Anatomy Was Groomed. During a Focal Animal Sample 1974). Combining the Three Sampling Procedures A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Anatomy Was Groomed. During a Focal Animal Sample 1974). Combining the Three Sampling Procedures A 3 Patterns and Functions of Grooming Behavior among the Common Indian Langur Monkey James J. McKenna For many primate species grooming is a predomin- These data suggest, particularly with respect to the ant and socially important behavior pattern diverse question of functions, that while grooming no doubt both in the form it can take and the social situations serves both social and hygienic functions, it does so in a within which it is initiated. While not the only behavior context apparently dictated -by social needs. It is con- that often cuts across different age and sex classes to cluded that social grooming among langur monkeys is promote cohesion, the tremendous amount of time highly integrated into almost all aspects of group life and energy invested by some species in this activity and, while not a reliable behavioral index to status suggests that as a bond-building mechanism social differentials between group members, it is important grooming has played an auxiliary if not complemen- in assessing role complexes. tary role in the evolution of primate sociality. Among primates social grooming is thought by in- Materials and Methods vestigators to provide useful information on many dif- The study group was housed in a dome-shaped ferent aspects of group life. Grooming data has been mesh enclosure approximately 35 feet high by 45 feet used to measure developmental processes in the in diameter. Starting from the enclosure floor six dif- mother-infant relationship (Hinde 1974, Kaufman ferent levels ofsitting bars and platforms, interspersed and Rosenblum 1969), to document the growing net- every three to five feet, allowed proper social and work of attachments and subgroup formations within spatial refuge. An L-shaped mesh partition support- and among different primate groups (Simonds 1973, ing plywood sitting platforms was situated in the 1974; Furuya 1957; Kummer 1968), and as an exam- center of the enclosure. ple of parental investment (Trivers 1974, Hrdy 1976). According to Jay's (1965) field data, the population While Sade (1965) and Lindberg (1973) demonstrated structure included an adult male (about ten years of how grooming patterns reinforce social relationships age with extracted canines and musculature fully de- and, in some cases, regulate behavior, Chance and veloped); eight adult females (all at least silx years of Jolly (1970) suggested that within private societies the age, primiparous or multiparous); four immature direction of grooming can be used as one criterion to juvenile females (all between the ages of two and four delineate "attention structures" and to assess the de- years of age); four immaturejuvenile males (with mus- gree of social centrality of leader individuals around culature not fully developed and incompletely which others are organized. More recently, Boese erupted canines); and, finally, two dependent infants (1976) used data collected on preferred grooming less than eight months old. clusters to propose that West African baboons (Papio To collect data on the form, frequencies, and dura- papio) exhibit harem units based upon sexual but not tions of behavioral patterns for group members three social exclusivity. different sampling techniques were utilized: focal be- Although an impressive number of works dealing havior samples, focal animal samples, and behavior primarily with the social functions of grooming exist, spot checks. A focal behavior sample involved a fifteen there is not unanimous agreement that grooming be- minute observation period wherein all grooming ac- havior has been interpreted and dealt with correctly, tivities were recorded and notation was made ofwhich nor that its function has been entirely understood. animal initiated the grooming interaction, which other Alexander (1974) argues that not enough attention animal received it, its context, and what part of the has been given to the disease-controlling functions of anatomy was groomed. During a focal animal sample grooming, particularly since grooming may have orig- all behavior exhibited by and toward one individual inally functioned only as a parasite-controlling (the focal animal) during a fifteen minute observation mechanism and not as an accomodation to group liv- period was recorded. The duration of the behavior, ing. To varying degrees Alexander's position receives the actor, the receiver and the participants were noted. support from Hutchins and Barash (1976) and Free- Behavior spot checks involved a random scan and land (1976) who strongly argue that a much closer look periodic survey of the momentary activities of each at the relationship between disease pressures and so- individual at the time of observation (see Altmann cial behavior is warranted. 