 Eisenberg 34 72,8 88.2 90.2 Johnson 79 72 92. Johnson 79 79 111 Johnson 62 55 91 Johnson 17 91 92 Ozer 65 74 88.78 Jacobs and 56 71.5 85.27 Shafer

Notes: (1)These scores are notorganized in relation to pre-, p_ost-, age, or othervariables. (2) N = 424, fromsample of 500 whoreceived DAP. APPENDIX F ANALYSIS OF CDCDIFFERENCES

A question basic towhether different programs producedifferent results is whetherdifferent centers show differentresults. An attempt to decide whethercenters varied for reasonsother than chance was made but failed.The failure is, however,interesting for two reasons. First, although differencesin improvement means are tenuous,differences in improvement variability aremarked and striking.Second, there is sig- nificant correlationbetween the size of meanimprovement and its vari- ance, whichhas strong implicationsfor the design needed to approach the evaluation of centerdifferences. We chose 15 centers atrandom from the list of102 centers that provided one .or more ofthe 134 matched pre- and post-PPVT scores. One of the 15 was thendropped because 17 of the18 D-scores it reported were zero(the other was one.Exhibit F-1 summarizes theresults. The natural logarithm ofthe estimate of the centervariance has approx- imately a normal distributionwith variance 2/(n. - 1),where nj is the number of D- scores fromthe jth center. So, forthe center of the table this standard deviation (ofthe variance estimates)is between .408 and one.The hypothesis that thevariances of D-scores arethe same from center to center isclearly untenable. The F ratio is 1.08, but evenignoring the unequal variances, there is some difficulty ininterpreting its meaning, since wehave a situation sometimes differentiated fromthe usual analysis ofvariance situation and called Model II.The alternative to the nullhypothesis for an analy- sis of variance by ranksis, however, that atbest these centers differ. That hypothesis is of someinterest. The value ofthe chi-square statis- tic for the rank analysisof variance is 20.97 on13 degrees of freedom, which is significant at the7.3-percent level (the meanranks are givenin Exhibit F-1). The analysisof average ranks,then, is m. best marginally conclusive.

F-1 EXHIBIT F-1 MEANS AND VARIANCES OF PPVT DIFFERENCES FROM 14 CENTERS Center from CentersNumberD-Scores of D-ScoreAverage Variance Varianceloge of Rank ofMean 1 VarianceRank of 1 Means of ItAlaik 138 013 41 -0.25 0.00 0.25 .. OD -1.39 OP MD MD - 15.0012.88 575 001 MD 501152091590 002 001 001750 001 7641 .4.674.252.001.86 35.3814.258.80 .... 2.173.572.65-- 243 253 - 24.4328.535.7533.0 19231212 001002985 017001 12 348 7.006.337.888.75 163.51127.27104.36 28.25 4.844.645.103.34 7658 10 748 36.0033.8347.6236.88 508020902490 006 019 003831 019 13 654 21.6714.259.409.77 679.80503.78146.2574.18 6.524.316.224.98 101211 9 1211 69 53.9055.1247.3157.42 The situation revealed here is one inwhich, as more centers are considered, the spread of the means,if it increases at all, increases with, but more slowly than, thespread of thevariance.'The efficient solution to this problem is to get more scores percenter rather than con- sidering more centers.

The correlationbetWeen ranks of means and variances is .752, which is significant at a level smallerthan .01.

F-3 _

APPENDIX G WORKER EVALUATION RESPONSES

The exhibit in this appendix gives the frequencies of responses of paid and volunteer workers from the approximately 1-percent sample CDC's to each item on the Paid and Voluntary Workers' Evaluation Form (see Appendix A for a sample of the form). Thus, particular hypotheses about the opinions of different types of workers may be checked by means of this data. The items are presented in order in the exhibit. For each item, a row identifies the type of worker; acolumn identifies a response cate- gory; and the overallX2value is given. AllX2'sare significant at the 5-percent level for the appropriate degrees of freedom except those for Items 22 and 23. It should be noted that the response categories change from one group of items to another and that,for some items, categories have been combined in order that sufficient cell sizes of expected frequencies may be achieved. The categories of workers represented in each row, from top to bottom, are: a. Professional Paid and Volunteer b. Neighborhood Paid c. Neighborhood Volunteer d. Other Paid and Volunteer

G-1 EXHIBIT G-1 ITEM RESPONSEFREQUENCIES

Item very Good Good Fair Poor Can't Evaluate 40(1) - 35 16 3 4 16 = 4

113(1) - 22 93 - 21 24 - 24 37 -. .9

272 83 24 179 41 35 39 6 23 49 12 9

54(1) 30 72 59 27 34 30 29

314 59 34 233 28 41 93 9 45 78 31 30

208 83 156 17 53 6 2 131 56 26 X2= 79.903 7. a.566 690 326 105 21 b. 649 567 189 28 32 c. 297 219 61 3 10 d. 125 149 64 14 28 x2 = 192.342 8. a..665 730 226 62 19 b. 644 61'7 151 19 29 c. 262 253 53 6 13 d. 133 159 53 4 30 x2= 114.44 Note: (1) Includes both "lair" and"poor." G-2 EXHIBIT G-1(Continued)

Item Very G ood Good Fair Poor Can't Evaluate 9. a.613 725 247 92 11 b.560 666 157 26 25 c. 253 251 54 12 11 d. 133 145 55 25 16 X2 = 94.029

6 - 24 x2= 67.685 11. a. 331 713 284 96 194 b. 301 590 136 15 321 c. 109 215 56 20 153 d. 52 118 42 23 114 x2= 208.202 12. a. 854 456 144 66 87 b.698 468 99 18 68 c. 280 174 45 14 35 d. 161 102 30 11 41 X2 = 57.767 13. a.742 551 182 54 73 b.642 502 106 22 67 c. 268 180 55 10 35 d. 159 95 31 5 55 X2 = 95.102 14. a. 258 452 273 174 421 b. 291 466 189 75 297 c. 121 179 83 31 131 d. 54 94 43 28 123 X2= 85.990 15. a. 157 346 234 291 525 b. 166 327 172 129 498 c. 72 131 93 39 204 d. 33 68 39 44 155 X2 = 89.591 16. a. 324 501 258 137 314 b.290. 396 162 77 349 c. 105 142 84 29 168 d. 65 90 50 12 121 X2 = 72.717 17. a. 317 612 286 137 172 b. 285 529 197 59 200 c. 127 164 88 29 116 d. 52' 112 50 29 92 x2= 110.307 Note: (1) Includes both "fair" and "poor." G-3 EXHIBIT G-1(Continued)

Very Much Occasionally Worthwhile Not ItemWorthwhile Worthwhile and Waste of Time Applicable a. 1,180 260 44 40 b. 1,025 217 14 14 c. 375 98 47 4 d. 246 65 5 19 x2=118.690 a. 1,055 327 58 84 b. 943 259 21 47 (2) c. 344 122 48 10 d. 238 69 6 22 x2 = 83.468 a. 1,282 214 18 10 b. 955 273 35 7 (3) c. 360 113 46 5 d. 252 66 7 10 x2=138.617 a. 1,225 280 14 5 b. 817 416 32 5 (4) c. 300 178 42 4 d. 213 97 8 17 Xx2 = 265 234 a. 1,292 220 9 3 b. 936 304 23 7 (5) c. 377 102 41 4 d. 247 64 8 .16 x2= 214.108 a. 1,094 349 40 41 b. 803 341 72 54 (6) c. 306 153 50 15 d. 206 91 16 22 x2= 77.775 a. 1,287 229 6 2 b. 930 292 38 10 (7) c. 344 130 44 6 d. 238 74 7 16 x2= 216.764 a.'1,306 206 7 5 b. 984 262 18 6 (8) c.371 115 34 4 d. 270 56 1 8 x2.= 150.943

G-4 EXI-TiBIT G-1(Continued)

No Change, Worse, Item Much Better Better Much Worse 19. a. 1,112 406 6 b. 879 381 10 (1) c. 322 195 7 d. 193 139 3 x2= 47.747 a. 873 641 10 b. 560 690 20 (2) c. 244 273 7 d. 155 171 9 x261.679 a. 728 756 40 b. 570 648 52 (3) c. 210 282 32 d. 134 177 24 x2= 30.047 a. 762 731 31 b. 569 667 34 (4) c. 212 288 24 d. 137 178 20 x2= 35.824 a. 548 939 37 b. 366 830 74 (5) c. 179 308 37 d. 98 213 24 x2= 46.081 a. 726 776 22 b. 511 721 38 (6) c. 222 275 27 d. 129 193 13 x2= 39.087 a. 1,023 485 16 b. 767 477 26 (7) c.,.317 198 9 d. 190 130 15 X2 = 35.737 a. 688 814 22 b. 511 715 44 (8) c. 216 293 15 d. 145 180 10 x2= 17.641 EXHIBIT G-1(Continued)

No Change, Worse, Item Much Better Better Much Worse a. 1,018 492 14 b. 818 435 17 (9) c. 309 203 12 d. 208 122 5 x2= 14.994 a. 616 809 99 b. 530 665 75 c. 216 278 30 d. 124 174 37 x2= 13.227 a. 402 745 377 b. 373 631 266 (.2) c. 175 251 98 d. 89 146 100 x2= 25.086 a. 376 815 333 b. 336 634 300 (3) c. 157 268 99 d. 97 152 86 x2= 14.766 ti a. 362 940 222 386 719 165 (4) b. c. ,171 292 61 d. 100 166 69 x2= 39.024 a. 312 944 268 b. 304 777 189 (5) c. 160 292 72 d. 82 179 74 x2= 34.343 a. 369 926 229 332 746 192 (6) b. c. 167 277 80 d. 101 172 62 x2= 19.811 EXHIBIT G-1(Continued)

