Pitch and Tone.Pdf

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pitch and Tone.Pdf PITCH AND TONE [This file is accompanied by five zip files that contain the sound recordings on which this analysis is based. These include the plosive codas, the plosive onsets, the fricative and affricate onsets, the nasal onsets, and the approximant onsets] The issue of suprasegmental phonological features such as tonality and stress are contentious in many Tibeto-Burman languages of the Eastern Himalayas. Possible realisations, distinctiveness and perceptions of these features may differ among native speakers, between native speakers and researchers, and among the researchers describing the language at different moments in time. Chinese linguists tend to describe every language as tonal, whereas Indian linguists generally do not mark tonal contrasts, even in languages that are phonologically the same whether spoken at the Tibetan or the Indian side of the border, as the descriptions of Puroik by Sun Hongkai et al. (1991) and Remsangpuia (2008) show. Other examples are the various descriptions of tone in Boro (see for an analysis Burling and Joseph 2010), in Lhasa Tibetan (see for a summary Mazaudon 2014: 590, 609), in rGyalronic languages (e.g. the analysis for Jiǎomùzú and the overview of other rGyalronic varieties in Prins (2011: 51-54)) and in Tshangla. As Morey (2014) mentioned, in many languages of the Eastern Himalayas, pitch and contour (change in pitch) are not the only defining features of tone. Rather, tone consists of a bundle of features that may include phonation (glottalisation and creakiness, breathiness, tenseness and laxness), duration and intensity. Not only Morey’s Tangsa, but also Watter’s Kham (), and other languages of the Eastern Himalayas display exactly this kind of complex interaction between different phonological features. Lowes (2009: ) describes the situation for Kurtoep where voiceless and voiceless aspirated obstruents are followed by the high tone, and voiced aspirated obstruents are followed by the low tone. TONE IN TSHANGLA The absence or presence of tone in Tshangla has been an issue that has divided linguists for a considerable time now. Depending on the origin and background of the speaker and the researcher, Tshangla is described as a language that has no tone, no distinctive tone, distinctive tone only on some onsets, or a language with distinctive high versus low register tone on all syllables. Perhaps the most interesting but unfortunately underrepresented feature of Das Gupta’s 1968 description of Dirang Tshangla is the assertion (1968:14) that ‘tone plays a significant part. As has been observed already, k and g, c and j, t and d, and p and b owe their distinction to their tonal values as well. Vowel lengths are also affected by tones’. He then lists the following examples: khu [rising] dog vs. khu [level] rice, sha [rising] meat vs. sha [level] tooth, ra [rising] brass vs. ra [level] paddy, shi [rising] excreta vs. shi [level] bamboo and nga [rising] fish vs. nga [level] full. Unfortunately, in the remainder of his work no further mention is made of lexical tone. Own research on Dirang Tshangla has thus far not resulted in conclusive evidence for minimal pairs that confirm the distinctiveness of tone in the Dirang Tshangla varieties. The data from Metok presented in Zhāng (1986), Anon. (1991), Sun and colleagues (1981) show a two-tonal (high level versus low rising) contrast on all syllables. In fact, the main difference in consonant inventory between standard Bhutan Tshangla and Pemakö Tshangla stems from the fact that Zhāng does not distinguish the voiced consonants from their unvoiced counterparts. Partially in line with what Das Gupta earlier asserted, Zhāng argues that the voiceless initial consonants are only present in syllables with a high tone, and that voiced initial consonants only occur in syllables with