1974). Combining the three sampling procedures a This paper examines the forms, the contexts, and total of 1500 observation hours were completed. the functions of social grooming behavior among the Tabulation of the data followed the procedure used common Indian langur money (Presbytis entellus). by Lindberg (1973), in which a grooming bout was 4 considered to be any paired occurrence of grooming 60% (n= 1146) classified as relaxed social grooming, involving a recognizable actor and receiver not inter- 4% took the form ofmutual grooming (animal A and B rupted by a third animal or by a major relocation of groom each other simultaneously); 53% involved one of the participants. Reciprocal grooming (animal allo-grooming (animal A grooms animal B); 42% in- A grooms B, then, animal B grooms A) was considered volved reciprocal grooming (animal A grooms animal as two different bouts. If three or more animals B, then, animal B grooms animal A); and, finally, in groomed simultaneously (animal A grooms animal B less than 1% of the relaxed social grooming total the while animal C grooms A), each grooming dyad was form of grooming was unclear, since observation scored separately. If more than ten seconds passed began after the animals had initiated the interaction. between bouts, or it a grooming sequence was termi- Aggressive behavior preceded 63% of tension- nated but begun again after an individual invited a reducing grooming bouts (667 or 35% of all social partner to resume grooming, then this was scored as a grooming bouts observed). Ventral-ventral embracing second grooming bout even though the participants was the second most frequent behavior to precede had previously groomed during the sampling period. tension-reducing grooming with 22% of the total. Sexual solicitations, social mountings, and social pre- Forms of Grooming senting were roughly equal in eliciting grooming re- Although the contexts were varied, three types of sponses (2.5%, 1%, and 4.2% respectively). grooming were recognized for captive Hanuman lan- The third and least frequently observed form of gurs: 1) relaxed social grooming, 2) tension-reducing grooming among captive langurs was self-grooming. grooming, and 3) self-grooming (see table 1). Groom- This form of solitary skin care occurred in 4% of the ing interactions involving more than one animal and total number of grooming behaviors observed. Al- not accompanied by aggressive or tense vocalizations, though spatial and, therefore, social isolation often postures, and/or facial expressions (grimaces) were preceded self-grooming, it was certainly not the only scored as bouts ofrelaxed social grooming. In contrast, condition in which it was initiated. Self-grooming any bout preceded by an embrace, a social present, often occurred between reciprocal grooming sessions aggressive behavior, a social or sexual mount, or a and/or following or preceding allo-grooming by either sexual solicitation involving tense vocalizations and/or one of the participants. facial expressions was scored as a tension-reducing groom. Finally, self-grooming was scored when an in- Frequency and Distribution of Grooming Patterns dividual licked or picked through its own skin, hair, or by Age and Sex Class teeth. Figure 1 shows the percentages by which age and sex classes acted and received Grooming Behavior social grooming (relaxed and tension-reducing grooming combined). Based SOCIAL (96%) SELF (4%) upon 94 fifteen-minute on each of the ____ samples adults RELAXED (60%) TENSION-REDUCING (35%) and immatures (excluding the neonates) and 399 focal la 0 _ behavior hours (1,596 fifteen-minutes observation periods) I found that during any one sample period an average of 3.6 individuals could be found either as - a 0q a<aaa, actors or receivers in grooming interactions. The adult male was involved in more grooming interactions than or A 0~~~j any other individual (331 10% of the total and received approximately twice as much grooming as he returned. a q As an age and sex class the adult females partici- in the actor role more than twice as as a- pated frequently they did in the receiver role (actor to receiver ratio for adult females was 1/2.2). While a t-test comparing the differences between the mean frequency of initiated grooming bouts per animal per class showed that adult females initiated and acted grooming at rates signific- Table 1. Taxonomy of the forms, contexts, and percentages of different grooming behaviors observed among captive Hanuman antly different (p. <01) than either immature males or langurs (P. entellus) females, there is much variation within age and sex classes which deserves attention. When adult females As Table 1 reveals, of the observed 1907 initiated are treated as a class of individuals grouped on the grooming behaviors 60% involved relaxed social basis of age and sex statuses the standard deviation was grooming, 35% involved tension-reducing grooming, 46 and the coefficient of variation was extremely high and 4% involved solitary or self-grooming. Utilizing (21.3). This figure assumes more meaning when it is Sparks (1967) distinctions, I computed that, of the considered that one adult female (Goldie) initiated far 5 time in grooming, males invest little and receive much Ad' A¶? (see Figure 2). 60 I S4 Not only did adult females initiate social grooming 0 more frequently and for longer periods of time, but ; 50 their grooming networks were much more extensive.