Item Much More More A Little. Not At All

21. a. 1,082 360 40 11 b. 855 292 62 17 (1) c. 326 145 32 6 d. 197 82 24 7 x2= 38.485 a. 1,065 385 32 8 (2) b. 796 343 69 9 c. 313 161 30 3 d. 198 98 8 6 x2= 48.712 a. 908 484 71 13 b. 634 449 100 14 (3) c. 229 205 54 7 d. 156 109 31 10 x2= 66.663 a. 760 591 90 21 26 (4) b. 529 464 159 c. 200 193 77 17 d. 132 123 36 11 x2= 73.484 22. a. 1,377 71 4 1 b. 1,116 51 6 0 c. 453 .27 3 d. 278 20 1 1 x2= 8.668 23. a. 1,266 146 22 12 b. 1,038 97 18 13 c. 413 53 9 "6 d. 250 38 8 4 x2= 10.321

G-7 APPENDIX H ADDITIONAL HEAD STARTSTUDIES

1966 are included here. Reports of studiesreceived after 15 March ra These studies fall intothree broad categories:follow-up studies of Head Start children intokindergarten or firstgrade, a test-reliabilitystudy, and studies of language'development and/or readingreadiness. A. Follow-Up Studies On 10 March 1966, a letter asking for reports onfollow-up eval- uations of participating children was sentfrom the Head Start Research and Evaluation Division, headed by Dr.Edmund G. Gordon, to a number of agencies which had sponsoredHead Start programs during the summer of 1965. Of the 18 respondents, 7 indicatedthat no follow-up program had been undertaken, and 11 indicated that avariety of kinds of follow-up activities had been performed. Follow-uptesting in cognitive areas (intelligence testing andacadeMic testing) was reported by film agencies; follow-up medical and dental examinationsand treatments were specified by six agencies; social/emotionalratings and evaluations were included in follow-up work by six agencies;and extended parent programs were mentioned by three' agencies. Typical examples of specific follow-up measuresincluded: Purchasing clothes for all children who neededthem to start the fall school year(Reference 117). Providing at least one balanced meal perday for each child (Reference 117). Continuing parents' programs(Reference 117). Making home visits to obtain parents'reactions to children's progress in school(References 118, 120). Using behavioral checklists forevaluation, including specific capabilities such as shoe-tieing,balancing, etc. (References 118,119).

H-1 Administering psychological tests to children who had been referred by teachers for special study (Reference 119). Contracting with psychiatrists to help children who need fur- ther assistance in adjustment to school life (Reference 119). Establishing cumulative folders to include Head Start in- formation (Reference 120). Continuing teacher in-service training in art and music (Reference 120). Administering Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests as part of a 1966 year-round Head Start program (Reference 120). While the respondents usually did not include actual data concern- ing the measurement made on the children, one respondent from a Texas school system (Reference 121) specified that, of the 54 children in the summer1965 Head Startprogram, 45 are enrolled locally in the first grade. Of these children, 50 percent are making norma.lprogress with 25 percent. considered "excellent ".; 25 percent are not doing as well as average. The report states: This would have little significance were it not understood that 98 percent of our Latin Americans repeat the first grade.It appears that 30 of the pupils have gained a full year by having 8 weeks of Head Start. Only four were listed as falling behind. Attendance has been very good, with one exception. The number of absences has been an average of 6 days out of 88. In addition to the above information on follow-up studies, the final report of Van Egmond's 0E0-funded independent research study was re- ceived (Reference 105). Much of the substance of his finalreport was pre- sented in his earlier progress report and has been discussed in the first volume of this report (pages IV-110 to IV-113). Van Egmond's final report describes in considerable detail the use of a "Collaboration Scale for the Analysis of Classroom Teaching Be- havior" to obtain descriptions of modes of teaching in eight Head Start classrooms during the summer 1965 program. The "Collaboration Scale" provides for observation of teaching tasks within a framework of three general and seven secondary categories, as follows.

H-2 1. Working on content or task a. Providing focus b. Development of focus c. Providing information d. Appraising effort 2. Maintaining social order a. Setting expectations b. Implementing action c. Appraising effort 3. Facilitating The investigators found a wide range of teaching styles and learn- ing opportunities in the eight classrooms, and felt that the "Collabora- tion Scale" could provide information to help identify teaching styles and learning opportunities for children. Further new information in Van Egmond's final report concerned performance measures of reading readiness for Head Start and non- Head Start children, all of whom were in the same kindergarten classes. Arrangements had been made to use the American School Reading Read- iness Tests Revised Edition, Form Xt but many children could not pro- duce scorable results.Thus, the test was rejected and the Metropoli- tan Readiness Test, Form S was used. The sample tested was composed of 144 children and included 35 Head Start girls, 36 Head Start boys, 41 non-Head Start girls, and 32 non-Head Start boys. A distinct trend was reported toward higher readiness scores in the non-Head Start children, compared to Head Start children of the same sex. However, none of the differences reported wasstatistically significant as determined by the t test.Exhibit H-1 summarizes the test results. According to Van Egmond's report, the clearest indications of dif- ferential effects of the Head Start program were seen in the interview responses of kindergarten teachers. Although they tended to seethe activity and exploratory behaviors of the Head Start children as class- room management problems, they found that afterseveral weeks of school, the Head Start children had learned to accommodate their be- havior to the teachers' expectations. H-3 EXHIBIT H-I MEAN TOTALREADING READINESSSCORES (VAN EGMOND)

Girls Boys Head StartNon-Head StartHead StartNon-Head Start 32 N 35 41 36 Mean Score 37.14 46.00 36.47 45.41 One of Van Egmond's major recommendations (in the final report) concerned the relationship between Head Start and the kindergarten pro- gram of the receiving public school system: There is a clear need for establishing a meaning- ful relationship between the Head Start program and the kindergarten program of the receiving public school system. Regardless of the quality of the summer experience for children, a nega- tive or unsympathetic orientation, or sheer lack of information on the part of the teacher who re- ceives the Head Start child will not provide an optimal opportunity for the continued growth of the learner.This could he accomplished by in- viting Head Start teachers and staff members to visit kindergarten classrooms in the neighbor- hood schools and encouraging the kindergarten teachers to visit Head Start classrooms during the summer. A definite need is indicated for providing an orientation for the receiving teacher regarding the program goals and activities of the Head Start centers. Information regarding the needs, experience, and performance of children as perceived by Head Start personnel should also be shared with the receiving teacher to aid in planning for the further development of the child.If the goals of the Head Start pro- gram are to be extended and given further de- velopment by the public and private school, then improved articulation between the two pro- grams is essential.(Reference 105) B. Draw-A-Person Test Reliability Study One of the independent research studies funded by 0E0 was di- rected by Dr. Dale B. Harris of Pennsylvania State University (Reference 49) and had as its main purpose the scoring of Goodenough Draw-A-Man pictures to atain inter-scorer score reliabilities.The calculations from which the reliability correlations were obtained were based upon independent rescorings of 10 percent of the drawings.1

1The number of drawings rescored wasnot reported.

H-5 Inter-Score Reliability Aloof Child Male Drawing FemaleDrawing 4 years, 6months to 4 years, 11months .949 .962 5 years to 5 years,5 months .959 .931 months to 5 years, 11months .956 .960 5 years, 6 .946 .940 6 years and over .962 Mixed .934 C. Language/Reading Readiness Studies Four of the reportsreceived in time for inclusion in thisdocument were concernedprimarily with studies of languagedevelopment or read- ing readiness. Dr. SolGordon's report on reading readiness programs in Mississippi is discussedfirst (Reference 42). Next, Dr. Jane Beasley Raph's language research reportis discussed (Reference 92). Attention is then given to the observations,findings, and recommendations of Mr. Melvyn Resnick, a linguist andHead Start consultant who observed nine Head Start centers containing alarge proportion of non-English speaking children (Reference 122). Finally, someof the findings of Dr. DonaldReiff (University of Rochester), who wascontracted by 0E0 to prepare an in- depth review and analysis ofall Head Start studies which dealt withlan- guage development, arepresented (Reference 123). Gordon's report on the effectiveness offive different reading readiness programs instituted inIssaquena and Sharkey Counties, Mis- sissippi provided thought-provokinginformation not so much on the re- search problems, as on thespecial problems encountered inestablish- ing a Head Start program in aparticular region of the country --the deep South; In regard to the study itself,189 children were pre-tested onthe Wide Range Reading ReadinessAchievement, the Draw-A-Man, andthe Write Own Name tests.Posttest data were obtained on150 children 7 weeks later. Actual test scores werenot yet available. The five reading readiness programsunder investigation are shown in Exhibit H-Z.

unreported 1Includes childrenunder 4 years, 5months and children with ages. . H-6 EXHIBIT H-2 EXPERIMENTAL PROGRAMS(S. GORDON)

No. of Children Program (Materials /Method) Using Program Allyn and Bacon Reading Readiness 20 Sylvia Ashton Warner 8 Doman and Delacato 20 Allen Linguistic Reading Method 20 Learn A Thon (Golden Books) 20 Although data were notreported, as mentionedabove, the inves- profit tigator commentedthat the childrenappeared, in general, to most from theAllyn and Baconmethod, which emphasized atraditional approach to readingreadiness. One interesting aspectof the study was the useof record players in the homes ofthe children. Aninexpensive record player was pur- Each family chased and placedin the home ofeach participating child. received threedifferent educationalrecords each week, and was requested to playeach record at least once aday. Discussions with data were parents indicatedthat they compliedwith the request, but not reported asto the effects ofthis in-the-homeeducational method. Head Start The manypolitical pressuresbrought to bear on the staff and participants werediscussed in the report,including barrage- ments by the KuKlux Klan, job lossby parents ofparticipating children, and the burning downof Head Startfacilities. Raph's report(Reference 92),"Development of aMethodology Verbalizations Used byPre- for Obtaining andAnalyzing Spontaneous A Pilot Study" Kindergarten Childrenin Selected HeadStart Programs: of discussed the useof tapedtranscriptions andnarrative descriptions chil- language samples toobtain spontaneouslanguage data from young dren. In addition, astudy was made oftwo approachesfor setting up samples: (1) simple,struc- astandard-stimulus forobtaining language It was not tured devices, and(2) semi-controlledfree-play situations. possible, at theconclusion of the study,to make aclear-cut recommen- effectiveness of dation for eitherapproach, since theresults (i.e., the children, the researchersin- the approaches)varied with the individual volved, and certaintemporal factors. Because of the manyproblems encounteredin typing thelanguage for language samples from thetaped transcriptions,the methodologies investigators sub- analysis had not yetbeen completed;however, the might be consid- mitted somepreliminary suggestionsof factors which included: length ered in attempts toanalyze languagesamples. These speech to total output, of sentences,verb tense,proportion of parts of

