Faculty & Research

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Faculty & Research Faculty & Research Me Versus We: Balancing Cooperation and Competition in Groups Through Emotional Algorithms by C. Loch S. Schneider and C. Galunic 2003/74/TM Working Paper Series ME VERSUS WE: BALANCING COOPERATION AND COMPETITION IN GROUPS THROUGH EMOTIONAL ALGORITHMS Christoph H. Loch, INSEAD and HP Labs Susan Schneider, HEC Genève Charles Galunic, INSEAD September 24, 2003 Working Paper Me versus We: Balancing Cooperation and Competition in Groups through Emotional Algorithms This paper examines emotional algorithms and their role in a fundamental dilemma that confronts human groups—whether actors should take care of “me” (compete) or take care of “we” (cooperate). We argue that human emotions, triggered in algorithmic fashion through four common, albeit culturally specified, mechanisms, powerfully direct humans to compete or cooperate. Drawing on evolutionary theory and work within evolutionary psychology, we first identify and characterize these hard-wired emotional algorithms, presenting evidence for their independent existence and influence. Their regulatory influence on human groups, however, can only be appreciated once we examine them as a system. We show how, as a system, these algorithms help explain the dynamic balance that members of human groups can (and often must) achieve between competition and cooperation. We derive three propositions regarding how these algorithms play out in groups and conclude with a discussion of this system’s evolutionary origins. We also suggest that understanding these dynamics can help leaders better manage cooperation and competition in organizational groups. 1. INTRODUCTION: THE FUNDAMENTAL DILEMMA OF SOCIAL GROUPS Most of human history happened in relatively small groups of between 50-150 people (Dunbar 1996a) where individuals were naturally and frequently faced with a fundamental dilemma: the pursuit of self-interest versus the pursuit of group interest. Excessive self-interest could mean a lack of coordinated effort, and so expose the group—and the individual—to greater environmental risks, such as other tribes and non- human predators (Sober and Wilson, 1998). Excessive cooperation, on the other hand— such as taking the vanguard position during battles—could leave individuals exploited by fellow members and faced with lower survival chances. Indeed, this dilemma of cooperation versus competition is timeless to our species and has attracted the attention of various scientists and approaches (e.g., Kramer 1991, Axelrod 1997, Dawes & Messick 2000). For instance, the conflict of “me versus we” is perhaps most famously explored in economics, in the form of the tragedy of the commons—i.e., the failure to maintain a public good because the individual gains the full and immediate benefits of use while sharing the cost of degradation (e.g., pastures, fisheries, pollution, see Hardin 1968). Here, self-interest and (indirect) competition is presumed to trump cooperation, and largely because of the imbalance of incentives facing an actor. In fact, the most common view of this dilemma is to regard it as essentially an interplay of the incentives facing the actor—whether to compete or cooperate is portrayed as an analytical response to the structure of incentives. Notwithstanding the usefulness of these approaches, their adoption of a predominantly rational view of this dilemma is excessively restricted. Whether an actor decides to compete or cooperate is presumed to be a question of reason and cognition, not 1 emotion, and so calculations appear relatively flexible and immediate, effectively uninfluenced by any deeper, latent structures of the human mind, such as our emotional proclivities. Indeed, much of the work on this dilemma within sociology and economics is based on a boundedly-rational decision maker weighing the tradeoffs, short-term and long, between cooperative and competitive behavior (e.g., Axelrod 1997, Frank 1988, Lin 1990, Runciman 1998, Burt 2000). Not surprisingly, much of the spotlight is placed on the logical nature of these tradeoffs, and so the human actor is effectively reduced to the role of a calculator, although, it is acknowledged, an imperfect one. While no one would dispute the importance of cognition to our understanding of this dilemma, we believe there is much more that informs human decision making when it comes to questions of cooperation or competition. For example, while economists have long proposed property rights and incentive systems to resolve the tragedy of the commons (e.g., Ostrom 1990), it has been shown that traditional pastoralists are well able to sustainably manage their commons not with property rights but through norms of mutual reciprocity and what has been called emotional interdependence (Niamir-Fuller 2002). Also, work in social psychology suggests that while competition for shared resources may create conflict within organizational groups, it is highly influenced by categorizations of group membership (i.e. the salience of higher-order commonalities) such that in-group versus out-group delineations skew any incentives that may be present (e.g., Kramer 1991, Dawes & Messick 2000). In other words, emotional responses to social arrangements, operating alongside cognition, have a role to play in the explanation of competitive and cooperative behavior. Moreover, neurologists have shown strong evidence that rational reasoning alone does 2 not enable humans to make good social decisions if divorced from their emotions (e.g., Damasio 1994, LeDoux 1996). And yet it is precisely the role of emotions that has largely been neglected in social dilemmas: It's not that human beings are not rational – we