Genetics, Environment, and Behavior Implications for Educational Policy

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Genetics, Environment, and Behavior Implications for Educational Policy Contributors V. ELVING ANDERSON ERNST CASPARI J. C. De FRIES THEODOSIUS DOBZHANSKY BRUCE E. ECKLAND LEE EHRMAN JOHN L. FULLER BENSON E. GINSBURG 1.1. GOTTESMAN LEONARD L. HESTON ARTHUR R. JENSEN L. ERLENMEYER-KIMLING GERALD E. McCLEARN AUBREY MANNING NEWTON E. MORTON ARNO G. MOTULSKY GILBERT S. OMENN P.A.PARSONS SATYA PRAKASH CLAUDINE PETIT WILLIAM S. POLLITZER W. R. THOMPSON E. TOBACH S. G. VANDENBERG PETER L. WORKMAN Genetics, Environment, and Behavior Implications for Educational Policy Edited by LEE EHRMAN Division of Natural Sciences State University of New York College at Purchase Purchase, New York GILBERT S. OMENN Division of Medical Genetics School of Medicine University of Washington Seattle, Washington ERNST CASPARI Department of Biology University of Rochester Rochester, New York ACADEMIC PRESS New York and London COPYRIGHT © 1972, BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Ill Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24/28 Oval Road, London NW1 LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOG CARD NUMBER: 72-84369 PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA List of Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' con• tributors begin. V. ELVING ANDERSON (172), Dight Institute for Human Genetics, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota ERNST CASPARI (1, 215, 307), Department of Biology, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York J. C. De FRIES (5), Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colo• rado, Boulder, Colorado THEODOSIUS DOBZHANSKY (72), Department of Genetics, University of California, Davis, California BRUCE E. ECKLAND (297), Department of Sociology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina LEE EHRMAN (307), Division of Natural Sciences, State University of New York, College at Purchase, Purchase, New York JOHN L. FULLER (17), Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Binghampton, New York XI xii LIST OF CONTRIBUTORS BENSON E. GINSBURG (290), Department of Biobehavioral Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut 1.1. GOTTESMAN (105), Department of Psychology, University of Minne• sota, Minneapolis, Minnesota LEONARD L. HESTON (99, 105, 240), Department of Psychiatry, Uni• versity of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota ARTHUR R. JENSEN (23, 240), Institute for Human Learning, Universi• ty of California, Berkeley, California L. ERLENMEYER-KIMLING (181), New York State Psychiatric Insti• tute, New York, New York GERALD E. McCLEARN (55), Institute for Behavioral Genetics, Univer• sity of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado AUBREY MANNING (48), Department of Zoology, University of Edin• burgh, Edinburgh, Scotland NEWTON E. MORTON (217, 247), Population Genetics Laboratory, University of Hawaii, Honolulu, Hawaii ARNO G. MOTULSKY (129), School of Medicine, University of Wash• ington, Seattle, Washington. GILBERT S. OMENN (129, 307), School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington P. A. PARSONS (75), Department of Genetics and Human Variation, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia SATYA PRAKASH (68), Department of Biology, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York CLAUDINE PETIT (27), Université Paris 7, Faculté des Sciences, Labo- ratorie de Biologie Animale, Paris, France WILLIAM S. POLLITZER (123), Department of Anatomy and Anthro• pology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina W. R. THOMPSON (209), Department of Psychology, Queens University, Kingston, Ontario E. TOB ACH (219), Department of Animal Behavior, American Museum of Natural History, New York, New York S. G. VANDENBERG (273), Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado PETER L. WORKMAN (26, 266), Department of Anthropology, Univer• sity of Massachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts Foreword At the request of the U.S. Office of Education, the National Academy of Sciences (NAS), jointly with the National Academy of Education (NAE) established the Committee on Basic Research in Education (COBRE) in 1968 to support the conduct of research of a fundamental character in