a low tone (Zhāng 1988:4). According to Zhāng, there is a distinct two-tone contrast, namely high-level or high-falling tone, which he represents by the high-level tone ˥ (55) and the low-level or low-rising tone which he represents as the low-rising tone ˨ (13) (Zhāng 1988:10-11). Thus, the voiceless plosives and fricatives /p/, /pʻ/, /t/, /tʻ/, /k/, /kʻ/, /ʔ/, /ts/, /tsʻ /, /tʂ/, /tʂʻ/, /ʨ/, /ʨʻ/, /s/, /ɬ/, /ʂ/ and /h/ and the complex onsets /pr/ and /pʻr/ are only present in the high-tone syllables, whereas the voiced plosives and fricatives /b/, /d/, /g/, /dz/, /dʐ/, /dʑ/ and /z/ and the complex onsets /br/ and /mr/ are only present in low-tone syllables. Zhāng does not present minimal pairs to confirm this theory. In fact, speakers themselves consider tone as non-distinctive, at least in the Didong dialect (Zhāng 1988:10). According to Zhāng, nine consonants can occur in both high-tone as well as low-tone syllables: [m], [n], [ŋ], [ȵ], [w], [l], [r], [j] and [ɕ] because of the change from [ʑ] to [ɕ]. He then continues to list 13 (near-) minimal pairs out of his total of 2128 lexemes. It is important to differentiate between native Tshangla lexemes and nativised borrowings and changed pronunciation as a result of contact with other languages. Thus, ŋa five might be realized as [ŋa˥] in Pemakö Tshangla under the influence of Tibetan high-register [ʼnga] (lnga), and jaŋ wealth, prosperity is a borrowing from Tibetan [ʼyang] (g.yang) through religious concepts such as jaŋ kʰukpe to conduct a ritual to accumulate wealth. Furthermore, in translating from one language to the other one has to be careful not to project semantic meanings onto the target language. For Bhutan Tshangla it was reported by authors such as Hoshi (1987:6), Egli-Roduner (1987), Wangdi (2005:56), van Driem (2001b) and Lowes (2006:94) that Tshangla does not have contrastive tone. This lack of tone distinction is even carried over by Tshangla speakers into other languages they might speak, including Dzongkha and Tibetan. In both languages, native Tshangla speakers do not pronounce the low-high initial tone contrast. This is a very typical feature by which Tshangla speakers of Dzongkha are easily distinguished from native Dzongkha speakers, or even Dzongkha speakers with another Central Bodish or even East Bodish language background, who are able to hear and produce tone differences. Andvik (1999:32, 2003) described Tshangla as contrasting high versus low tone following sonorant consonants for a small subset of words with some dialects currently replacing a contrast in voicing of initial obstruents with a contrast of high/low tone on the following vowel. Grollmann, finally, in the description of Bjokapakha Tshangla stated that “the differentiation of minimal pairs on the basis of register tones in Bjokapakha is, if present at all, only in the process of being built up and by no means an elaborated strategy”, with “not enough homophone lexemes to assume systematic tone differences in Bjokapakha”. As proposed in Bodt and Gyatso (2012) and Bodt (2012), ongoing research on the various Tshangla dialects in Bhutan and elsewhere is pointing to both the existence of distinctive tone in some varieties, the recent loss in other varieties, and ongoing tonogenesis in again other varieties. In Bodt (2012: 200) I argued that the various Tshangla varieties display a shift from distinctive syllable-final consonant clusters (Dirang Tshangla), to distinctive tone (Pemakö Tshangla), to distinctive morphophonological features and homonymy (Bhutan Tshangla). But there appear to be several independent and sometimes contradicting pathways to the development of distinctive lexical tone, including a loss of voicing of plosives, a loss of coda consonant clusters and the influence of coda glottal stops, and the introduction of tonal distinction through loanwords from language where lexical tone or register tone is