are. The point is that we are not only rational. What makes us human is the addition of a rational mind to a preexisting emotional base (Massey 2002, 2; see also Nowak et al. 2000). That emotions should matter to human decision making and behavior is receiving increasing attention amongst organizational scholars (e.g., Heath, Bell, and Sternberg 2001; Lawler, Thye & Yoon 2000). The role of emotions in cooperative versus competitive behavior, however, has been under-explored. It deserves particular attention because recent research into the constitution of human emotions and their origins in human evolution suggests that certain emotional responses are algorithmic in nature (Le Doux 1996, Damasio 1999) and tightly intertwined with the dilemma of cooperation vs. competition that has been ever present throughout the evolutionary history of our species. The human mind is deeply rooted with emotion-laden algorithms that help direct behavior in the presence of dilemmas in cooperation. For example, the work of neurologist Damasio (1999) is compelling in its depiction of the automaticity and evolutionary functionality of emotions. They are seen as … complicated collections of chemical and neural responses, forming a pattern; all emotions have some kind of regulatory role to play, leading in some way or another to the creation of circumstances advantageous to the organism. (…) Notwithstanding the reality that learning and culture alter the expression of emotions, (…) they are biologically determined processes. (…) The considerable amount of individual variation and the fact that culture plays a role in shaping some inducers does not deny the fundamental stereotypicity, automaticity, and regulatory purpose of the emotions (Damasio 1999: 51). Others have also suggested that emotions operate as an unconscious, “hard-wired” intelligence, which proved—on average—adaptive in the past (Plotkin 1993) and serve a 3 regulatory role. According to this view, emotions are behavioral predispositions that can offer survival advantage—such as the preparation for evasive or aggressive action in a threatening situation (see Griffiths 1997, Damasio 1999). Focusing specifically on competition vs. cooperation, evolutionary theorists argue that the dilemma of taking care of “me versus we” is imprinted on our minds as rules-of- thumb or algorithms that operate through the arousal of emotions and have evolved to become programmed and inherited (Cosmides & Tooby 1992). Of course, these algorithms do not operate by emotion alone, but neither are they simply composed of conscious cognitive analysis—our mental capacities are simply not sufficient (e.g., LeDoux 1996, Damasio 1994 and 1999). These “me versus we” algorithms are triggered by generic social circumstances and, in turn, arouse emotions. As they are not emotions themselves, we will simply refer to them as emotional algorithms. What is important is that certain and specific social circumstances are capable of triggering emotional states which are experienced as ends in themselves, capable of giving satisfaction just through the fulfillment of accompanying behavior—for example, helping a friend in need can be emotionally satisfying in itself, regardless of the favors it engenders. Our basic argument is that these emotion-laden algorithms significantly inform the dilemma of competition versus cooperation and deserve our attention. Relatedly, most theories of emotion address the basic emotions, such as fear, anger, joy, sadness, disgust, guilt, etc., and largely concern the release of these emotions through events that are singularly focused on an individual and the fulfillment of his/her interests, such as his/her success or failure to attain goals (Scherer 1988, Ekman & Friesen 1971; Frijda 1993, LeDoux 1996). They do not, however, target social dilemmas, 4 such as competition versus cooperation. Certainly, emotions are
Recommended publications
  • Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 95, No. 1, 212–221 0022-3514/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212 Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience Mark Schaller and Damian R. Murray University of British Columbia Previous research has documented cross-cultural differences in personality traits, but the origins of those differences remain unknown. The authors investigate the possibility that these cultural differences can be traced, in part, to regional differences in the prevalence in infectious diseases. Three specific hypotheses are deduced, predicting negative relationships between disease prevalence and (a) unrestricted sociosex- uality, (b) extraversion, and (c) openness to experience. These hypotheses were tested empirically with methods that employed epidemiological atlases in conjunction with personality data collected from individuals in dozens of countries worldwide. Results were consistent with all three hypotheses: In regions that have historically suffered from high levels of infectious diseases, people report lower mean levels of sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness. Alternative explanations are addressed, and possible underlying mechanisms are discussed. Keywords: culture, disease prevalence, extraversion, openness to experience, sociosexuality People’s personalities differ, and some of that individual vari- For example, Schmitt (2005) and his collaborators in the Inter- ability is geographically clumped. But why is that so? How are we national Sexuality Description Project assessed worldwide vari- to understand the origins of regional differences in personality? A ability in chronic tendencies toward either a “restricted” or “unre- complete response to that question will surely require attention to stricted” sociosexual style.