edu• cation. This Committee is currently composed of a group of distinguished scien• tists, with Patrick Suppes (Stanford University) as Chairman, and James S. Coleman (The Johns Hopkins University) as Vice Chairman, and includes the following members: John B. Carroll (Educational Testing Service), Ernst W. Caspari (University of Rochester), Bruce K. Eckland (University of North Carolina), Robert M. Gagné (Florida State University), Wayne xiu xiv FOREWORD H. Holtzman (The Hogg Foundation for Mental Health, University of Tex• as), H. Thomas James (The Spencer Foundation), Arthur W. Melton (University of Michigan), Julius B. Richmond, M. D. (Harvard Medical School), A. Kimball Romney (University of California at Irvine), Edgar H. Schein (Massachusetts Institute of Technology), and Theodore W. Schultz (University of Chicago). The program is administered by the Division of Behavioral Sciences of the National Research Council. Henry David, Exec• utive Secretary of the Division, is the Project Director. Also serving on the Committee in earlier years were: R. Taylor Cole (Duke University), Lawrence A. Cremin (Teachers College, Columbia University), John I. Goodlad (University of California at Los Angeles), Louis Hartz (Harvard University), and Fritz Machlup (Princeton Universi- ty). In its first two years of life, COBRE developed a project grant support program in the behavioral sciences in support of basic research on problems relevant to education. The selected projects were funded by the Office of Education. For its third year, a special small grant program directed toward recent doctoral recipients was established. The purposes of the grant pro• gram are to support research which will contribute to fundamental knowl• edge, and will deepen understanding of the critical problems in educational theory, policy and practice. In this effort, the behavioral sciences include anthropology, economics, geography, linguistics, political science, psycholo• gy, and sociology, and also the relevant areas of the biological sciences, en• gineering, history, philosophy, and other sciences. A second phase of third-year activity was a series of eight research work• shops. These workshops were invitational, informal, 15-20 participants and ran from five to ten days. Each workshop was directed by a member of COBRE, and their general goals were to identify significant researchable questions in the area, to define regions or groups of research efforts, and to identify individual scientific contributors. This book is derived from such a workshop held at Wainwright House, Rye, New York in October 1971 under the direction of Ernst W. Caspari and the coordination of Lee Ehrman. The workshop was convened under the title "Genetic Endowment and Environment in the Determination of Be• havior," and concentrated on the contributions of geneticists and psycholo• gists. Specific, advance contributions were commissioned, and Dr. Ehrman prepared a summary and overview of the workshop. This book, with changes and additions, is a derivative of this process. COBRE, the NAS and NAE, and the sponsors, the U. S. Office of Edu• cation, hope that this book will contribute to a richer and sharper develop• ment of genetic and biological research contributions to the problems of ed- FOREWORD XV ucation. We thank the participants and the contributors, the director, the co• ordinator (and the editors) for their efforts. SHERMAN ROSS Executive Secretary Committee on Basic Research In Education Preface From Sunday, October 3, 1971 through Friday, October 8, at Wain- wright House, Rye, New York, with the State University of New York, Col• lege at Purchase, Purchase, New York, as host, a workshop was held on Ge• netic Endowment and Environment in the Determination of Behavior. This was the eighth and final workshop in a series concerned with the social, po• litical, and biological aspects of educational policy. A research workshop on the genetics of behavior and learning cuts across many disciplines. At the very least, those of animal behavior, anthropology, biochemical genetics, cytogenetics, demography, ecology, ethology, evolu• tion, population genetics, psychiatry, psychology, and sociology are inti• mately involved—this is not to omit the new interdisciplinary field of behav- xvii XVlll PREFACE ior genetics itself. This hybrid subject has recently been