General Introduction

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

General Introduction Have You Heard About Hairy-Armed Killers, Defiled Steaks and Contaminated Condiments? Affect, Interpersonal Relationships and Urban Legend Transmission Author Rogers, Shae Published 2008 Thesis Type Thesis (PhD Doctorate) School School of Psychology DOI https://doi.org/10.25904/1912/3200 Copyright Statement The author owns the copyright in this thesis, unless stated otherwise. Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/367088 Griffith Research Online https://research-repository.griffith.edu.au Have You Heard about Hairy-Armed Killers, Defiled Steaks, and Contaminated Condiments? Affect, Interpersonal Relationships, and Urban Legend Transmission Shae Rogers BBehSc, BPsych (Hons) School of Psychology, Health & Griffith Psychological Health Research Centre GRIFFITH UNIVERSITY Submitted as partial fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Clinical Psychology), June 2007. i ABSTRACT The aim of the present thesis is to test a model of urban legend transmission by examining patterns of transmission, the interpersonal functions of transmission, and the role of affect. Urban legends are virulent and persist despite efforts to prevent, cease, or reduce their transmission. The thesis reviews existing explanations for urban legend transmission before presenting the current model to be tested. The model in the present thesis incorporates core social motives (Fiske, 2000, 2004), conversational pragmatics (Grice, 1975), and rationality of purpose (Evans, Over, & Manktelow, 1993) to examine the transmission process within the social context. The model proposes that urban legends are transmitted for the purpose of achieving core social motives such as belonging, by increasing social bonding and affiliation between known others. The model draws on rationality of purpose and conversational pragmatics to help explain the unusual behaviour of the teller (transmitting implausible stories as though they are true) and receiver (failing to question the plausibility of the legend) respectively, in the transmission process. Chapter 2 presents Study 1, which aimed to examine the primary motivation for transmitting an urban legend (meeting core social motives), the optimal social distance for transmission, the role of affect, and the likelihood of transmission of an urban legend when compared to a vivid and neutral story. Study 1 found a clear pattern of transmission of urban legends to particular characters, with close friends the most preferred receivers followed by acquaintances and then strangers. The legend was rated as more likely to be transmitted to the close friend than were the vivid or neutral stories; the legend was rated as equally likely to be transmitted to the acquaintance as was the vivid story, but the legend was rated as more likely to be transmitted to the acquaintance than was the neutral story. There were no differences in the rated likelihood of transmission between stories for the stranger. Further, the presence of negative affect ii alone did not predict the likelihood of transmission. The legend was rated as the least believable story and the most likely to be told to the close friend (over the vivid story which was a more plausible account of danger), suggesting that warning others is not the primary motivation for transmission. Chapter 3 presents Study 2, in which the legend type was changed, using a fear based legend rather than the disgust based legend used in Study 1. However, the findings from Study 2 were generally consistent with Study 1. The main difference was that Study 2 found that the legend was rated the most likely story to be told to all characters, followed by the vivid, then the neutral, stories. Chapter 4 presents the qualitative information relating to the reasons for legend transmission obtained from Studies 1 and 2. Coding the open-ended responses provided valuable information about specific motivations of transmission that could be measured. This chapter outlines how the materials for Study 3 were developed. Chapter 5 presents Study 3, which aimed to test specific motivations of transmission based on the relationship the teller has with the receiver, and legend type. This study found that motivations of transmission such as gauging the other person’s reaction, sharing their own reaction, and the story being disturbing, shocking, or alarming were rated highly across all three characters. These motivations may be consistent with a social sharing of emotion explanation. This explanation suggests that people share emotional events with others because the sharing experience brings about interpersonal benefits such as the strengthening of social bonds and the improvement of interpersonal relationships (Zech & Rime, 2005). A teller sharing an urban legend and expressing their reaction to it or encouraging the receiver to share his or her own reaction, may lead to a positive social sharing experience, which may be beneficial for relationship maintenance. The diffuse transmission of legends and their spread across social networks may be explained by the receivers’ engaging in secondary social iii sharing. Urban legend transmission becoming a form of social sharing in an interaction, would be beneficial in meeting core social motives, such as belonging, by developing and enhancing affiliation and social bonds. Chapter 6 presents the main findings and implications and the directions for future research. Overall, there was support for some aspects of the model. There was some support for the hypothesis that the motivation for transmission is