1i-8 variety of vocabulary, conceptformation, cognitive development,inter- nal mediation, .purposeof the verbal interaction, particularconcepts, use ofcomparatives and superlatives,functional uses of statements, nature of interactions,grammatical errors, articulationomissions, distortions, and substitutions.Each of the foregoing factors was men- tioned in terms of thepossible indications or clues theymight provide concerning differentiallanguage development among children. Resnick (Reference 122)observed nine Head Start classes which were composed of children,teachers, and aides with varyinglingual back- grounds. In some classesthere were aides who spoke only Spanish to the Spanish-speakingchildren, while in others the speakingof English only was strongly encouraged. He found that manyteachers were "hampered by the lackof reli- able evaluating proceduresthat would tell them just howmuch English a child knew,in both productive and receptiveaspects." After his ob- servations of and consultationswith the staffs at the various centers facing the problems of teachingEnglish, Resnick recommendedthat native English-speaking and non-Englishspeaking children should beplaced in the same classes, with a strongpredominance of English-speakingchil- dren to serve as languagemodels. In his summary andrecommendations, Resnick noted hisimpres- sion that: The non-English speakingchildren who learned the most English in theeight weeks were those whose teachers and staff spoke tothem in Eng- lish. A rather obvious point,but since so many of the teachers, aides, and volunteersspoke the children's native language to them,it bears watching and correction in futureHead Start programs. Children who are largely self -sufficient, intro-. verted perhaps... will require atype of program and attention that the moreaggressive Spanish- speaking children do not need,if they are to learn English as rapidly asother children. With such children a specially trainedstaff is prob- ably necessary. The staff'sability and willing- ness (and willpower) to speak English toall the children iscrucial. H -9 After reviewing all Head Start studies which concerned language development, Reiff (Reference 123) stated in his report: "It is only one in- dication of the complexity and consequent difficulty of such assessment, and of the magnitude of the failure of attempts to do such assessment, that not one shred of systematically gathered, linguistically interest- ing data is available in any of the 1965 research project reports." Reiff suggested that Caldwell's Preschool Inventory could be used to obtain information about the children's receptive and expressive ver- bal reservoirs, and that the Inventory might be meaningfully scored in such a way as to extract this linguistic information. Although some investigators had seen the Peabody Picture Vocab- ulary Test as a possible measure of verbal ability, Reiff felt that, be- cause of the problems of intelligibility across dialects: "the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, as a test of receptive language ability, is... less than worthless." Reiff indicated that, at the conclusion of various language studies, there was a heightened awareness on the part of the various investigators as to the difficulties inherent in gathering data on children's language. His report concluded with a number of suggestions for improving fur- ther research efforts in the language development of young children. The recommendations concerned data-gathering techniques ("systematic, exhauative observation techniques," though not developed, are needed and recommended by the author); listings of populations from which data should be gathered for comparative purposes; the identification of expert teachers for use as models; and the development of regional centers for providing language training facilities and consultants to teachers. APPENDIX I

THE PRESCHOOL INVENTORY ANDBEHAVIOR INVENTORY

In this appendix, we shall sumMarizethe results cf analyses of differences and regressed differences for thePreschoca Inventory (PSI) and Behavior Inventory (BI) matched scoresfrom the 1- percent sample. The statistical analyses and tests madefor each test were the same as those made for the PPVT (seeAppendix B) and will not be described again here. However, the scoring systemused for each test (and their subtests) will be described in some detail. Weshall also present the results of an analysis of intertestrelationships. A. The Preschool Inventory (PSI) The original version of the PSI--theversion used in the Summer 1965 Head Start program--consisted of161 items constructed by Dr. Bettye M. Caldwell (Reference13). A number of factors influenced the construction of the test, but basically,"It was decided to concentrate on specific achievements representing whatthe child brought with him to the educational experience rather than canbroad areas of cognitive function- ing that might predict how well he woulddo in thefuture."'Thus, the original intent was to make the Inventory moreof an educational (achieve- ment) test than a psychological (ability) one.In the author's words, should be "interpreted as a measure ofperformance rather than potential."1 A copy of the test is presented inAppendix A of this volume. The scoring weight for each item, provided byCaldwell, is listed in Exhibit I-1. In the scoring of tests in the 1-percentsample, Item 3 (the Draw-A-Man/ Woman item) was omitted; thus,the total possible score for the PSI in this analysis is 293. Exhibit I-1 alio shows the groupings ofitems according to intended subtests.(The subtest to which a given item belongsis identified by a

1Loc.cit., page 3.

I- 1 EXHIBIT I-1PRESCHOOL INVENTORY SUB TESTS AND ITEMSCORES

Factor Item No. Subtest Response Score Category

1-2 1 yes 2 III no 0 3 5 human figure drawings N/A 4.1

4-7 1 yes 2 4.1 no 0

9-10 1 3 or more names 2 I or 2 names none 0

11-20 1 gives name 2 Ill shows 1 wrong or does notknow 0 21-35 2 right IA wrong 0 36 2 counts to 4 or 5 2 IA counts to 2 or 3 1 counts to 1 or less 0 37 shows correctly 1 does not allow 0 38 2 knows 1 IA does not know 0 39-42 2 right 1 IA wrong 0 43-47 2 right 2 IA wrong 0 48-51 right 1 IV wrong 0 52-55 1 gives name 2 IB points 1 wrong ordoes not know 0 gives similar object 0 56 5 correct reproduction ofline 2 57 5 correct reproduction ofcircle 4 58 5 correct reproduction ofsquare 6 59 .5 correct reproduction oftriangle 8 60-90 3 right 1 III, IB wrong 0 EXHIBIT I-1(Continued)

Factor Item No. Subtest Response Score Category

91 1 10 or more responses 4 IB 7 to 9 responses 3 4 to 6 responses 2 1 to 3 responses 1 C responses 0 92-99 3 names right 2 IB gives right 1 gives wrong 0 names wrong 0 100-105 3 says right 2 IB points right 1 points wrong 0 says wrong 0 106-110 3 says right 2 w shows right 1 says or shows wrong 0 111-123 4 2 2 N 1 1 wrong or does notknow 0 124-134 6 each part of the response thatis 1 III correct; i. e. , score peritem may range from 0 to4 135-142 4 function 2 N association 1 wrong or does notknow 0 143-148 5 yes 2 11= no 0 149-161 7 yes 3 MI no 0

Subtest Name Total Points Vocabulary 49 -Basic Information and 36 -Number Concepts andOrdination -Concepts I:Size, Shape, Motion, andColor 57 Concepts II: Time,Object Class, and Social 42 - 32 -Visual Motor 38 -Following Instructions -Independence and Self-Help 2-3939

I- 3 EXHIBIT I-1 (Continued)

Factor Category No. of Items Factor IA - ConceptActivation, Numerical 19 Factor IB - ConceptActivation, Sensory 19 Factor III - Personal-Social Responsiveness 26 Factor IV - AssociativeVocabulary 21 Total 85

1-4 letter on the left-hand side of the exhibit. ) Asthe.PSI was originally constructed and, scored by Caldwell, there were sevensubtests, the names of which arelisted at the end of the exhibit. Caldwell and Soule (Reference 13) analyzed a sample of pretest scores. Exhibit 1-2 presentsthe means and standard deviations of the standardization sample reported by the authors, and Exhibit 1-3 gives the subtest/total test correlations which theyobtained. On the basis of (1) these score correlations, (2) afactor analysis of items, and (3) an item difficulty analysis, Caldwell selected a set of 85items represent- ing four factors and a range of itemdifficulties for each factor group.1 This set of items constitutes a revised Preschool Inventory, tobe used research and evaluation of the 1966 Head Start programs. Wehave identified the sets of items representing thehighest factor loadings of a given factor in Exhibit I-1 with Roman numerals onthe right-hand side of the exhibit. Not all of the items in some rows arenecessarily used in a factor group. However, the identification servesin a general way to indicate groupiigs of items in therevised PSI.The scoring weights of items on the revised PSI are, with a few exceptions,1 for right and .; . 0 for wrong. analyses' Of thei3Si. We have, as stated earlier, made the same 11. as were performedwith the PPVT; that is, we have examined both gain (D) scores (absolute differences), and adjusted(aA) mean scores, or what we have called regresseddifferences (see Appendix B). For each set of PSI scores, we have made analyses based on varioussocio-economic factors, which constitute a subset of those usedfor the analysis of PPVT scores. Exhibit 1-4 shows the results obtained forthe zero-factor distri- butions of matched (pre- and post-) total andsubtest scores for the orig- inal PSI.The first column on the left identifiesthe name of each sub- test (the top row refers to total testscores). The next column gives the number of subjects (N) in the1-percent sample for whom matched

1There were some modifications of theitems themselves.