graced with its own journal, Behavior Genetics. Its editors (Professors S. G. Vandenberg and J. C. DeFries of the University of Colorado) found it necessary to state the following in their introductory address : "It is most clear from recent events that the misunderstandings inherent in the old nature-nurture controversy are not dead and buried, but alive and well. In fact, this topic seems to gen• erate today as much emotional reaction with as little information as in the past. Perhaps when appreciation of the substantive and methodological in• formations of behavior genetics becomes more widespread people will be able to cope more effectively with such issues." It seemed wise, therefore, to make the theme of our workshop, Genetic Endowment and Environment in the Determination of Behavior.1 Our reason for planning this workshop
Recommended publications
  • SOHASKY-DISSERTATION-2017.Pdf (2.074Mb)
    DIFFERENTIAL MINDS: MASS INTELLIGENCE TESTING AND RACE SCIENCE IN THE TWENTIETH CENTURY by Kate E. Sohasky A dissertation submitted to the Johns Hopkins University in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Baltimore, Maryland May 9, 2017 © Kate E. Sohasky All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT Historians have argued that race science and eugenics retreated following their discrediting in the wake of the Second World War. Yet if race science and eugenics disappeared, how does one explain their sudden and unexpected reemergence in the form of the neohereditarian work of Arthur Jensen, Richard Herrnstein, and Charles Murray? This dissertation argues that race science and eugenics did not retreat following their discrediting. Rather, race science and eugenics adapted to changing political and social climes, at times entering into states of latency, throughout the twentieth century. The transnational history of mass intelligence testing in the twentieth century demonstrates the longevity of race science and eugenics long after their discrediting. Indeed, the tropes of race science and eugenics persist today in the modern I.Q. controversy, as the dissertation shows. By examining the history of mass intelligence testing in multiple nations, this dissertation presents narrative of the continuity of race science and eugenics throughout the twentieth century. Dissertation Committee: Advisors: Angus Burgin and Ronald G. Walters Readers: Louis Galambos, Nathaniel Comfort, and Adam Sheingate Alternates: François Furstenberg
    [Show full text]
  • Genes and Development: an Early Chapter in German Developmental Biology
    Int..I. Dey. BioI. 40: 83-87 (] 9%) 83 Genes and development: an early chapter in German developmental biology ULRICH GROSSBACH* Chair of Developmental Biology, Third Department of Zoology- Developmental Biology, University of Gottingen, Germany Gene action and its spatial and temporal control is a crucial time. However, the physical nature of the genes and the mecha- constituent of development. From a modern point of view, it is nisms of gene action were completely unknown. To elucidate therefore surprising that genetic concepts and methods were of such mechanisms in developmental processes was the aim of a no importance in the work of the outstanding German develop. group of biologists who worked at the University of Gbttingen in mental biologists Hans Driesch and Hans Spemann. Both were the decade from 1925 to 1935. apparently not interested in genetics. The dramatic progress in In these years, Gbttingen was a world centre of mathematics. classical genetics cannot have escaped Oriesch's attention, The physics and chemistry departments also belonged to the especially as T.H. Morgan had worked in his laboratory. but it is leading institutions in their field. Biology was very small. There easy to see that it had no place in his "vitalistic" biology. Much were two chairs of botany and one of zoology. Alfred KOhn, a stu- less obvious is that Spemann's critical and open mind should dent of August Weismann, became director of the Zoology have never led him to consider genetic influences in develop- Department in 1920. He started a cooperation with the physicist ment. Theodor Boveri had interpreted the decisive influence of Pohl on color vision in insects but went soon over to develop- cytoplasm onto the fate of the blastomeres in Ascaris develop- ment and genetics.