the need to meet core social motives by enhancing affiliation and strengthening social bonds. This finding also supports the rationality of purpose component of the model which suggests that if people are meeting goals through transmission, then their behaviour is in fact rational. There was also support for the hypothesis that the presence of negative affect alone is not sufficient motivation for transmission. There was no support in any of the studies for the hypothesis that acquaintances would be preferred targets for transmission. The current thesis did not examine transmission from the receiver’s perspective and this would be a useful direction for future research. The pattern of transmission was clearly established in this thesis and it is clear that interpersonal relationships have an important role in urban legend transmission. It is hoped that future research examines further the functions and consequences of urban legend transmission to understand the role of transmission in the social context and ultimately to prevent the negative outcomes that can be associated with constant recirculation of these legends. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First, I would like to acknowledge the ongoing guidance and supervision provided by my principal supervisor Dr Stefano Occhipinti. He has been very supportive throughout my candidature and encouraged me to pursue a topic I feel very passionate about. He has been instrumental in providing assistance in developing my research skills and has provided a constructive environment in which to share thoughts and discuss ideas. My associate supervisor Dr Liz Jones has also provided exceptional support and feedback during my candidature, particularly during the final phase while I was writing the document. Her continued encouragement and sound professional advice have been greatly appreciated. I am also thankful for the support provided by the Griffith Psychological Health Research Centre and fellow PhD students who have been generous with support and advice. Thanks to Lori Leach and Michelle McDowell for their coding expertise and to Andrea Kittila for proof reading the thesis with such attention to detail. Thanks also to Mr Paul Grieve for his assistance throughout my candidature. His support, friendship, and encouragement particularly during the more difficult periods during my candidature were invaluable. Completion of this thesis would not have been possible without the support of my family: Dad, Mum, and Glenn. I am particularly thankful for the love, support, and patience they have continued to provide. I cannot imagine going through this experience without them; their understanding and belief in me have motivated and encouraged me throughout this process. Lastly, thanks also to my partner Glynn, whose love and friendship has provided a strong foundation of support that continues to give me strength. Without these people, completion of my postgraduate studies would not have been possible. v STATEMENT OF ORIGINALITY This work has not been previously submitted for a degree or diploma in any university. To the best of my knowledge and belief, the thesis contains no material previously unpublished or written by another person except where due reference is made in the thesis itself. Shae Rogers. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... i ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................ iv STATEMENT OF ORIGINALITY ................................................................................. v TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................ vi LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................................... xi LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................... xiii
Recommended publications
  • Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 95, No. 1, 212–221 0022-3514/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212 Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience Mark Schaller and Damian R. Murray University of British Columbia Previous research has documented cross-cultural differences in personality traits, but the origins of those differences remain unknown. The authors investigate the possibility that these cultural differences can be traced, in part, to regional differences in the prevalence in infectious diseases. Three specific hypotheses are deduced, predicting negative relationships between disease prevalence and (a) unrestricted sociosex- uality, (b) extraversion, and (c) openness to experience. These hypotheses were tested empirically with methods that employed epidemiological atlases in conjunction with personality data collected from individuals in dozens of countries worldwide. Results were consistent with all three hypotheses: In regions that have historically suffered from high levels of infectious diseases, people report lower mean levels of sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness. Alternative explanations are addressed, and possible underlying mechanisms are discussed. Keywords: culture, disease prevalence, extraversion, openness to experience, sociosexuality People’s personalities differ, and some of that individual vari- For example, Schmitt (2005) and his collaborators in the Inter- ability is geographically clumped. But why is that so? How are we national Sexuality Description Project assessed worldwide vari- to understand the origins of regional differences in personality? A ability in chronic tendencies toward either a “restricted” or “unre- complete response to that question will surely require attention to stricted” sociosexual style.