1-5 SAMPLE OF EXHIBIT 1-2 RESULTSOFPRESCH0014,INVENTORY - HEAD STARTCHILDREN L/(CALDWELL)

The 161 test items \v.:meintended to measure a child'sperformance in the following areas:basic information andvocabulary; number con- cepts and ordination;concepts of size, shape,motion, and color (I); concepts of time,object class, and socialfunction (U); visual-motor performance; the followingof instructions; andindependence and self- help. Means andstandard deviations for thepreliminary standardiza- Start children were: tion sample of Head Standard Area N Mean Deviations Information and vocabulary 387. 35.57 7.61 Numerical relations 389 23.78 6.85 Concepts I 389 40.51 11.25 Concepts II 374 27.29 7.94 10.80 Visual-motor 389 31.28 Following of 5.70 instructions 389 30.38 Independence and self-help 389 31.30 7.46 44.68 Total 372 219.65 Note: (1) FromReference 13. EXHIBIT 1-3INTERCORRELATIONS AMONGSUBTESTS OF THE PRESCHOOLINVENTORY(1) (CALDWELL)

(N for correlations rangesfrom 171 to 302)

COUNT CONC 1 CONC 2VISMO FOLIN INDEP TOTAL

VOCAB(2) .68 .75 .69 .54 .56 .35 .86 COUNT .78 .65 .58 .56 .40 .87 CONC 1 .73 .66 .57 .36 .93 CONC 2 .46 .50 .33 .84

VISMO .40 .40 .72 .26 .68 .44

Notes: (1) From Reference 13. (2) VOCAB is subtest "BasicInformation and Vocabulary." COUNT is subtest"Number Concepts and Ordination," CONC 1 is subtest "ConceptsI." CONC 2 is subtest "ConceptsIX." VISMO is subtest"Visual-Motor Performance." FOLIN is subtest "FollowingInstructions." INDEP is subtest"Independence and Self-Help."

I-7 EXHIBIT 1-4 COMPARISON OF PRESCHOOL INVENTORY (PSI) SCORES (NATIONWIDE) Test N MeanPretest (7) Difference (TS)Pre-Post Difference (alRegressed IIIX 0 Y D rX, Y 3.2.1. NumericalInformationTotal relations' and vocabulary 423423422 208.82 22.5634.98 12.31*(1)1.83*2.76* 19.2297.6610.48 51.94 7.479.11 49.29 8.717.56 44.03 8.426.65 38.61 6.007.26 .55.62.61 1.4s 4.5. Concepts III (sine,(time, shape, social color)function) 4204.Z2 40.0626.70 1.55*1.74* 12.8916.65 12.55 9.07 12.30 9.14 10.54' 8.44 7.529.45 .57.64 CO Note:8.7.6. (1) An asterisk ( *) indicttes p <.05. Independence-selfFollowingVisual-motor instructions help 422 30.29.7924.58 87 2.50*1.21* .69* 31.1217.65'13.11 9.567.867.99 7.387.947.41 11.74 8.467.27 7.337.196. X 6 .12.43.56

la...r.. scores could be obtained for each subtest. The third column lists the pretest mean (TC for each test.The next columns list, respectively, the mean gain score (b) for each test, and the adjusted or regressed differences(aA) for each test.Standard deviations for X,Y,D, and aAare given in the next four columns, and the last column to the right lists the correlation coefficients for pretest and posttest scores of each test. An mean gains are positive and significant at the 5- percent level, although the gain for the Following Instructions subtest is mar- ginal (p = .0465). Exhibit I-5 summarizes the results of 1-factor analyses of total PSI scores. This exhibit presents,in compact format, the same information (except for differences in order or arrangement between posttest means) that is presented in the 1-factor tables of PPVT results (see Section IV of the first volume of this report). This exhibit, however, presents all factors in a single exhibit, rather than in separate exhibits for each factor. The first column lists factor names and levels, and the second column, sample sizes for each group. The third column shows the pretest mean(X) for each factor level, as well as the unweighted average for the whole group clas- sified by the factor indicated. The next column lists the mean gain score (D.) for the group named by the jth level of the factor. The next column gives the adjusted mean or regressed difference score(aA.for each group, and also the unweighted averagefor the factor group as a whole. The columns following list differences of various types.The col- umn labeledit.- Xkgives the differences between pretest means for groups or levels within a factor classification.The subscripts j and k refer to different levels of a factor. For example, for the age factor, are three le gels: "over5," "5," and "under 5." The 37. -Xkk difference in the "over 5" row is the difference between level a and level b--that is, the pretest mean of the "over 5" group minus the pretest mean of the 5-year-old group.The X.-Xk difference in the row labeled "5" is the difference in pretest means of the 5-year-olds (b) and the under 5-year-olds (c).The difference in the row labeled "under 5" is the difference between the over 5-year-olds (a) and theunder 5 -year- olds (c). For the 3 -level groups, the descending order inthe X. -k

I- 9 EXHIBITFACTOR 1-5 AND LEVEL COMPARISON OF PSI Pretest 7., TOTAL SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES aj - a 1. AGE a. Over 5 5 210143 Mean (Se) 203.65225.28 13.54*10. '''j 48*(11 102.59 96.82, aj 21.63*19.50* k ,' a 5.784.58 20.92* .71 i' 5.38 ,. -3.3,9-3.07 -3.17- .11 b.Unweightedc. Average Under 5 58 204.36184.16 13.65*16.'93* 92.2397.21 41.12* 10.36 - 20.21* -4.98- .40 -6.46 3.28 . 2. SEX b.a. Female Male 166192 206.12212.38 14.37*12. 00* 9.64* 98.6596.4897.56 6.26 - 2.1.6 .0-.... 3.13 -1.08 1.08 -4.72 -2.36 2.36 3. RACE a.UnweightedUnweightedb. Negro Average Average White 216204 209.25213.35211.38215.31 12.11.21*11.87* 53* 96.4897.5698.65 3.94 - 2.16 -1.97 1.97 -1.08 1.08 -1.32 - .66 .66 4. FAMILY INTACTNESS b.a.Unweightedc. Mother Only Average FatherBoth Parents Only 286 1868 218.21237.06210.10207.48 -..... 12.92*10.28*8.509.41* 105. 43 95.3297.7599.50 -29.58 26. 95 2.62 -10.- 7. 11 68 2.43 -10.73 18.848.11 -4.18-1.75 5.93 4.423.50 .91 -1.78-. .86 2.64 5. FAMILY6. URBANIZATION INCOME Unweightedb.a.. Less than Average $3, 000 $3, 000 or More 343 80 208.208.13208.55 98 10.70*13. 29* 8.10* 95.9798.7493.20 .86 - 5.54 . 43.43 -2.77 2.77 -5.20 -2.60 2.60 Note: (1) An asterisk (*) indicates PUnweightedb.a. < UrbanRural Average 295 87 212.52218.03207.00 05 10.07*12.94*15.82* 104.9794.7199.84 -11.03 - 10.26* 5.52 -5.13* 5. 13* -5.74 -2.87 2.87

- "Ir ''..t M.Y14.441. SPW'r - column is always a - b ,b- c,and a - c .Similarly, the column gives the differencesof adjusted (regressed) differences labeledgi k fromeach other and in the sameorder as for the X. -Xkkdifferences.1 The column labeledI-3r gives the differences of the factor level pretest meansfrom the unweighted averageof factor group means. The - column gives this samedifference for the ad!*usted differ- ences.The final two columns ofstatistics show the differencesof differ-. ences foreach factor level from eachother (in the same a-b, b - c , a - c order asbefore) and from the totalunweighted average difference (D) for the factor group. Anasterisk (*) in all casesindicates a signif- icant difference or contrastat the .05 level. Examination ofExhibit T-C reveals that, aside fromthe number of significantly different from zero, asexpected, there .D scores that were were only twofactors which had significantdifferences.There was a significant difference in pretestperformance as a function of age,with the youngest childrenhaving the lowest scores andthe oldest having the highest ones. The two extremepretest means (for"over 5" and "under 5") also differed fromthe total unweighted mean.That is, they were significantly different from a groupmean of zero.However, there were no differentialeffects observed for thedifferent age groups on eitherde- pendent variable.That is, no group madesignificantly more or less gain than the others.The youngest childrendid make greater absolute gains than the olderchildren. Nevertheless,it is not possible to accept the hypothesis that anyof the observed clustersof differences or changes belong to a differentpopulation of differencesfrom the others for this sample. (The readerwill recall that there was asignificant difference

2- lInsome exhibits,the X: - Xk has beenomitted from the text for level factors. Thevaluesare simplydouble those for theTi- indication applies toboth statistics wherever values, and a significance those a1.21 aqterisk(311). is shown. The samerelationship applies to values that have beenomitted. . -tik and D. -k between the adjusted orregressed differences of differences of PPVT scores for the ageclassification. ) The other factorclassification in which a significant difference occurred is Urbanization.As with PPVT measures, the rural children scored higher on the pretestof the PSI, although not significantly so. However, on the PSI,the urban children also appear to have madeless gain than expected, asmeasured by adjusted or regressed differences. This result is comparablein nature to that obtained with the PPVTwhen scores wereclassified by the age of the child(see Exhibit IV-9 of the first volume of this report).It is true, however, that the data canalso be interpreted to meanthat the rural children in this samplegained or improved significantly morethan the urban and suburban children on the total achievementtest.Mi., hypnchawis is supported particularly by examination of the pretest meansand the change scores.In any case, it may be concluded thatwhen PSI total scores are groupedaccording to where the child lives,there is, for the covariance measure, amain effect attributable to thecharacteristic and a differential effect ofthe treatment.) Otherwise, as may be seen inExhibit 1-5, there were no signif- icant differences associated withfactor classifications. Trends in pre- test means and change scores weresimilar to those foUnd with the PPVT. Whites scored higher thanNegroes; males scored higherthan females; the higher income groupscored higher than the lower one, etc. Exhibits 1-6 through 1-12 presentanalyses of each of the PSI sub- tests. We shall not comment oneach table in detail.The results are, in general, the same asthose found for the PSI total scores.The follow- ing points, however, areworth particular attention.