    [Show full text]
  • Alfred Kuhn, His Work and His Contribution to Molecular Biology
    Int..I. De\'. Bio!' 40: 69-75 (1996) 69 Alfred Kuhn, his work and his contribution to molecular biology ALBRECHT EGELHAAF* Zoologisches Institut der Universitat K61n, K61n, Germany Among the developmental biologists of this century Alfred evolution" (Kuhn 1959) were of great impact for Kuhn. Kuhn is an outstanding personality. Of greatest impact are his Weismann's research projects and objects laid the foundation for studies on the action of genes in development that maps him a his interests in developmental physiology. pioneer of the "one gene-one enzyme" hypothesis. This aspect After his promotion (1908) Kuhn became second assistant of will be in the center of the following report. However, it would be Weismann. Following his habilitation (1910) he worked as unjustified in view of his universality and broad influence not to "Privatdozent" and subsequently became "a.o. Professor" (asso- mention his other scientific interests comprising an astonishing ciate professor) at the Zoological Institute in Freiburg. Research variety of themes and objects. Much of this work can be traced periods in Naples and Heidelberg helped to enlarge his experi~ back to his early period of scientific activity in Freiburg. ences in a variety of projects. During World War I he was a med- Kuhn himself published only a short biographical account on ical orderly (1915-1918). The following two years he cooperated his scientific career until 1937 (Kuhn, 1959). He was born on with the famous embryologist Karl Heider at the Zoological April 22nd, 1885 in Baden Baden (South West Germany), Institute in Berlin. In 1920, Kuhn received the call to become full attended the gymnasium in Freiburg im Breisgau and began professor in Gbttingen.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychometric G: Definition and Substantiation
    Psychometric g: Definition and Substantiation Arthur R. Jensen University of Culifornia, Berkeley The construct known as psychometric g is arguably the most important construct in all of psychology largelybecause of its ubiquitous presence in all tests of mental ability and its wide-ranging predictive validity for a great many socially significant variables, including scholastic performance and intellectual attainments, occupational status, job performance, in- come, law abidingness, and welfare dependency. Even such nonintellec- tual variables as myopia, general health, and longevity, as well as many other physical traits, are positively related to g. Of course, the causal con- nections in the whole nexus of the many diverse phenomena involving the g factor is highly complex. Indeed, g and its ramifications cut across the behavioral sciences-brainphysiology, psychology, sociology-perhaps more than any other scientific construct. THE DOMAIN OF g THEORY It is important to keep in mind the distinction between intelligence and g, as these terms are used here. The psychology of intelligence could, at least in theory, be based on the study of one person,just as Ebbinghaus discov- ered some of the laws of learning and memory in experimentswith N = 1, using himself as his experimental subject. Intelligence is an open-ended category for all those mental processes we view as cognitive, such as stimu- lus apprehension, perception, attention, discrimination, generalization, 39 40 JENSEN learning and learning-set acquisition, short-term and long-term memory, inference, thinking, relation eduction, inductive and deductive reasoning, insight, problem solving, and language. The g factor is something else. It could never have been discovered with N = 1, because it reflects individual di,fferences in performance on tests or tasks that involve anyone or moreof the kinds of processes just referred to as intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • The Biological Laboratory
    LONG ISLAND BIOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION ANNUAL REPORT OF THE BIOLOGICAL LABORATORY COLD SPRING HARBOR LONG ISLAND, NEW YORK 1950 TABLE OF CONTENTS The Long Island Biological Association Officers 5 Board of Direectors 5 Committees 6 Members 7 Report of the Director 11 Reports of Laboratory Staff 20 Report of Summer Investigators 34 Course of Bacteriophages 40 Course on Bacterial Genetics 42 Phage Meeting 44 Nature Study Course 47 Cold Spring Harbor Symposia Publications 49 Laboratory Staff 51 Summer Research Investigators 52 Report of the Secretary, L. I. B. A. 53 Report of the Treasurer, L. I. B. A. 55 THE LONG ISLAND BIOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION President Robert Cushman Murphy Vice-President Secretary Arthur W. Page E. C. Mac Dowell Treasurer Assistant Secretary Grinnell Morris B. P. Kaufmann Director of The Biological Laboratory, M. Demerec BOARD OF DIRECTORS To serve until 1954 Amyas Ames Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y. Robert Chambers Marine Biological Laboratory George W. Corner Carnegie Institution of Washington Th. Dobzhansky Columbia University Ernst Mayr American Museum of Natural History Mrs. Walter H. Page Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y. Willis D. Wood Huntington, N. Y. Toserveuntil 1953 H. A. Abramson Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y. M. Demerec The Biological Laboratory Henry Hicks Westbury, N. Y. Dudley H. Mills Glen Head, N. Y. Stuart Mudd University of Pennsylvania Medical School Robert Cushman Murphy American Museum of Natural History John K. Roosevelt Oyster Bay, N. Y. To serve until 1952 W. H. Cole Rutgers University Mrs. George S. Franklin Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y. E. C. Mac Dowell Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y.