distinctive, in particular Tibetan and Dzongkha. Tone in Tshangla, though, remains elusive, and unless detailed descriptions of the many Tshangla varieties become available will probably remain elusive in the near future. TONE IN DUHUMBI In order to analyse the absence or presence of tone in Duhumbi, an approach was taken as also advocated by various authors such as Morey (2014) and Mazaudon (2014). First, tonal contrasts were identified, then, the phonetic details of these contrasts were explored. The identification of tonal contrasts was through a combination of identifying minimal and near-minimal pairs. These minimal and near-minimal pairs were then discussed in detail with the main consultant, in which the core phrases became khengdeq [kʰʲɛŋdḛʔ] ‘it pulls’ and khengbang [kʰɛŋbaŋ] ‘it doesn’t pull’ or, in frequent code-shifting to Dirang Tshangla, the cognates jangkla [ʥaŋkla] and jangkmala [ʥaŋkmala] or in a curious creole of both as jangteq [ʥaŋtḛʔ] and jangpang [ʥaŋpaŋ]. ‘Pulling’ was the way in which the informant expressed what she described as a need to ‘pull up’ a consonant from somewhere, rather than simply letting it flow from the point of articulation outward. In the end, perhaps, simply accepting this extremely clever laywoman’s view of the issue turned out to quite neatly pin-point at the core of the suprasegmental phonological feature of tone. PLOSIVE CODAS AND PITCH CONTRAST The first general observation that can be made is that syllables ending in rhymes with a plosive consonant /k, t, p/ as well as the constricted vowel rhymes /-aq, -eq, -oq, -uq/ are almost unequivocally realised with a rising-falling pitch by all speakers. This observation implies that the rising-falling pitch pattern is a co-articulated, inherent feature of closed rhymes ending on the plosive consonants /k, t, p/ or a glottal stop/glottal constricted vowel. Characteristically, the onset of the syllable does not influence the pitch pattern: a rising-falling pitch pattern has been attested following all the permitted onsets and onset clusters, and there are no minimal pairs in which both onset and rhyme are the same and only the pitch pattern is distinct.
Recommended publications
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Phonetics Interact with Phonology During Tone Sandhi?
    How does phonetics interact with phonology during tone sandhi? Bijun Ling Tongji University [email protected] ABSTRACT whether the phonological system affects the phonetic interaction of consonant and f0. This paper investigated the phonetics and phonology Shanghai Wu, a northern Wu dialect of Chinese, of consonant–f0 interaction in Shanghai Wu. Bi- offers a good study case for this research question. syllabic compound nouns, which form tone sandhi Shanghai Wu has five lexical tones, which can be domain, were elicited within template sentences with described by three features [27]: F0 contour: falling two factors controlled: lexical tones (T1[HM], (T1) and rising (T2-T5); Tonal register: high (T1, T2, T3[LM], T5[LMq]) and consonant types (obstruents T4) and low (T3, T5); and Duration: long (T1-T3) and & nasals). Results showed that although the base tone short (T4, T5). They exhibit interesting co-occurrence contrast of the second syllable is neutralized by patterns with both the onset and coda of the tone- phonological tone sandhi rules, the onset f0 of the bearing syllable. Syllables with voiceless onsets only second syllable with low tones (T3) is significantly allow tones that start in the high register, i.e. T1, T2 lower than that with high tone (T1). Furthermore, and T4; while voiced onsets co-occur with tones that such difference cannot be just attributed to the start in the low register, i.e. T3 and T5. Interestingly, consonant perturbation, because it also exists when the sonorant consonants could occur with both high the consonant (i.e. /m/) is the same for all three tones.