1Wehave throughout interpretedsignificant differences in distributions of differences of differences toimply a differential effect ofHead Start treatnentza, the subgroupsinvolved in the analysis. There are,of cour alternative interpretations. Theinterpretation made here, ho never, seems mostuseful or feasible to us in theinterest of illumi- nating results and futureresearch implications anddirections.

I -12 1:=0 En= Tme C3 = t=3 C=I EXHIBIT 1-.6 COMPARISON OF PSI SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES .... SUBTEST 1: FACTOR AND LEVEL INFORMATION AND VOCABULARY N - Mean (3C-)Pretest 15 j a. J X. -X j k ,^ j - c/Z_ lc X - j /\ - aa. D.. - D j k D. - D j 1. AGE b.a. Over 5 5 181134 33.9837.61 2.99*2. 87*(1) 20.5718.98 , 2.533.63* -0 34 1,,1 59 25 -0. 37 3.26* -0.64 0.94 /' - -1.53-0.12 -0.59-0.47' Unweightedc. .A.1 le vrge Under 5 1"...rnr . 42 34.3531.45 4.52*3.46* 19.6319. 32 6.16* I -2.90* -0.30 -1.65 1.06 2. SEX Unweightedb.a. MaleFemale Average 188177 .. 35.35.1135.29 20 2.3.11*2.73* 92* 19.4819.6319.34 -0.18 -0.18 -0.30 0, 30 -0.09 0.09 -0.15 0.15 -0.38 0. 38 -0.19 0.19 4.3. FAMILY RACE INTACTNESS Unweightedb.a. Negro Average White 152167 35.35.88 7563 3..3.36*2. 75*06* 19.8820.2419.51 -0.24 0.24 -0.72 0.72- -0.12 0.12 -0.36 0.36 -0.60 0.60 -0.30 0.30 5. FAMILY INCOME Unweightedc.b.a. Mother Only Average FatherBoth Parents Only 257 5916 36.3634.7036.7539.19 2.25*1.90*O.3.07* 38 19.1918.8119.3619.40 -4.48-2.83-1.65 - 0.590.55O. 39 -0.39-2. 04 2.44 -0.38 0.170.21 2.701.880.82 -1.52 0.351.17 b.a. Less than $3, 000 $3, 000 or More 294 75 34.9735.59 3.31*1.77* 19.7618.52 -0.62 0.62 . -1,. 24 1.. 24 -O. 31 0. 31 -0.62 0.62 -1.54 1.54 -0.77 0.77 6. URBANIZATION a.Unweighted Average Urban 263 83 35.0435. "28 4.2.47*2.54* 36* 19.1418.95 -0.28 - -2. 02* 2. 02* -0.14 0. 14 -1.01* 1.01* -1.90 1.90 - -0.95 0.95 Note: (1) An asterisk ( *) indicates PUnweightedb. < . 05. Average Rural - 35.1835.33 3.41* 19.9720.98 -0.28 SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES - SUB TEST 2: EXHIBITFACTOR 1-7AND LEVEL COMPARISONNUMERICAL OF RELATIONSPSI Mean (X)Pretest 5 J . na. 5E j . - ii k di ;" /\ci,_ A . - 'Ci. 3 - /ci D j - 5 k . - ii 1. AGE 127 24.82 1. 65* (1) 11.16 3. 37* . 0.870.5 ?. 2.87* 0.120.63 -0. 77 0.15 -0.46 0.15 2. a.c.b. OverSEXUnweighted Under 5 5 Average 5 178 40 1 21.4521.9519.58 2.15*2.28*2.43* 10.6410.52 9.77 5.24*1.87* 1.39 -2.-0.50 37* -0.75 -0.62 0. 30 3. Unweightedb.a.RACE MaleFemale Average 170183 22.6221.8423. 39 .2. 01*2. 1.99*03* 10.6710.4010.95 -1. 56 1.56 -0.56 0.56 -0. 78 0.78 -0.28 0. 28 -0, C4 0. 04 -0. 02 0. 02 4. b.a.FAMILYUnweighted INTACTNESS Average WhiteNegro t 147161 24.3022.8821.45 2.2.47*1.64* 05* 10.6810.9510.82 -2.86* 2.86*- -0.26 0.26 -1.43* 1.43* -0.13 0.670, .3 -0.82 1.310.82 -0.41 0.411.13 5. b.a.FAMILYUnweightedc. Mother INCOMEOnly Average FatherBoth Parents Only 247 5715 23.26.22.6522.54 7507 2.43*1.30*0.1.12 33 11.0610. 3239 9.80 -3.-3.4E:-0. 1152 - -0.52 0.751.27 -1.10-1.21 2. 31 -0.59-0. 08 2.0.79 10 -0.17-0.96 6. Unweightedb.a.URBANIZATION Less than Average $3, 000 or More $3, 000 284 72 22.8921.3522. 12 2. 2.00*03* 02* 110. O.1 O. 1880 49 -1.54 -1.54 -0.62 0.62 -0.77 0. 77 -0. 31 0. 31 -0. 04 0. 16 -0. 02 0. 02 , Note: (1) An asterisk a.Unweightedb. Urban Rural Average (*) indicates P < . 05. 253 84 23.1524.21.98 32 2.1.76* 2.24*71* 11.1012.10.18 01 -2.34 2.34- -1.84* 1.84* -1.17 1.17 -0.92* 0.92* -.96- . 96 -0.4u 0.48 EXHIBIT I.8 COMPARISON OF PSI SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES . SUBTEST 3: FACTOR1. AGE AND LEVEL CONCEPTS I Mean (N)Pretest ±3j ^ a. J . - /\ ^a J - G. . - , ^ J - aa. . - 13 k B. j - i5 . b.a.Unweightedc. Average Under5Over 5 5 173124 42- 40.3443.4038.7432.48 0.901.83*3.2901.312, 1 16.2615.3816.3417.06 10.9Z* 7.86*3.05- 0.960.73-1.68 -6.26* 4.66*1.60- -0.880.800.08- -0.41-2.38-1.97 - -0.93-0.52 -1.45 2. SEX Unweightedb.a. MaleFemale Average 177 - 39.6139.9739.25 2.07*2.05*2.03* 16.8016.6916.91 -- - -0.36 -0.36 -0.11 0.11- -- -0.02 0.02- 3. RACE a.UnweizhtedUnweightedb. White Aver Average Negro 151154 - 42.9840.9338.88 2.62*1.76*0.89 17.4017.1016.89 -- - -2.05* 2.05*- -0.10 -0.10 -.. -0.87 -0.87 4. FAMILY INTACTNESS c.b.a.Unweighted Mother Only Average FatherBoth Parents Only 248 57-14 42.2946.3640.9839.54 0.210.262.08*0.85 16.6017.4815.5316.81 -6.81-5.37-1.44 -0.67-1.95 -1.28 -1.31-2.75 4.06- -1.08 0.870.20- 0,07-1.871.82 -0.64-0.59 -1.23 5. FAMILY INCOME Unweightedb.a. Less than Average $3,000 $3, 000 or More 281 73- 39.8440.1039.58 2. 0.2117*1.19* -F. 16.0217.1014.94 -- - -0.26 0.26- -1.08 -1.08 - -0.98 -0.98 6.Note: URBANIZATION (1) An asterisk(*) indicates P

14 111 4,06 1 41. LLY..ou.srucsa uop wa.s.Y on4416..e Conc-epts I (size, shape, .11.1111J tion, and color) subtest were the pretest meansof the rural children sig- nificantly higher than those of theurban/suburban children (see Exhibit 1-8).However, on most subtests there was a trend towardbetter ini- tial achievement performance of the ruralchildren. Males tended to perform a little better than females on most subtests, although not significantly so. On theNumerical Relations sub- test, the difference between male andfemale pretest means was very close to the .05 bound and could be consideredsignificantly different for purposes of furtherinvestigation or research. Males had the only neg- ative D-scores observed so far (seeExhibit I-11).It is interesting to speculate as to why there should have been nosignificant changes in per- formance on this subtest for either males orfemales. In general, whites scored higherthan Negroes on the pre- test. However, on the Independenceand Self-Help subtest, the situation was reversed, andNegroes scored significantly higherthan whites (see Exhibit I- 12). With the exception of urban versusrural children noted above, there were no significantdifferences in the sample analyzed in gains on either measure attributable to afactor classification or a dif- ferential effect of the Head Starttreatment. In light of the emphasis placed onproblems of perceptual discrimination among culturally deprivedchildren, it is interesting to note the sigAificant differences amongpretest means of different sub- groups of children onthe Concepts I subtest (see ExhibitI-8).This

I -20 subtest (according to Caldwell) tapsdiscriminations of shape, size, color, and motion.

B. The Behavior Inventory (BI), The Behavior Inventory is a setof 50 rating scales, of which 25 refer to positive attributes ofchildren and 25 to negative ones. Ratings of each child were supposed tobe completed by teachers in theSummer 1965 Project Head Start atthe beginning and end of the program.The BI can provide two setsof scores: (1) anoverall adjiinfrnArd- szi`erri2,271,1 (2) a separate adjustment scorefor each of nine behavioral categories. A copy of the BehaviorInventozy is presented in Appendix A.Ex- hibit 1-13 shows the itemsthat are included in the positive andnegative categories and in the ninebehavioral categories. Thecategories are numbered as they will benumbered in tables to follow.Items were scored on a 4-point scale.For positive items, a ratingof "Very Much Like" was given a score of 4.For negative items, that category was given a score of 1.Thus, total adjustment scorescould range from 50 (maximum negative) to 200(maximum positive) if all scales were come pleted. The range of scoresfor the various behavioralcategories de- pends, of course, on thenumber of items in the category. The same analyses weremade of the Behavior Inventory as were made of the PPVT and thePSI, in order to provide acomparable set of measures. Thus,analyses were made oftotal adjustment scores andof each behavioral category scorefor those matched(pre/post) forms re- coverable in the 1-percentsample. It is possible to analyze scoresfor each item in theBehavior In- ventory.This was not done forthe 1-percent sample data.However, the author of the OperationHead Start Behavior Inventory,Dr. Edward F. Zig ler, provided anindex of the consistencyof raters' responses. A "consistency" or"lie" score can be calculatedfrom the responses to five selected pairs of polaritems. The index based onthe pairs of items is derived as follows.