    [Show full text]
  • The Biological Laboratory
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Institutional Repository LONG ISLAND BIOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION ANNUAL REPORT OF THE BIOLOGICAL LABORATORY COLD SPRING HARBOR LONG ISLAND, NEW YORK 1951 LONG ISLAND BIOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION INCORPORATED 1924 ANNUAL REPORT OF THE BIOLOGICAL LABORATORY FOUNDED 1890 SIXTY-SECOND YEAR 1951.1952 The Biological Laboratory was organized in 1890 as a department of the Brooklyn Institute of Arts and Sciences.It was financed and directed by a Board of Managers, consisting mainly of local residents.In 1924 this group incorporated as the Long Island Biological Association and took over the administration of the Laboratory. TABLE OF CONTENTS The Long Island Biological Association Officers 5 Board of Directors 5 Committees 6 Former Officers and Board Members 7 Members 9 Message from the President 13 Report of the Director 15 Reports of Laboratory Staff 24 Reports of Summer Investigators 41 Course of Bacteriophages 47 Course on Bacterial Genetics 48 Phage Meeting 50 Nature Study Course 52 Cold Spring Harbor Symposia Publications 55 Laboratory Staff 57 Summer Research Investigators 58 Report of the Secretary, L. I. B. A. 59 Report of the Treasurer, L. I. B. A. THE LONG ISLAND BIOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION President Amyas Ames Vice-President Assistant Secretary Jane N. Page E. C. Mac Dowell Vice-President El Treasurer Assistant Secretary Grinnell Morris B. P. Kaufmann Director of the Biological Laboratory, M. Demerec BOARD OF DIRECTORS To serve until 1956 Mark H. Adams New York University Crispin Cooke Huntington, N. Y. Mrs. George S.
    [Show full text]
  • An Analysis of Stormfront Discussions on Race
    “So Much for Darwin” An Analysis of Stormfront Discussions on Race Dianne Dentice Stephen F. Austin State University ABSTRACT Even though people who actively participate in the white nationalist movement appear to be a relatively small percentage of the global popula- tion, it appears that racist and anti-Semitic attitudes continue to inform a new generation of white nationalists, many of whom populate discussion forums on Stormfront, online since 1995 and billed as the first Internet site in the hate genre. Membership in extremist groups and support for sites like Stormfront embody specific attitudes about race, the importance of the exis- tence of biological races, intellectual superiority of whites, and justification for these beliefs that is sometimes framed with a religious perspective such as Christian Identity. This project is based on a content analysis of Storm- front discussion forums posted online between the years 2010 and 2017. The findings indicate that some discussants are willing to admit the exis- tence of small percentages of mixed DNA without giving up their white identity. They understand the concept of mass migration and human breed- ing patterns. Others stand ready to argue these points and discredit DNA testing whenever possible. Hatred of Jews is a recurrent theme throughout Stormfront forums, no matter the topic of discussion and both William Pierce and Wesley Swift are gaining currency with younger discussants on the site. Additionally and maybe most importantly, a mentoring system is in place where more mature and seasoned white racialists share race science anecdotes and articles from J. Philippe Rushton and Arthur Jensen, among others.