Recommended publications
  • Effects of Hierarchical Steepness on Grooming Patterns in Female Tibetan Macaques (Macaca Thibetana)
    fevo-09-631417 February 27, 2021 Time: 15:50 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 04 March 2021 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.631417 Effects of Hierarchical Steepness on Grooming Patterns in Female Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) Dong-Po Xia1,2*†, Xi Wang2,3†, Paul A. Garber4,5, Bing-Hua Sun2,3, Lori K. Sheeran6, Lixing Sun7 and Jin-Hua Li2,3* 1 School of Life Sciences, Anhui University, Hefei, China, 2 International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology, Hefei, China, 3 School of Resources and Environmental Engineering, Anhui University, Hefei, China, 4 Department of Anthropology and Program in Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States, 5 International Centre of Biodiversity and Primate Conservation, Dali University, Dali, China, 6 Department of Anthropology and Museum Studies, Central Washington Edited by: University, Ellensburg, WA, United States, 7 Department of Biological Sciences, Central Washington University, Ellensburg, Lucja A. Fostowicz-Frelik, WA, United States Institute of Paleobiology (PAN), Poland Hierarchical steepness, defined as status asymmetries among conspecifics living in the Reviewed by: Małgorzata Arlet, same group, is not only used as a main characteristic of animal social relationships, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poland but also represents the degree of discrepancy between supply and demand within Jundong Tian, Zhengzhou University, China the framework of biological market theory. During September and December 2011, *Correspondence: we studied hierarchical steepness by comparing variation in grooming patterns in two Dong-Po Xia groups of Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana), a primate species characterized by [email protected] a linear dominance hierarchy.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Food Sharing Behavior and Social Grooming Networks in Desmodus Rotundus with Agent-Based Models
    IN PRESS, UC DAVIS McNAIR SCHOLARS JOURNAL 2019 Evolution of Food Sharing Behavior and Social Grooming Networks in Desmodus rotundus with Agent-Based Models Alery Tenorio† Faculty Mentor: Jeffrey Schank‡ College of Biological Sciences†, College of Letters and Science‡ The common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus, is an example of a non-primate social animal that shares food. Females live in social groups of 8-12, with their offspring and perform social behaviors, such as food sharing and social grooming. Vampire bats share food with other females in their social group by regurgitating their bloodmeals to certain group members. Vampire bats groom certain group members. While social grooming has been studied, its contribution to fitness is unknown. This behavior often occurs before food sharing, possibly to identify cheats, who accept food sharing but do not share. Excluding mother-daughter interactions, social grooming and food sharing usually occurs within subgroups of social groups. These subgroups within social groups are highly associated group members of 2 to 4 and their offspring. While many studies focus on entire social groups or populations, this paper lays the groundwork for investigating whether social grooming is a mechanism for assessing whether bats will share blood meals. The proposed investigation will be conducted by producing agent-based models of vampire bat social groups. These models represent individuals that behave in essential ways like vampire bats and will allow us to characterize by emergent behavior and population-level impacts of behavior. Our goal is to determine whether blood-meal sharing can evolve given or understand of the physiology, behavior, and population structure of vampire bats.
    [Show full text]
  • How Mammals Stay Healthy in Nature: the Evolution of Behaviours to Avoid Rstb.Royalsocietypublishing.Org Parasites and Pathogens
    How mammals stay healthy in nature: the evolution of behaviours to avoid rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org parasites and pathogens Benjamin L. Hart and Lynette A. Hart Review School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA BLH, 0000-0003-2342-6058; LAH, 0000-0002-7133-6535 Cite this article: Hart BL, Hart LA. 2018 How mammals stay healthy in nature: the evolution Mammals live and thrive in environments presenting ongoing threats from of behaviours to avoid parasites and parasites in the form of biting flies, ticks and intestinal worms and from pathogens as wound contaminants and agents of infectious disease. Several pathogens. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373: strategies have evolved that enable animals to deal with parasites and patho- 20170205. gens, including eliminating away from the sleeping–resting areas, use of an http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0205 array of grooming techniques, use of saliva in licking, and consuming med- icinal plant-based compounds. These strategies all are species-specific and Accepted: 2 April 2018 reflect the particular environment that the animal inhabits. This article is part of the Theo Murphy meeting issue ‘Evolution of pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours’. One contribution of 14 to a Theo Murphy meeting issue ‘Evolution of pathogen and parasite avoidance behaviours’. 1. Introduction Subject Areas: From biting flies and ticks, to intestinal worms, to wound contaminants and to behaviour, health and disease and infectious diseases, animals living in nature are in an environment that presents epidemiology, evolution an ongoing threat to health and even survival from the standpoint of parasites and pathogens.