I- 21 EXHIBIT 1-13BEHAVIOR INVENTORYITEMCATEGORIES(1)

iveItems (-) Dimension Positive Items (+) Neia...... t: 7 1. Sociability 33 35 16 Cooperation 24 Politeness 38 45 37

13 4 2. Independence 40 Dependence 21 '44 Si

5 14 3. Curiosity 22 Enthusiasm 30 39 32 Exploration 46 Creativity 43 3 4. Persistence 11 20 28 26 5. Emotionality 1 23 36 31 42 48 49 11 6 6. Self-Confidence 9 15 18

2 10 7. Jealousy 19 Attention-Seeking 27 17 8. Achievement 8 25 34 49 47 12 9. Leadership 50

numbered on theOperation Note: (1) Item numbersrefer to the items as Head Start BehaviorInventory Form (see copyin Appendix A).

I- 22 For each pair, agreement ordisagreement with both itemswould indicate an inconsistent ornondiscriminatory response.In each case where this occurs, a scoreof 1 will be given.The range of this score will therefore be from0 to 5.Under this scoring system, aninconsist- of ent ornondiscriminating set of responsesis related to the magnitude this score--i.e.,the higher the score,the greater the inconsistencyof the responses. For each of the fivepairs of items, the scoringis as follows: a. If the (+) itemreceives a score of 3 or4 and the (-) item receives a score of1 or 2, the response tothe pair of items is incon- sistent and a scoreof 1 is given. b. If the (+) itemreceives a score of 1 or2 and the (-) item receives a score of3 or 4, the response tothe pair of items is incon- sistent and a scoreof 1 is given. the pair c. All other combinationsindicate that the response to is consistent, and a scoreof 0 is given. The five pairs of items are asfollows:

35 versus 7 44 ver sus 4 30 versus 22 23 versus 26 25 versus 17 Consistency scores werecalculated for each formin the matched sample and correlatedwith total adjustment scoresand with subtests or categories1 (Sociability), 3(Curiosity), and 5(Emotionality). Exhibit 1-14 lists statisticsfor Behavior InventoryTotal Adjust- ment scores for asample of 320 matchedtests. The testslisted in the left-hand column are: BIT : Behavior InventoryTotal AdjustmentScore BI: Sociability 1 BI: Independence 2 BI: Curiosity 3

I- 23 EXHIBIT 1-14 COMPARISON OF BEHAVIOR INVENTORY TOTAL ADJUSTMENT AND SUBTEST SCORES 2.1. Test(1) BI1BIT i 320 N . MeanPretest 5f 127.67 22.16 1.03 .66*(2) D 14.09 3.49cr X 19.83 aD5.38 4.5.3. M4B13B12 320320 20.8614.22 9.55 - .17 .49*.93* 2.855.612.83 2.342.965.40 8.6.7. BIB15B17 6 320 22.1711.12 8.60 - .37* .04.56 2.652.635.11 2.652.335.32 10. Notes:9. (1) See text for test and subtest names. BI (2) An98 aaterisk (*) indicates p <.05. 320 13.98 5.01 - .10.23 3.161.51 1.543.03 BI4: Persistence : Emotionality BI5 BI6: Self-Confidence Attention-Seeking BI7: Jealousy, BI :Achievement 8 BI: Leadership 9 Different subsets have different means,partially due to the fact that different numbers ofitems comprise subsets, as wasnoted earlier. It is interesting toobserve in Exhibit I-14the number of negative15- scores. Twoof thmse are ,significant atthe 5-percent level for a two - arAd BI Considering the traits thatthese scales are tailed test (BI4 6). dealing with, andconsidering that the majorityof both parents and teachers sampl I feltthat the children hadgenerally improved in self- confidence and infinishing what they start,these results are puzzling. One possibility isthat, in the process oflosing over 90 percent ofthe matched 1-percent sample scoresfor various reasons, somesort of unusual selection processhas occurred. Thesample of children for whom there werematched pre/post BehaviorInventory scores'available is quite different incomposition on varioussocio-economic variables from either the PPVTsample or the PSI sample.This may be seen by noting the N's fordifferent factor levels inthe exhibits of 1-factor Behavior Inventorycomparisons that follow.However, it is also pos- sible that the BehaviorInventory scales and theWorker Evaluation Form items in fact tapquite different judgmentsand concepts in re- spondents about seeminglysimilar areas of observation. Exhibits 1-15 through 1-24list the comparisonsof Behavior In- ventory scores by age, sex,race, income,and urbanization.Tabula- tions for mother'seducation (High Schoolgraduates versus non-High School graduates) weremade; however, errorsin the data prevented the reporting of resultsfor this factor.In addition, tabulations were made for familyintactness. However, sincethe N for "FatherOnly" was zeroand for "Mother Only" wassix, this factor wasdropped from further consideration.Moreover, there was a caseinvolving the income

I- 25 EXHIBIT I-15 TOTALCOMPARISON ADJUSTMENT OF BEHAVIOR INVENTORY SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES - FACTOR AND LEVEL Mean (5C) D. J /'a. J . - Xk ." ai - /\ ak Xi - X - 1 /\ ai - a ./\ D. - Dk - D. - D - 1. AGE b.c.a. OverUnder5 5 5 . 210138 36 - 131.129.02127. 0800 -1.20 4. 11 07*( ) ?165.1769.39 . 53 -4.-2.06-2. 0802 -2.-6.37 15 4.22 -0.01-2. 04 2. 05 -3.53* 2. 0.6984 -5.31-0. 05 5.26 -3.52 1.1.74 78 2. SEX b.a.UnweightedUnweighted Male Average Average Female 205184 - 129.128.25129.25127.24 04 2. 1.281.111.4533* 68.67.2467.5966.90 69 -2.002.00 -0.70 0.70 -1.00 1.00 -0.35 0.35 -0.34 0.34 -0.17 0.17 3. RACE Unweightedb.a. Average NegroWhite 186173 - 128.127.91128.49 19 -0.232.831.30 67.2468.6265.85 -0.56 0.56 -2.78 2.78 -0.28 0.28 -1.39 1.39 -3.06 3.06 -1.53 1.53 5.4. URBANIZATIONFAMILY INCOME b.a.Unweighted Average $3,Less 000 than or More $3, 000 362 38 - 127.128.62126.11 36 3.29*5.820. 77 68.8066.70. 92 68 -2.52 2.52 -3. 76 3.76 -I. 26 1. 26 -1. 88 1.88 -5. 04 5. 04 -2.52 2. 52 Note: (1) An Asterisk (*) indicates pb.a.