    [Show full text]
  • ABC+Q: Contour Segments and Tones in (Sub)Segmental Agreement by Correspondence
    Inkelas & Shih 25 May 2013 ABC+Q: Contour segments and tones in (sub)segmental Agreement by Correspondence Sharon Inkelas (UC Berkeley) & Stephanie S Shih (Stanford/UC Berkeley) [email protected], [email protected] 0 INTRODUCTION Phonological theory has long been challenged by the behavior of contour segments and contour tones in harmony patterns. (1) Contour segments: segments with distinct gestures sequenced in time (Sagey 1986; et seq.) a. pre- and post-nasalized segments (e.g., nd, dn) b. affricates (e.g., ʧ) c. pre- or post-laryngealized segments (e.g., kʰ, ʰk) d. contour tones (e.g., ǎ, â, ǎ` ) e. diphthongs (e.g., aɪ) f. on- and off-glides (e.g., ju, kʷ) (a), (c), see Steriade 1993; 1994; (b), see Lombardi 1990; see also Banner-Inouye 1989 on flaps (2) Not covered in this talk: complex segments involving distinct gestures which overlap in time a. labiovelars, /l/ b. partially laryngealized vowels c. clicks, implosives, ejectives (3) Conflicting behavior by contour segments in harmony patterns: participate in phonology as whole units their subsegmental parts act independently (4) This talk: ABC+Q Propose new subdivided, quantized segmental representations, which allow for a better characterization of the behavior of contour and simplex segments. Quantized segmental subdivisions are grounded in phonetic reality and tuned to typological phonological generalizations incorporated in Agreement by Correspondence (ABC) theory (Hansson 2001, Rose & Walker 2004). ABC+Q marries the representational strength of previous approaches, including Autosegmental Theory, Aperture Theory, and Articulatory Phonology, with the locality and similarity design features of ABC, to capture the unity and internal complexity of segments.
    [Show full text]
  • Consonant Types and F0 in Kiowa
    UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2011) CONSONANT TYPES AND F0 IN KIOWA ANNA JURGENSEN UNIVERSITY OF CALIFONRIA, BERKELEY 62 UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2011) Abstract Since the work of House and Fairbanks (1953) and Lehiste and Peterson (1961) it has been established that the prevocalic voiced stops produce downward perturbations and prevocalic voiceless stops produce upward perturbations on the fundamental frequency (F0) of the following vowel. The effects of stops with other laryngeal settings, such as aspirated and ejective, are not as clear (Hombert 1978). Additionally, the effects on F0 in tonal languages may differ from those in nontonal languages, both in the magnitude of the effect on vowels of different tone and in the duration of the effect in general (Gandour 1974). Using pre-recorded elicitations in Kiowa, a tonal, Kiowa-Tanoan language of southwestern Oklahoma, the effects of prevocalic voiced, voiceless unaspirated, voiceless aspirated, and ejective stops on the F0 of the following vowel were measured and analyzed. Using WaveSurfer, the F0 of the vowel was measured every 20 ms for a duration of 100 ms. The measurements were categorically averaged for comparison between types of stop and tone level. The analysis shows that voiced and voiceless stops in Kiowa produce the predicted downward and upward perturbations, and provides evidence that aspirated stops produce a higher perturbation on vowels than do voiceless unaspirated, and that ejective stops produce a downward perturbation on F0 instead of a neutral effect, as has been previously assumed (Hombert 1978). 63 UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2011) It has been well established that the laryngeal setting of a prevocalic stop has an intrinsic effect on the fundamental frequency (F0) of the vowel that follows.