    [Show full text]
  • Population-Specific Social Dynamics in Chimpanzees COLLOQUIUM
    PAPER Population-specific social dynamics in chimpanzees COLLOQUIUM Edwin J. C. van Leeuwena,b,1, Katherine A. Croninc,d, and Daniel B. M. Haune,f,g aSchool of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, KY16 9JP St Andrews, Scotland; bMax Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6500 AH Nijmegen, The Netherlands; cLester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago, IL 60614; dCommittee on Evolutionary Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637; eLeipzig Research Centre for Early Child Development, Faculty of Education, Leipzig University, 04109 Leipzig, Germany; fDepartment for Early Child Development and Culture, Faculty of Education, Leipzig University, 04109 Leipzig, Germany; and gMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Edited by Douglas L. Medin, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, and approved February 27, 2018 (received for review January 2, 2018) Understanding intraspecific variation in sociality is essential for as a pressing question in the study of psychological and behavioral characterizing the flexibility and evolution of social systems, yet its diversity: Beyond isolated accounts of tradition formation in non- study in nonhuman animals is rare. Here, we investigated whether human animals, is there any indication that nonhuman animals chimpanzees exhibit population-level differences in sociality that exhibit intraspecific population-level variation in their everyday so- cannot be easily explained by differences in genetics or ecology. We cial interactions that might be instigated by cultural processes? compared social proximity and grooming tendencies across four One seminal case demonstrating the plausibility of learned, semiwild populations of chimpanzees living in the same ecological population-level differences in sociality was reported by Sapolsky Papio anubis environment over three consecutive years, using both linear mixed and Share (31) in their study of olive baboons ( ).
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Causes And
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Causes and Consequences of Parasitism in the California Fiddler Crab, Uca Crenulata A Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Evolution, Ecology, and Organismal Biology by Adrienne Brooke Mora December 2013 Dissertation Committee: Dr. Marlene Zuk, Chairperson Dr. Daphne Fairbairn Dr. Bradley Mullens Copyright by Adrienne Brooke Mora 2013 The Dissertation of Adrienne Brooke Mora is approved: _______________________________________________ _______________________________________________ _______________________________________________ Committee Chairperson University of California, Riverside ! ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I owe a large debt of gratitude to the many people who contributed to the completion of this dissertation. First and foremost, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Marlene Zuk. Marlene, you always encouraged me to push myself and taught me to develop my own voice and mind. You were also a strong advocate for my research, and with your support I was inspired to reach beyond my comfort zone and take on new challenges that greatly contributed to my intellectual and personal growth. I would also like to thank Dr. Daphne Fairbairn for her help and guidance throughout my graduate career. Daphne, you gave me great insights into the life of an academic, and helped me learn to think critically and set high standards for myself. When my working space was jeopardized, you jumped in and offered your lab space to me. I am forever grateful to you for your support during this time, as I would not have been able to complete my experiments were it not for you. I consider both you and Marlene to be great role models.