    [Show full text]
  • Feeling and Decision Making: the Appraisal-Tendency Framework
    Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: Extending the Appraisal-Tendency Framework The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Lerner, Jennifer S., Seunghee Han, and Dacher Keltner. 2007. “Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: Extending the Appraisal- Tendency Framework.” Journal of Consumer Psychology 17 (3) (July): 181–187. doi:10.1016/s1057-7408(07)70027-x. Published Version 10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70027-X Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:37143006 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Feelings and Consumer Decision Making 1 Running head: FEELINGS AND CONSUMER DECISION MAKING Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: The Appraisal-Tendency Framework Seunghee Han, Jennifer S. Lerner Carnegie Mellon University Dacher Keltner University of California, Berkeley Invited article for the Journal of Consumer Psychology Draft Date: January 3rd, 2006 Correspondence Address: Seunghee Han Department of Social and Decision Sciences Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Phone: 412-268-2869, Fax: 412-268-6938 Email: [email protected] Feelings and Consumer Decision Making 2 Abstract This article presents the Appraisal Tendency Framework (ATF) (Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Lerner & Tiedens, 2006) as a basis for predicting the influence of specific emotions on consumer decision making. In particular, the ATF addresses how and why specific emotions carry over from past situations to color future judgments and choices.
    [Show full text]
  • Urban Legends
    Jestice/English 1 Urban Legends An urban legend, urban myth, urban tale, or contemporary legend is a form of modern folklore consisting of stories that may or may not have been believed by their tellers to be true. As with all folklore and mythology, the designation suggests nothing about the story's veracity, but merely that it is in circulation, exhibits variation over time, and carries some significance that motivates the community in preserving and propagating it. Despite its name, an urban legend does not necessarily originate in an urban area. Rather, the term is used to differentiate modern legend from traditional folklore in pre-industrial times. For this reason, sociologists and folklorists prefer the term contemporary legend. Urban legends are sometimes repeated in news stories and, in recent years, distributed by e-mail. People frequently allege that such tales happened to a "friend of a friend"; so often, in fact, that "friend of a friend has become a commonly used term when recounting this type of story. Some urban legends have passed through the years with only minor changes to suit regional variations. One example is the story of a woman killed by spiders nesting in her elaborate hairdo. More recent legends tend to reflect modern circumstances, like the story of people ambushed, anesthetized, and waking up minus one kidney, which was surgically removed for transplantation--"The Kidney Heist." The term “urban legend,” as used by folklorists, has appeared in print since at least 1968. Jan Harold Brunvand, professor of English at the University of Utah, introduced the term to the general public in a series of popular books published beginning in 1981.
    [Show full text]
  • Fact Or Fiction?
    The Ins and Outs of Media Literacy 1 Part 1: Fact or Fiction? Fake News, Alternative Facts, and other False Information By Jeff Rand La Crosse Public Library 2 Goals To give you the knowledge and tools to be a better evaluator of information Make you an agent in the fight against falsehood 3 Ground rules Our focus is knowledge and tools, not individuals You may see words and images that disturb you or do not agree with your point of view No political arguments Agree 4 Historical Context “No one in this world . has ever lost money by underestimating the intelligence of the great masses of plain people.” (H. L. Mencken, September 19, 1926) 5 What is happening now and why 6 Shift from “Old” to “New” Media Business/Professional Individual/Social Newspapers Facebook Magazines Twitter Television Websites/blogs Radio 7 News Platforms 8 Who is your news source? Professional? Personal? Educated Trained Experienced Supervised With a code of ethics https://www.spj.org/ethicscode.asp 9 Social Media & News 10 Facebook & Fake News 11 Veles, Macedonia 12 Filtering Based on: Creates filter bubbles Your location Previous searches Previous clicks Previous purchases Overall popularity 13 Echo chamber effect 14 Repetition theory Coke is the real thing. Coke is the real thing. Coke is the real thing. Coke is the real thing. Coke is the real thing. 15 Our tendencies Filter bubbles: not going outside of your own beliefs Echo chambers: repeating whatever you agree with to the exclusion of everything else Information avoidance: just picking what you agree with and ignoring everything else Satisficing: stopping when the first result agrees with your thinking and not researching further Instant gratification: clicking “Like” and “Share” without thinking (Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Disgust: Evolved Function and Structure