11611111111110111111.110111WthWNIElillikigUMMIMaigididiAL4 ONAAfir ..1.4.A-M2L.L.ii.u..rA,MidN. MAWLM EXHIBIT I-;21 SUBTESTCOMPARISON 6: SELF-CONF1DENCE OF BEHAVIOR INVENTORY SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES - FACTOR AND. LEVEL AGE Mean (5C-.)Pretest D._ /-\ a. J X. - X /-\ a. - a j /*\ k X. - X - /\a. - a J /\ -D. - D 1 - k D. - D 3 2. SEXa.Unweightedc.b. -Under 5 Average 5Over 5 210138 36 7.698.288.528.62 -0.-O.-0.43-0.26 19 304,4 .1) ' 4.3.88 310809 0.0, -938210 -0.430.200. 23 -0.58 -0.240. 34 -0.21-0. 01 0.22- -0. 0724 -0. 17 -0. 14 0. -1004 3. RACEUnweighteda.b. Male Average Female 205184 8.8.268.75 50 -0.-0.19-0. 35* 52* 4.4.19 1115 -- -- -0.25 0.25- -0. 0' 0.04- - -0. 17 0.17- 4. FAMILYTJnweightedb.a. INCOME Average NegroWhite 186173 8.538.408.65 -O.-0.51*-0.29 40* 4.=1.29 3-.11 94 - - -0.12 -0. 12 -0.17 -0. 17 -- -0.11 0. -11 5. URBANIZATIONUnweighteda.b. Average $3,Under 000 or$3, More 000 362 38 8.458.8.54 37 -0.19-0. 40* 0. 03 4.294. 4612 I - -- -0.09 -0. 09 -0. 17 0. -17 -- -0.21 0.21- 1::= = = c I= C=1 =1 = It= Note: Unweightedb.a. Average (1) An asteriskRuralUrban (*) indicates p < . 05. 270 92 8.598.8.49 70 -0.-0.57* 24 0. 09 4.314.3.93 69 =1 I=3 = =1 I=1 = = - - -0.10 0. -10 -0.38 .0. 38 - -0.33* 0.33*- =3 = =D I EXHIBIT 1-22 SUBTESTCOMPARISON 7: JEALOUSY, OF BEHAVIOR ATTENTION-SEEKING INVENTORY SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES - . .. . . - FAC TOR AND LEVEL N Mean (X)Pretest D. J /N a. J X. - X k a. - aj ,. k X. - X - a.. - J D. - D j k D. - D j . 2. SEX1. AGE Unweightedc.b.a. Over 5 Average Under5 5 210138 36 11.3811.3311.2011.09 -0.120.100.220. 21 5. 4.73350302 -0.-0.63-0. 74 12 '' -0.34-0.62 0.28 -0.17-0.29 0. -46 , r-0.30-0. 02 0. 32 -0.-4.35 01 0. 33 -0.23 O.0.11 12 . RACEUnweightedb.a. Male Average Female 205-184 11.1911.4710. 92 -0.15 0.0.20 03 4.884.824. 93 -0.54* 0.54* -0.10 0.10 -0. 274 -0.27* ) -0.05 -0. 05 -0.34 0. 34 -0.17 0.17 4. Unweightedb.a.FAMILY AverageINCOME NegroWhite -186173 11.1511.2211.09 -0.05 0.0.16 05 4.895.024.75 -0.12 0.12 -0.26 0.26 -0.06 0.06 -0.13 0.13 -0.20 0.20 -0.100.10 . URBANIZATIONUnweightedb.a. Average $3,Under 000 or$3, More 000 362 38 10.10.7411.25 99 -0. 02 0.250. 53 5.4.85 1802 -0. 52 0.52 -0. 32 0. 32 -0.26 0.26 -0.16 0. 16 -0.54 0.54 -0.27 0.27 Note: (1) An asterisk (*) indicates b.a.Unweighted Average RuralUrban 270 92 p < . 05.11.4311.3111..'.9 -0.-0.20 08 0. 04 4.824.4.89 76 -0.24 -0.24 -0. 12 -0. 12 -0.12 0.12 -0.06 0. 06 -0.24 0. 24 -0.12 0. 12 __.....er.prer-nw.pmrrnvmomwmur.*wwgririrmP±"wunrgwrm pmmirwortwpww. puvr"-."'"""w"'"`""7"v"r""''''' " ''' EXHIBIT 1-23 SUBTESTCOMPARISON 8: ACHIEVEMENT OF BEHAVIOR INVENTORY SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES - FACTOR AND. LEVEL AGE Mean (5?)Pretest D. /\a. J X. - X k ^ j - a . - a. - a J D. - D j k D - D j [ UnweightPc.b.a. Average Under5Over 5 5 210138 36 - 13.9913.5313.8714.57 -0.17-0.33-0. 01 5.945.745.726.35 -1.040.340.71 a -0.01 -0.610.62 -0.-0.12 46 -0.58 -0.20-0.21 -0.41 -0.16 -0. 3115 -0.16 -0.0.15 00 2. SEX Unweightedb.a. Average FemaleMale 205184 - 14.13.8914.28 09 -0.-0.32 1906 5.966.5.92 01 -0. 38 -0.38 -0.08 -0. 08 -0. 19 -0.19 -0.04 O.- 04 -0.26 0. -26 -0.13 O.- 13 3. LACE Unweightedb.a. Average NegroWhite 186173 - 14.13.95 0514 -0.-0.21-0.28 24 5. 5.885.90R9 -0.20 -0.20 -0.02 0. -02 -0.10 0.10- -0.01 -0.01 -0. 06 0.06- j -0. 03 -0.03 5.4. URBANIZATION FAMILY INCOME b.a.Unweighted Average $3,Under 000 $3, or 000More 362 38 - 14. 110600 -0. 26 0. 0537 6.196.485. 90 -0.12 -O. 12 -0.58 0.58- -0. 06 -0. 06 -0. 29 -0.29 -0. 64 -0.64 -0. 32 -0. 32 . Unweightedb.a. Average RuralUrban 270 92 - 14.14.33 1804 -0.-0.29 1824 5. 6.075.8495 -0.280.28- -0.24 -0.24 -0. 140.14- -0. 12 -0. 12 -0. 10 -0.10 -0.05 0.05- EXHIBITFACTOR I -.24AND COMPARISONLEVEL OF BEHAVIOR INVENTORY SCORES BY FACTOR CATEGORIES tr B T ES T 9: LEADERSHIP Mean (3C)Pretest D._ j /\ a. J X. - aj - ak X. X /\ 0.. - J a- D. - Dk D. - D - 1. b.a.AGE Over 5 5 210138 5.134.64 -0.00 0. 3941) 2.422.59 -0.48* 0.10 / /-0.35 0.17 -0.29 0.19 -0.004 0.17 /' 0.44 0.13 Unweightedc. Average Under 5 36 - 4. 5.0393 0. 3926* 2. 2,.59 77 i -0. 38 - -0. 18 - 0. -09 , -0.17 - -0.40 O.- 00 -0.26 0.13- 2. Unweightedb.a.SEX Average FemaleMale 205184 - 4.5.4.76 1595 0. 1616* 2.512.2.41 60 -0. 40* -0. 40* 1 -0. 18 -0. 18 -0. 20* -0. 20* -0. 09 -0. 09 -- -0. 001 0. -001 3. Unweightedb.a.RACE White Average Negro 186173 - 4.4.835.12 97 -0.02 O.0.32* 15 2.612.402. 50 -0.30 -0. 30 -0.20 -0.20 -0.15 -0.15 -0.10 -O.10 -0. 34*0. -34* -0. 17* -0.17* 4. Unweightedb.a.FAMILY INCOME Average $3,Under 000 $3, or 000More 362 38 - 4.4.84 9198 0.0.13 3926* 2.2.692.48 59 -0. 14 -0. 14 -0.20 0.20- -0. 07 -0. 07 -0. 10 0.10- -0.26 -0. 26 -0.13 -0. 13 5. b.a.URBANIZATION RuralUrban 270 92 - 4. 979699 0. 0.0900 17 2. 2.533544 -0. 04 -0. 04 -0. 18 -0. 18 -0. 02 -0. 02 -0. 09 -0. 09 -0. 18 -0. 18 -0. 09 0.09- Note: (1) An asterisk (*) indicates pUnweighted < Average _ . . 05. factor in which the data obtained were in error.Thus, there is a blank row in one of theexhibits(I-16).1There is one exhibit for the Total Adjustment score (BIT) and one foreach of the nine categories of scales (BIthrough BI The latter are, as elsewhere,in the order listed and 1 9). named earlier. We shall not undertake adetailed analysis of the results as sum- marized in each exhibit.The asterisks, which indicate asignificant difference at the 5-percent level, make itrelatively easy for the reader, who is by now very familiar withthe format and meaning of the results, to select his own observationsfor interpretation.It should be noted here again that tests of the significanceof D.'s were two-tailed tests.There are, however, anumber of relationships shown by the datain the ex- hibits which should be noted. Forexample, consider Exhibit 1-15, which presents results of analysesof the Total Adjustment scores on theBe- havior Inventory.It may be seen that the order ofstanding on pretest scores on anumber of classifications is reversed fromthat which has been typical of the two performance tests(PPVT and PSI). The younger children scored higher than the olderchildren, girls scored higher than boys, the lower income children scoredhigher than the higher income children, and the urban children scoredhigher than the rural children. The differences are not statisticallysignificant.Nonetheless, there is a consistent reversalof tendencies or trends observedelsewhere. Fur- thermore, there are a number of casesthroughout in which the groups with the higher pretest meansshowed noticeably 'arger gains between pretest and posttest.This tendency is reflected in some ofthe signifi- cant differences ofdifferences and adjusted differences indicated in the exhibits by asterisks.

1To the best of our knowledge,the data presented throughout is accurate,. Errors arising from various sourceshave been eliminated.It is possible, however, that an occasional anomaly maystill exist. We have attempted to facilitate the evaluation ofdata and the elimination of misleadinginfor- mation by presenting as much data aspossible to permit others to check results. We are keenly aware of theimportance of reliable information in as newly explored an area as thatof the socially disadvantaged child.

1- 36 Finally, there are four subtests inwhich Negroes were ratedhigher on the averagethan whites, although again thedifferences are not statis- tically significant. The trends found in the resultswith the Behavior Inventory areof particular interest, since thisinstrument was the only onespecifically designed to make a comprehensiveasses'rnent of detailedsocial and emotional behavioral variables. ThePsychological Screening Procedure provided more or less globalimpressions of the children.The Behavior Inventory calls out more specificbehavioral characteristics. As will be seen in theanalyEl3 of intertest correlationswhich follows, it appears that theBehavior Inventory has indeedmeasured a unique and crucial area ofdevelopment of culturally deprivedchildren (see Exhibit II-8 in the firstvolume of this report). C. Intertest and IntratestRelationships A question of great interest concernsthe relationships between and within the three main testinstruments used in the1965 Project Head Start.To obtain informationabout such relationships, itappeared fea- sible to perform a canonicalanalysis, using as theantecedent or "pre- dictor" variables various total andsubtest scores available, and asthe outcome or "criteria"variables the D- scoresobtained on the tests (see Reference 126 for a discussionof the methodology).The tests selected for analysis were: PPVT PSI Total Score (PSIT) PSI Vocabulary and Information(PSI1) PSI Numerical Relations(PSI2) PSI Concepts II (PSI4) PSI Following Instructions(PSI6) BITotal Adjustment Score (BIT) BI Sociability (B11) BI Curiosity '(BI3) BI Emotionality (BI5) BI Consistency Score(Bic)