    [Show full text]
  • IQ Guru Talks to Skeptic Magazine? Given the Chance to Explain How He
    Heredity (2003) 90, 346–347 & 2003 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0018-067X/03 $25.00 www.nature.com/hdy BOOK REVIEW IQ guru talks to Skeptic magazine? welfare state that will long continue to support the Given the chance to explain how he country’s Black underclass. Who were Jensen’s friends in martyrdom and what chose to endure denunciation for was the organization of the London School of which he ‘fascism’, psychologist Arthur Jensen became the revered intellectual head? (The journal holds his peace Intelligence recently devoted a Special Issue to him, calling him ‘A King among Men.’) What was the plan of Intelligence, Race, and Genetics: Conversations campaign for dealing with the rise of political correctness which was making even discussion of race impossible? with Arthur R Jensen Miele’s book has no index allowing tracing, but such F Miele crown princes of the London School as Phil Rushton Westview Press, Oxford. 2002; 236 pp. (who first linked the Mongoloid–Caucasian–Negroid d16.99, hardback. ISBN 0-8133-4008-X continuum to a wide range of psychological features, Reviewed by Chris Brand including sexuality – Rushton, 1985) and Richard Lynn Heredity (2003) 90, 346–347. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800226 (who first noticed evidence that Black African IQ was actually only 70 – Lynn, 1991) certainly do not loom large in Jensen’s ‘conversation.’ Scrupulously aiming to avoid In 1969, Arthur Jensen (today Emeritus Professor at controversy, the man who first so sensationally attracted Berkeley) began his career of martyrdom with his it now disdains any interesting propositions about the Harvard Educational Review article saying that educability, human condition.
    [Show full text]
  • Hans Eysenck's Theory of Intelligence, and What It Reveals About Him
    Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 116–127 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Hans Eysenck's theory of intelligence, and what it reveals about him Linda S. Gottfredson School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA article info abstract Article history: Hans Eysenck was a highly analytical, objective, independent-minded experimentalist. He personified the biolog- Received 18 March 2016 ical perspective of the Galton–Spearman ‘London School of Psychology’, which he led for many decades. His first Accepted 6 April 2016 (1939) and last publications (1998) were on intelligence. Returning to the topic in the 1960s, he formulated, tested, and promulgated the theory that general intelligence (g) is a biological phenomenon with broad social Keywords: consequences. I examine the status of Eysenck's theory, advances in the field, and social reactions to them during Hans J. Eysenck – – London School of Psychology the 1960s 1970s, 1980s 1990s, and since 2000. My perspective is that of a sociologist who, in testing alternative Intelligence theories of social inequality, was drawn inexorably into the intelligence literature, policy debates over fairness in g factor employee selection, and first-hand observation of the sort of controversies he experienced. Eysenck's 1979 and Intelligence theory 1998 textbooks on intelligence mark developments in his theory and supporting evidence during the first two Biological psychology periods. They exhibit considerable knowledge about the philosophy and history of science, and the nature of sci- Scientificcontroversy entific controversy. Advances in intelligence since 2000, in particular, from neuroimaging and molecular genetics, Social inequality vindicate his biological perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • Philosophy of Science and to Transform These Spotlights in Time Inspire Our Future Success and Development
    Table of Contents Overview of the First 40 Years ... 00 • • 00 •••• 00 •• 00 •• 00 00. 2 Annual Lecture Series, 1960-2002 ..................... 6 Visiting Fellows and Scholars Program ........... 14 Lunchtime Colloquium .................................... 17 Conferences and Workshops .. ... .... ................... 18 Public Lecture Series ........................................ 26 Advisory Board .......... .. .... .. .. ............... :... ........ 00 26 Resident Fellows and Associates .. ............... .. ... 27 Center Publications ... ............... .. .. .. .... ... ... ........ 2 8 Archives of Scientific Philosophy in the 20th Century .............................. ............ 30 Major Funding Sources ... ................................. 31 CENTER CHRONOLOGY • In 2001-2002, the Center for Philosophy of Scie nce celebrates 40 years of in· 9/1/60 Acaaemic Vice CHancellor Ctiarles• H. Peak:e appoints Aaolf Grun- novation and accomplishment. The timeline included here highlights many baum as Andrew Mellon Professor of Philosophy with a twin mandate to of the Center's remarkable achievements and most memorable moments. establish a first-class center for philosophy of science and to transform These spotlights in time inspire our future success and development. the Department of Philosof:!hy into a leading department in the country. Andrew Mellon chair in philosophy to an unusually promis­ rated sixd1 in one category and eighth d1e main foci of Griinbaum's administra­ ing young scholar, someone so young that the age d1reshold in a second. In a confidential report tion. He relinquished his adnlinistrative of forty years for the Mellon Professorships had to be waived prepared in August 1965 for the Pitt appointment as Center Director in 1978 in order to secure Griinbaum for the chair. Perhaps no ap­ University Study Committee, Philosophy when he became its first chairman, a posi­ pointment at any university has returned greater dividends was among three departments identi- tion he continues to hold.