    Psychological Review © 2012 American Psychological Association 2013, Vol. 120, No. 1, 65–84 0033-295X/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030778 Disgust: Evolved Function and Structure Joshua M. Tybur Debra Lieberman VU University Amsterdam University of Miami Robert Kurzban Peter DeScioli University of Pennsylvania Brandeis University Interest in and research on disgust has surged over the past few decades. The field, however, still lacks a coherent theoretical framework for understanding the evolved function or functions of disgust. Here we present such a framework, emphasizing 2 levels of analysis: that of evolved function and that of information processing. Although there is widespread agreement that disgust evolved to motivate the avoidance of contact with disease-causing organisms, there is no consensus about the functions disgust serves when evoked by acts unrelated to pathogen avoidance. Here we suggest that in addition to motivating pathogen avoidance, disgust evolved to regulate decisions in the domains of mate choice and morality. For each proposed evolved function, we posit distinct information processing systems that integrate function-relevant information and account for the trade-offs required of each disgust system. By refocusing the discussion of disgust on computational mechanisms, we recast prior theorizing on disgust into a framework that can generate new lines of empirical and theoretical inquiry. Keywords: disgust, adaptation, evolutionary psychology, emotion, cognition Research concerning disgust has expanded in recent years (Ola- selection pressure driving the evolution of the disgust system, but tunji & Sawchuk, 2005; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 2009), and there has been less precision in identifying the selection pressures contemporary disgust researchers generally agree that an evolu- driving the evolution of disgust systems unrelated to pathogen tionary perspective is necessary for a comprehensive understand- avoidance (e.g., behavior in the sexual and moral domains).
    [Show full text]
  • “La Llorona” As a Liminal Archetypal Monster in Modern Latin American Society
    eTropic 16.1 (2017): ‘Tropical Liminal: Urban Vampires & Other Bloodsucking Monstrosities’ Special Issue | 67 The Role of the Internet in the Endurance of “La Llorona” as a Liminal Archetypal Monster in Modern Latin American Society David Ramírez Plascencia University of Guadalajara-SUV, México Abstract Monsters are liminal beings that not only portray fears, proscriptions and collective norms, they are also embedded with special qualities that scare and, at the same time, captivate people’s inquisitiveness. Monstrosities are present in practically all cultures; they remain alive, being passed from one generation to another, often altering their characteristics over time. Modernity and science have not ended people’s belief in paranormal beings; to the contrary, they are still vivid and fresh, with contemporary societies updating and incorporating them into daily life. This paper analyses one of the most well-known legends of Mexico and Latin America, the ghost of “La Llorona” (the weeping woman). The legend of La Llorona can be traced to pre-Hispanic cultures in Mexico, however, the presence of a phantasmagoric figure chasing strangers in rural and urban places has spread across the continent, from Mexico and Central America, to Latino communities in the United States of America. The study of this liminal creature aims to provide a deep sense of her characteristics – through spaces, qualities and meanings; and to furthermore understand how contemporary societies have adopted and modernised this figure, including through the internet. The paper analyses different versions of the legend shared across online platforms and are analysed using Jeffrey Jerome Cohen’s (1996) theoretical tool described in his work Monster Culture (Seven Theses), which demonstrates La Llorona’s liminal qualities.
    [Show full text]
  • Experiments in Clustering Urban-Legend Texts
    How to Distinguish a Kidney Theft from a Death Car? Experiments in Clustering Urban-Legend Texts Roman Grundkiewicz, Filip Gralinski´ Adam Mickiewicz University Faculty of Mathematics and Computer Science Poznan,´ Poland [email protected], [email protected] Abstract is much easier to tap than the oral one, it becomes feasible to envisage a system for the machine iden- This paper discusses a system for auto- tification and collection of urban-legend texts. In matic clustering of urban-legend texts. Ur- this paper, we discuss the first steps into the cre- ban legend (UL) is a short story set in ation of such a system. We concentrate on the task the present day, believed by its tellers to of clustering of urban legends, trying to reproduce be true and spreading spontaneously from automatically the results of manual categorisation person to person. A corpus of Polish UL of urban-legend texts done by folklorists. texts was collected from message boards In Sec. 2 a corpus of 697 Polish urban-legend and blogs. Each text was manually as- texts is presented. The techniques used in pre- signed to one story type. The aim of processing the corpus texts are discussed in Sec. 3, the presented system is to reconstruct the whereas the clustering process – in Sec. 4. We manual grouping of texts. It turned out that present the clustering experiment in Sec. 5 and the automatic clustering of UL texts is feasible results – in Sec. 6. but it requires techniques different from the ones used for clustering e.g. news ar- 2 Corpus ticles.