    [Show full text]
  • On Basque Affricates
    On Basque Affricates JOSE IGNACIO HUALDE University of Southern California 1. Introduction * The representation of affricates and other non-steady-state segments posed se­ rious problems to linear phonological theories. Hierarchical models such as the one introduced in elements (1985) offer the possibility of representations that capture the behavior of these segments in a much more conspicuous way than in earlier models. Working within a hierarchical theory of phonological representations, Sagey (1986) proposes that affricate segments differ from simple segments in that they contain two ordered features [-cont] [+ cant]. Given this representation, Sagey makes the prediction that affricates will behave as stops for rules involving their left edge, but as fricatives for rules sensitive to their right edge. Sagey pre­ sents data from a number of languages which offer support for this prediction. In this paper, I will argue, basing myself on Basque data, that, whereas a representation of affricates as segments containing two ordered features [-cont] [+ cont] is essentially correct, it does not necessarily follow--· from that represen­ tation that these segments-'wilI always show edge effects'. As I will show, affricates in Basque are treate~ as stops with respect to a deletion rule involving their right side, but sensitive to -the feature [~cont]. I will argUe for a representation of affricates as segments fontaining two 'ordered supralaryngeal nodes. 2. Basque Stop Deleti()n In Basque, sequences of two [-cont] segments are not allowed if the first of the two adjacent segments also has the feature [-son]. There is a rule which simplifies these sequences, affecting the first of the two stops.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Duration and Sonority on Contour Tone Distribution— Typological Survey and Formal Analysis
    The Effects of Duration and Sonority on Contour Tone Distribution— Typological Survey and Formal Analysis Jie Zhang For my family Table of Contents Acknowledgments xi 1 Background 3 1.1 Two Examples of Contour Tone Distribution 3 1.1.1 Contour Tones on Long Vowels Only 3 1.1.2 Contour Tones on Stressed Syllables Only 8 1.2 Questions Raised by the Examples 9 1.3 How This Work Evaluates The Different Predictions 11 1.3.1 A Survey of Contour Tone Distribution 11 1.3.2 Instrumental Case Studies 11 1.4 Putting Contour Tone Distribution in a Bigger Picture 13 1.4.1 Phonetically-Driven Phonology 13 1.4.2 Positional Prominence 14 1.4.3 Competing Approaches to Positional Prominence 16 1.5 Outline 20 2 The Phonetics of Contour Tones 23 2.1 Overview 23 2.2 The Importance of Sonority for Contour Tone Bearing 23 2.3 The Importance of Duration for Contour Tone Bearing 24 2.4 The Irrelevance of Onsets to Contour Tone Bearing 26 2.5 Local Conclusion 27 3 Empirical Predictions of Different Approaches 29 3.1 Overview 29 3.2 Defining CCONTOUR and Tonal Complexity 29 3.3 Phonological Factors That Influence Duration and Sonority of the Rime 32 3.4 Predictions of Contour Tone Distribution by Different Approaches 34 3.4.1 The Direct Approach 34 3.4.2 Contrast-Specific Positional Markedness 38 3.4.3 General-Purpose Positional Markedness 41 vii viii Table of Contents 3.4.4 The Moraic Approach 42 3.5 Local Conclusion 43 4 The Role of Contrast-Specific Phonetics in Contour Tone Distribution: A Survey 45 4.1 Overview of the Survey 45 4.2 Segmental Composition 48
    [Show full text]
  • Looking Into Segments Sharon Inkelas and Stephanie S Shih University of California, Berkeley & University of California, Merced
    Looking into Segments Sharon Inkelas and Stephanie S Shih University of California, Berkeley & University of California, Merced 1 Introduction It is well-known that the phonological ‘segment’ (consonant, vowel) is internally dynamic. Complex segments, such as affricates or prenasalized stops, have sequenced internal phases; coarticulation induces change over time even within apparently uniform segments. Autosegmental Phonology (e.g., Goldsmith 1976, Sagey 1986) captured the internal phasing of complex segments using feature values ordered sequentially on a given tier and ‘linked’ to the same timing unit. Articulatory Phonology (e.g., Browman & Goldstein 1992, Gafos 2002, Goldstein et al. 2009) captures internal phases through the use of coordinated gestures, which overlap in time with one another but are aligned to temporal landmarks. Segments can be internally dynamic in a contrastive way. Affricates differ from plain stops or plain fricatives in being sequentially complex; the same difference obtains between prenasalized vs. plain stops, between contour and level tones, and so forth. Segments can also be dynamic in a noncontrastive way, due to coarticulation with surrounding segments. To the extent that phonological patterns are sensitive to contrastive or noncontrastive segment-internal phasing, the phasing needs to be represented in a manner that is legible to phonological grammar. However, contemporary phonological analysis couched in Optimality Theory, Harmonic Grammar and similar approaches is very highly segment-oriented. For example, Agreement by Correspondence theory, or ABC (Hansson 2001, 2010; Rose & Walker 2004; Bennett 2013; inter alia) and other surface correspondence theories of harmony and disharmony are theories of segmental correspondence. The constraints in these theories refer to segments as featurally uniform units, and do not have a way of referencing their internal phases.