    [Show full text]
  • Flynn-2018.Pdf
    Journal of Criminal Justice xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Criminal Justice journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jcrimjus Academic freedom and race: You ought not to believe what you think may be true James R. Flynn Psychology Department, University of Otago, Dunedin 9001, New Zealand ABSTRACT There should be no academic sanctions against those who believe that were environments equalized, genetic differences between black and white Americans would mean that blacks have an IQ deficit. Whether the evi- dence eventually dictates a genetically caused deficit of nil or 5 or 10 or 20 IQ points is irrelevant. The hypothesis is intelligible and subject to scientific investigation. If that is so, you must have already investigated it if you are to know what is true or false. To prohibit others from investigation or publication of their results is to designate certain truths as the property of an elite to be forbidden to anyone else. It is to insulate them from whatever new evidence the scientific method may provide that would modify belief. A word to those who seek respectability by banning race/gene research: how much respectability would you get if your position were stated without equivocation? What if you were to openly say genetic equality between the races may or may not be true; and that is exactly why I forbid it to be investigated. Or: “I do not know if genetic equality is true and do not want anyone else to know.” There should be no academic sanctions against those who believe than white Americans.” First, that it makes a racial distinction and that that were environments equalized, genetic differences between black there are no such things as pure races, that is, there are no groups of and white Americans would mean that blacks have an IQ deficit.
    [Show full text]
  • A Lynx Natural Brain Endocast from Ingarano (Southern Italy; Late Pleistocene): Taphonomic, Morphometric and Phylogenetic Approaches
    Published by Associazione Teriologica Italiana Volume 26 (2): 110–117, 2015 Hystrix, the Italian Journal of Mammalogy Available online at: http://www.italian-journal-of-mammalogy.it/article/view/11465/pdf doi:10.4404/hystrix-26.2-11465 Research Article A lynx natural brain endocast from Ingarano (Southern Italy; Late Pleistocene): Taphonomic, Morphometric and Phylogenetic approaches Dawid A. Iurinoa,b,∗, Antonio Proficob,c, Marco Cherinb,d, Alessio Venezianob,e, Loïc Costeurb,f, Raffaele Sardellaa,b aDipartimento di Scienze della Terra, Sapienza Università di Roma, Piazzale Aldo Moro 5, 00185, Rome, Italy bPaleoFactory, Sapienza Università di Roma, Piazzale Aldo Moro 5, 00185, Rome, Italy cDipartimento di Biologia Ambientale, Sapienza Università di Roma, Piazzale Aldo Moro 5, 00185 Roma, Italy dDipartimento di Fisica e Geologia, Università degli Studi di Perugia, Via Alessandro Pascoli, 06123 Perugia, Italy eSchool of Natural Sciences and Psychology, John Moores University, James Parsons Building, Byrom Street L3 3AF, Liverpool, United Kingdom fNaturhistorisches Museum Basel, Augustinergasse 2, 4001 Basel, Switzerland Keywords: Abstract Natural brain endocast Carnivora A natural brain endocast from the Late Pleistocene site of Ingarano (Apulia, Southern Italy) has Felidae been investigated in detail using CT scanning, image processing techniques and Geometric Morpho- Lynx metrics to obtain information about the taxonomy and taphonomy of the specimen. Based on its paleontology characteristically felid shape, we compared several measurements of the endocast with those of the geometric morphometrics brains of living Felidae, with a special emphasis on Panthera pardus, Lynx lynx and Felis silvestris phylogenetic signal earlier reported from the same locality. The applied combination of techniques revealed that this specimen is morphometrically closest to the brains of lynxes, and so can be reported as the first Article history: natural endocranial cast of Late Pleistocene Lynx sp.
    [Show full text]