    [Show full text]
  • Feeling and Decision Making: the Appraisal-Tendency Framework
    Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: Extending the Appraisal-Tendency Framework The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Lerner, Jennifer S., Seunghee Han, and Dacher Keltner. 2007. “Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: Extending the Appraisal- Tendency Framework.” Journal of Consumer Psychology 17 (3) (July): 181–187. doi:10.1016/s1057-7408(07)70027-x. Published Version 10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70027-X Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:37143006 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Feelings and Consumer Decision Making 1 Running head: FEELINGS AND CONSUMER DECISION MAKING Feelings and Consumer Decision Making: The Appraisal-Tendency Framework Seunghee Han, Jennifer S. Lerner Carnegie Mellon University Dacher Keltner University of California, Berkeley Invited article for the Journal of Consumer Psychology Draft Date: January 3rd, 2006 Correspondence Address: Seunghee Han Department of Social and Decision Sciences Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Phone: 412-268-2869, Fax: 412-268-6938 Email: [email protected] Feelings and Consumer Decision Making 2 Abstract This article presents the Appraisal Tendency Framework (ATF) (Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Lerner & Tiedens, 2006) as a basis for predicting the influence of specific emotions on consumer decision making. In particular, the ATF addresses how and why specific emotions carry over from past situations to color future judgments and choices.
    [Show full text]
  • Disgust: Evolved Function and Structure
    Psychological Review © 2012 American Psychological Association 2013, Vol. 120, No. 1, 65–84 0033-295X/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030778 Disgust: Evolved Function and Structure Joshua M. Tybur Debra Lieberman VU University Amsterdam University of Miami Robert Kurzban Peter DeScioli University of Pennsylvania Brandeis University Interest in and research on disgust has surged over the past few decades. The field, however, still lacks a coherent theoretical framework for understanding the evolved function or functions of disgust. Here we present such a framework, emphasizing 2 levels of analysis: that of evolved function and that of information processing. Although there is widespread agreement that disgust evolved to motivate the avoidance of contact with disease-causing organisms, there is no consensus about the functions disgust serves when evoked by acts unrelated to pathogen avoidance. Here we suggest that in addition to motivating pathogen avoidance, disgust evolved to regulate decisions in the domains of mate choice and morality. For each proposed evolved function, we posit distinct information processing systems that integrate function-relevant information and account for the trade-offs required of each disgust system. By refocusing the discussion of disgust on computational mechanisms, we recast prior theorizing on disgust into a framework that can generate new lines of empirical and theoretical inquiry. Keywords: disgust, adaptation, evolutionary psychology, emotion, cognition Research concerning disgust has expanded in recent years (Ola- selection pressure driving the evolution of the disgust system, but tunji & Sawchuk, 2005; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 2009), and there has been less precision in identifying the selection pressures contemporary disgust researchers generally agree that an evolu- driving the evolution of disgust systems unrelated to pathogen tionary perspective is necessary for a comprehensive understand- avoidance (e.g., behavior in the sexual and moral domains).