    [Show full text]
  • Contour Tone Distribution in Luganda
    Contour Tone Distribution in Luganda Katharine Dutcher and Mary Paster Pomona College 1. Introduction The traditional model of contour tone distribution (see, e.g., Hyman 2003) uses phonological units to describe which syllable types can bear rising or falling tones (see Zhang 2004 for an overview). A common pattern is one in which contour tones are permitted on long vowels but not on short vowels; this is explained in the traditional model by assuming that the mora is the tone-bearing unit (TBU) and there is a one-tone-per-mora restriction that prohibits contour tones on short vowels. An alternative to the phonological approach is one in which contour tone distribution is phonetically based (see, e.g., Gordon 2001, 2006, Zhang 2001, 2004). In the phonetic model, there is no role for the mora in determining contour tone distribution; instead, it is duration and/or sonority that determine whether a contour can occur on a given syllable. In this paper, we show how Luganda bears on the choice between the two models, favoring the phonological approach over the phonetic one. As we will show, the mora is crucial to explaining contour tone distribution in Luganda, and the duration-based phonetic model makes incorrect predictions for this language. 2. Two competing approaches In the phonetic approach proposed by Zhang (2001), contour tones need a minimum sonorous rime duration (SRD) in order to be realized on a given syllable. SRD is defined as the duration of the syllable nucleus plus any sonorant consonant(s) in the coda, but crucially not including any obstruent coda consonants.
    [Show full text]
  • Acoustic Phonetics Lab Manual
    LING 380: Acoustic Phonetics Lab Manual Sonya Bird Qian Wang Sky Onosson Allison Benner Department of Linguistics University of Victoria INTRODUCTION This lab manual is designed to be used in the context of an introductory course in Acoustic Phonetics. It is based on the software Praat, created by Paul Boersma and David Weenink (www.praat.org), and covers measurement techniques useful for basic acoustic analysis of speech. LAB 1 is an introduction to Praat: the layout of various displays and the basic functions. LAB 2 focuses on voice onset time (VOT), an important cue in identifying stop consonants. The next several labs guide students through taking acoustic measurements typically relevant for vowels (LAB 3 and LAB 4), obstruents (LAB 4) and sonorants (LAB 6 and LAB 7). LAB 8 discusses ways of measuring phonation. LAB 9 focuses on stress, pitch-accent, and tone. LAB 9 and LAB 10 bring together the material from previous labs in an exploration of cross-dialectal (LAB 10) and cross-linguistic (LAB 9) differences in speech. Finally, LAB 11 introduces speech manipulation and synthesis techniques. Each lab includes a list of sound files for you to record, and on which you will run your analysis. If you are unable to generate good-quality recordings, you may also access pre-recorded files. Each lab also includes a set of questions designed to ensure that you – the user – understand the measurements taken as well as the values obtained for these measurements. These questions are repeated in the report at the end of the lab, which can serve as a way of recapitulating the content of the lab.
    [Show full text]
  • Uu Clicks Correlate with Phonotactic Patterns Amanda L. Miller
    Tongue body and tongue root shape differences in N|uu clicks correlate with phonotactic patterns Amanda L. Miller 1. Introduction There is a phonological constraint, known as the Back Vowel Constraint (BVC), found in most Khoesan languages, which provides information as to the phonological patterning of clicks. BVC patterns found in N|uu, the last remaining member of the !Ui branch of the Tuu family spoken in South Africa, have never been described, as the language only had very preliminary documentation undertaken by Doke (1936) and Westphal (1953–1957). In this paper, I provide a description of the BVC in N|uu, based on lexico-statistical patterns found in a database that I developed. I also provide results of an ultrasound study designed to investigate posterior place of articulation differences among clicks. Click consonants have two constrictions, one anterior, and one posterior. Thus, they have two places of articulation. Phoneticians since Doke (1923) and Beach (1938) have described the posterior place of articulation of plain clicks as velar, and the airstream involved in their production as velaric. Thus, the anterior place of articulation was thought to be the only phonetic property that differed among the various clicks. The ultrasound results reported here and in Miller, Brugman et al. (2009) show that there are differences in the posterior constrictions as well. Namely, tongue body and tongue root shape differences are found among clicks. I propose that differences in tongue body and tongue root shape may be the phonetic bases of the BVC. The airstream involved in click production is described as velaric airstream by earlier researchers.