I -37 8

u Correlation matrices, canonical correlation coefficients, and canonical vector weights were obtained using an existing canonical anal-. Q ysis program.1 As it turned out, the total number of matched forms for all vari- ables was 43. Nonetheless, some of the interrelationships that emerged D are noteworthy and are includedin this report for their value in direct - ing attention to hypotheses to be examined in future studies.Exhibit 11 1-25 shows correlation coefficients obtained between the pretest vari- ables. For ease of reading and analysis, only those coefficients that o exceeded ±0.35 were put in the table.The one exception is the Bekavior Inventory Consistency score (Bic), for which the intercorrelationshave been reported, regardless.Exhibit 1 26 gives the correlation coefficients 11 for the difference scores on the te.:+ variables.Exhibit 1-27 presents the intercorrelations of the pretest variables ("predictors") and change scores u ("criteria") for all variables.In this case, all coefficients of +0.30 or more are stated. Although ourcutoff level for listing a coefficient was n arbitrary--i.e., not based on a calculation of significance--we shall

speak as if the coefficients listed were significant and those unlisted not El significant.(The correlation of variables with themselves is, of course, 1.0; this value has also been omitted from the tables. ) D The most striking fact about Exhibit 1-25, the pretest score inter- correlations, is the lack of correlation between any of the three instru- ments. To the extent that this small sample is at all indicative, each o of the instruments appears to be measuring a different fUnction. The intra-PSI correlations can be examined in the light of pretest 11 intercorrelations reported by Caldwell for her standardization sample (see Exhibit 1-3).She found a higher intercorrelation between subtest 1 ii (Information and Vocabulary) and the total test score than that reported 0

1Dix.on, W. J., ed. , "BMD 06M Canonical Analysis," BMD Biomedical il Computer Programs. Los Angeles: Health Sciences Computing Facility, U Department of Preventive Medicine and Public Health, University of California, 1 January 1964 0

I -38 o

0 EXHIBIT 1-25 INTERCORRELATIONS OF PPVT, PSI, AND BI PRETEST SCORES (1) PPVT PPVT - PSI _ T PSI - 1 PS'PSI2 - PSI - 4 PSI - 6 - BI - 1 BI a 3 BI - 5 BI - C PSPSI I1 /112 T -- --- .67 - - .39.74 s .67 ------PSIBIT 64 ------.58 - a- .77 -- .77 a .73 - -.13 - BI 1 - - - a- - -- - a- .78 - .56 - -.04-.17 Note:BI (1) Only correlations larger than ± .35 are shown, except for correlations the Behavior Inventory Consistency score (Bic). -N - - a - - = 43. - - a concerning a - A EXHIBIT 1-26 INTERCORRELATIONS PPVT PSI T PSI 1 OF PPVT, PSI, AND BIPSI2 PSI 4 PSI 6 DIFFERENCE BIT BI1 SCORES") BI 3 BIs BIC PPVT - - - - I - - - - . - a - PSI1PSITPSI 2 -- -- .62 a- .53 ------a- --Oa . -4.1 - -- PSIPSIBIT 46 ------.65 - - .84 - .80 - .70 - -.67 -- BI 3 ------.75 - .52.76 - -.75-.46-.62 BIC-Note:BI (1) Only 5 Behavior Inventory Consistency score correlations larger than ±- .35 are - - - (BIC). N = 43.shown, except -for correlations - - - concerning- the - - C=3 C:=3 eZ2 eme e=s c=1 c= C=3 =3 =I C:0 EXHIBIT 1-27 INTERCORRELATIONS AND DIFFERENCE SCORES OF PPVT, PSI, AND BI PRETEST (CRITERIA)(1) (PREDICTORS) ..r.14an. a M orp.rn,...... AJ PredictorsPPVT IIIMINOMINW11 PI'VT -.32 D PSI -TD PSI - ID PSI - 2D PSI -4D Criteria PSI - 6D BITD - BI -.311D BI3D - BI - 5D BICD - PSITPSI2PS/1 -- -.41 a- -- - .35 - - a- -a - I --a - PSIPSI6 4 .30 -a -.40 - -- -- .4S.31 - .43 ------.23 - BIBIlBIT 3 ------.- - - -a -- -- - .14.15.19 Note:BIBI5 (1) Only correlations larger than ± Behavior Inventory Consistency score (BI C). N = - - - .30 are Cdown, except for correlations concerning- - 43. - .58 .67 .62 the .40 -.74 in Exhibit Similarly, the correlations in theexhibit between PSIT and PSIand PSIare lower thanthose reported by Caldwell. However, 1 4 the correlations reported herebetween PSIT and PSI6, andPSI4.and. PSI6, are about the same asthose found by Caldwell. The Behavior Inventory subtestsselected all appear to correlate fairly highly with the TotalAdjustment score (BIT), and somewhat less so with eachother. There is a small but consistentlynegative relation- ship between the ConsistencyScore .(BIc) and the other Behavior Inven- tory scores. That is,the better (lower) the consistency score,the higher (better) the total orsubtest scores.1The negative correlations thus indicate a type ofvalidity of the consistency scores, sincelow con- sistency scores are indicativeof an absence of contradictoryratings. The data in Exhibit 1-26 suggestwhat would be expected from the correlations in Exhibit1-25--namely, that pretest scores on variables that are correlated arepositively correlated with change scores onthose variables. An exception isthe correlation of D- scores foundbetween PSI/ and PSI2, which did nothave a .significant pretest intercorrelation. Of further interest is the verystrong negative relationshipbetween the BIscores and theother BI change scores. A BIchange score was obtained by subtracting a pretestBic score from the posttest Bic score. Thus, the greater the change towardconsistency, the lower the BI change score. The negativecorrelations shown in Exhibit1-26 indicate, therefore, that improvementsfrom pretest to posttest on thevarious tests are correlatedwith an increase in consistency ordiscrimination of the raters. Exhibit 1-27 lists intercorrelationsbetween pretest vai 'ables (labeled "predictors" here) anddifference scores (which we havecalled "criteria"). The negative correlations(for example, between PPVT and PPVTD) are what wouldbe expected from the generalobservation

litshould be remembered that ahigh total or subtest score indicates generally a high rating onpositive attributes and a low rating onnegative ones.

I- 42 that the lower pretest scorers tend to show relatively more gain than the higher ones. The positive correlations are less obviously interpretable. Finally, Exhibit 1-28 lists canonical vectors for the three largest canonical correlation coefficients (RC). Frequently, only the first root an corresponding canonical correlation coefficient is of interest, although it has been found that other roots and correlations may be of interest for some research problems (Reference 126, page 37). We have therefore listed all eleven canonical correlations in order, and provided the vector weights for the second and third, as well as the first, since these R 13 were so close to the first.The vectors are the weights of the variables whose linear combination maximizes the corre- lation between the canonical variates.In view of the ezlormous losses of samples that occurred in arriving at the inputs for this analysis (it will be recalled that N = 43), we will not attempt to speculate on pos7 Bible interpretations of the vectors, although a detailed examination of relationships could reveal hypotheses of further interest. We have throughout this report attempted to provide data with which others may make their own interpretations and evaluations in addition or in contra- distinction to ours, in order not to pre-empt the interests and special skills of experts it the various areas of child development.It is in this spirit that the vectors are reported here. It is our opinion that the canonical analysis, as it turned out, has been of value primarily for the correlation matrices that were generated. Future research studies of Head Start children, however, may well in- clude hypothesei about continuous multiple antecedent and criterion var- iables that would make canonical analysis a valuable tool. D. Conclusion There is little in the analyses presented here that changes con- clusions already drawn in Section V of the first volume of this report. The following points seem warranted, however, even though a few are repetitions of earlier statements.

I- 43 EXHIBIT 1-28 CANONICAL VECTORS 'CSR THE THREE HIGHEST CANONICAL CORRELATIONS") Predictors 1. R .. = .87 Ciiteria Predictors 2. R -.C.-- .82 Predictors 3. R .77 .3.24.60 .!.) BI5PSI T .34.53 BITBIc .39.60 BITPSI 6 .48.50 Criteria PSIBI 1 6 .60.90 BITBI 2 .49.59Criteria PSIBI1 T .05.10 PSIPSI6BI 3 2 .16.10.21 PSI 61 .10.19.31 PSIPSIBI c 41 .20.34.38 PSIPSI.PPVT 1 .16.41.44 PSI 61 .20.27.44 BITPPVTBI C -.05-.048 .04 PSIBIPPVT T -.13 .001.01 PSI2.PPVTBI 5 4 -.23 .01.09 PSIBI5 2T .03.04.19 BIBIT 3 4 -.24 .01 BIPSI c 42 .08.19 PSIBI 3 61 -.76-.57-.32 BIcPSIBI 1 41 -.54-.31-.27 PSI1313BI 1 T -.40-.35-.25 PPVTBI 13 -.73-.55-.12 PSIBI5PSI2. c T -.90-.84-.55-.47 BIPSIPPVT 35 T -.96-.37-.19-.18 BIPSI 5 24 Note: (1) Remaining canonical correlations: 4.7.6.5. 0.390.520.660.75 11.10. 8.9. 0.010.100.170.32 --...-i. Results in cognitive and perce?tual areas of functioning typically show positive improvement or gain even for the brief interval between pre- and posttesting. While problems ofreliability and prac- tice effects do enter into an evaluation of results, we areinclined to agree with Pierce-Jones,et al., who wrote after analyzing their own and others' findings of pre/post differences onthe PPVT and PSI: "We are disposed, on suchgrounds, to think. that the changes we have found are real ones ratherthan mere statistizal artifacts." (Reference 86). Not only are norms badly needed for the newer tests, but particular attention should, be paid to the composition ofstandardization groups. Theconsistent and pervasive findings with respect to sex and urbanization, for example, and the te:adencies associated withincome level, suggest that norms based onmiddle-class urban or suburban children may be very misleading when applied to culturallydeprived groups. There appear to be diffe:rent orders of relationships between social and emotional behavioral measures and socio-economiccharac- teristics of the children from those observed in the morecognitive areas of performance. There is a strong need forreliable data and measures of performance in this realm, and for furtherstudy of relationships be- tween this aspect of developmental behaviorand cognitive functioning in culturally deprived groups. The importance of attention to the mechanics of data collec- tion and processing must again be stressed. There has been little consistent evidence of a differential effect of treatment associated with sub-classificationsof the sample of Head Start children analyzed in this study.This does not necessarily mean that such effectsdid not occur. There is an urgent need forfuture studies specifically designed toinvestigate this area.