    [Show full text]
  • Storytelling
    Please do not remove this page Storytelling Anderson, Katie Elson https://scholarship.libraries.rutgers.edu/discovery/delivery/01RUT_INST:ResearchRepository/12643385580004646?l#13643502170004646 Anderson, K. E. (2010). Storytelling. SAGE. https://doi.org/10.7282/T35T3HSK This work is protected by copyright. You are free to use this resource, with proper attribution, for research and educational purposes. Other uses, such as reproduction or publication, may require the permission of the copyright holder. Downloaded On 2021/09/24 13:02:38 -0400 Chapter 28- 21st Century Anthropology: A Reference Handbook Edited by H. James Birx Storytelling Katie Elson Anderson, Rutgers University. Once upon a time before words were written, before cultures and societies were observed and analyzed there was storytelling. Storytelling has been a part of humanity since people were able to communicate and respond to the basic biological urge to explain, educate and enlighten. Cave drawings, traditional dances, poems, songs, and chants are all examples of early storytelling. Stories pass on historical, cultural, and moral information and provide escape and relief from the everyday struggle to survive. Storytelling takes place in all cultures in a variety of different forms. Studying these forms requires an interdisciplinary approach involving anthropology, psychology, linguistics, history, library science, theater, media studies and other related disciplines. New technologies and new approaches have brought about a renewed interest in the varied aspects and elements of storytelling, broadening our understanding and appreciation of its complexity. What is Storytelling? Defining storytelling is not a simple matter. Scholars from a variety of disciplines, professional and amateur storytellers, and members of the communities where the stories dwell have not come to a consensus on what defines storytelling.
    [Show full text]
  • Urban-Legend-PUPIL.Pdf
    PUPIL Like the wood we use for our furniture, the paper used to print this catalog is FSC certified and comes from responsible forests. OUR BRAND VALUES Urban Legend, a brand by CasaMia Interior Ltd. Born from the will to create accessible Designs that do not compromise innovation, quality and style, “Urban Legend” is a freedom brand first and foremost We create what we like. We believe the best way to keep our innovative and creative spirit alive every day is to love what we do, and to do it with purpose. We constantly re-invent ourselves. Our Designs cannot be classified under one particular style, but rather incorporate different languages to create unique statements. Our Design vision is very focused on what we call our “emotional intelligence”, or our capacity to be moved by a simple detail, a shape or a texture. Our style is minimalist yet sophisticated, warm and “There is an invisible, subconscious authentic yet resolutely contemporary, innova- tive and sometimes conservative. These contra- feeling that connects us to the dictions, we do not fight them, but we integrate things we love. It is this very intimate them fully into our work, and they define the very and ethereal emotion that we strive unique “Urban Legend” signature, between Arti- san well kept knowhow and “modern lifestyles”. to ignite through our products” D.Jamet Head of Design p4 p5 OUR PRODUCTS Our furniture is manufactured in Vietnam, by “work- shop-sized” facturies that are both highly skilled and cost-efficient. We put a high priority to work with people who take pride in what they do, and always / FSC timbers aim at achieving higher goals with us! / Human-scaled 100% of the wood we use in production is imported and comes from sustainable forests.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploding Phones and Dangerous Bananas: Perceived Precision and Believability of Deceptive Messages Found on the Internet
    EXPLODING PHONES AND DANGEROUS BANANAS: PERCEIVED PRECISION AND BELIEVABILITY OF DECEPTIVE MESSAGES FOUND ON THE INTERNET Stefano Grazioli Ronald C. Carrell McIntire School of Commerce McCombs School of Business University of Virginia University of Texas at Austin [email protected] [email protected] Abstract Urban legends are untrue messages that circulate widely in a social environment and that have a wide audience that believes them to be true. We argue that the Internet facilitates the spreading of these deceptive messages and that in some cases this carries with it social and managerial consequences. We contribute to current research on dysfunctional forms of communication by showing that targets of deceptive messages judge lower-precision messages to be untruthful, and that some (but not all) targets use higher precision as a proxy for truth. Overall, these results caution us not to use precision as a proxy for truth when evaluating reports and rumors such as those that flood Internet mailboxes and chat rooms. Introduction Urban legends are narratives that (a) originate and spread anonymously or semi-anonymously, (b) are often found in several versions, (c) have a wide audience, and (d) are believed by many to be true despite the lack of supporting evidence or despite the existence of clear contradictory evidence (Brunvand 2000; Heath, et al 2001). Examples of urban legends include reports of alligators inhabiting city sewer systems, razor blades being placed inside apples, phones that cause explosions, and bacteria-laden bananas. Google and the Open Directory Project,1 as well as dozens of other web sites,2 contain hundreds of examples of such narratives.