    [Show full text]
  • UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics
    UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics Title WPP, No. 107: Acoustic Study of Georgian Stop Consonants Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/63t1324h Author Vicenik, Chad Publication Date 2008-09-30 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, No. 107, pp.1-30 An Acoustic Study of Georgian Stop Consonants Chad Vicenik [email protected] Abstract This study investigates the acoustic properties of ejective, voiced and voiceless aspirated stops in Georgian, a Caucasian language, and seeks to answer two questions: (1) which acoustic features discriminate the three stop manners and (2) do Georgian stops undergo initial strengthening, and if so, is it syntagmatic or paradigmatic strengthening? Five female speakers were recorded reading words embedded into carrier phrases and stories. Acoustic measures include closure duration, voicing during the closure, voicing lag, relative burst intensity, spectral moment of bursts, phonation (H1-H2) and f0. Of these, voicing lag, voicing during the closure, mean burst frequency, H1-H2 and f0 could all be used to discriminate stop manner, but stop manners did not differ in closure duration or relative burst intensity. Georgian stops did show initial strengthening and showed only syntagmatic enhancement, not paradigmatic enhancement. Stops showed longer closure durations, longer voicing lags, less voicing during the closure and higher H1-H2 values in higherprosodicpositions. 1. Introduction Georgian, a Caucasian language spoken in Georgia, has three stop manners: voiceless aspirated, voiced and ejective (Shosted & Chikovani2006). Its stop inventory is given below in Table 1. This study examines the stop consonants of Georgian and will look at a number of acoustic measures in order to describe the similarities and differences between ejectives and the other stop manners present in the language.
    [Show full text]
  • High Tone Lowering and Raising in Tsua
    Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus, Vol. 54, 2018, 1-16 doi: 10.5842/54-0-772 High tone lowering and raising in Tsua Timothy K. Mathes Department of Linguistics, New York University, USA E-mail: [email protected] Andy Monthusi Chebanne Faculty of Humanities, University of Botswana, Botswana E-mail: [email protected] Abstract The Tsua language is an Eastern Kalahari Khoe language of Botswana (Chebanne 2014). Tsua tone production displays complex Fundamental Frequency (F0) trajectories. Lexical data show that this language has three tone levels: High (H), Mid (M), and Low (L), with the following surface tone melodies: [HH, HM, HL, MM, MH, ML]. High tones may be lowered when they occur following voiced obstruents, aspirated obstruents or the glottal fricative /h/ in root-initial position, a typologically rare pattern. This results in two depressed melodies: depressed HM [DH-M] and depressed HL [DH-L]. Both clicks and non-clicks participate in this interaction. We refer to this tonal depression pattern as High Tone Lowering (HTL). HTL may be formally accounted for via the Low tone insertion rule: → L / [-sonorant, +slack] ___ H [-H] #. Recent analysis suggests that Super High (SH) tones are derived from /H/ when docked to the high vowels [i], [u] and are not phonemic. For example,∅ underlying /HL/ tű ù “to collect and remove ash” is produced with SH-L tones on the surface. We refer to this as High Tone Raising (HTR). Elderkin (1988) reports a similar finding in Ju|ʼhoan in which a sequence of successive extra- high tones is only found in click-initial morphs when the final vowel is [i] or [u].
    [Show full text]