    [Show full text]
  • Grooming As an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’S Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur Garnettii)
    The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Master's Theses Spring 5-2017 Grooming as an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’s Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii) Jennie L. Christopher University of Southern Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Comparative Psychology Commons, Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Christopher, Jennie L., "Grooming as an Agonistic Behavior in Garnett’s Small-Eared Bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii)" (2017). Master's Theses. 289. https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses/289 This Masters Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by The Aquila Digital Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of The Aquila Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected]. GROOMING AS AN AGONISTIC BEHAVIOR IN GARNETT’S SMALL-EARED BUSHBABY (OTOLEMUR GARNETTII) by Jennie L. Christopher A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate School and the Department of Psychology at The University of Southern Mississippi in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science Approved: ________________________________________________ Dr. Heidi Lyn, Committee Chair Assistant Professor, Psychology ________________________________________________ Dr. Alen Hajnal, Committee Member Associate Professor, Psychology ________________________________________________ Dr. Richard Mohn,
    [Show full text]
  • Pan Troglodytes)
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Masters Theses Graduate School 12-2005 Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Crystal E. Anderson University of Tennessee - Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Anderson, Crystal E., "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). " Master's Thesis, University of Tennessee, 2005. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes/579 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Crystal E. Anderson entitled "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships of a Captive Group of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)." I have examined the final electronic copy of this thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in Psychology. Richard Saudargas, Major Professor We have read this thesis and recommend its acceptance: Gordon Burghardt, Debora Baldwin Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Crystal Elizabeth Anderson entitled "Exploring Behavior and Social Relationships ofa Captive Group ofChimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)." I have examined the final paper copy ofthis thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree ofMaster ofArts, with a major in Psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Females, Food, Family, and Friendship
    Evolutionary Anthropology 85 NEWS Females, Food, Family, and Friendship or much of the last twenty ances, female philopatry, well-differ- student in the late 1970s, I was ad- years, females have occupied entiated female relationships, and vised by a well-respected primatolo- Fcenter stage in theoretical and male dispersal. On the other hand, gist not to study the sources of varia- empirical analyses of primate social when within-group contest competi- tion in reproductive success among organization. Richard Wrangham1 tion is weak and between-group con- female bonnet macaques because I was one of the first to give females top test competition is strong, we expect wouldn’t find any. But I did, and so billing, when he suggested that social- to see egalitarian dominance relation- have many primatologists since ity has evolved in primates because it ships among females, group-level co- then.10–13 enhances females’ access to resources. alitions against other groups, female If female reproductive success var- Female nutritional needs drive the philopatry, and poorly differentiated ies, then we can ask how the form and plot because female fitness is largely female relationships.4 features of females’ social relation- determined by their access to re- Socioecological models shine the ships contribute to variability in their sources, while male fitness is mainly spotlight on females, and primatolo- fitness.14 Widespread (but not univer- affected by their access to receptive gists have spent much of the last sal) evidence that female dominance females. This means that females dis- twenty years collecting data on female rank influences reproductive perfor- tribute themselves in space according life histories, feeding behavior, and mance provides the first clue that so- to how their food is distributed, and social relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • Coping with Acute Crowding by Cebus Apella
    International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 27, No. 5, October 2006 (C 2006) DOI: 10.1007/s10764-006-9070-z Coping with Acute Crowding by Cebus apella Megan L. van Wolkenten,1,2,3 Jason M. Davis,1 May Lee Gong,1 and Frans B. M. de Waal1,2 Received September 29, 2005; accepted November 16, 2005 Published Online October 18, 2006 Traditionally, scientists believed that crowded housing conditions increased aggression in animals. Recent research on captive primates fails to support a linear relationship between population density and aggressive behavior. Our study is the first to investigate the effects of acute crowding on a New World primate. We observed brown capuchins (Cebus apella) under 2 different spatial conditions: a control that offered the full indoor space in the enclosure and a short-term experimental condition that restricted the capuchins to ap- proximately half the amount of space of the control condition. We compared rates of self-grooming, aggression, contact sitting, social grooming, and play between both conditions. We conducted a mixed factorial ANOVA at the individual level while taking subject sex and age class into account. Intense aggression, play, and social grooming decreased significantly in crowded conditions, suggesting that capuchins avoid social encounters if spatially confined. Their strategy for coping with acute crowding via a decrease in all forms of social behavior is intermediate between chimpanzee and macaque strategies in similar experiments. The only behavior to increase under crowding was a nonsocial, self-directed behavior with the potential of reducing arousal: self-grooming. KEY WORDS: aggression; Cebus apella; coping model; crowding; husbandry.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Parasites on Human Behaviour: an Evolutionary Perspective