    [Show full text]
  • Urban Legend
    Urban legend “Urban tale” redirects here. For the rock band, see Urban 1 Origins and structure Tales. For other uses, see Urban legend (disambiguation). An urban legend, popular legend, urban myth, ur- The term “urban legend,” as used by folklorists, has ap- peared in print since at least 1968.[3] Jan Harold Brun- vand, professor of English at the University of Utah, in- troduced the term to the general public in a series of popular books published beginning in 1981. Brunvand used his collection of legends, The Vanishing Hitchhiker: American Urban Legends & Their Meanings (1981) to make two points: first, that legends and folklore do not occur exclusively in so-called primitive or traditional so- cieties, and second, that one could learn much about ur- ban and modern culture by studying such tales. Many urban legends are framed as complete stories with plot and characters. The compelling appeal of a typical urban legend is its elements of mystery, horror, fear or hu- [4] The "Bunny Man Bridge”, a legend tripping destination. mor. Often they serve as cautionary tales. Some urban legends are morality tales that depict someone, usually a child, acting in a disagreeable manner, only to wind up in trouble, hurt, or dead.[4] ban tale, or contemporary legend is a form of modern folklore consisting of fictional stories, often with macabre elements deeply rooted in local popular culture. These legends can be used for entertainment purposes, as well 2 Propagation and belief as for semi-serious explanations for random events such as disappearances and strange objects. As Jan Brunvand points out[5] antecedent legends includ- Despite its name, an urban legend does not necessarily ing some of the motifs, themes and symbolism of these originate in an urban area.
    [Show full text]
  • Fundamentals of Mathematical Theory of Emotional Robots
    Department of Education and Science, Russian Federation Perm State University FUNDAMENTALS OF MATHEMATICAL THEORY OF EMOTIONAL ROBOTS MONOGRAPH Oleg G. Pensky, Kirill V. Chernikov 2010 Perm, RUSSIA Abstract In this book we introduce a mathematically formalized concept of emotion, robot’s education and other psychological parameters of intelligent robots. We also introduce unitless coefficients characterizing an emotional memory of a robot. Besides, the effect of a robot’s memory upon its emotional behavior is studied, and theorems defining fellowship and conflicts in groups of robots are proved. Also unitless parameters describing emotional states of those groups are introduced, and a rule of making alternative (binary) decisions based on emotional selection is given. We introduce a concept of equivalent educational process for robots and a concept of efficiency coefficient of an educational process, and suggest an algorithm of emotional contacts within a group of robots. And generally, we present and describe a model of a virtual reality with emotional robots. The book is meant for mathematical modeling specialists and emotional robot software developers. Translated from Russian by Julia Yu. Plotnikova © Pensky O.G., Chernikov K.V. 2010 2 CONTENTS Introduction 5 1. Robot’s emotion: definition 7 2. Education of a robot 12 3. Parameters of a group of emotional robots 22 4. Friendship between robots: fellowship (concordance) 24 5. Equivalent educational processes 26 5.1. Mathematical model of equivalent education processes 27 5.2. Alternative to an objective function under coincidence of time steps of real and equivalent education processes 29 5.3. Generalization in case of noncoincidence of time steps of real and equivalent education processes 33 6.
    [Show full text]