    [Show full text]
  • Fundamentals of Mathematical Theory of Emotional Robots
    Department of Education and Science, Russian Federation Perm State University FUNDAMENTALS OF MATHEMATICAL THEORY OF EMOTIONAL ROBOTS MONOGRAPH Oleg G. Pensky, Kirill V. Chernikov 2010 Perm, RUSSIA Abstract In this book we introduce a mathematically formalized concept of emotion, robot’s education and other psychological parameters of intelligent robots. We also introduce unitless coefficients characterizing an emotional memory of a robot. Besides, the effect of a robot’s memory upon its emotional behavior is studied, and theorems defining fellowship and conflicts in groups of robots are proved. Also unitless parameters describing emotional states of those groups are introduced, and a rule of making alternative (binary) decisions based on emotional selection is given. We introduce a concept of equivalent educational process for robots and a concept of efficiency coefficient of an educational process, and suggest an algorithm of emotional contacts within a group of robots. And generally, we present and describe a model of a virtual reality with emotional robots. The book is meant for mathematical modeling specialists and emotional robot software developers. Translated from Russian by Julia Yu. Plotnikova © Pensky O.G., Chernikov K.V. 2010 2 CONTENTS Introduction 5 1. Robot’s emotion: definition 7 2. Education of a robot 12 3. Parameters of a group of emotional robots 22 4. Friendship between robots: fellowship (concordance) 24 5. Equivalent educational processes 26 5.1. Mathematical model of equivalent education processes 27 5.2. Alternative to an objective function under coincidence of time steps of real and equivalent education processes 29 5.3. Generalization in case of noncoincidence of time steps of real and equivalent education processes 33 6.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Article (PDF)
    Journal of Robotics, Networking and Artificial Life, Vol. 2, No. 4 (March 2016), 247-251 Dynamic Behavior Selection Model based on Emotional States for Conbe-I robot Wisanu Jitviriya and Jiraphan Inthiam Computer Science and Systems Engineering, Kyushu Institute of Technology. 680-4, Kawazu, Iizuka, Fukuoka, 820-8502, Japan. [email protected], [email protected] Eiji Hayashi Mechanical Information Science and Technology, Kyushu Institute of Technology. 680-4, Kawazu, Iizuka, Fukuoka, 820-8502, Japan. [email protected] Abstract Currently, the rapid development of non-industrial robots that are designed with artificial intelligence (AI) methods to improve the robotics system is to have them imitate human thinking and behavior. Therefore, our works have focused on studying and investigating the application of brain-inspired technology for developing the conscious behavior robot (Conbe-I). We created the hierarchical structure model, which is called “Consciousness-Based Architecture: CBA” module, but it has limitation in managing and selecting the behavior that only depends on the increase and decrease of the motivation levels. Consequently, in this paper, we would like to introduce the dynamic behavior selection model based on emotional states, which develops by Self-organizing map learning and Markov model in order to define the relationship between the behavioral selection and emotional expression model. We confirm the effectiveness of the proposed system with the experimental results. Keywords: Behavior selection model, Self-organizing map (SOM) learning, Markovian model. group. But the conventional (CBA) model has limitation 1. Introduction in managing and selecting the behavior that only Nowadays, the focus of research of service robots is depends on the increase and decrease of the motivation a development of the robots that are able to express levels.
    [Show full text]
  • Leila Cohn, MA
    Somatic Emotional Selection An Evolutionary Path Leila Cohn, MA Abstract This article presents Stanley Keleman’s Formative Psychology, theory and method, as part of the contemporary paradigm of life as an evolving, self-forming, interconnected system. It emphasizes the contribution of Formative Psychology to the understanding of the human process from a bio-psychological and evolutionary perspective and relates Formative theory and its somatic-emotional methodology to neuroscience research findings involving human consciousness and evolution. The article establishes a correlation between Edelman’s neural selection and the occurrence of somatic emotional selection within the practice of the Formative method and states that Keleman’s somatic emotional methodology stands as an asset to human participation in the process of evolution. Keywords Body – Formative Psychology – Human Evolution – Neural Selection – Somatic Psychotherapy. My first contact with Formative Psychology happened when I read Stanley Keleman’s Emotional Anatomy in 1986. I first met Stanley in 1988 when I moved to the Bay Area with the specific intention of learning about Formative theory and practice. As a result of this move, I entered a whole new life cycle, personally and professionally, that kept me in California for the next six years until I moved back to Brazil to found the Center of Formative Psychology in Rio de Janeiro in 1995. The Center now stands as a growing community that includes a professional team of nine Formative psychotherapists who continue forming and developing the Center. The main impact Stanley Keleman´s Formative Psychology had on me was its evolutionary paradigm (Keleman, 1975, 1979, 1985, 1989). Formative Psychology’s cosmological vision and biological ground provided me with a fulfilling view of the human being as an ever self-forming living system interacting with itself and with the world around it, moving towards its own growth and maturation.