    ISSN 2029-8587 PROBLEMS OF PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY Volume 5, 2013 46 THE EFFECTS OF PARASITES ON HUMAN BEHAVIOUR: AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE Pavol Prokop Trnava University, Trnava, Slovakia; Institute of Zoology, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia E-mail: [email protected] Peter Fedor Comenius University, Bratislava, Slovakia E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Parasites and predators possess serious threats to humans throughout our evolutionary history. Although the impact of predators in modern world is lower than previously, parasites still infl uence morbidity and mortality of contemporary humans. From the evolutionary perspective, selective pressures caused by parasites should favour individuals that successfully avoid objects/subjects which transmit parasitic diseases and/or those who are successful in combats with them. There is growing evidence for the existence of evolved anti-parasite behaviours in animals, beginning with invertebrates and ending with primates. Avoidance of infected individu- als, grooming and self-medication are most common behavioural strategies that are believed to reduce the transmission of parasites. Here we review recent evidence suggesting that several behavioural and emotional traits in humans evolved as a response to selective pressure caused by parasites. Disgust sensitivity and patho- gen threat may be a mechanism that generates both a cross-cultural variation in preferences for certain facial traits, as well as drivers of religious diversity, political stability of countries, parenting cognitive abilities and prevalence of extraversion and openness to new ideas. Finally, we declare areas for a future research based on questions guided by the evolutionary perspective. Key words: evolution, human, parasites, psychology. Introduction Archaeological evidence had revealed that organisms in the world change over time.
    [Show full text]
  • Co-Evolution of Neocortex Size, Group Size and Language in Humans
    Co-evolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans http://www.bbsonline.org/documents/a/00/00/05/65/bbs000005... Below is the unedited preprint (not a quotable final draft) of: Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4): 681-735. The final published draft of the target article, commentaries and Author's Response are currently available only in paper. € For information about subscribing or purchasing offprints of the published version, with commentaries and author's response, write to: [email protected] (North America) or [email protected] (All other countries). € CO-EVOLUTION OF NEOCORTEX SIZE, GROUP SIZE AND LANGUAGE IN HUMANS R.I.M. Dunbar Human Evolutionary Biology Research Group Department of Anthropology University College London London WC1E 6BT Keywords Neocortical size, group size, humans, language, Macchiavellian Intelligence Abstract Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and apes. To maintain the stability of the large groups characteristic of humans by grooming alone would place intolerable demands on time budgets. It is suggested that (1) the evolution of large groups in the human lineage depended on the development of a more efficient method for time-sharing the processes of social bonding and that (2) language uniquely fulfills this requirement.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Grooming Used As a Commodity in Wild White-Handed Gibbons, Hylobates Lar?
    Animal Behaviour 82 (2011) 801e809 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Is grooming used as a commodity in wild white-handed gibbons, Hylobates lar? Claudia Barelli a,b,*, Ulrich H. Reichard c,1, Roger Mundry d,2 a Reproductive Biology Unit, German Primate Centre b Sezione di Biodiversità Tropicale, Museo delle Scienze c Department of Anthropology, Southern Illinois University Carbondale d Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology article info Biological market theory is an extension of the idea of reciprocal altruism, as a mechanism to explain fl Article history: altruistic acts between unrelated individuals in a more exible system of exchanging commodities. In Received 13 December 2010 nonhuman primates, social grooming has been used to test predictions in both contexts: reciprocal Initial acceptance 4 February 2011 altruism or biological markets. Specifically, in species in which males cannot coerce females to mate, Final acceptance 1 July 2011 males may attempt to interchange grooming for mating, particularly when females are fertile and mating Available online 17 August 2011 opportunities are more likely to arise. We tested whether wild white-handed gibbon males from Khao MS. number: 10-00867R Yai National Park, Thailand, increased their grooming activity when the female partner was fertile. Adult females and males of our study population are codominant (in terms of aggression), they live in pairs or Keywords: small multimale groups and mate promiscuously. We analysed grooming episodes during 2760 biological market observation-hours in 12 groups separately for females’ reproductive condition (cycling/pregnant/ commodity lactating) based on noninvasive measures of ovarian hormone activity.
    [Show full text]