    [Show full text]
  • From Sexuality to Eroticism: the Making of the Human Mind
    Advances in Anthropology, 2016, 6, 11-24 Published Online February 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/aa http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/aa.2016.61002 From Sexuality to Eroticism: The Making of the Human Mind Ferdinand Fellmann1, Rebecca Walsh2 1Department of Philosophy, Chemnitz University of Technology, Chemnitz, Germany 2Department of Philosophy, Münster University, Münster, Germany Received 27 November 2015; accepted 23 February 2016; published 26 February 2016 Copyright © 2016 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Abstract This paper proposes that the human mind in its creativity and emotional self-awareness is the re- sult of the evolutionary transition from sexuality to eroticism. Eroticism is arrived at and defined by the high amount of energy displayed in animal sexuality. We propose that the unique human emotional intelligence is due to this “overflow” of mating energy. What from the survival view- point looks like an enormous waste of time and energy reveals itself to be an unexpected psycho- logical benefit. The diversion of sexual energy from procreation—a process that results in erotic fantasies—turns intimacy into a source of human self-consciousness. This places different empha- sis on the meaning of eroticism and provides a coherent scenario of mental development beyond mere cognitive capacities. Arguments are presented on how erotic imagination, or sexual excita- tion as an end in itself, promotes the human propensity for explorative curiosity; data from ethol- ogy, psychology, sociology, and neuroscience are presented to support these arguments.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Emotion Theory About?
    Emotion Modelling with Human Belief Revision in Computer Games BY Yan Ma A Thesis Submitted to Saint Mary’s University, Halifax, Nova Scotia in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Applied Science March, 2007, Halifax, Nova Scotia Copyright: Yan Ma, 2007 Approved: Dr. Joseph Maclnnes (Supervisor) Approved: Dr. Sageev Oore (Examiner) Dr.Camilla Holmvall (Examiner) Dr.Thomas Trappenberg (Examiner) Date: Dec 19, 2006 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Library and Bibliotheque et Archives Canada Archives Canada Published Heritage Direction du Branch Patrimoine de I'edition 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-29011-8 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-29011-8 NOTICE: AVIS: The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive exclusive license allowing Library permettant a la Bibliotheque et Archives and Archives Canada to reproduce,Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver, publish, archive, preserve, conserve,sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public communicate to the public by par telecommunication ou par I'lnternet, preter, telecommunication or on the Internet,distribuer et vendre des theses partout dans loan, distribute and sell theses le monde, a des fins commerciales ou autres, worldwide, for commercial or non­ sur support microforme, papier, electronique commercial purposes, in microform,et/ou autres formats. paper, electronic and/or any other formats. The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriete du droit d'auteur ownership and moral rights in et des droits moraux qui protege cette these.
    [Show full text]
  • Religion's Evolutionary Landscape
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 713–770 Printed in the United States of America Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion Scott Atran CNRS–Institut Jean Nicod, 75007 Paris, France; and Institute for Social Research–University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1248 [email protected] http://www.institutnicod.org Ara Norenzayan Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4 Canada [email protected] www.psych.ubc.ca/~ara Abstract: Religion is not an evolutionary adaptation per se, but a recurring cultural by-product of the complex evolutionary landscape that sets cognitive, emotional, and material conditions for ordinary human interactions. Religion exploits only ordinary cognitive processes to passionately display costly devotion to counterintuitive worlds governed by supernatural agents. The conceptual founda- tions of religion are intuitively given by task-specific panhuman cognitive domains, including folkmechanics, folkbiology, and folkpsy- chology. Core religious beliefs minimally violate ordinary notions about how the world is, with all of its inescapable problems, thus en- abling people to imagine minimally impossible supernatural worlds that solve existential problems, including death and deception. Here the focus is on folkpsychology and agency. A key feature of the supernatural agent concepts common to all religions is the triggering of an “Innate Releasing Mechanism,” or “agency detector,” whose proper (naturally selected) domain encompasses animate objects rele- vant to hominid survival – such as predators, protectors, and prey – but which actually extends to moving dots on computer screens, voices in wind, and faces on clouds. Folkpsychology also crucially involves metarepresentation, which makes deception possible and threatens any social order.
    [Show full text]
  • Dynamic Emotional Language Adaptation in Multiparty Interactions with Agents
    Dynamic Emotional Language Adaptation in Multiparty Interactions with Agents Bahar Irfan∗ Anika Narayanan James Kennedy [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] CRNS, University of Plymouth Disney Research Los Angeles Disney Research Los Angeles Plymouth, UK Glendale, California, USA Glendale, California, USA ABSTRACT content in responses. However, as research identifies additional In order to achieve more believable interactions with artificial potential areas of deployment in service [41] and entertainment [18] agents, there is a need to produce dialogue that is not only rel- roles, there is a need for agents to deliver responses that are not evant, but also emotionally appropriate and consistent. This paper only correct, but emotionally appropriate. presents a comprehensive system that models the emotional state Emotional language adaptation is of particular relevance in sys- of users and an agent to dynamically adapt dialogue utterance se- tems that consider the development of a dynamic social relationship lection. A Partially Observable Markov Decision Process (POMDP) between an agent and a user in addition to information delivery, with an online solver is used to model user reactions in real-time. such as in storytelling [22], tutoring [1, 2, 12–14, 19, 29], games [40], The model decides the emotional content of the next utterance therapy [9, 20, 36] and personal assistants [26]. The application of based on the rewards from the users and the agent. The previous dynamic emotions and related human-like behaviors including per- approaches are extended through jointly modeling the user and sonality, affective behavior, and memory to an agent’s language agent emotions, maintaining this model over time with a memory, aid in creating an empathetic and believable interaction experi- and enabling interactions with multiple users.
    [Show full text]
  • Durham Research Online
    Durham Research Online Deposited in DRO: 13 June 2019 Version of attached le: Published Version Peer-review status of attached le: Peer-reviewed Citation for published item: Stubberseld, Joseph M. and Dean, Lewis G. and Sheikh, Sana and Laland, Kevin N. and Cross, Catharine P. (2019) 'Social transmission favours the `morally good' over the `merely arousing'.', Palgrave communications., 5 (1). p. 3. Further information on publisher's website: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0269-y Publisher's copyright statement: This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Additional information: Use policy The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that: • a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.
    [Show full text]
  • Society for Judgment and Decision Making, November 24-25, 2002
    Society for Judgment and Decision Making Preliminary Annual Meeting Schedule November 24-25, 2002 Hyatt and Westin Hotels Kansas City, MO Saturday, November 23, 2002 4:30 – 5:30 pm Early Registration (outside of Psychonomics Decision Making Sessions) 5:30 – 7:00 pm Graduate Student Social Event 6:00 – 9:00 pm Board Meeting (Meet in Hyatt hotel lobby at 6:00 p.m.) Sunday, November 24, 2002 7:30 - 11:00 Registration (Westin, outside meeting rooms) 7:30 - 9:00 Continental Breakfast 8:00 - 9:20 Paper Session 1a, 1b, 1c (Westin) 9:25 – 10:45 Paper Session 2a, 2b, 2c (Westin) 10:45- 11:00 Coffee Break 11:00 – 12:20 Paper Session 3a, 3b, 3c (Westin) 12:21 – 1:29 pm ☺ Lunch on your own 1:30 – 2:30 Invited Address by Daniel Ellsberg “The Allais and Ellsberg Paradoxes: 40 Years Later” (Westin) 2:40 – 4:10 Poster Session 1 (Hyatt) 4:20 – 5:40 Paper Session 4a, 4b, 4c (Westin) 5:50 – 7:20 Poster Session 2 (Hyatt) 7:30 on Social Event at Push Monday, November 25, 2002 7:30 - 9:00 Continental Breakfast 8:20 - 9:40 Paper Session 5a, 5b, 5c (Westin) 9:45 –11:20 Business Meeting and Einhorn Young Investigator Award 11:25 - 12:45 Paper Session 6a, 6b (Westin) 12:50 - 2:30 Luncheon Student Poster Award presented by JD Jasper Presidential Address by George Loewenstein 2:45 - 4:05 Paper Session 7a, 7b (Westin) Session 1a What If The Grass Is Greener On The Other Side? The Psychology Of Deferred Decisions Veinott, Elizabeth (University of Michigan); Yates, J.
    [Show full text]